Basal ganglia output reflects internally-specified movements
Abstract
How movements are selected is a fundamental question in systems neuroscience. While many studies have elucidated the sensorimotor transformations underlying stimulus-guided movements, less is known about how internal goals – critical drivers of goal-directed behavior – guide movements. The basal ganglia are known to bias movement selection according to value, one form of internal goal. Here, we examine whether other internal goals, in addition to value, also influence movements via the basal ganglia. We designed a novel task for mice that dissociated equally rewarded internally-specified and stimulus-guided movements, allowing us to test how each engaged the basal ganglia. We found that activity in the substantia nigra pars reticulata, a basal ganglia output, predictably differed preceding internally-specified and stimulus-guided movements. Incorporating these results into a simple model suggests that internally-specified movements may be facilitated relative to stimulus-guided movements by basal ganglia processing.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.13833.001eLife digest
An important role of the nervous system is to allow us to move around in the world. These movements are typically influenced by the goal that we want to achieve (for example, finding food) as well as stimuli that we sense in our environment (for example, the smell of pizza). Yet we understand little about how the brain controls these sorts of goal-directed movements, even under normal conditions. This lack of basic understanding presents a big problem when it comes to treating movement disorders like Parkinson’s disease.
For a long time, a collection of brain regions called the basal ganglia have been known to be important for controlling movements, although the specific role that they play in this process is not well understood. Does the brain activity that controls movements differ depending on whether the movement is made in response to a stimulus or not?
Using mice, Lintz and Felsen have now recorded the activity of individual neurons in the basal ganglia that signal to other brain regions as the animals performed a behavioral task. Different trials in the task required the mouse to make two types of otherwise-identical movements: movements based on a stimulus, and movements based on recent experiences (and not triggered by a stimulus). The output activity of the basal ganglia differed under these two conditions, suggesting that the basal ganglia may play different roles in each type of movement.
From the results, Lintz and Felsen could make some predictions about how the basal ganglia influence the activity of downstream regions of the nervous system that control movement. Further studies are now required to test these predictions.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.13833.002Introduction
As we interact with the world, our movements are selected based on external sensory stimuli and internal variables representing action value, learned stimulus-response contingencies, and prior experiences (Gold and Shadlen, 2007). Selecting the movement associated with the most desirable outcome requires appropriately weighting each of these factors. While the neural substrates for movements based on external sensory stimuli have been the focus of much research (Hall and Moschovakis, 2003), where, how, and when internal goals influence movement selection is less well understood. The basal ganglia (BG) are known to be involved in motor control (Hikosaka and Wurtz, 1989; Mink, 1996), contributing to movement selection by modulating inhibition on competing downstream motor structures (Basso and Wurtz, 2002; Di Chiara et al., 1979; Hikosaka and Wurtz, 1985). In particular, the BG have been thought to bias the selection of movements towards those associated with the highest value (Hikosaka et al., 2006). This 'value-biasing' hypothesis is supported by much evidence showing that activity in several BG nuclei is modulated, prior to stimulus presentation, by reward expectation (Bryden et al., 2011; Handel and Glimcher, 2000; Hikosaka et al., 2006; Kawagoe et al., 1998; Sato and Hikosaka, 2002) such that movements toward high-value targets are disinhibited relative to movements toward low-value targets (Hikosaka et al., 2006; Lauwereyns et al., 2002). Anatomical evidence is consistent with a primary role for the BG in mediating the integration of value-based information into motor plans (Bolam et al., 2000; Gerfen and Surmeier, 2011). However, movement selection may also be guided by other internal representations, such as recent movements and their outcomes (Corrado et al., 2005; Fecteau and Munoz, 2003; Lau and Glimcher, 2005). We therefore asked whether BG activity mediates the influence of internal goals, in addition to value, on movement selection.
We reasoned that, if this were the case, BG output would differ when selecting equally valuable stimulus-guided and internally-specified movements. Specifically, we would expect that internally-specified movements would be promoted relative to otherwise-identical stimulus-guided movements, just as more valuable movements have been shown to be promoted relative to otherwise-identical less valuable movements (Hikosaka et al., 2006; Sato and Hikosaka, 2002). Notably, it is has been proposed that Parkinsonian patients exhibit more pronounced bradykinesia when initiating internally-specified than stimulus-guided movements because the latter engage pathways outside of the BG (Glickstein and Stein, 1991). However, whether the BG themselves are differentially engaged by these two types of movements has not been tested.
We distinguished between these two possibilities by recording from neurons in the substantia nigra pars reticulata (SNr), an output nucleus of the BG critical for orienting movements (Basso and Sommer, 2011; Handel and Glimcher, 1999; Hikosaka and Wurtz, 1983a), in mice performing a behavioral task in which a sensory stimulus either was or was not informative of the rewarded direction of an orienting movement. Using a design akin to that of other recent studies (Pastor-Bernier and Cisek, 2011; Seo et al., 2012; Ito and Doya, 2015), in alternating blocks of trials, the rewarded direction was either determined by a sensory cue or by internal representations informed by recent trial history. Critically, we designed the task such that correct movements were equally valuable in both conditions. We found that SNr activity predictably differed between these two conditions, supporting the idea that the BG mediate the influence on movement selection of internal goals. We interpret these results, in the context of a simple model of BG output (Hikosaka et al., 2006), as suggesting that internally-specified movements may be promoted over stimulus-guided movements by BG activity.
Results
Behavioral assay dissociates stimulus-guided and internally-specified movements
We trained mice on a delayed-response spatial choice task comprised of interleaved blocks of 'stimulus-guided' (SG) trials, in which the direction of movement is selected based on a sensory stimulus (Uchida and Mainen, 2003), and 'internally-specified' (IS) trials, in which the direction of an otherwise-identical movement is selected based on internal representations informed by recent trial history (see Materials and methods; Figure 1A,B). In each trial of the task, the mouse is presented with a binary odor mixture at a central port, waits for an auditory go cue, and moves to the left or right reward port for a water reward. In SG trials, the dominant component of the odor mixture – which varies trial by trial – determines the side at which reward will be delivered, while in IS trials, a balanced mixture of the two odors is always presented but reward is delivered at only one side throughout the block (see Materials and methods; Figure 1B). Thus, while both trial types require the mouse to sample the stimulus, in SG trials the stimulus indicates that the rewarded side is determined by the odor mixture and in IS trials the stimulus indicates that the rewarded side is determined by the recent history of choices and outcomes. We found that mice were able to infer (unsignaled) transitions between the SG and IS blocks and switch their response mode accordingly: during SG blocks, mice were equally likely to choose the left and right port (Figure 1C, gray boxes) reflecting a dependence on the odor mixture (Figure 1D), while during IS blocks, mice reliably returned to the same (rewarded) port in each trial (Figure 1C, white boxes). We quantified performance in IS blocks by calculating, for each block, the percentage of correct trials and the number of error events, defined as a run of consecutive incorrect choices (Figure 1E). Finally, we reasoned that if a mouse were to recognize that a given trial belonged to an IS block, it could prepare its movement in advance and would therefore be able to reach the reward port faster (Niemi and Näätänen, 1981; Poulton, 1950; Seo et al., 2012). Indeed, across the population of sessions, we found that reaction time – defined as the time from the go cue to reward port entry – was shorter in IS trials than in the 'easy' SG trials in the same session [Figure 1F; we used only easy SG trials (mixture ratios of 95/5, 80/20, 20/80, and 5/95) to control for a potential dependence of reaction time on difficulty; population of sessions: p = 1.7 × 10−11, paired t-test; individual sessions: IS shorter than SG in 46/108, SG shorter than IS in 3/108, p<0.05, Wilcoxon rank-sum test; ipsiversive and contraversive trials compared separately]. Together, these data suggest that, as intended, the direction of movement in SG blocks is selected based on the stimulus while the direction of movements in IS blocks is selected based on recent trial history. We therefore utilized this behavioral assay to compare how stimulus-guided and internally-specified movements are mediated by the BG, as described below.
SNr activity differs for stimulus-guided and internally-specified movements
If the BG integrates not only value but also other internal representations, then stimulus-guided and internally-specified movements may differentially engage the BG despite being equally valuable. In this case, we would predict that BG output would depend on whether the movement was internally specified or stimulus guided, and specifically, in our task, on the degree to which recent trial history is informative of rewarded direction. To test this prediction we examined activity in the SNr, a BG output known to be involved in orienting movements (Basso and Sommer, 2011; Handel and Glimcher, 2000, 1999; Hikosaka and Wurtz, 1983a, 1983b). We recorded from 296 well-isolated left SNr neurons (see Materials and methods; Figure 2) in four mice performing the task (see Supplementary file 1). Data from one example neuron are shown in Figure 3A,B, segregated by reward port selected (ipsilateral vs. contralateral to the recording side) and by trial type (SG vs. IS). The activity of this neuron clearly depends on both movement direction and trial type. To examine these dependencies across the population of neurons we first examined firing rate during the delay epoch, defined as the time from odor valve open to the time of odor port exit (Figure 1A, gray box), which most directly captures, across trial types, activity underlying selection of the direction of movement (but since activity in IS trials may, by design, reflect direction selection even before stimulus delivery, we subsequently examine activity in other epochs). Based on the firing rate during this epoch in SG and IS trials, we then calculated direction preference (see Materials and methods). This value ranges from -1 (strongly 'prefers' ipsiversive) to 1 (strongly prefers contraversive), where 0 represents no preference. We found that 216/296 neurons displayed a significant direction preference (p<0.05) during the delay epoch, with about as many preferring ipsiversive (94/216) as contraversive (122/216) movements (Figure 3C). Since SNr activity has been shown to exhibit both movement-related increases and decreases (Bryden et al., 2011; Gulley et al., 2002, 1999; Handel and Glimcher, 1999; Sato and Hikosaka, 2002), we next asked whether a relationship existed between direction preference and the sign of activity change during the delay epoch, relative to baseline (see Materials and methods). We found that 188/296 neurons exhibited an increase in activity during this epoch, 91/296 exhibited a decrease, and 17/296 exhibited no change (Table 1), consistent with previous studies (Bryden et al., 2011; Gulley et al., 2002, 1999; Handel and Glimcher, 2000, 1999; Sato and Hikosaka, 2002). Within these groups, neurons exhibited a preference for ipsiversive, contraversive, or neither direction in roughly equal numbers (Table 1).
We next examined whether activity during the delay epoch of direction-selective neurons (Figure 3C, black bars) differed between SG and IS trials, in two complementary ways. First, we examined whether the difference in activity between ipsiversive and contraversive trials depended on whether the movement was stimulus guided or internally specified. Across our population, neurons tended to show a larger difference in firing rate preceding ipsiversive and contraversive movements in IS than in SG trials [Figure 3D; ipsiversive-preferring neurons: p = 2.2 × 10−10, paired t-test; contraversive-preferring neurons: p = 4.0 × 10−4, paired t-test]. Second, we determined whether neurons were trial-type-dependent by comparing firing rates between SG and IS trials in which the selected movement was correct and in the preferred direction of the neuron; we then repeated this comparison for the antipreferred direction. For trials in the preferred direction, we found that the activity of approximately half of the direction-selective neurons was modulated by trial type (101/216; p<0.05, unpaired t-test), with more neurons exhibiting higher activity in IS trials than SG trials (84/101 vs. 17/101; p = 2.6 × 10−11, X2 test). Conversely, for trials in the antipreferred direction, we again found that the activity of approximately half of the direction-selective neurons was modulated by trial type (109/216; p<0.05, unpaired t-test; 158/216 direction-selective neurons were modulated by trial type in at least one direction), but that more neurons exhibited higher activity in SG trials than IS trials (76/109 vs. 33/109; p = 3.8 × 10−5, X2 test). Therefore, while we found that neurons were about equally likely to prefer upcoming ipsiversive and contraversive movements (Figure 3C), their activity depended, in a predictable manner, on trial type (Figure 3D).
While these findings suggest that the BG differentially mediate internally-specified and stimulus-guided movements, as we had predicted, a few differences between SG and IS trials may have contributed to this observation. We therefore sought to identify these differences and determine their influence, in several ways. First, we reasoned that, if neural activity indeed reflects trial type, firing rate would systematically change during the IS block as the mouse increasingly based its movement choice on internal representations instead of the stimulus (recall that the transitions from SG to IS blocks were unsignaled). To test this idea, we calculated the correlation between the trial-by-trial firing rate during the delay epoch and the extent to which the mouse had learned that its movement choice should be internally specified, estimated with a reinforcement learning algorithm (see Materials and methods). We performed this analysis on the 158 neurons with firing rates that depended on both direction and trial type, separately for choices in the preferred and antipreferred direction. Figure 4A shows data from an example neuron displaying a significant correlation for trials in the preferred direction of the neuron (r = 0.65, p = 7.0 × 10−9), and no correlation for trials in the antipreferred direction (r = 0.066, p = 0.60). Overall, 77/158 neurons exhibited a significant correlation (p<0.05) between firing rate and the number of consecutive correct trials for either direction [Figure 4B,C; 35/77 for trials in the preferred direction (red circles), 29/77 for trials in the antipreferred direction (blue circles), and 13/77 for trials in both directions (purple circles)], with more positive correlations for trials in the preferred direction (p = 2.4 × 10−6, X2 test) and negative correlations for trials in the antipreferred direction (p = 5.9 × 10−6, X2 test), as we would expect given the pattern of results shown in Figure 3D. These results support the idea that SNr activity reflects the degree to which movements are selected based on internal representations.
Modulation of SNr activity by task-relevant variables
We next examined the potential influence of other factors on the observed difference in neural activity during SG and IS trials. One difference between these trial types, by design, is that in IS trials the decision (to move left or right) is relatively easy, while in some SG trials this decision is more difficult (Figure 1D). The difficulty of this decision – or an associated variable, such as uncertainty, or the estimated value of each movement direction – could, in principle, affect SNr activity. Were this the case, we would expect to observe a difference in activity between those SG trials requiring an easy discrimination (mixture ratios of 95/5, 80/20, 20/80, and 5/95) and those SG trials requiring a 'difficult' discrimination (mixture ratios of 60/40, 50/50, and 40/60), since easy trials resulted in a larger fraction of correct choices (p = 3.4 × 10–27, paired t-test; see Figure 1D), corresponding to a higher likelihood of reward. We therefore compared firing rate during the delay epoch between easy and difficult SG trials, separately for trials in the ipsiversive (Figure 5A) and contraversive (Figure 5B) direction, for the 216 direction-selective neurons (Figure 3C, black bars). We found that the activity of some individual neurons depended on difficulty (or an associated variable) (ipsiversive direction: 39/216 neurons; contraversive direction: 31/216 neurons, p<0.05, 1-way ANOVA across mixture ratios, Figure 5A,B), as would be predicted by the value-biasing view of BG function. However, there was little overlap (purple circles) between this small population of difficulty-dependent neurons (blue circles) and those neurons that we classified as trial-type-dependent [ipsiversive direction: 21/109 trial-type-dependent, and 18/107 non-trial-type-dependent, neurons exhibited difficulty dependence (these ratios did not differ: p = 0.32, X2 test); contraversive direction: 16/101 trial-type-dependent, and 15/115 non-trial-type-dependent, neurons exhibited difficulty dependence (these ratios did not differ: p = 0.56, X2 test); p<0.05, 1-way ANOVA across mixture ratios]. These results suggest that differences in decision difficulty, uncertainty, and the value associated with the direction of movement do not account for trial-type dependence or the differences in activity between SG and IS trials shown in Figure 3D.
We then examined whether reaction time (which differs between trial types; Figure 1F) and the choice on the previous trial (which, by design, is more likely to correlate with the current choice in IS blocks than SG blocks; Figure 1C) could explain the difference in activity between SG and IS trials. Preliminary analyses of each of these factors in isolation indicated that, as opposed to discrimination difficulty or an associated variable such as value (Figure 5), they often correlated with firing rate during the delay epoch. In order to determine the relative influence of these factors, as well as other factors that correlate with firing rate – current choice (Figure 3C) and trial type (Figure 3D) – on neural activity during the delay epoch, we performed a linear regression analysis with previous choice, current choice, trial type and reaction time as predictor variables (see Materials and methods). By considering all of these factors simultaneously, this analysis provides an unbiased method for determining their influence on neural activity.
Across our population of neurons, the vast majority were influenced by at least one of these factors (281/296, p<0.05), and we found neurons with firing rates influenced by all possible combinations of factors (Figure 6A). Consistent with the results shown in Figure 3C and D, respectively, this analysis confirms that, as the mouse is selecting its direction of movement, the activity of many SNr neurons was modulated by current choice (167/296) and trial type (142/296). We also found that the activity of many neurons depended on reaction time (111/296). Surprisingly, the largest fraction of neurons exhibited activity modulated by previous choice (188/296). This is particularly interesting because this variable is critical for determining, in an IS block, which direction is associated with reward.
Given that movements can initially be selected earlier in IS than SG trials (Figure 3A,B), we wondered how firing rate at other times during the trial depended on previous choice, current choice, trial type, and reaction time. We therefore extended our regression analysis to examine how firing rate is modulated by these factors during overlapping 100 ms windows throughout the trial (see Materials and methods). In the example shown in Figure 6B, the activity of the neuron is modulated by previous choice (cyan line) – i.e., the confidence interval (shading) for this coefficient does not include 0 – even before the odor is delivered (odor valve open), and this influence persists until the movement is initiated (odor port exit). The current choice (black line) does not influence neural activity until ramping up just prior to movement initiation, but then continues to exert an influence for the remainder of the trial. The trial type (magenta line), meanwhile, exerts a moderate influence on the firing rate – specifically, activity is higher for IS trials – until just before movement initiation, after which this influence is diminished. Reaction time was a relatively poor predictor of firing rate (not shown, for clarity). To examine the dynamics of the weights of these factors across our population of neurons, we calculated the fraction of neurons with significant weights in each time window (Figure 6C). The pattern of results was similar to that shown in the example neuron (Figure 6B). Before the odor is delivered, the firing rates of about half of the neurons are influenced by the previous choice. However, as the odor is sampled, the influence of the previous choice decreases and the influence of the current choice increases, with about two thirds of neurons exhibiting a significant weight for the current choice by the time the movement is initiated. Interestingly, trial type modulates the activity of about one third of neurons throughout the trial. The influence of reaction time is strongest during movement but has relatively little influence on the population (not shown). These results indicate that SNr activity dynamically reflects trial type and other task-relevant variables throughout the trial, as would be expected if the BG are differentially involved in mediating stimulus-guided and internally-specified movements.
Discussion
We have shown that SNr activity preceding orienting movements depends on whether the direction of movement was indicated by a stimulus or was specified by internal variables (Figures 3, 4). While we designed the task such that correct movements were equally valuable across these conditions (Figure 1), given imperfect (and stochastic) choice behavior, the experienced value was not necessarily identical. However, the dependence on trial type could not be accounted for by differences in the estimated value of each movement direction – or an associated variable, such as difficulty or uncertainty in selecting the movement – between the trial types (Figure 5). In some neurons this dependence could be explained, in part, by the choice on the previous trial (Figure 6A), which is informative of the rewarded direction in IS blocks. Over the course of the trial, while the influence on SNr activity of the previous choice decreased and that of the current choice increased, as might be expected given the demands of the task, the influence of trial type remained relatively constant (Figure 6C). These results suggest that the SNr is differentially engaged by stimulus-guided and internally-specified movements.
Previous studies in primates have shown that movement-related SNr activity was higher for memory-guided than visually-guided saccades (Hikosaka and Wurtz, 1983a), and that SNr stimulation had a larger effect on memory- than visually-guided saccades (Basso and Liu, 2007). Movements selected based on a remembered stimulus can be thought of as internally specified, and in this sense our results (Figure 3D) are consistent with these findings and demonstrate that they generalize across species and movement types (saccades and full-body orienting). However, the rewarded direction in both memory- and visually-guided trials was indicated by the stimulus, which was not the case in our IS trials, in which we sometimes observed that direction preference emerged even before stimulus delivery (see example in Figure 3A,B). Further, the difference between direction preference in IS and SG trials during the delay epoch was correlated with the difference between preference in IS and SG trials during the epoch from odor port entry to odor valve open (r = 0.47, p = 4.8 × 10−18). These results demonstrate that, in IS trials, the direction of movement was initially selected independent of the stimulus, which contributes to the difference in activity during the delay epoch that we observe between SG and IS trials. In addition, while Hikosaka and Wurtz (1983a) examined only neurons that exhibited a decrease in activity around the time of contraversive saccades, we examined increasing and decreasing neurons that prefer both ipsiversive and contraversive movement (Gulley et al., 2002, 1999; Handel and Glimcher, 2000; Sato and Hikosaka, 2002) and found that all of these groups exhibited a difference in activity between stimulus-guided and internally-specified movements. Therefore, the differences we observed between internally-specified and stimulus-guided movements extend our understanding of SNr function.
Interestingly, patients with Parkinson’s disease and other BG pathologies have been reported to exhibit greater deficits in the initiation of internally-specified than visually-guided movements (Forssberg et al., 1984; Laplane et al., 1984; Azulay et al., 1999). While the neural basis for this phenomenon is not well understood and remains an active area of study (Distler et al., 2016), it has been suggested that visual cues engage (intact) sensorimotor pathways outside of the BG, such as the cerebellum (Glickstein and Stein, 1991). Our results suggest that differential processing of internally-specified and visually-guided movements within the BG themselves may also contribute to this clinical observation.
As noted above, other studies have found that movement-related SNr activity is modulated by the relative value associated with a movement (Bryden et al., 2011; Sato and Hikosaka, 2002), including whether the movement will be rewarded at all (Handel and Glimcher, 2000). This value dependence likely arises from dopaminergic input to the BG that is thought to convey reward-related information (Schultz et al., 1997), and has been accounted for by a model in which, prior to stimulus presentation, reward expectation modulates striatal inputs to the SNr in order to bias downstream superior colliculus (SC) activity such that the most valuable movement is facilitated (Hikosaka et al., 2006; Wolf et al., 2015). We propose that a similar model can also explain how internally-specified movements, more generally, are facilitated (Figure 7).
To illustrate this idea, consider how, given the data presented here, the relative activity between ipsiversive-preferring left and right SNr neurons would relate to an upcoming rightward movement (we consider relative, rather than absolute, activity since this is most directly relevant to the decision – move left vs. move right – required by our task). Left and right SNr neurons would exhibit a larger difference in activity in IS trials than in SG trials (Figure 3D, left). If ipsiversive-preferring SNr neurons primarily project to the ipsilateral SC (Hikosaka and Wurtz, 1983b), then a downstream left SC neuron, the activity of which promotes rightward movement (Felsen and Mainen, 2012; Horwitz and Newsome, 2001; Stubblefield et al., 2013) will receive less inhibition from the left SNr when the movement is internally specified, thereby facilitating rightward movements that are internally specified (Figure 7). Preceding the same movement, contraversive-preferring left and right SNr neurons would also exhibit a larger difference in activity in IS trials than in SG trials (Figure 3D, right). If contraversive-preferring SNr neurons comprise the 'crossed' projection to the contralateral SC (Jiang et al., 2003), then a downstream right SC neuron would receive more inhibition from the left SNr when the movement is internally specified, again facilitating the rightward movement. However, we observed that slightly more SNr neurons prefer contraversive than ipsiversive movement (Figure 3C) but many fewer SNr neurons project to the contralateral than ipsilateral SC, particularly in rodents (Beckstead et al., 1981; Deniau et al., 1977; Gerfen et al., 1982; Jayaraman et al., 1977), and contraversive-preferring SNr neurons may preferentially project to non-tectal targets. Thus, SNr activity may be consistent with the facilitation of internally-specified contraversive movements. Our results therefore extend the model underlying the value-biasing view of BG function (Hikosaka et al., 2006) by suggesting that the influence of the SNr on downstream motor regions is modulated by internal representations in addition to value.
In summary, we have shown that SNr activity depends on whether otherwise-identical movements are specified by internal representations of task variables or guided by an external stimulus. We suggest that this dependence may reflect a facilitation for internally-specified movements, consistent with the view that, although movements are often made in response to sensory stimuli, internal representations of priors play a critical role in guiding motor output (Wolpert and Landy, 2012). Our results are sufficiently consistent with results in primate SNr (Handel and Glimcher, 2000, 1999; Hikosaka and Wurtz, 1983a; Liu and Basso, 2008; Sato and Hikosaka, 2002) that they can inform the interpretation of previous studies (e.g., our proposed extensions of the model explaining the value-biasing role of the BG described above), while also offering novel insight into BG function. Future studies can utilize the task established here, in the experimentally-advantageous awake-behaving mouse model (Carandini and Churchland, 2013), to examine whether the difference in SNr activity preceding internally-specified and stimulus-guided movements is established by local processing or via striatal inputs (Hikosaka et al., 2006; Lauwereyns et al., 2002) and to further elucidate how the BG control goal-directed movements.
Materials and methods
Animal subjects
Request a detailed protocolAll experiments were performed according to protocols approved by the University of Colorado School of Medicine Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. We used male adult C57BL/6J mice (n = 4, determined by estimating the number of neurons required for our analyses and by the number of neurons recorded per mouse in initial experiments; aged 7–14 months at the start of experiments; Jackson Labs) housed in a vivarium with a 12-hr light/dark cycle with lights on at 5:00 am. Food (Teklad Global Rodent Diet No. 2918; Harlan) was available ad libitum. Access to water was restricted to the behavioral session to motivate performance; however, if mice did not obtain ~1 ml of water during the behavioral session, additional water was provided for ~2–5 min following the behavioral session (Smear et al., 2011; Thompson and Felsen, 2013). All mice were weighed daily and received sufficient water during behavioral sessions to maintain >85% of pre-water restriction weight.
Behavioral task
Request a detailed protocolIn general, mice were first trained to perform an odor-guided spatial choice task – which was comprised of 'stimulus-guided' (SG) trials – as described in Stubblefield et al. (2013), and were then trained to perform 'internally-specified' (IS) trials. Briefly, each mouse was water-restricted and trained to interact with three ports (center: odor port; sides: reward ports) along one wall of a behavioral chamber (Island Motion). In each trial, the mouse entered the odor port, triggering the delivery of an odor; waited 488 ± 104 ms (mean ± SD) for a go signal (auditory tone); exited the odor port; and entered one of the reward ports (Figure 1A). Premature exit from the odor port resulted in the unavailability of reward on that trial. Odors were comprised of binary mixtures of (+)-carvone and (-)-carvone, commonly perceived as caraway and spearmint, respectively; an enantiomeric odor pair was selected to control for differences in molecular structure of odorant stimuli. In each SG trial, one of seven odor mixtures was presented via an olfactometer (Island Motion): volume (+)-carvone/(-)-carvone = 95/5, 80/20, 60/40, 50/50, 40/60, 20/80, or 5/95. Mixtures in which (+)-carvone > (-)-carvone indicated reward availability only at the right port and mixtures in which (-)-carvone > (+)-carvone indicated reward availability only at the left port [we therefore refer to (-)-carvone as the 'left odor' (e.g., Figure 1D) for simplicity]. In trials in which (+)-carvone = (-)-carvone, the probability of reward at the left and right ports, independently, was 0.5. Reward, consisting of 4 μl of water, was delivered by transiently opening a calibrated water valve 10–100 ms after reward port entry. Odor and water delivery were controlled, and port entries and exits were recorded, using custom software (available at https://github.com/felsenlab; adapted from C. D. Brody) written in MATLAB (MathWorks).
Mice learned to perform SG trials within ~48 sessions (1 session/day); detailed training stages are described in Stubblefield et al. (2013). Mice required an additional ~5 sessions to learn to perform interleaved blocks of SG and IS trials. In every IS trial the 50/50 mixture was presented, and reward was available only at one side throughout the block. Mice were first introduced to interleaved blocks, each of which required 25 correct trials to advance to the next block. Once they performed ~70% of trials in the session correctly, the number of correct trials required per block was increased to 50. Mice performed 5 blocks (SG, IS, SG, IS, SG) per session (Figure 1B); the side associated with reward switched between each IS block. Upon completing training, mice were implanted with microdrives for neural recording (see below). During each of the 54 recording sessions, mice performed 321.81 ± 89.49 (mean ± SD) trials.
Surgery
Request a detailed protocolDetails of the surgical procedure are provided in Thompson and Felsen (2013). Briefly, once the mouse was fully trained on the task, it was anesthetized with isoflurane and secured in a stereotaxic device, the scalp was incised and retracted, 2 small screws were attached to the skull, and a craniotomy targeting the left SNr was performed, centered at 3.27 mm posterior from bregma and 1.4 mm lateral from the midline (Paxinos and Franklin, 2004). A VersaDrive 4 microdrive (Neuralynx), containing 4 independently adjustable tetrodes, was affixed to the skull via the screws, luting (3M), and dental acrylic (A-M Systems). A second small craniotomy was performed in order to place the ground wire in direct contact with the brain. After the acrylic hardened, a topical triple antibiotic ointment (Major) mixed with 2% lidocaine hydrochloride jelly (Akorn) was applied to the scalp, the mouse was removed from the stereotaxic device, the isoflurane was turned off, and oxygen alone was delivered to the animal to gradually alleviate anesthetic state. Mice were administered sterile isotonic saline (0.9%) for rehydration and an analgesic (Ketofen; 5 mg/kg) for pain management. Analgesic and topical antibiotic administration was repeated daily for up to 5 days, and animals were closely monitored for any signs of distress.
Electrophysiology
Request a detailed protocolNeural recordings were collected using four tetrodes, wherein each tetrode consisted of four polyimide-coated nichrome wires (Sandvik; single-wire diameter 12.5 μm) gold plated to 0.2–0.4 MΩ impedance. Electrical signals were amplified and recorded using the Digital Lynx S multichannel acquisition system (Neuralynx) in conjunction with Cheetah data acquisition software (Neuralynx).
Tetrode depths were adjusted approximately 23 hr before each recording session in order to sample an independent population of neurons across sessions. To estimate tetrode depths during each session we calculated distance traveled with respect to rotation fraction of the screw that was affixed to the shuttle holding the tetrode. One full rotation moved the tetrode ~250 μm and tetrodes were moved ~62.5 μm between sessions. The final tetrode location was confirmed through histological assessment using electrolytic lesions and tetrode tracks (see below).
Offline spike sorting and cluster quality analysis was performed using MClust software (MClust-3.5, A.D. Redish, et al.) in MATLAB. Briefly, for each tetrode, single units were isolated by manual cluster identification based on spike features derived from sampled waveforms (Figure 2B). Identification of single units through examination of spikes in high-dimensional feature space allowed us to refine the delimitation of identified clusters by examining all possible two-dimensional combinations of selected spike features. We used standard spike features for single unit extraction: peak amplitude, energy (square root of the sum of squares of each point in the waveform, divided by the number of samples in the waveform), and the first principal component normalized by energy. Spike features were derived separately for individual leads. To assess the quality of identified clusters we calculated two standard quantitative metrics: L-ratio and isolation distance (Schmitzer-Torbert et al., 2005). Clusters with an L-ratio of less than 0.70 and isolation distance greater than 6.5 were deemed single units, which resulted in the exclusion of 12% of the identified clusters. Although units were clustered without knowledge of interspike interval, only clusters with few interspike intervals less than 1 ms were considered for further examination. Furthermore, we excluded the possibility of including data from the same neuron twice by ensuring that both the waveforms and response properties sufficiently changed across sessions. If they did not, we conservatively assumed that we were recording from the same neuron, and only included data from one session.
Lesioning and histology
Request a detailed protocolTo verify final tetrode location we performed electrolytic lesions (100 μA, ~1.5 min per lead) after the last recording session. One day following lesion, mice were overdosed with an intraperitoneal injection of sodium pentobarbital (100 mg/kg) and transcardially perfused with saline followed by ice-cold 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA) in 0.1 M phosphate buffer (PB). After perfusion, brains were submerged in 4% PFA in 0.1 M PB for 24 hr for post-fixation and then cryoprotected for 24 hr by immersion in 30% sucrose in 0.1 M PB. The brain was encased in the same sucrose solution, and frozen rapidly on dry ice. Serial coronal sections (60 μm) were cut on a sliding microtome for reconstruction of the lesion site and tetrode tracks. Fluorescent Nissl (NeuroTrace, Invitrogen) was used to identify cytoarchitectural features of the SNr and verify tetrode tracks and lesion damage within or below the SNr. Images of SNr (see Figure 2A) were captured with a 10x objective lens, using an LSM 5 Pascal series Axioskop 2 FS MOT confocal microscope (Zeiss).
Analyses and statistics
All analyses were performed in MATLAB.
Direction preference
Request a detailed protocolTo quantify the selectivity of single neurons for movement direction, we used an ROC-based analysis (Green and Swets, 1966). This analysis calculates the ability of an ideal observer to classify whether a given spike rate was recorded in one of two conditions (here, preceding leftward or rightward movement). We defined 'preference' as 2(ROCarea − 0.5), a measure ranging from −1 to 1, where −1 signifies the strongest possible preference for left, 1 signifies the strongest possible preference for right, and 0 signifies no preference (Feierstein et al., 2006). Statistical significance was determined with a permutation test: we recalculated the preference after randomly reassigning all firing rates to either of the two groups arbitrarily, repeating this procedure 500 times to obtain a distribution of values, and calculated the fraction of random values exceeding the actual value. We tested for significance at α = 0.05. Trials in which the movement time (between odor port exit and reward port entry) was > 1.5 s were excluded from all analyses. Neurons with fewer than 100 trials of each type (SG and IS) or with a firing rate below 2.5 spikes/s for either trial type or across the entire session (Fc, described below), were excluded from all analyses.
Sign of activity change during delay epoch
Request a detailed protocolWe calculated the normalized response (NR) for each neuron as NR = Ft/Fc where Ft is the mean firing rate in the 'test' window (delay epoch) and Fc is the mean firing rate in the 'control' window (Sato and Hikosaka, 2002) across all trials in the preferred direction of the neuron (or, for neurons with no direction preference, across all trials). Since the structure of our task does not include a natural 'control' epoch – i.e., in which the animal is in a motionless state unaffected by task demands – our control window was defined as the time of odor port entry to reward port exit (i.e., the duration of the trial). Neurons with NR < 1 were defined as decreasing and neurons with NR > 1 were defined as increasing (Table 1). Note that, by convention, a decreasing neuron that decreases more for contraversive than ipsiversive movement would be considered to have an ipsiversive direction preference (as calculated above), because firing rate is higher for ipsiversive movement (cf. Sato and Hikosaka, 2002).
Reinforcement learning model
Request a detailed protocolIn order to estimate the value associated with each direction of movement, we iteratively updated the value of each direction in each trial as , where is the reward for the given direction in the previous trial in which that direction was chosen [0 for unrewarded (which includes trials in which the correct choice was made but the odor port was exited before the go signal) and 1 for rewarded] and is the learning rate (we set ; values of 0.03 and 0.3 did not affect the results). The value of each direction was updated independently. This estimate is based on the Q-learning algorithm (Sutton and Barto, 1998; note that we excluded a term for maximum future value because this was independent of the choice on the current trial). therefore ranged from 0 to 1. Since we calculated in each trial of the session (including SG and IS trials), it tended to start near 0.5 (but was not exactly 0.5) at the beginning of each IS block. In well-behaved IS blocks, tended to asymptotically approach 1 as the mouse consistently returned to the rewarded port. was calculated separately for the ipsiversive and contraversive directions and was only updated in trials in which that direction was selected.
Regression model
Request a detailed protocolTo assess the influence of several factors on SNr activity (Figure 6), we fit the electrophysiological data with a multi-variable linear regression model of the form
where is the firing rate during the delay epoch,
in the previous trial,
in the current trial,
,
,
represents the mean firing rate across trials during the delay epoch, and , , , and represent the influence on firing rate of the previous choice, the current choice, the trial type, and the reaction time corresponding to that trial. Positive values for and indicate that firing rate is increased by contraversive choices and negative values indicate that firing rate is increased by ipsiversive choices. Positive values for indicate that firing rate is increased by stimulus-guided trials and negative values indicate that firing rate is increased by internally-specified trials. The sign of indicates the sign of the correlation between reaction time and firing rate. We used the MATLAB function fitlm to estimate the βs and calculate their significance and confidence intervals. We also performed this same regression analysis with two additional terms, for value [estimated in IS trials with the reinforcement learning model (see above) and in SG trials as the average performance by mixture ratio within the block (Figure 1D)], and the interaction between value and trial type. We found that, while the firing rate of some neurons was influenced by these additional factors, as expected given the value-biasing view of BG function (Hikosaka et al., 2006), including them – or including the log of the value – did not affect the overall results shown in Figure 6A. To examine how firing rate throughout the trial depended on these factors (Figure 6B,C), we repeated the above analysis with respect to firing rate in overlapping 100 ms bins, shifted by 10 ms, aligned to three behavioral events: odor valve open, odor port exit and reward port entry.
References
-
Context-dependent effects of substantia nigra stimulation on eye movementsJournal of Neurophysiology 97:4129–4142.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00094.2007
-
Neuronal activity in substantia nigra pars reticulata during target selectionJournal of Neuroscience 22:1883–1894.
-
A comparison of the intranigral distribution of nigrotectal neurons labeled with horseradish peroxidase in the monkey, cat, and ratJournal of Neuroscience 1:121–125.
-
Synaptic organisation of the basal gangliaJournal of Anatomy 196:527–542.https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1469-7580.2000.19640527.x
-
Impact of expected value on neural activity in rat substantia nigra pars reticulataEuropean Journal of Neuroscience 33:2308–2317.https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07705.x
-
Probing perceptual decisions in rodentsNature Neuroscience 16:824–831.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3410
-
Linear-Nonlinear-Poisson models of primate choice dynamicsJournal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 84:581–617.https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2005.23-05
-
Exploring the consequences of the previous trialNature Reviews Neuroscience 4:435–443.https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1114
-
Midbrain contributions to sensorimotor decision makingJournal of Neurophysiology 108:135–147.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.01181.2011
-
Is parkinsonian gait caused by a regression to an immature walking pattern?Advances in Neurology 40:375–379.
-
Crossed connections of the substantia nigra in the ratThe Journal of Comparative Neurology 207:283–303.https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.902070308
-
Modulation of striatal projection systems by dopamineAnnual Review of Neuroscience 34:441–466.https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-113641
-
Paradoxical movement in parkinson's diseaseTrends in Neurosciences 14:480–482.https://doi.org/10.1016/0166-2236(91)90055-Y
-
The neural basis of decision makingAnnual Review of Neuroscience 30:535–574.https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.113038
-
BookSignal Detection Theory and PsychophysicsNew York, USA: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
-
Quantitative analysis of substantia nigra pars reticulata activity during a visually guided saccade taskJournal of Neurophysiology 82:3458–3475.
-
Contextual modulation of substantia nigra pars reticulata neuronsJournal of Neurophysiology 83:3042–3048.
-
Basal ganglia orient eyes to rewardJournal of Neurophysiology 95:567–584.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00458.2005
-
Visual and oculomotor functions of monkey substantia nigra pars reticulata. III. Memory-contingent visual and saccade responsesJournal of Neurophysiology 49:1268–1284.
-
Visual and oculomotor functions of monkey substantia nigra pars reticulata. IV. Relation of substantia nigra to superior colliculusJournal of Neurophysiology 49:1285–1301.
-
Modification of saccadic eye movements by GABA-related substances. II. Effects of muscimol in monkey substantia nigra pars reticulataJournal of Neurophysiology 53:292–308.
-
Target selection for saccadic eye movements: prelude activity in the superior colliculus during a direction-discrimination taskJournal of Neurophysiology 86:2543–2558.
-
Expectation of reward modulates cognitive signals in the basal gangliaNature Neuroscience 1:411–416.https://doi.org/10.1038/1625
-
Pure psychic akinesia with bilateral lesions of basal gangliaJournal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry 47:377–385.https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.47.4.377
-
Dynamic response-by-response models of matching behavior in rhesus monkeysJournal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 84:555–579.https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2005.110-04
-
Substantia nigra stimulation influences monkey superior colliculus neuronal activity bilaterallyJournal of Neurophysiology 100:1098–1112.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.01043.2007
-
The basal ganglia: focused selection and inhibition of competing motor programsProgress in Neurobiology 50:381–425.https://doi.org/10.1016/S0301-0082(96)00042-1
-
Foreperiod and simple reaction timePsychological Bulletin 89:133–162.https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.89.1.133
-
Neural correlates of biased competition in premotor cortexJournal of Neuroscience 31:7083–7088.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5681-10.2011
-
BookThe Mouse Brain in StereotaxicCoordinates (2nd edn)Amsterdam: Elsevier Academic.
-
Perceptual Anticipation and Reaction TimeQuarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 2:99–112.https://doi.org/10.1080/17470215008416582
-
Role of primate substantia nigra pars reticulata in reward-oriented saccadic eye movementJournal of Neuroscience 22:2363–2373.
-
Optogenetic investigation of the role of the superior colliculus in orienting movementsBehavioural Brain Research 255:55–63.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2013.04.040
-
Activity in mouse pedunculopontine tegmental nucleus reflects action and outcome in a decision-making taskJournal of Neurophysiology 110:2817–2829.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00464.2013
-
Speed and accuracy of olfactory discrimination in the ratNature Neuroscience 6:1224–1229.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1142
-
An integrative role for the superior colliculus in selecting targets for movementsJournal of Neurophysiology 114:2118–2131.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00262.2015
-
Motor control is decision-makingCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology 22:996–1003.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2012.05.003
Article and author information
Author details
Funding
National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (R01NS079518)
- Gidon Felsen
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Acknowledgements
We thank Michele Basso, Abigail Person, Jaclyn Essig, Andrew Wolf, and Elizabeth Stubblefield for comments on the manuscript; Angie Ribera for use of the confocal microscope; and John Thompson, Jamie Costabile, and Quang Dang for experimental and technical assistance. This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health (R01 NS079518).
Ethics
Animal experimentation: This study was performed in accordance with the recommendations in the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals of the National Institutes of Health, 8th edition. All experiments were performed according to protocol #90209(12)1D, approved by the University of Colorado School of Medicine Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. All surgeries were performed under isoflurane anesthesia and all perfusions were performed following an overdose of sodium pentobarbital. Quality of life was improved with enriched living environments and dietary treats while every effort was made to minimize suffering.
Copyright
© 2016, Lintz et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 1,705
- views
-
- 464
- downloads
-
- 10
- citations
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Cell Biology
- Neuroscience
Experience shapes the brain as neural circuits can be modified by neural stimulation or the lack of it. The molecular mechanisms underlying structural circuit plasticity and how plasticity modifies behaviour are poorly understood. Subjective experience requires dopamine, a neuromodulator that assigns a value to stimuli, and it also controls behaviour, including locomotion, learning, and memory. In Drosophila, Toll receptors are ideally placed to translate experience into structural brain change. Toll-6 is expressed in dopaminergic neurons (DANs), raising the intriguing possibility that Toll-6 could regulate structural plasticity in dopaminergic circuits. Drosophila neurotrophin-2 (DNT-2) is the ligand for Toll-6 and Kek-6, but whether it is required for circuit structural plasticity was unknown. Here, we show that DNT-2-expressing neurons connect with DANs, and they modulate each other. Loss of function for DNT-2 or its receptors Toll-6 and kinase-less Trk-like kek-6 caused DAN and synapse loss, impaired dendrite growth and connectivity, decreased synaptic sites, and caused locomotion deficits. In contrast, over-expressed DNT-2 increased DAN cell number, dendrite complexity, and promoted synaptogenesis. Neuronal activity modified DNT-2, increased synaptogenesis in DNT-2-positive neurons and DANs, and over-expression of DNT-2 did too. Altering the levels of DNT-2 or Toll-6 also modified dopamine-dependent behaviours, including locomotion and long-term memory. To conclude, a feedback loop involving dopamine and DNT-2 highlighted the circuits engaged, and DNT-2 with Toll-6 and Kek-6 induced structural plasticity in this circuit modifying brain function and behaviour.
-
- Cell Biology
- Neuroscience
Sorting nexin 4 (SNX4) is an evolutionary conserved organizer of membrane recycling. In neurons, SNX4 accumulates in synapses, but how SNX4 affects synapse function remains unknown. We generated a conditional SNX4 knock-out mouse model and report that SNX4 cKO synapses show enhanced neurotransmission during train stimulation, while the first evoked EPSC was normal. SNX4 depletion did not affect vesicle recycling, basic autophagic flux, or the levels and localization of SNARE-protein VAMP2/synaptobrevin-2. However, SNX4 depletion affected synapse ultrastructure: an increase in docked synaptic vesicles at the active zone, while the overall vesicle number was normal, and a decreased active zone length. These effects together lead to a substantially increased density of docked vesicles per release site. In conclusion, SNX4 is a negative regulator of synaptic vesicle docking and release. These findings suggest a role for SNX4 in synaptic vesicle recruitment at the active zone.