A saturation hypothesis to explain both enhanced and impaired learning with enhanced plasticity

Abstract

Across many studies, animals with enhanced synaptic plasticity exhibit either enhanced or impaired learning, raising a conceptual puzzle: how enhanced plasticity can yield opposite learning outcomes? Here we show that recent history of experience can determine whether mice with enhanced plasticity exhibit enhanced or impaired learning in response to the same training. Mice with enhanced cerebellar LTD, due to double knockout (DKO) of MHCI H2-Kb/H2-Db (KbDb-/-), exhibited oculomotor learning deficits. However, the same mice exhibited enhanced learning after appropriate pre-training. Theoretical analysis revealed that synapses with history-dependent learning rules could recapitulate the data, and suggested that saturation may be a key factor limiting the ability of enhanced plasticity to enhance learning. Moreover, optogenetic stimulation designed to saturate LTD produced the same impairment in WT as observed in DKO mice. Overall, our results suggest that recent history of activity and the threshold for synaptic plasticity conspire to effect divergent learning outcomes.

Article and author information

Author details

  1. TD Barbara Nguyen-Vu

    Department of Neurobiology, Stanford School of Medicine, Stanford, United States
    For correspondence
    ngbabs@gmail.com
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0002-4708-1982
  2. Grace Q Zhao

    Department of Neurobiology, Stanford School of Medicine, Stanford, United States
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared.
  3. Subhaneil Lahiri

    Department of Neurobiology, Stanford School of Medicine, Stanford, United States
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0003-2028-6635
  4. Rhea R Kimpo

    Department of Neurobiology, Stanford School of Medicine, Stanford, United States
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared.
  5. Hanmi Lee

    Department of Neurobiology, Stanford School of Medicine, Stanford, United States
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared.
  6. Surya Ganguli

    Department of Neurobiology, Stanford School of Medicine, Stanford, United States
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared.
  7. Carla J Shatz

    Department of Neurobiology, Stanford School of Medicine, Stanford, United States
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared.
  8. Jennifer L Raymond

    Department of Neurobiology, Stanford School of Medicine, Stanford, United States
    For correspondence
    jenr@stanford.edu
    Competing interests
    Jennifer L Raymond, Reviewing editor, eLife.

Funding

National Institutes of Health (RO1DC04154,RO1NS072406,R21NS057488,P30DC10363)

  • Jennifer L Raymond

National Science Foundation (Graduate Research Fellowship)

  • TD Barbara Nguyen-Vu

Burroughs Wellcome Fund

  • Surya Ganguli

Genentech Foundation

  • Hanmi Lee

James S. McDonnell Foundation

  • Jennifer L Raymond

National Institutes of Health (F31DC010547)

  • TD Barbara Nguyen-Vu

National Institutes of Health (F32NS058060)

  • Grace Q Zhao

National Institutes of Health (RO1MH07166)

  • Carla J Shatz

National Institutes of Health (NS069375)

  • Jennifer L Raymond

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: All experimental procedures were approved by the Administrative Panel on Laboratory Animal Care at Stanford University under animal care and use committee (IACUC) Protocol #9143, titled 'Vestibular and Visual Control of Eye Movements in Mice'.

Copyright

© 2017, Nguyen-Vu et al.

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License permitting unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. TD Barbara Nguyen-Vu
  2. Grace Q Zhao
  3. Subhaneil Lahiri
  4. Rhea R Kimpo
  5. Hanmi Lee
  6. Surya Ganguli
  7. Carla J Shatz
  8. Jennifer L Raymond
(2017)
A saturation hypothesis to explain both enhanced and impaired learning with enhanced plasticity
eLife 6:e20147.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.20147

Share this article

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.20147

Further reading

    1. Neuroscience
    2. Physics of Living Systems
    Moritz Schloetter, Georg U Maret, Christoph J Kleineidam
    Research Article

    Neurons generate and propagate electrical pulses called action potentials which annihilate on arrival at the axon terminal. We measure the extracellular electric field generated by propagating and annihilating action potentials and find that on annihilation, action potentials expel a local discharge. The discharge at the axon terminal generates an inhomogeneous electric field that immediately influences target neurons and thus provokes ephaptic coupling. Our measurements are quantitatively verified by a powerful analytical model which reveals excitation and inhibition in target neurons, depending on position and morphology of the source-target arrangement. Our model is in full agreement with experimental findings on ephaptic coupling at the well-studied Basket cell-Purkinje cell synapse. It is able to predict ephaptic coupling for any other synaptic geometry as illustrated by a few examples.

    1. Neuroscience
    Sven Ohl, Martin Rolfs
    Research Article

    Detecting causal relations structures our perception of events in the world. Here, we determined for visual interactions whether generalized (i.e. feature-invariant) or specialized (i.e. feature-selective) visual routines underlie the perception of causality. To this end, we applied a visual adaptation protocol to assess the adaptability of specific features in classical launching events of simple geometric shapes. We asked observers to report whether they observed a launch or a pass in ambiguous test events (i.e. the overlap between two discs varied from trial to trial). After prolonged exposure to causal launch events (the adaptor) defined by a particular set of features (i.e. a particular motion direction, motion speed, or feature conjunction), observers were less likely to see causal launches in subsequent ambiguous test events than before adaptation. Crucially, adaptation was contingent on the causal impression in launches as demonstrated by a lack of adaptation in non-causal control events. We assessed whether this negative aftereffect transfers to test events with a new set of feature values that were not presented during adaptation. Processing in specialized (as opposed to generalized) visual routines predicts that the transfer of visual adaptation depends on the feature similarity of the adaptor and the test event. We show that the negative aftereffects do not transfer to unadapted launch directions but do transfer to launch events of different speeds. Finally, we used colored discs to assign distinct feature-based identities to the launching and the launched stimulus. We found that the adaptation transferred across colors if the test event had the same motion direction as the adaptor. In summary, visual adaptation allowed us to carve out a visual feature space underlying the perception of causality and revealed specialized visual routines that are tuned to a launch’s motion direction.