Systemic pharmacological suppression of neural activity reverses learning impairment in a mouse model of Fragile X syndrome
eLife assessment
This important manuscript follows up on previous findings from the same lab supporting the idea that deficits in learning due to enhanced synaptic plasticity are due to saturation effects. Compelling evidence is presented that behavioral learning deficits associated with enhanced synaptic plasticity in a transgenic mouse model can be rescued by manipulations designed to reverse the saturation of synaptic plasticity. In particular, the finding that a previously FDA-approved therapeutic can rescue learning could provide new insights for biologists, psychologists, and others studying learning and neurodevelopment.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.92543.3.sa0Important: Findings that have theoretical or practical implications beyond a single subfield
- Landmark
- Fundamental
- Important
- Valuable
- Useful
Compelling: Evidence that features methods, data and analyses more rigorous than the current state-of-the-art
- Exceptional
- Compelling
- Convincing
- Solid
- Incomplete
- Inadequate
During the peer-review process the editor and reviewers write an eLife Assessment that summarises the significance of the findings reported in the article (on a scale ranging from landmark to useful) and the strength of the evidence (on a scale ranging from exceptional to inadequate). Learn more about eLife Assessments
Abstract
The enhancement of associative synaptic plasticity often results in impaired rather than enhanced learning. Previously, we proposed that such learning impairments can result from saturation of the plasticity mechanism (Nguyen-Vu et al., 2017), or, more generally, from a history-dependent change in the threshold for plasticity. This hypothesis was based on experimental results from mice lacking two class I major histocompatibility molecules, MHCI H2-Kb and H2-Db (MHCI KbDb−/−), which have enhanced associative long-term depression at the parallel fiber-Purkinje cell synapses in the cerebellum (PF-Purkinje cell LTD). Here, we extend this work by testing predictions of the threshold metaplasticity hypothesis in a second mouse line with enhanced PF-Purkinje cell LTD, the Fmr1 knockout mouse model of Fragile X syndrome (FXS). Mice lacking Fmr1 gene expression in cerebellar Purkinje cells (L7-Fmr1 KO) were selectively impaired on two oculomotor learning tasks in which PF-Purkinje cell LTD has been implicated, with no impairment on LTD-independent oculomotor learning tasks. Consistent with the threshold metaplasticity hypothesis, behavioral pre-training designed to reverse LTD at the PF-Purkinje cell synapses eliminated the oculomotor learning deficit in the L7-Fmr1 KO mice, as previously reported in MHCI KbDb−/−mice. In addition, diazepam treatment to suppress neural activity and thereby limit the induction of associative LTD during the pre-training period also eliminated the learning deficits in L7-Fmr1 KO mice. These results support the hypothesis that cerebellar LTD-dependent learning is governed by an experience-dependent sliding threshold for plasticity. An increased threshold for LTD in response to elevated neural activity would tend to oppose firing rate stability, but could serve to stabilize synaptic weights and recently acquired memories. The metaplasticity perspective could inform the development of new clinical approaches for addressing learning impairments in autism and other disorders of the nervous system.
Introduction
Since its discovery, long-term synaptic plasticity has been of great interest to neuroscientists as a therapeutic target for brain disorders, especially disorders affecting learning and memory. Scientific and technological advances have provided an array of tools for enhancing synaptic plasticity. In some cases, experimental manipulations that augment plasticity have succeeded in augmenting learning (Tang et al., 1999; van Praag et al., 1999; Lee and Silva, 2009). However, in many cases, manipulations that augment plasticity have impaired learning (Migaud et al., 1998; Uetani et al., 2000; Gu et al., 2002; Cox et al., 2003; Rutten et al., 2008; Navakkode et al., 2022). Surprisingly, there have been few attempts to reconcile these conflicting findings with a mechanistic explanation for why enhancing synaptic plasticity can have opposite effects on learning. Such mechanistic insight about how enhanced synaptic plasticity functions in vivo could facilitate the development of this approach as a viable clinical intervention for learning disorders, recovery from stroke or brain injury, dementia, addiction, and other neurological and psychiatric disorders.
Recently, we proposed a testable hypothesis about what can go wrong with augmented plasticity in vivo, based on experimental and theoretical analysis of learning in mice with enhanced associative synaptic plasticity in the cerebellum (Nguyen-Vu et al., 2017). Associative LTD at the cerebellar PF-Purkinje cell synapses (PF-Purkinje cell LTD) has been implicated in certain cerebellum-dependent learning tasks and not others, based in part on the observation of selective learning impairments in mouse lines with impaired PF-Purkinje cell LTD (reviewed in Raymond and Medina, 2018; De Zeeuw et al., 2021). Initially, we expected that mice with enhanced PF-Purkinje cell LTD would exhibit the exact opposite behavioral phenotype as mice with impaired PF-Purkinje LTD, that is, enhancement of learning on the same behavioral tasks in which mice with impaired PF-Purkinje cell LTD exhibit impaired learning. Contrary to this expectation, double knockout of the major histocompatibility class I molecules MHCI H2-Kb and H2-Db (MHCI KbDb−/−), which enhances PF-Purkinje cell LTD (McConnell et al., 2009), results in the very same, specific oculomotor learning impairment as observed in mice with impaired PF-Purkinje cell LTD (Nguyen-Vu et al., 2017). To explain the puzzling observation that the enhancement of a plasticity mechanism could yield the same behavioral phenotype as its impairment, we hypothesized that enhanced LTD prevents learning by allowing spontaneous activity in the circuit to aberrantly recruit and saturate this form of plasticity, making it unavailable at the specific synapses where it is needed to support learning. Two key predictions of this hypothesis were confirmed experimentally by previous work: optogenetic stimulation of the circuit designed to induce PF-Purkinje cell LTD before training recapitulated in WT mice the same, specific oculomotor learning deficit observed in the MHCI KbDb−/− mice with enhanced LTD; and a behavioral manipulation designed to reverse PF-Purkinje cell LTD before oculomotor training reversed the learning deficit in MHCI KbDb−/− mice (Nguyen-Vu et al., 2017).
Here, we further tested the hypothesis that the enhancement of PF-Purkinje cell LTD can result in its aberrant recruitment by ongoing neural activity and consequent increased threshold for its induction and reduced availability to support learning. First, we replicated key behavioral findings in a different line of mice with enhanced PF-Purkinje cell LTD. Purkinje cell-specific knock out of the Fragile X gene Fmr1 enhances PF-Purkinje cell LTD (Koekkoek et al., 2005). We show that these L7-Fmr1 KO mice are selectively impaired on LTD-dependent oculomotor learning tasks, and that their learning deficit can be reversed with behavioral pre-training designed to reverse PF-Purkinje cell LTD, as previously reported in the MHCI KbDb−/− mice with enhanced PF-Purkinje cell LTD. We then test a new prediction about a pharmacological treatment to reverse the learning deficit in mice with enhanced associative synaptic plasticity. The experimental results support and extend the influential concept of a history-dependent sliding threshold for synaptic plasticity (Bienenstock et al., 1982) as a regulator of learning.
Results
Selective learning impairment in mice with enhanced associative long-term depression in the cerebellum
We assessed oculomotor learning in mice lacking expression of the Fragile X gene Fmr1 in cerebellar Purkinje cells, which have been shown to have enhanced PF-Purkinje cell LTD in vitro (Koekkoek et al., 2005). Purkinje-cell-specific Fmr1 knock out mice were generated by crossing conditional Fmr1 knockout mice (Mientjes et al., 2006) with mice expressing Cre under the control of the L7/Pcp2 promoter (Zhang et al., 2004; see Materials and methods). We tested the ability of these L7-Fmr1 KO mice to adaptively modify their eye movement responses to vestibular and visual stimuli, and compared their performance on different oculomotor learning tasks that have previously been shown to have different sensitivity to perturbations of PF-Purkinje cell LTD.
We first assessed adaptive modification of the vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR). The VOR stabilizes images on the retina by using the vestibular sensory input caused by a head movement to drive an oppositely directed eye movement response. Learning can adjust the amplitude of this oculomotor reflex to improve the stabilization of visual images on the retina for successful navigation in the world (Gonshor and Jones, 1973; Ito et al., 1974a; Miles and Fuller, 1974; Broussard and Kassardjian, 2004; Gittis and du Lac, 2006; Cullen, 2023). Mice were trained to adaptively increase or decrease their VOR amplitude using two types of vestibular-visual stimulus pairings (Figure 1; Boyden and Raymond, 2003; Boyden et al., 2004). When a vestibular stimulus (1 Hz sinusoidal rotation about an earth-vertical axis with peak velocity of ± 10°/s) was paired with oppositely directed motion of a large-field visual stimulus for 30 min (Figure 1A, left; see Materials and methods), this induced an adaptive learned increase in the eye movement responses of wild type (WT) mice to the vestibular stimulus alone (VOR-increase learning; Figure 1A, right, black; p=7.37 × 10–4, 0 vs 30 min, Tukey). When the vestibular stimulus was instead paired with motion of a visual stimulus in the same direction as the head (Figure 1B, left), this induced an adaptive learned decrease in the eye movement responses of WT mice to the vestibular stimulus alone (VOR-decrease learning; Figure 1B, right, black; p=0.001, 0 vs 30 min, Tukey).
Both VOR-increase and VOR-decrease learning are cerebellum dependent (Ito et al., 1974b; Robinson, 1976; Lisberger et al., 1984; Nagao, 1983; Michnovicz and Bennett, 1987; Pastor et al., 1994; Koekkoek et al., 1997; McElligott et al., 1998). However, manipulations that impair or enhance PF-Purkinje cell LTD have previously been found to selectively alter VOR-increase learning, with less or no effect on VOR-decrease learning (Li et al., 1995; Boyden et al., 2006; Hansel et al., 2006; Guo et al., 2014; Kimpo et al., 2014; Nguyen-Vu et al., 2017; Kakegawa et al., 2018; Zhang et al., 2023). Accordingly, the L7-Fmr1 KO mice with enhanced PF-Purkinje cell LTD were selectively and profoundly impaired on VOR-increase learning. Unlike the WT control group, L7-Fmr1 KO mice exhibited no significant change in the amplitude of their VOR after 30 min of VOR-increase training (Figure 1A, red; p=0.97, L7-Fmr1 KO, 0 vs 30 min; p=3.47 × 10–5, L7-Fmr1 KO vs. WT, 30 min; Tukey). In contrast, VOR-decrease learning in the L7-Fmr1 KO mice was robust and indistinguishable from that of their WT littermates (Figure 1B, red; p=1.10 × 10–5, L7-Fmr1 KO, 0 vs 30 min; p=0.091, L7-Fmr1 KO vs. WT, 30 min; Tukey). The VOR-increase learning deficit was observed in both male and female L7-Fmr1 KO mice (Figure 1—figure supplement 1). Baseline oculomotor performance of L7-Fmr1 KO mice was normal, as were the eye movement responses to the paired presentation of visual and vestibular stimuli used for both types of training (Figure 1—figure supplement 2), suggesting that there was no deficit in the vestibular, visual or oculomotor functions required to perform the learning tasks; rather the L7-Fmr1 KO mice have a selective deficit in learning. These results support previous findings that manipulations of PF-Purkinje cell LTD selectively affect VOR-increase learning, and that the enhancement of PF-Purkinje cell LTD impairs rather than enhances this form of learning.
Behavioral pre-training eliminates learning impairment in L7-Fmr1 KO mice with enhanced LTD
A key question is why the enhancement of PF-Purkinje cell LTD would impair LTD-dependent learning. One potential explanation is that the enhancement of LTD allows the spontaneous activity in the cerebellar circuit to aberrantly recruit this mechanism and increase the threshold for its further recruitment, reducing its availability to support new LTD-dependent learning. If this is the case, then manipulations that prevent or reverse excessive PF-Purkinje cell LTD before training should reset the circuit to a state compatible with new LTD-dependent learning, and thereby improve VOR-increase learning in the L7-Fmr1 KO mice. To test this prediction, we first employed a behavioral approach designed to reverse PF-Purkinje cell LTD in the oculomotor cerebellum before training.
In wild-type mice, VOR-decrease training can rapidly reverse any behavioral evidence of prior VOR-increase learning, which suggests that VOR-decrease training can reverse any plasticity induced during VOR-increase learning, including any PF-Purkinje cell LTD (Boyden and Raymond, 2003), presumably through the induction of PF-Purkinje cell LTP (Lev-Ram et al., 2003; Shim et al., 2022). Accordingly, VOR-decrease pre-training was previously found to reverse the oculomotor learning deficit in MHCI KbDb-/- mice with enhanced PF-Purkinje cell LTD (Nguyen-Vu et al., 2017). We tested whether the same behavioral pre-training intervention could also eliminate the learning deficit in L7-Fmr1 KO mice (Figure 2A–C).
L7-Fmr1 KO and WT mice were subjected to 30 min of VOR-decrease pre-training followed by 30 min of VOR-increase training. In WT mice, there were adaptive changes in the amplitude of the VOR during both the pre-training and training periods—first a decrease and then an increase in the eye movement response to the vestibular stimulus alone (Figure 2B, black; VOR-decrease, dotted lines, p=0.02, WT –30 vs 0 min; VOR-increase, solid lines, p=0.001, WT 0 vs 30 min; Tukey). The L7-Fmr1 KO mice exhibited adaptive changes in the VOR during both the pre-training and training periods that were statistically indistinguishable from WT (Figure 2B, red; VOR-decrease, dotted lines, p=0.18, L7-Fmr1 KO vs. WT, 0 min, Tukey; Figure 2B, bar graphs, p=0.17, L7-Fmr1 KO vs. WT, VOR-increase from 0 to 30 min, t-test; Figure 2—figure supplement 1). Although in the absence of pre-training, VOR-increase training failed to induce any significant change in the VOR of the L7-Fmr1 KO mice (Figure 2A, red, solid lines and bar graph; p=0.99, L7-Fmr1 KO, 0 vs 30 min, Tukey), the same VOR-increase training procedure did induce a significant increase in VOR amplitude when delivered to the same cohort of mice after VOR-decrease pre-training (Figure 2B, red, solid lines and bar graph, p=0.001, 0 vs 30 min, Tukey). In other words, the ability of the L7-Fmr1 KO mice to learn in response to the VOR-increase training varied with the recent history of experience (Figure 2, compare red bars in A vs. B; p=0.01, VOR-increase learning of L7-Fmr1 KO without vs. with pre-training, paired sample t-test). Pre-training experience did not have the same effect in WT mice. The amount of learning exhibited by WT mice in response to VOR-increase training was not enhanced after VOR-decrease pre-training (Figure 2, compare black bars in A vs.B; p=0.41, paired sample t-test). Thus, VOR-decrease pre-training had different effects on the L7-Fmr1 KO and WT mice, putting the L7-Fmr1 KO mice, but not the WT mice, into a state more compatible with VOR-increase learning.
A second behavioral pre-training procedure, habituation of the VOR, induced by presentation of the vestibular stimulus alone in complete darkness (Vestibular only pre-training), had effects similar to those of VOR-decrease pre-training on subsequent VOR-increase learning. After thirty minutes of Vestibular only pre-training, subsequent VOR-increase learning in the L7-Fmr1 KO mice was comparable to that of their WT littermates (Figure 2C, red vs. black bars; p=0.84, L7-Fmr1 KO vs. WT, VOR-increase from 0 to 30 min, t-test).
Pharmacological suppression of neural activity the day before training eliminates learning impairment of L7- Fmr1 KO mice with enhanced LTD
The preceding results are consistent with the hypothesis (Nguyen-Vu et al., 2017) that in mice with enhanced PF-Purkinje cell LTD, spontaneous activity in the circuit can induce LTD and thereby increase the threshold for its subsequent induction, and that behavioral pre-training can alter neural activity in a manner that prevents or reverses this increased threshold for LTD in vivo, thereby reversing the learning impairment. Since PF-Purkinje cell LTD is driven by co-activation of cerebellar parallel fibers and climbing fibers (Ito and Kano, 1982; Ito, 1982; Linden and Connor, 1995), pharmacological suppression of neural activity should also prevent the induction and increase in the threshold for LTD during the pre-training period, and restore the capacity for subsequent LTD-dependent learning in mice with enhanced LTD. We tested this prediction by administering the benzodiazepine diazepam, a positive allosteric modulator of GABAA receptors, to enhance inhibition and suppress neural activity in the L7-Fmr1 KO mice during the period preceding VOR-increase training. Diazepam has been shown to reduce neural firing in cerebellar neurons and neural responses to vestibular stimuli (Ryu and McCabe, 1974; Barmack and Pettorossi, 1980). We assessed VOR learning 2 hr after a single, systemic dose of diazepam, immediately after recovery from diazepam (18–24 hr after administration), and 1 week later.
The acute effect of diazepam administration was to impair learning. There was no effect of diazepam on the baseline amplitude of the VOR response measured in the dark 2 hr after diazepam (Figure 3—figure supplement 1), contrary to what has been reported in rabbit (Barmack and Pettorossi, 1980). However, when VOR-increase training was delivered 2 hr after systemic administration of diazepam, VOR-increase learning was profoundly impaired in WT as well as L7-Fmr1 KO mice (Figure 3—figure supplement 2).
It is not surprising that the acute effect of suppressing neural activity was to impair learning. The key question was whether this suppression of activity could reset the circuit to a state compatible with subsequent LTD-dependent learning. Therefore, VOR learning was tested after recovery from the acute effects of diazepam. Diazepam has a long half-life of ~24 hr (Riss et al., 2008), therefore mice were allowed to recover in their home cage for 18–24 hr after diazepam administration, and then VOR learning was tested after recovery from this prolonged period of pharmacological suppression of neural activity (Figure 3A). Remarkably, the L7-Fmr1 KO mice exhibited robust VOR-increase learning, comparable to their WT littermates (Figure 3B, top, red vs. black; p=0.86, L7-Fmr1 KO vs. WT, 30 min, Tukey). Although the same individual L7-Fmr1 KO had exhibited no significant learning in response to VOR-increase training in the absence of the pharmacological pre-treatment (p=0.99, 0 vs 30 min, data for subset of mice in Figure 1A used in Figure 3 experiments, Tukey), diazepam pre-treatment eliminated this learning deficit.
The enhancement of learning by diazepam pre-treatment was temporary. When the same mice were re-tested one week after diazepam administration, the L7-Fmr1 KO mice again failed to learn in response to VOR-increase training (Figure 3C, red; p=0.12, 0 vs 30 min, Tukey). Thus, diazepam pre-treatment could restore the VOR circuit of L7-Fmr1 KO mice to a state compatible with VOR-increase learning, but this effect was transient.
Specificity of the effects of pre-training treatments on learning
The ability of behavioral and pharmacological pre-training interventions to enhance learning was specific to mice with enhanced PF-Purkinje cell LTD and to the type of VOR learning task. Wild type mice did not exhibit enhanced VOR-increase learning after diazepam pre-treatment (Figure 3B top vs. Figure 2A, black, p=0.55; Figure 3B vs. 3 C, top, black, p=0.99; paired sample t-test). Moreover, there was no effect of diazepam pre-treatment on VOR-decrease learning in either the WT or L7-Fmr1 KO mice (compare Figure 3B, bottom vs. Figure 1B; p=0.91, WT, 1 day post-diazepam vs. control, VOR-decrease at 30 min, paired sample t-test; p=0.37, L7-Fmr1 KO, 1 day post-diazepam vs. control, VOR-decrease at 30 min, paired sample t-test; p=0.11, L7-Fmr1 KO vs. WT 1 day post-diazepam, VOR-decrease at 30 min, Tukey). Thus, both the learning impairment in the L7-Fmr1 KO mice and the effects of diazepam pre-treatment were selective for VOR-increase learning, consistent with previous evidence that this form of VOR learning is more dependent on PF-Purkinje cell LTD than VOR-decrease learning (Li et al., 1995; Boyden et al., 2006; Hansel et al., 2006; Guo et al., 2014; Kimpo et al., 2014; Nguyen-Vu et al., 2017; Kakegawa et al., 2018; Jang et al., 2020; Shim et al., 2022; Zhang et al., 2023).
Previous work has also suggested a selective contribution of PF-Purkinje cell LTD to VOR learning induced with high-frequency (≥1 Hz) vestibular and visual stimuli, with less contribution of LTD when VOR learning is induced with low-frequency (≤0.66 Hz) vestibular and visual stimuli (Boyden et al., 2006; Nguyen-Vu et al., 2017). We found a trend for low-frequency (0.5 Hz, Figure 4) as well as high-frequency (1 Hz, Figures 1—3) VOR-increase learning to be impaired in the L7-Fmr1 KO mice (Figure 4A, left, red vs. black; p=0.06, L7-Fmr1 KO vs. WT, 30 min, Tukey). However, the low-frequency learning impairment was not reversed by the pre-training procedures that reversed the high-frequency learning impairment. Neither behavioral pre-training (Figure 4A, red, middle, p=0.47, L7-Fmr1 KO, VOR-decrease Pre-training vs. no Pre-training; right, p=0.35, L7-Fmr1 KO, Vestibular only Pre-training vs. no Pre-training; paired sample t-test), nor treatment with diazepam 18–24 hr before training (Figure 4B, compare red bars; p=0.66, L7-Fmr1 KO, saline vs. diazepam, paired sample t-test), reversed the impairment of low-frequency VOR-increase learning in the L7-Fmr1 KO mice, in contrast to their effectiveness at reversing the impairment of high-frequency VOR-increase learning (Figures 2 and 3). This is consistent with the hypothesis that the behavioral and pharmacological pre-training manipulations selectively restore the capacity for learning tasks that depend on PF-Purkinje cell LTD.
To further test the specificity of the effects of Fmr1 knockout and diazepam pre-treatment for learning tasks that depend on PF-Purkinje cell LTD, we tested optokinetic reflex (OKR) adaption, which is the oculomotor learning task for which there is arguably the strongest evidence for a critical role of PF-Purkinje cell LTD (Takeuchi et al., 2008; Wang et al., 2014; Inoshita and Hirano, 2018; Kakegawa et al., 2018). The OKR was elicited by rotating a striped drum about an earth-vertical axis centered on the head of the mouse, using a sinusoidal velocity profile with frequency of 1 Hz and peak velocity of ± 10°/s (Figure 5), with the head restrained and stationary. Baseline OKR gain was not significantly different in the L7-Fmr1 KO and WT mice (Figure 5—figure supplement 1). In WT mice, 60 min of training with the optokinetic stimulus induced a learned increase in the amplitude of the OKR response, as the mice learned to more closely match their eye movements to the motion of the optokinetic stimulus and thus improve stability of its image on the retina (Figure 5A, black, p=0.001, 0 vs 60 min, Tukey). The L7-Fmr1 KO mice with enhanced PF-Purkinje cell LTD had significantly impaired OKR adaptation (Figure 5A, red vs. black; p=1.27 × 10–4, L7-Fmr1 KO vs. WT, 60 min; p=0.87, L7-Fmr1 KO, 0 vs 60 min; Tukey). This learning deficit was completely eliminated by pre-treatment with diazepam 18–24 hr before training (Figure 5B). Indeed, the L7-Fmr1 KO mice not only learned better 18–24 hr after diazepam than they did without this pre-treatment (Compare Figure 5A vs. B, red; p=0.0001, saline vs. diazepam pre-treatment, 60 min, Tukey), but their learning after diazepam pre-treatment was even enhanced relative to WT (Figure 5B, red vs. black; p=0.02, L7-Fmr1 KO vs. WT, 60 min, Tukey). Thus, the OKR adaptation results support the findings from high-frequency VOR-increase learning suggesting a deficit in PF-Purkinje cell LTD-dependent learning in the mice with enhanced LTD that varies dramatically with the recent history of activity in the circuit.
Discussion
A question of central scientific and clinical importance is why the enhancement of synaptic plasticity can impair rather than enhance learning. One hypothesis is that a lower threshold for the induction of plasticity might cause it to be over-recruited during training, at synapses that should not have undergone plasticity in addition to synapses where it would support adaptive behavioral changes, thereby corrupting the memory trace (Migaud et al., 1998; Koekkoek et al., 2005). An alternative hypothesis is that the enhancement of plasticity might allow spontaneous activity in the circuit to aberrantly recruit the plasticity mechanism even before training begins, and thereby reduce its availability during training to support new learning (Figure 6). Nguyen-Vu et al., 2017 described this reduced availability as saturation of plasticity, but more generally, the reduced availability could reflect an increased threshold for the induction plasticity (Bienenstock et al., 1982; Leet et al., 2022). This threshold metaplasticity hypothesis differs from the over-recruitment hypothesis by suggesting that the enhanced plasticity mechanism is under- rather than over- recruited during training. It also differs by suggesting that the problem with enhanced plasticity arises because of what it does to the circuit before training, rather than how it functions during training, and therefore more readily accounts for effects of pre-training manipulations on learning in mice with enhanced plasticity. The current findings thus bolster the evidence from Nguyen-Vu et al., 2017 suggesting threshold metaplasticity rather than over-recruitment as the cause of impaired learning in mice with enhanced associative LTD at the cerebellar parallel fiber-Purkinje cell synapses.
We found that L7-Fmr1 KO mice with enhanced PF-Purkinje cell LTD exhibit impaired rather than enhanced learning on oculomotor learning tasks in which PF-Purkinje cell LTD has been implicated, as previously shown for MHCI KbDb−/− mice. In both mouse lines, behavioral manipulations designed to prevent or reverse the induction of PF-Purkinje cell LTD during the period before training reversed the learning impairment. The additional finding that pharmacological suppression of neural activity with diazepam can enhance subsequent learning in mice with enhanced associative LTD provides new evidence that these mice are not incapable of LTD-dependent learning. Rather, the interaction of the ongoing neural activity in the circuit with the enhanced plasticity appears to create a state in which LTD is unavailable to support learning. However, this state of reduced availability can be reversed when the patterns of neural activity that create it are eliminated. The capacity for PF-Purkinje cell LTD-dependent learning is dynamically regulated by the recent history of activity.
Comparison across closely related cerebellum-dependent learning tasks reveals the specific behavioral consequences of enhanced PF-Purkinje cell LTD. Although it was the main candidate mechanism of cerebellum-dependent learning for many decades, there is growing evidence that PF-Purkinje cell LTD contributes selectively to certain cerebellum-dependent learning tasks, and not others (Shibuki et al., 1996; Boyden et al., 2006; De Zeeuw et al., 2021). Oculomotor learning is particularly advantageous for analyzing the function of PF-Purkinje cell LTD during learning because this plasticity mechanism is thought to contribute differentially to a set of closely related oculomotor learning tasks that all depend on the same vestibular, visual and motor signaling pathways through the cerebellar flocculus. Despite the shared cerebellar and extra-cerebellar circuitry, a number of experimental approaches, including ex vivo slice physiology (Inoshita and Hirano, 2018; Jang et al., 2020; Shim et al., 2022), optogenetic stimulation (Kimpo et al., 2014; Nguyen-Vu et al., 2017; Zhang et al., 2023) and studies of oculomotor learning in mice with impaired LTD (Boyden et al., 2006; Hansel et al., 2006; Kakegawa et al., 2018) have suggested a selective contribution of PF-Purkinje cell LTD to OKR adaptation and VOR-increase learning induced by high-frequency (≥1 Hz) vestibular and visual stimuli, with less or no contribution to VOR-decrease learning or VOR-increase learning induced with lower frequency vestibular and visual stimuli.
In both the L7-Fmr1 KO and MHCI KbDb−/− mice with enhanced PF-Purkinje cell LTD, the learning impairments and the enhancement of learning by behavioral or pharmacological pre-training manipulations were remarkably selective for the oculomotor learning tasks in which PF-Purkinje cell LTD has been most strongly implicated. Both lines of mice were profoundly impaired on high-frequency VOR-increase learning and OKR adaptation. The effects of behavioral and pharmacological pre-training were strikingly specific for the same oculomotor learning tasks. Moreover, pre-training only enhanced learning in the L7-Fmr1 KO and MHCI KbDb−/− mice, and not WT mice, consistent with the pre-training selectively reversing limitations caused by enhanced LTD, rather than generally enhancing cerebellum-dependent learning. In the L7-Fmr1 KO mice, low-frequency as well as high-frequency VOR-increase learning was impaired. However, the behavioral and diazepam pre-treatments designed to prevent or reverse LTD during the period before training only improved the high-frequency and not the low-frequency VOR-increase learning in the L7-Fmr1 KO mice, suggesting different mechanistic underpinnings of the low- and high-frequency impairments. In other words, the deletion of Fmr1 from Purkinje cells may have two distinct effects: enhancement of PF-Purkinje cell LTD, which recapitulates the high-frequency VOR-increase learning phenotypes observed in the MHCI KbDb−/− mice with enhanced LTD, plus disruption of an additional cellular mechanism that contributes to low-frequency VOR-increase learning. Overall, in the two lines of mice with enhanced PF-Purkinje cell LTD, both the learning impairments and the effects of manipulations designed to reverse or prevent LTD before training were remarkably selective for the specific oculomotor learning tasks with the strongest evidence for a contribution of PF-Purkinje cell LTD. This oculomotor learning task specificity strengthens the evidence that the behavioral phenotypes in the L7-Fmr1 KO and MHCI KbDb−/− mice are a reflection of the enhanced LTD at PF-Purkinje cell synapses rather than other functional properties of the Purkinje cells, their synapses, or other cells or plasticity mechanisms within the same circuit.
The very similar behavioral phenotypes observed when PF-Purkinje cell LTD is enhanced by manipulating different molecular cascades further strengthens the evidence that their shared effect of enhancing LTD is responsible for their shared learning impairments, rather than other, off-target effects of the molecular manipulations. MHCI H2- Db acts on MAP kinase and integrin via interaction with immune receptors such as PirB (Shatz, 2009), whereas Fmr1 acts by inhibiting mGluR-dependent dendritic protein translation (Huber et al., 2002). Cell-type-specific manipulations localize the site where loss of expression of MHCI H2-Db or Fmr1 yields the oculomotor learning deficits to the Purkinje cells. Each yields enhancement of PF-Purkinje cell LTD, as measured by the ability of protocols that fail to induce LTD in slices from WT mice to induce LTD in slices from MHCI KbDb−/− (McConnell et al., 2009) and L7-Fmr1 KO mice (Koekkoek et al., 2005). Along with this shared effect of enhancing PF-Purkinje cell LTD, loss of MHCI H2-Db or Fmr1 expression from the Purkinje cells may each have additional effects on the intrinsic and synaptic properties of Purkinje cells, which are likely to be different for the two molecules. These latter, ‘off-target’ effects may contribute to the impairment of L7-Fmr1 KO mice on low-frequency VOR-increase learning (Figure 4), which was not observed in MHCI KbDb−/− mice (Figure 1—figure supplement 1, Nguyen-Vu et al., 2017). Off-target effects also might have contributed to the different cerebellar learning phenotypes reported previously in the two lines of mice – enhanced rotorod learning in the MHCI KbDb−/− mice (McConnell et al., 2009), but impaired eyeblink conditioning in the L7-Fmr1 KO mice, global Fmr1 KO mice and Fragile X patients (Koekkoek et al., 2005). However, the behavioral tasks used in those previous studies were also different, and our current results highlight the importance of the specific choice of behavioral task for assessing cerebellar learning, and the differential dependence of different cerebellar learning tasks on specific molecular and cellular processes within the cerebellum.
The current findings and the metaplasticity conceptual framework could guide the development of new clinical approaches for Fragile X syndrome and a range of other neurological and psychiatric conditions with enhanced associative plasticity. Pre-treatment with the FDA-approved drug diazepam restored the capacity for high-frequency VOR-increase learning and OKR adaptation in the L7-Fmr1 KO mice without compromising other forms of oculomotor learning or baseline oculomotor performance. This was true even for VOR-decrease learning, which may depend on LTP of the same population of PF-Purkinje cell synapses that undergo LTD during VOR-increase learning (Shim et al., 2022). In other words, diazepam pre-treatment fully rescued the learning impairments with no apparent side effects on other, closely related functions of the same neural circuitry. This specificity enhances its therapeutic potential. At the same time, this approach of suppressing neural activity during a pre-training period may be generally applicable to any learning task, motor or cognitive (Rochefort et al., 2013; Badura et al., 2018; Ashburn et al., 2020; Frontera et al., 2020; Stoodley and Tsai, 2021; Hwang et al., 2022), that is impaired by enhanced associative LTD. Pharmacological suppression of neural activity should suppress PF-Purkinje cell LTD throughout the cerebellum, and hence may have the general effect of restoring all regions of the cerebellum to a state compatible with new LTD-dependent learning. This generality of the pharmacological approach stands in contrast to the behavioral pre-training approach, which would require extensive additional knowledge and experimentation to design the appropriate behavioral pre-treatment to reset each functional region of the cerebellum to a state compatible with LTD-dependent learning, and different behavioral pre-treatments would be required to target each of the many functional regions supporting the myriad motor and cognitive functions of the cerebellum. Thus, from a practical standpoint, pharmacological pre-treatment to prevent or reverse the recruitment of LTD before training and thereby lower the threshold for its subsequent recruitment during training is a more feasible and general approach to restoring the capacity for PF-Purkinje cell LTD-dependent learning. Such an approach could be tested even when the specific plasticity mechanism responsible for a learning deficit has not been identified, because lower activity should reduce induction of most associative plasticity mechanisms. Thus, the approach of limiting neural activity during a period before training to reset elevated thresholds for plasticity may be broadly applicable throughout the brain for resetting neural circuits to a state compatible with adaptive plasticity and new learning. Consistent with this, suppression of neural activity in the retina has been successfully employed to reset the visual circuitry and enable recovery from amblyopia in adult mice and cats (Fong et al., 2021). The suppression of neural activity may be an especially useful approach if plasticity is pathologically enhanced in areas like the cerebellum or basal ganglia, with a high level of spontaneous spiking activity.
The results predict history-dependent changes in the availability of PF-Purkinje cell LTD to support learning due to activity-dependent changes in the threshold for LTD. Threshold metaplasticity has not been directly documented at these synapses, however several factors that influence climbing fiber-induced calcium influx or the probability of LTD induction have been identified (reviewed in Zang and De Schutter, 2019), which could provide the mechanistic substrate for threshold metaplasticity. These include plasticity of the climbing fiber synapse onto the Purkinje cell (Hansel and Linden, 2000), plasticity of Purkinje cell dendritic excitability (Ohtsuki et al., 2012), changes in the number of spikes in a climbing fiber burst (Mathy et al., 2009; Medina and Lisberger, 2008), changes in short-term plasticity mechanisms at the PF-Purkinje cell synapses (Hunley et al., 2023), and plasticity of inhibitory synapses onto the Purkinje cells (Kano et al., 1992; Kawaguchi and Hirano, 2007; Rowan et al., 2018).
The concept of experience-dependent changes in the threshold for synaptic plasticity has been highly influential in theoretical and computational neuroscience (Abraham and Bear, 1996; Benusková et al., 1999; Cooper and Bear, 2012; Hulme et al., 2012; Lee, 2022), since the foundational work of Bienenstock et al., 1982. However, experimental evidence for whether and how such threshold metaplasticity supports the function of neural circuits has been limited, and derived largely from studies of the effects of sensory deprivation on the functional connectivity of circuits (Mioche and Singer, 1989; Kirkwood et al., 1996; Philpot et al., 2003; He et al., 2007; Yee et al., 2017) rather than studies of learning per se. Analysis of threshold metaplasticity in the context of cerebellum-dependent learning and associative LTD offers a new perspective on the Bienenstock, Cooper,and Munro (BCM) model (Bienenstock et al., 1982). Most fundamentally, the present results predict threshold metaplasticity at synapses where the plasticity is not Hebbian. A sliding threshold for plasticity has been conceived as a mechanism for countering an instability inherent in Hebbian LTP whereby correlated pre- and post-synaptic activity strengthens a synapse, which leads to an increase in correlated activity, which in turn leads to further strengthening. An increased threshold for LTP in response to an increase in neural activity would counter this instability and provide a mechanism to stabilize firing rates and synaptic weights within a desired range (van Rossum et al., 2000; Toyoizumi et al., 2014; Yger and Gilson, 2015; Zenke et al., 2017). In contrast, plasticity at the cerebellar PF-Purkinje cell synapse is described as ‘anti-Hebbian’ because the associative form of plasticity is LTD. Associative LTD lacks the instability inherent in Hebbian LTP. Moreover, an increased threshold for LTD in response to an increase in neural activity or a decreased threshold for LTD in response to decreased neural activity would tend to oppose rather than support the stability of firing rates of the postsynaptic Purkinje cells. Yet, the present results provide evidence for these activity-dependent changes in the threshold for cerebellar LTD. Thus, rather than supporting homeostatic control of firing rates, the central function of threshold metaplasticity at these synapses may be to limit the amount of plasticity. In addition, the finding that manipulations of neural activity during the pre-training period had different effects in the mice with enhanced LTD than in WT mice suggests that changes in the threshold for plasticity may be driven, not directly by firing rates, but by the recent history of activity-dependent induction of plasticity (Montgomery and Madison, 2002; Lev-Ram et al., 2003; Hunley et al., 2023; Abraham, 2008; Martin and Kosik, 2002; Redondo and Morris, 2011) A plasticity-driven increase in the threshold for further plasticity could serve to protect newly acquired memories from being overwritten (Fusi et al., 2005; Benna and Fusi, 2016). A second potential function would be to separate memories acquired in close succession onto the synapses of different Purkinje cells, in contrast to findings in the amygdala, where there is evidence that a plasticity-driven decrease in the threshold for further plasticity supports the allocation of memories acquired in close succession to the same neurons (Han et al., 2007; Benna and Fusi, 2016; Cai et al., 2016; Rashid et al., 2016; Lau et al., 2020).
Conclusion
We leveraged the relatively simple and well understood physiology and function of the cerebellum and oculomotor system to develop and test a new hypothesis to explain why enhanced plasticity often impairs rather than enhances learning. The current results, along with the previous work by Nguyen-Vu et al., 2017, provide convergent evidence that a lower threshold for synaptic plasticity can result in its aberrant recruitment by ongoing activity in a circuit, resulting in an increased threshold for its subsequent induction and hence the impairment of learning. This threshold metaplasticity perspective may be useful in considering the impact of enhanced plasticity not only in the cerebellum, but in other brain areas as well, and for developing new clinical approaches for reversing maladaptive plasticity and resetting neural circuits to a state compatible with adaptive plasticity and new learning. More generally, the present results highlight the principle that synaptic properties do not control learning in isolation but interact with the patterns of neural activity in the corresponding circuits to control the capacity for new learning. The implication is that learning deficits associated with abnormal plasticity are not necessarily permanent, but in some cases can be remedied with appropriate reset of the circuit, opening up the possibility for therapeutic approaches targeting neural activity as well as the plasticity mechanisms themselves.
Materials and methods
All experimental procedures were approved by the Administrative Panel on Laboratory Animal Care (APLAC protocol # 9143) at Stanford University.
Mice
Mice with the Fmr1 gene knocked out selectively from cerebellar Purkinje cells were generated through the following breeding strategy. First, homozygous female mice whose Fmr1 gene, located on the X-chromosome, was floxed (Fmr1 conditional knockout, cKO; Mientjes et al., 2006) were crossed with male mice expressing L7/Pcp2-Cre on an autosome (L7/Pcp2-Cre Jdhu; The Jackson Laboratory, Stock No. 010536; Zhang et al., 2004). The L7/Pcp2-Cre Jdhu line expresses Cre-recombinase in a manner that is highly selective for Purkinje cells. Male offspring from this first cross were mated with females homozygous for the Fmr1 cKO allele to generate offspring homozygous for Fmr1 cKO, with some mice L7/Pcp2-Cre-positive and some L7/Pcp2-Cre-negative. Cre-positive offspring of this second cross are referred to as L7-Fmr1 KO, and their Cre-negative littermates were used as controls and referred to as wild type (WT). Genotyping was performed by Transnetyx Inc on ear-clipped samples to confirm the presence of the floxed Fmr1 allele in all mice and the presence or absence of Cre using RT-qPCR.
Mice were kept on a reversed 12 hr light/12 hr dark cycle, and behavioral experiments were conducted during the dark cycle of the mice. After surgical implantation (see below), mice were housed individually in standard cages and provided food and water ad libidum. Male and female mice 8–22 weeks old were used in the behavioral experiments. Similar learning deficits were observed in male and female L7-Fmr1 KO mice (Figure 1—figure supplement 1), therefore results were pooled across sex.
Surgery
Request a detailed protocolMice underwent surgery between 8 and 12 weeks of age to implant hardware for restraining the head and measuring eye movements, as described previously (Payne and Raymond, 2017; Nguyen-Vu et al., 2017). Mice were anesthetized with 1.5–2.5% isoflurane. An incision was made in the scalp and a custom-made head post (Shapeways Inc) was attached to the top of the skull using dental acrylic (Relyx Unicem Self-Adhesive Universal Resin Cement, Aplicap Capsule Refills-3M). Two stacked neodymium magnets with a total size of 0.75x2 mm (grade N50, axially magnetized, https://supermagnetman.com/) were implanted beneath the conjunctiva on the temporal side of the left eye. An angular magnetic field sensor (HMC1512, Honeywell Inc) was soldered to an 8-pin connector and attached to the skull above the eye using dental acrylic, in a plane parallel to horizontal (nasal-temporal) eye movements. Eye movements were measured by detecting changes in the magnetic field created by movements of the magnet implanted on the eye (Payne and Raymond, 2017). Mice recovered from surgery for at least five days before experiments were performed.
Behavioral experiments
Request a detailed protocolMice were acclimatized to the laboratory for at least 20 min after transport from the animal care facility before the start of an experiment. Experiments were conducted in a light-proof, sound-attenuated chamber (IAC acoustics). The head of the mouse was secured by attaching its head post to a restrainer, which was then attached to a vestibular turntable controlled by a Carco Model 823 rate table and Model 405D controller. The turntable delivered vestibular stimuli to the mouse by rotation about a yaw (earth-vertical) axis centered on the head of the mouse. An optokinetic drum controlled by a Yaskawa AC-Servo SGMCS-02B3B11 motor provided visual stimulation by rotation about an earth-vertical axis aligned with that of the vestibular turntable. The drum was made of translucent white plastic, and had alternating black and white stripes, with each stripe subtending approximately 7.5° of the visual field, illuminated by an LED light strip attached to the rim of the drum. Eye movements were recorded using the method described in Payne and Raymond, 2017.
Experiments to assess VOR learning consisted of testing blocks and training blocks. Testing blocks consisted of three 45 s tests of the eye movement response to the vestibular stimulus delivered alone in complete darkness, that is, the VOR. The vestibular stimulus was sinusoidal vestibular turntable rotation at 1 Hz or 0.5 Hz with a peak velocity of ± 10°/s. The three 45 s VOR tests in a block were separated by 10 s with the turntable stationary. Training blocks were ten minutes long, and were repeated three times for a total of 30 min training, with a testing block following each training block. For VOR-increase training, the vestibular stimulus used for testing the VOR (1 Hz or 0.5 Hz) was paired with oppositely directed motion of the illuminated optokinetic drum with the same peak velocity (±10°/s). For VOR-decrease training, the vestibular stimulus used for testing was paired with motion of the optokinetic drum in the same direction with the same velocity, so that the drum was stationary relative to the head of the mouse. In behavioral pre-training experiments, the pre-training consisted of three 10 min blocks of either VOR-decrease training or delivery of the vestibular stimulus alone in the dark (Vestibular only), with a testing block before each training block. Calibration of the signals from the magnetic sensor used to record eye movements was performed after the experiment, as described in Payne and Raymond, 2017.
Experiments to assess OKR adaptation consisted of sixty 50 s long blocks of training with 1 Hz sinusoidal rotation of the optokinetic drum with peak velocity of ± 10°/s, with the vestibular turntable stationary. Each 50 s block of training was followed by 10 s in darkness.
Prior to some experiments (Figure 3, Figure 3—figure supplements 1 and 2, Figure 4, Figure 5, Figure 5—figure supplement 1), mice received a single IP injection of 0.4, 0.5, or 2.5 mg/kg diazepam (in saline) or saline control. After diazepam or saline administration, mice were returned to the home cage, and then behavioral testing was performed either 2 hr, 18–24 hr, and/or 1 week later.
Each mouse underwent multiple behavioral experiments, with at least two days between successive experiments. The same cohort of mice was used for the experiments shown in Figures 1 and 2, with the order of the experiments randomized. A subset of the same cohort was then used for the diazepam experiments shown in Figure 3. A separate cohort of mice was used for the low-frequency training experiments shown in Figure 4, with the order of randomized for the behavioral pre-training conditions shown in Figure 4A (no pretraining, VOR-decrease pre-training and Vestibular only pre-training) followed by the diazepam pre-treatment experiments in Figure 4B, with randomized order for drug and saline conditions. Another separate cohort of mice was used for the OKR adaptation experiments shown in Figure 5. The order of experiments with diazepam and saline treatment was pseudorandomized. The experimenters collecting behavioral data were blinded to the genotype and the diazepam versus saline treatment.
Analysis of eye movement measurements
Request a detailed protocolSignals from the magnetic sensor related to eye position were fourth-order low-pass (15 Hz) Butterworth filtered and then digitally differentiated to obtain eye velocity using a windowed (30 ms) Savitzky-Golay filter. Eye velocity data from each VOR test or OKR block were fit with a 1 Hz or 0.5 Hz sinusoid. Values deviating from the sinusoidal fit by more than 31°/s were identified as saccades or movement artifacts and excluded, along with data from 50 ms before and after. Segments of data less than 10ms in duration were also excluded. The entire 45 s VOR or 50 s OKR test was excluded if more than 45% of the data points were excluded, Subsequently, the remaining eye velocity data underwent a second round of fitting using sinusoids at frequencies of either 1 Hz or 0.5 Hz. The amplitude of this second sinusoidal fit provided the measure of the amplitude of the eye movement response, Values from the three VOR tests in a block were averaged. VOR learning (ΔVOR) was calculated as the percentage change in the VOR amplitude following each 10 min block of training relative to the baseline VOR amplitude measured before training. For OKR, values from the first three 50 s OKR blocks were averaged to obtain the pre-training baseline, and values from the last three blocks (58–60 min) were averaged to obtain the post-training OKR measure. OKR learning (ΔOKR) was calculated as the percentage change in the OKR amplitude post- vs. pre-training. Learned changes in the OKR relative to pre-training are also reported for blocks 10, 20, 30, 40, and 50. Eye movement gain was calculated as the ratio of eye movement amplitude to either vestibular (VOR) or visual (OKR) stimulus amplitude.
Statistical analysis
Request a detailed protocolData were analyzed with a Shapiro-Wilk test of normality, followed by a two-factor repeated measures ANOVA with posthoc Tukey or by a two-sample or paired sample t-test, executed in OriginPro 2022 software. A value of p less than 0.05 was considered significant. Data are plotted as mean ± SEM.
Data availability
All code used for data acquisition (https://github.com/RaymondLab/Code/tree/Master/Experiment%20Protocols) and analysis (https://github.com/RaymondLab/Code/tree/Master/Tools/VOR_Analysis) is available at https://github.com/RaymondLab/Code (copy archived at RaymondLab, 2024). Underlying data for each figure is available in the corresponding Excel file.
References
-
Metaplasticity: the plasticity of synaptic plasticityTrends in Neurosciences 19:126–130.https://doi.org/10.1016/s0166-2236(96)80018-x
-
Metaplasticity: tuning synapses and networks for plasticityNature Reviews. Neuroscience 9:387.https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2356
-
Cerebellar function in children with and without dyslexia during single word processingHuman Brain Mapping 41:120–138.https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24792
-
Influence of a GABA agonist, diazepam, on the vestibuloocular reflexes of the rabbitBrain Research Bulletin 5:705–712.https://doi.org/10.1016/0361-9230(80)90116-1
-
Computational principles of synaptic memory consolidationNature Neuroscience 19:1697–1706.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4401
-
Computational study of experience-dependent plasticity in adult rat cortical barrel-columnNetwork 10:303–323.
-
Cerebellum-dependent learning: the role of multiple plasticity mechanismsAnnual Review of Neuroscience 27:581–609.https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144238
-
The BCM theory of synapse modification at 30: interaction of theory with experimentNature Reviews. Neuroscience 13:798–810.https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3353
-
Mice lacking Tropomodulin-2 show enhanced long-term potentiation, hyperactivity, and deficits in learning and memoryMolecular and Cellular Neurosciences 23:1–12.https://doi.org/10.1016/s1044-7431(03)00025-3
-
BookVestibular motor controlIn: Cullen KE, editors. Handbook of Clinical Neurology. Elsevier. pp. 31–54.https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-323-98818-6.00022-4
-
Diversity and dynamism in the cerebellumNature Neuroscience 24:160–167.https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-020-00754-9
-
Intrinsic and synaptic plasticity in the vestibular systemCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology 16:385–390.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2006.06.012
-
Proceedings: Changes of human vestibulo-ocular response induced by vision-reversal during head rotationThe Journal of Physiology 234:102P–103P.
-
Impaired conditioned fear and enhanced long-term potentiation in Fmr2 knock-out miceThe Journal of Neuroscience 22:2753–2763.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-07-02753.2002
-
Cerebellar encoding of multiple candidate error cues in the service of motor learningThe Journal of Neuroscience 34:9880–9890.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5114-13.2014
-
Experience-dependent recovery of vision following chronic deprivation amblyopiaNature Neuroscience 10:1134–1136.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1965
-
Calcium-dependent but action potential-independent BCM-like metaplasticity in the hippocampusThe Journal of Neuroscience 32:6785–6794.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0634-12.2012
-
ConferenceParallel fiber metaplasticity mediated by cannabinoid type 1 receptorsProgram No. 213.05. 2023 Neuroscience Meeting Planner.
-
Cerebellar circuits for classical fear conditioningFrontiers in Cellular Neuroscience 16:836948.https://doi.org/10.3389/fncel.2022.836948
-
Cerebellar control of the vestibulo-ocular reflex--around the flocculus hypothesisAnnual Review of Neuroscience 5:275–296.https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ne.05.030182.001423
-
Intrinsic plasticity of cerebellar purkinje cells contributes to motor memory consolidationThe Journal of Neuroscience 40:4145–4157.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1651-19.2020
-
The role of neuronal excitability, allocation to an engram and memory linking in the behavioral generation of a false memory in miceNeurobiology of Learning and Memory 174:107284.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2020.107284
-
The molecular and cellular biology of enhanced cognitionNature Reviews. Neuroscience 10:126–140.https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2572
-
Metaplasticity framework for cross-modal synaptic plasticity in adultsFrontiers in Synaptic Neuroscience 14:1087042.https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsyn.2022.1087042
-
Metaplasticity: a key to visual recovery from amblyopia in adulthood?Current Opinion in Ophthalmology 33:512–518.https://doi.org/10.1097/ICU.0000000000000901
-
Reversing cerebellar long-term depressionPNAS 100:15989–15993.https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2636935100
-
Long-term synaptic depressionAnnual Review of Neuroscience 18:319–357.https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ne.18.030195.001535
-
Signals used to compute errors in monkey vestibuloocular reflex: possible role of flocculusJournal of Neurophysiology 52:1140–1153.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1984.52.6.1140
-
Synaptic tagging -- who’s it?Nature Reviews. Neuroscience 3:813–820.https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn942
-
Effect of cerebellar inactivation by lidocaine microdialysis on the vestibuloocular reflex in goldfishJournal of Neurophysiology 79:1286–1294.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1998.79.3.1286
-
Effects of rapid cerebellectomy on adaptive gain control of the vestibulo-ocular reflex in alert goldfishExperimental Brain Research 66:287–294.https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00243305
-
The generation of a conditional Fmr1 knock out mouse model to study Fmrp function in vivoNeurobiology of Disease 21:549–555.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nbd.2005.08.019
-
Cerebellar role in adaptation of the goldfish vestibuloocular reflexJournal of Neurophysiology 72:1383–1394.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1994.72.3.1383
-
Evidence for altered NMDA receptor function as a basis for metaplasticity in visual cortexThe Journal of Neuroscience 23:5583–5588.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-13-05583.2003
-
Computational principles of supervised learning in the cerebellumAnnual Review of Neuroscience 41:233–253.https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-080317-061948
-
Making memories last: the synaptic tagging and capture hypothesisNature Reviews Neuroscience 12:17–30.https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2963
-
Benzodiazepines in epilepsy: pharmacology and pharmacokineticsActa Neurologica Scandinavica 118:69–86.https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0404.2008.01004.x
-
Adaptive gain control of vestibuloocular reflex by the cerebellumJournal of Neurophysiology 39:954–969.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1976.39.5.954
-
The cerebellum: a new key structure in the navigation systemFrontiers in Neural Circuits 7:35.https://doi.org/10.3389/fncir.2013.00035
-
Enhanced long-term potentiation and impaired learning in phosphodiesterase 4D-knockout (PDE4D) miceThe European Journal of Neuroscience 28:625–632.https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06349.x
-
effects of diazepam and dimenhydrinate on the resting activity of the vestibular neuronAerospace Medicine 45:1177–1179.
-
ConferencePlasticity mechanisms for bidirectional oculomotor learningProgram No. 213.05. 2022 Neuroscience Meeting Planner.
-
Adaptive prediction for social contexts: The cerebellar contribution to typical and atypical social behaviorsAnnual Review of Neuroscience 44:475–493.https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-100120-092143
-
Stable Hebbian learning from spike timing-dependent plasticityThe Journal of Neuroscience 20:8812–8821.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-23-08812.2000
-
A metaplasticity view of the interaction between homeostatic and Hebbian plasticityPhilosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences 372:20160155.https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2016.0155
-
Models of metaplasticity: A review of conceptsFrontiers in Computational Neuroscience 9:138.https://doi.org/10.3389/fncom.2015.00138
-
Climbing fibers provide graded error signals in cerebellar learningFrontiers in Systems Neuroscience 13:46.https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2019.00046
-
The temporal paradox of Hebbian learning and homeostatic plasticityCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology 43:166–176.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2017.03.015
Article and author information
Author details
Funding
National Institutes of Health (R01 DC004154)
- Jennifer L Raymond
National Institutes of Health (R01 NS072406)
- Jennifer L Raymond
Simons Foundation (543031)
- Jennifer L Raymond
Stanford University (Dean's Fellowship)
- Amin MD Shakhawat
National Eye Institute (R01 EY031972)
- Jennifer L Raymond
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Acknowledgements
We thank David Nelson’s group at Baylor College of Medicine for providing Fmr1 cKO mice. We thank Sriram Jayabal for his technical advice, Macarena Martinez Rey and Daniel Geimer for their support in managing the mouse colony, Maxwell Gagnon and Brian Angeles for writing code for data acquisition and analysis, and all members of the JLR laboratory for their support and input. In preparing this manuscript, we made a conscious effort to address citation bias. Following the approach outlined in Dworkin et al., 2020, we used open source code to assess the gender balance of our citations based on the first names of the first and last authors (Zhou et al., 2020). Excluding self-citations, our article includes citations as follows: 56.98% man/man, 9.30% man/woman, 23.26% woman/man, and 10.47% woman/woman citations. For comparison, the proportions obtained from articles in the top five neuroscience journals (Dworkin et al., 2020) are as follows: 58.4% man/man, 9.4% man/woman, 25.5% woman/man, and 6.7% woman/woman. Our references also contain 21.96% author of color (first)/author of color(last), 18.55% white author/author of color, 21.03% author of color/white author, and 38.46% white author/white author.
Ethics
All experimental procedures were approved by the Administrative Panel on Laboratory Animal Care (APLAC protocol # 9143) at Stanford University.
Version history
- Sent for peer review:
- Preprint posted:
- Reviewed Preprint version 1:
- Reviewed Preprint version 2:
- Version of Record published:
Cite all versions
You can cite all versions using the DOI https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.92543. This DOI represents all versions, and will always resolve to the latest one.
Copyright
© 2023, Shakhawat et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 607
- views
-
- 62
- downloads
-
- 2
- citations
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Neuroscience
Behavioral and pharmaceutical interventions reverse defects associated with increased cerebellar long-term depression in a mouse model of Fragile X syndrome.
-
- Neuroscience
The concept that dimeric protein complexes in synapses can sequentially replace their subunits has been a cornerstone of Francis Crick’s 1984 hypothesis, explaining how long-term memories could be maintained in the face of short protein lifetimes. However, it is unknown whether the subunits of protein complexes that mediate memory are sequentially replaced in the brain and if this process is linked to protein lifetime. We address these issues by focusing on supercomplexes assembled by the abundant postsynaptic scaffolding protein PSD95, which plays a crucial role in memory. We used single-molecule detection, super-resolution microscopy and MINFLUX to probe the molecular composition of PSD95 supercomplexes in mice carrying genetically encoded HaloTags, eGFP, and mEoS2. We found a population of PSD95-containing supercomplexes comprised of two copies of PSD95, with a dominant 12.7 nm separation. Time-stamping of PSD95 subunits in vivo revealed that each PSD95 subunit was sequentially replaced over days and weeks. Comparison of brain regions showed subunit replacement was slowest in the cortex, where PSD95 protein lifetime is longest. Our findings reveal that protein supercomplexes within the postsynaptic density can be maintained by gradual replacement of individual subunits providing a mechanism for stable maintenance of their organization. Moreover, we extend Crick’s model by suggesting that synapses with slow subunit replacement of protein supercomplexes and long-protein lifetimes are specialized for long-term memory storage and that these synapses are highly enriched in superficial layers of the cortex where long-term memories are stored.