Experience-dependent shaping of hippocampal CA1 intracellular activity in novel and familiar environments
Abstract
The hippocampus is critical for producing stable representations of familiar spaces. How these representations arise is poorly understood, largely because changes to hippocampal inputs have not been measured during spatial learning. Here, using intracellular recording, we monitored inputs and plasticity-inducing complex spikes (CSs) in CA1 neurons while mice explored novel and familiar virtual environments. Inputs driving place field spiking increased in amplitude – often suddenly – during novel environment exploration. However, these increases were not sustained in familiar environments. Rather, the spatial tuning of inputs became increasingly similar across repeated traversals of the environment with experience – both within fields and throughout the whole environment. In novel environments, CSs were not necessary for place field formation. Our findings support a model in which initial inhomogeneities in inputs are amplified to produce robust place field activity, then plasticity refines this representation into one with less strongly modulated, but more stable, inputs for long-term storage.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.23040.001Introduction
The hippocampus is a major component of the mammalian brain’s machinery for learning and memory (Andersen et al., 2007). In humans, hippocampal damage results in an inability to convert the ongoing experience of items and events from daily life into long-term memories (Scoville and Milner, 1957). In rodents, the hippocampus has been shown to be critically involved in encoding spatial layouts and locations of importance (O'Keefe and Conway, 1978; Morris et al., 1982). Neural representations of novel (i.e., previously unencountered) environments form rapidly in the rodent hippocampus (Hill, 1978; Wilson and McNaughton, 1993; Frank et al., 2004; Leutgeb et al., 2004) and consist of place cells with location-selective ‘place field’ spiking (O'Keefe and Dostrovsky, 1971). Place field activity stabilizes (i.e., becomes more similar across successive traversals of the environment) with experience (Wilson and McNaughton, 1993; Frank et al., 2004; Leutgeb et al., 2004; Cacucci et al., 2007), and the persistence of fields over time provides the neural basis of spatial memory (O'Keefe and Conway, 1978; Thompson and Best, 1990; Liu et al., 2012; Mankin et al., 2012; Ziv et al., 2013).
What processes underlie the formation of hippocampal memory representations? Synaptic plasticity has been strongly implicated in spatial learning-based behaviors (Morris, 1989; Tsien et al., 1996) as well as changes in place field activity (Bostock et al., 1991; McHugh et al., 1996; Mehta et al., 1997, 2000, 2002; Ekstrom et al., 2001; Lever et al., 2002; Cacucci et al., 2007; Bittner et al., 2015). However, the role of plasticity in creating new place cell representations is unresolved.
First, is plasticity necessary for place cell activity in novel environments? Pharmacological and genetic manipulations of NMDA receptors (NMDARs) suggest that synaptic plasticity is not needed for place fields to appear in novel environments (McHugh et al., 1996; Kentros et al., 1998). However, intracellular recordings suggest that specific, plasticity-inducing events are required to generate each new place field (Bittner et al., 2015). In particular, intracellular experiments have shown that large, calcium-mediated complex-spike (CS) events (Kandel and Spencer, 1961; Wong and Prince, 1978; Traub and Llinás, 1979; Harvey et al., 2009; Epsztein et al., 2011; Grienberger et al., 2014), which trigger synaptic plasticity in vitro (Takahashi and Magee, 2009) and are sufficient to artificially create place fields, always co-occur with the spontaneous appearance of new fields in familiar environments (Bittner et al., 2015). Since, by definition, all place fields in novel environments are new, does their appearance require CSs?
Second, does plasticity strengthen place fields with experience? Within a behavioral session, hippocampal CA1 place field firing rates increase with repeated traversals through each location (Mehta et al., 1997, 2000), which is consistent with a strengthening of the inputs to fields via Hebbian (Hebb, 1949) and spike timing-dependent (Markram et al., 1997; Bi and Poo, 1998) plasticity mechanisms. Across days, though, the opposite occurs, with the average firing rate of CA1 place cells being lower in familiar compared to novel environments (Nitz and McNaughton, 2004; Karlsson and Frank, 2008). In each case, what changes are occurring to the underlying inputs?
Third, what underlies the stable representation of familiar spaces? The low and sometimes variable (Mankin et al., 2012; Ziv et al., 2013; Rubin et al., 2015) firing rates of CA1 place fields in familiar environments work against having a stable representation of space. However, when a place cell fires, the reliability of where it fires increases with experience (Wilson and McNaughton, 1993; Leutgeb et al., 2004; Cacucci et al., 2007). What changes in inputs produce this overall stability?
Addressing these questions requires measuring the inputs to individual place cells and CSs while animals form new spatial representations, and after environments have become familiar. Therefore, we performed whole-cell intracellular recordings of hippocampal neurons in mice exploring both novel and familiar environments – a comparison which has previously been limited to extracellular recording. Unlike extracellular recording, intracellular recording allows access to inputs via the subthreshold membrane potential (Vm), to CSs, and to intrinsic cellular properties such as the AP threshold that shape how inputs are converted into spiking output. Our experiments employed virtual reality-based methods in which animals moved through visually defined virtual environments by running in place (Hölscher et al., 2005; Harvey et al., 2009; Chen et al., 2013; Ravassard et al., 2013), which facilitated switching between different environments during intracellular recording. We assessed experience-dependent changes in subthreshold inputs and other intracellular features within a single exploration session in a given environment. We compared findings from sessions in novel environments to those in environments that had become familiar through multiple exploration sessions across several days. The results provide answers to specific plasticity-related questions and, more generally, help to bridge cellular and systems approaches to hippocampal learning. Overall, the findings support a model in which novel environments induce a rapid amplification of input activity in CA1, then the spatial tuning of inputs becomes less strongly modulated, but more reliable, across the entire environment with experience. The result provides the basis for a stable hippocampal representation of familiar spaces.
Results
A model of hippocampal spatial memory formation
A model of the formation of place cell representations that fits previous experimental observations is shown in Figure 1. It shows the firing rate and subthreshold Vm of a CA1 place cell in both novel and familiar environments as a function of the animal’s location. Here, the spatially tuned inputs driving place field spiking are modeled as being initially small, then, due to plasticity, growing with experience (McHugh et al., 1996; Mehta et al., 1997, 2000; Savelli and Knierim, 2010; D'Albis et al., 2015). The observed decrease in firing rate when the environment has become familiar is hypothesized to be due to a lower baseline Vm, resulting in reduced spiking output in spite of a larger input-based subthreshold Vm hill in the place field. The lower baseline could arise from the observed increase in inhibitory interneuron firing rates with familiarization (Wilson and McNaughton, 1993; Frank et al., 2004; Nitz and McNaughton, 2004). The inputs driving initial place field firing would be present from the onset of exploration and be based on the existing synaptic weight matrix without the need for CSs or plasticity, consistent with the finding that the majority of place fields are present during the first traversal of a novel environment (Hill, 1978). Instead, CS occurrence during some of the traversals (laps) across the environment could serve to strengthen inputs for long-term place field stability. This is in agreement with experiments showing that NMDAR-dependent plasticity is not required for place field formation during novel environment exploration, but is necessary for place field stability across days (Kentros et al., 1998). The larger subthreshold hill relative to out-of-field inputs would provide the basis for a reliable and long-lasting representation of familiar environments. We tested the features of this model with intracellular recordings of place field activity in behaving animals.

A model of intracellular changes underlying the formation of hippocampal CA1 place cell representations.
Hypothesized intracellular features underlying place field activity in novel and familiar environments that are consistent with previous studies. Each side represents the membrane potential (Vm) as a function of the animal’s location in a 1-dimensional environment. With repeated traversals of the environment, inputs underlying place field spiking are strengthened due to Hebbian or spike timing-dependent plasticity, which is reflected in a larger amplitude depolarization of the Vm from the baseline Vm level in familiar environments. The experimentally observed higher AP rate during novel experience is explained by a more depolarized baseline, which could result from the known reduction in CA1 inhibitory interneuron firing rates in novel environments. A lower AP threshold could potentially also contribute to higher AP rates in novel environments (lower, gray dotted line). The spatial tuning of inputs within the place field region is presumed to stabilize with experience, possibly due to plasticity triggered by intracellular complex spikes (CSs). Previous experimental data does not clearly inform what may occur regarding the stability of inputs outside of the field. Figure 7 shows our results from testing the features of this model with intracellular recordings.
Subthreshold Vm hill amplitude grows during novel environment exploration
Whole-cell current-clamp recordings of dorsal hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons were obtained in adult head-fixed mice as they navigated around novel (NOV) and familiar (FAM) 1-dimensional (1-D) virtual maze environments linked to a spherical treadmill (Figure 2A–B; 32 cells from 15 mice; mean ± SD, recording duration: 8.3 ± 7.1 min, recording duration/epoch: 4.3 ± 4.0 min, laps/epoch: 8.9 ± 6.0; maze lengths: 135–240 cm; ‘epoch’ refers to the entire continuous period in a given maze). Virtual reality (VR) facilitated switching between distinct environments during intracellular recording (Figure 2C–D), while the head-fixed behavior in the track-like mazes yielded repeated exposures to all spatial locations within an environment without backtracking. Each cell’s location-based activity with respect to a common reference (the fixed-location primary reward zone) was uncorrelated across distinct mazes (Figure 2—figure supplement 1), suggestive of global remapping (O'Keefe and Conway, 1978; Muller and Kubie, 1987; Leutgeb et al., 2005) between the virtual environments. The animals’ licking shifted from occurring just after reward delivery at the primary reward zone in novel environments to occurring just before delivery (i.e., predictively) in familiar environments (Figure 2E and Figure 2—figure supplement 2A), providing behavioral evidence of spatial learning.

Whole-cell intracellular recording of mouse hippocampal CA1 neurons in novel and familiar virtual maze environments.
(A) Top: Top view of virtual reality apparatus showing mouse on a spherical treadmill surrounded by an image of a maze environment projected onto a cylindrical screen (225° arc). Middle: The four virtual maze environments used in the study. Overhead view of 3-D scene models of the mazes (above). Photos of the rear-projected virtual mazes on the cylindrical screen taken from above and behind the animal position (below). Arrow shows location and perspective of the animal in the virtual maze (above) and on the spherical treadmill (below). Bottom: Example behavioral training and recording protocol. (B) Example whole-cell intracellular recording in a novel maze. Vm (top), AP rate (middle), and subthreshold Vm (bottom) for the first 16 laps in linearized position coordinates. Yellow asterisks mark times (every 20 s) when current injection was applied to probe series and input resistance and evoked spiking response. Note: ‘Subthreshold Vm’ refers to the Vm after APs and CSs have been removed. (C) Activity of same cell shown in (B) as the animal first explored a familiar maze (left), then a novel maze (middle), then was re-exposed to the initial familiar maze (right). Overall AP rate (top) and subthreshold Vm (middle) in each of these three epochs. Note: ‘Epoch’ refers to the entire continuous period in a given maze. White arrow: Running direction. White asterisk: Primary reward location. Example Vm traces (bottom) from single traversals (gray bars) through place fields in each epoch (APs truncated). (D) Overall epoch activity from (C) in linearized coordinates. (E) Left: Lick rate in spatial bins immediately surrounding primary reward zone (RDZ) in novel and familiar mazes. Right: Predictive licking behavior in spatial bins immediately preceding RDZ (Pre-RDZ lick rate in Hz) in familiar versus novel mazes. *p<0.05.
We first checked the amplitude of the main subthreshold Vm hill during individual maze epochs for evidence of increases in synaptic strength. In novel environments, the peak amplitude increased between the initial (1–2) and late (6-end) laps (Figure 3A, left, 3B), similar to the number of laps over which substantial changes in place fields have been observed in extracellular studies (Mehta et al., 2000; Ekstrom et al., 2001). This was the case when considering the peak subthreshold Vm within the field determined from the average subthreshold Vm as a function of location over the entire epoch (median ± SE, NOV initial laps: 1.9 ± 0.5 mV, late laps: 3.1 ± 0.6, p=0.003, n = 14, Figure 3A, left), or, to allow for spatial jitter, the peak in each lap irrespective of location (‘lap peak’, NOV initial laps: 3.0 ± 0.4 mV, late laps: 4.0 ± 0.6, p=0.004). The increase in amplitude is consistent with a strengthening of inputs to place fields via Hebbian, spike timing-dependent, or other (Golding et al., 2002; Dudman et al., 2007) mechanisms of synaptic plasticity, and indicates rapid learning in CA1 in novel environments. In contrast, no increases in amplitude were observed within familiar mazes (p=0.47, Figure 3A, right; lap peak: p=0.70).

Subthreshold Vm depolarizations underlying hippocampal CA1 place fields grow in novel environments and are larger in novel compared to familiar mazes.
(A) Peak subthreshold Vm in the initial laps 1–2 versus the late laps 6-end for all novel (NOV, left) and familiar (FAM, right) mazes. The peak refers to the peak value within the field determined from the epoch-averaged subthreshold Vm as a function of location. Here and elsewhere: Box marks 25-75th percentile values, horizontal line marks median. (B) Peak subthreshold Vm within the (spiking) place field in the initial versus late laps for all place fields in novel mazes. (C)-(D) Examples of place fields that formed in a lap later than the first lap in a novel maze. Subthreshold Vm for each lap at expanded (and fixed) voltage scale (right) and overlaid (bottom right). (E) Left: Peak subthreshold Vm within the place field in the laps before (Pre-Active) versus after (Post) the first lap with spiking in the field. This includes only those place fields in novel mazes that formed in a lap later than the first lap. Note that Post includes the first lap with spiking in the field. Right: Peak subthreshold Vm within the place field in the first lap with spiking in the field (First-Active) versus in the laps afterwards (Post’). This includes all place fields in novel mazes, including those that first spiked within the field in lap 1. In (C, right) and (D, right): Pre-Active laps (shades of blue), First-Active lap (green), Post’ laps (shades of red), Post laps (green and shades of red). (F) Mean subthreshold Vm inside versus outside the field in the laps before place field spiking for the novel maze place fields in (E, Left). (G) Overall epoch peak firing rate for all recorded pairs of novel and familiar mazes (NOV-FAM maze pairs) in which cell was active in at least one of the mazes in the pair. (H) Overall epoch peak subthreshold Vm for all NOV-FAM maze pairs whether or not cell was active or silent in either maze. (I) Same as (H) except for NOV-FAM maze pairs in which the cell had a place field in at least one of the mazes in the pair. (J) Peak subthreshold Vm within the Vm-defined field in the initial laps 1–2 (left) and late laps 6-end (right) of all NOV-FAM maze pairs. P-values for paired comparisons from Wilcoxon signed-rank tests. *p<0.05; **p<0.01.
To further address possible mechanisms, we examined the subthreshold Vm amplitude with respect to the first lap with spiking in the place field. We decomposed the novel maze increase in peak subthreshold Vm amplitude within the place field region (NOV initial laps: 3.0 ± 0.7 mV, late laps: 4.8 ± 0.8, n = 9, p=0.039, Figure 3B; in contrast, FAM initial laps: 3.9 ± 1.3, late laps: 3.0 ± 0.6, n = 10, p=0.63) into two parts. For the subset of novel epochs in which place field spiking did not occur in the first lap, the peak subthreshold Vm in the place field increased sharply from the laps before to the laps after spiking began (Figure 3C–D and E, left). However, there was no consistent increase after the first spiking lap, including cases in which place field spiking occurred in the first lap of the epoch (First-Active lap: 4.0 ± 0.9 mV, Post’ laps: 4.1 ± 0.5, n = 9, p=1.0, Figure 3E, right). The sharp increase suggests mechanisms of plasticity that do not rely on postsynaptic spiking (Golding et al., 2002; Dudman et al., 2007). The lack of increase in peak subthreshold Vm amplitude after the first spiking lap was also observed for place fields in familiar mazes (p=0.42). In all five cases in which place field spiking was not present in the first lap of a novel epoch, the mean Vm in the laps before spiking was greater inside versus outside the eventual place field (Figure 3F; also see Figure 3C–D, bottom right). This suggests that inputs were biased towards the locations of the place fields before spiking occurred.
Subthreshold Vm hill amplitude is smaller in familiar environments
We then compared the amplitude of subthreshold Vm hills in familiar versus novel environments. First, in agreement with previous extracellular recordings from freely moving rats (Nitz and McNaughton, 2004; Karlsson and Frank, 2008), the peak firing rate of active cells was higher in novel environments (peak of average firing rate as a function of location over the entire epoch, NOV: 5.9 ± 2.1 Hz, FAM: 1.7 ± 0.5, n = 10 NOV-FAM maze pairs with at least one epoch being active, p=0.004, Figure 3G; lap peak: NOV: 13.0 ± 2.3 Hz, FAM: 7.7 ± 2.1, p=0.004, Figure 3—figure supplement 1B), and the proportion of cells that were active was greater (69% of NOV epochs and 47% of FAM epochs).
According to the model in Figure 1, the strength of place field inputs should either continue to increase or remain strengthened with additional experience, with the lower firing rates in familiar environments being explained by a more hyperpolarized baseline Vm and/or higher action potential (AP) threshold. However, we found that the spike threshold (Figure 4A) and baseline (Figure 4B) did not differ between novel and familiar environments. Instead, the amplitude of the peak subthreshold Vm was smaller in familiar environments (NOV: 2.4 ± 0.4 mV, FAM: 1.8 ± 0.2, n = 14 active and silent NOV-FAM maze pairs, p=0.042, Figure 3H; lap peak: p=0.002, Figure 3—figure supplement 1C; active maze pairs subset: NOV: 3.2 ± 0.4 mV, FAM: 1.9 ± 0.2, n = 8, p=0.023; note that including only active pairs controls for the lower proportion of active cells in familiar mazes; peak in place field: NOV: 2.5 ± 0.4 mV, FAM: 1.6 ± 0.1, n = 10 maze pairs with at least one epoch having a place field, p=0.004, Figure 3I). Furthermore, there was no sign that any part of the increase in amplitude during novel exploration remained by the time environments became familiar (p=0.52, Figure 3J, left). The second highest subthreshold Vm peak in each environment was not, however, larger in novel compared to familiar environments (NOV: 1.4 ± 0.1 mV, FAM: 1.4 ± 0.1, n = 14, p=1.0).

Somatic excitability does not differ between novel and familiar mazes.
(A) Mean AP threshold for all cells recorded in a NOV-FAM maze pair that were active in both mazes. (B) Overall epoch baseline subthreshold Vm for all cells recorded in a NOV-FAM maze pair. (C) Left: Example intracellular current injection sequence and responses (to probe RN and evoked firing) from a NOV-FAM maze pair. Right: Mean somatic RN for all cells recorded in a NOV-FAM maze pair. (D) Mean number of APs evoked by the positive step in the current injection sequence (normalized to median value for each step size). (E) Mean AP threshold (mV) versus AP rate (Hz) for all NOV (top) and FAM (bottom) maze epochs with ≥1 AP. (F) Left: Baseline subthreshold Vm (mV) for all NOV (top) and FAM (bottom) epochs with no spontaneous APs. Right: Baseline subthreshold Vm versus mean AP rate for all NOV (top) and FAM (bottom) epochs with ≥1 AP. The baseline Vm was more depolarized in epochs with ≥1 AP than completely silent epochs (NOV: p=0.002, FAM: p<0.001). *p<0.05; **p<0.01.
Together, these results indicate that hippocampal CA1 neurons responded uniquely to novelty with enhanced inputs, leading to higher firing rates than in familiar environments. Another likely contributor to the increased spiking in novel environments was larger temporal fluctuations of the subthreshold Vm, reflecting the instantaneous input activity about the mean hill amplitudes described so far. Single place field traversals in novel and familiar mazes could display robust theta (~5–10 Hz) fluctuations (Figure 3—figure supplement 2A). As in freely moving rats (Lee et al., 2012), the theta-band power was slightly positively correlated with mean subthreshold Vm (Figure 3—figure supplement 2B), implying that peak theta power should be somewhat higher in novel environments. Indeed, we found slightly increased in-field theta power (5–10 Hz power1/2 in mV, NOV: 0.79 ± 0.09, FAM: 0.68 ± 0.06, n = 14, p<0.001), as well as gamma power (25–100 Hz power1/2 in mV: NOV: 0.33 ± 0.03, FAM: 0.30 ± 0.02, p=0.001), and Vm standard deviation (NOV: 1.31 ± 0.12 mV, FAM: 1.19 ± 0.10, p=0.011) in novel mazes (Figure 3—figure supplement 2C–E). Although small in amplitude, the extra instantaneous bump to the Vm hills at the peaks of these fluctuations likely adds to the enhanced spiking responses during novel exploration.
Hippocampal firing rates have been shown to increase with running speed (McNaughton et al., 1983). Here, the larger subthreshold responses in novel mazes were not attributable to differences in running behavior. Animals ran at similar overall speeds (NOV: 16.6 ± 1.3 cm/s, FAM: 15.8 ± 0.7, n = 14, p=0.63, Figure 3—figure supplement 1D) and paused (periods >1 s with speed <2 cm/s) with similar frequency (p=0.078, Figure 3—figure supplement 1E) in novel and familiar mazes. Furthermore, the instantaneous speed of the animal was not correlated with the instantaneous subthreshold Vm across novel and familiar epochs (median ± SE of Pearson’s r value per epoch: 0.01 ± 0.02, p=1.0). However, the differences in reward-related predictive licking (Figure 2E) and speed profile (see below) in novel versus familiar mazes provide behavioral evidence that the animals recognized the novelty and familiarity of the environments in VR.
Somatic excitability does not contribute to enhanced responses in novel environments
We next investigated cellular properties for possible contributions to the enhanced subthreshold and spiking responses in novel mazes. Differences in postsynaptic excitability could affect the magnitude of depolarization elicited by a given presynaptic input, as well as affect the spiking response to a given subthreshold Vm depolarization. However, somatic excitability did not differ between novel and familiar environments according to four measures: spike threshold, baseline (resting) Vm, input resistance, and evoked spike count (Figure 4A–D).
As already described, the AP threshold did not differ between novel and familiar environments (NOV: −47.5 ± 1.4 mV, FAM: −48.0 ± 1.7, n = 7, p=1.0, Figure 4A; also see Figure 4—figure supplement 1A). Also, as described, the baseline Vm – which can relate to excitability as well as to inputs – did not differ (NOV: −55.5 ± 1.3 mV, FAM: −56.6 ± 1.2, n = 14, p=0.63, Figure 4B and Figure 4—figure supplement 1B–D). A closer analysis of the baseline revealed no evidence of a role in the larger subthreshold or spiking responses during novel exploration. First, there were no transient changes in baseline found around the time of the switch between mazes (Figure 4—figure supplement 1E). In particular, there was no transient depolarization of the baseline that could trigger a sustained increase in activity during novel exploration. Furthermore, considering each neuron individually, the difference between a cell’s baseline in novel and familiar epochs could not account for the difference in peak subthreshold Vm (p=0.37, Figure 4—figure supplement 1F). A higher somatic membrane input resistance (RN) in novel mazes could cause larger subthreshold Vm hills in response to otherwise similar inputs. However, somatic RN did not differ between novel and familiar mazes (p=0.30, Figure 4C), and any differences within individual neurons could not account for the larger peak Vm responses in novel environments (p=0.43, Figure 4—figure supplement 1G). Lastly, subthreshold Vm hills of a given amplitude could potentially result in different spiking output even with a similar AP threshold (e.g., due to an increased propensity to burst). However, the number of spikes evoked by depolarizing current steps (0.1–0.2 nA, 0.1 s) did not differ between novel and familiar environments (p=0.55, Figure 4D).
Although we found no changes in somatic excitability between novel and familiar mazes during an individual recording, we asked how intrinsic properties, such as the AP threshold, might regulate spiking activity across different recordings. In agreement with previous findings (Epsztein et al., 2011), we found that the AP threshold was negatively correlated with the mean AP rate in novel mazes (p=0.041, Figure 4E, top). Interestingly, no such correlation was found in familiar mazes, for which the mean AP rate was instead positively correlated with the baseline Vm (p=0.003, Figure 4F, bottom right). In addition, the baseline Vm was more depolarized in epochs with ≥1 AP than in epochs with no spontaneous activity for both novel and familiar mazes (NOV: p=0.002, FAM: p<0.001, Figure 4F). These results suggest that in a novel environment, intrinsic cellular excitability helps to determine the initial place field representation. Then, when the environment has become familiar, inputs can drive the stored spatial representations in CA1 irrespective of excitability.
Spatial tuning of subthreshold Vm stabilizes with experience
As increases in the strength of place field inputs did not appear to be sustained, we looked for other evidence of lasting spatial learning. Stabilization of spatially tuned firing with experience is a sign of hippocampal learning (Wilson and McNaughton, 1993; Mehta et al., 1997, 2000, 2002; Kentros et al., 1998; Leutgeb et al., 2004; Frank et al., 2004; Cacucci et al., 2007). Experience-dependent changes in the spatial tuning of inputs have not previously been studied with intracellular recording. Moreover, using extracellular recording, changes cannot be inferred where there is little or no spiking, such as regions outside of place fields and in silent cells. Therefore, we analyzed the tuning of inputs across the whole environment as reflected in the shape of the entire subthreshold Vm curve versus location (Figure 1), and changes in this tuning with experience.
We assessed spatial tuning as a function of experience by computing the linear correlation of AP rate or subthreshold Vm activity in each lap with the average activity over the entire epoch (‘overall epoch’) (Figure 5A and Figure 5—figure supplement 1A), where each correlation was computed across all spatial bins (i.e., the entire extent of the maze). To begin with, high or low spatially correlated activity could be observed in novel or familiar mazes (Figure 5—figure supplement 2), and an individual cell’s correlation score in novel mazes did not predict its score in familiar mazes (or vice versa) for AP rate (p=0.16) or subthreshold Vm (p=0.83). We therefore compared spatial correlation scores for novel versus familiar epochs independent of paired exposures. All epochs were included, with the following exception. In our experiments, for each recording, the animal was generally first exposed to a familiar maze followed by a novel or other familiar maze. In many cases (10/32 cells), this first familiar maze recording was also the first exposure to any maze for the animal on that day. We suspected that spatial correlations could be disrupted in these first familiar epochs due to nonspecific behavioral factors (Figure 2—figure supplement 2B), thus partially masking differences from novel epochs. More generally, this is a factor that may be useful to consider in future VR studies. With all first epochs of the day (whether familiar or novel) excluded (Figure 5B; see Figure 5—figure supplement 1B for the same analysis including all epochs; note, excluding the initial epoch of each day did not alter any of the previous findings such as the difference in peak AP rate and subthreshold Vm amplitudes, Figure 5—figure supplement 3; similarly, excluding the region surrounding the primary reward location did not alter these results), we observed that AP rate correlation scores were, again, generally high and similar (p=0.29) in familiar (Pearson’s r: 0.65 ± 0.14) and novel (0.58 ± 0.09) mazes (Figure 5B, top).

Spatial tuning of inputs throughout the entire environment stabilizes with experience.
(A) Example of two cells recorded in a novel (left) and familiar (right) maze. AP rate (top) and subthreshold Vm (bottom) activity (normalized to the peak value in each lap) for each lap, color-coded based on the spatial correlation score (Pearson’s r) of the lap’s AP rate or subthreshold Vm profile with the overall epoch profile. (B) Left: Mean spatial correlation of the AP rate (top), subthreshold Vm in all locations (middle), and out-field (i.e., excluding the Vm-defined field region) subthreshold Vm (bottom), averaged across all individual laps shown in the corresponding panels on the right. This includes all cells recorded in a NOV or FAM maze (unpaired, minimum of 4 laps required). Right: Individual lap AP rate (top), subthreshold Vm (middle), and out-field subthreshold Vm (bottom) spatial correlation scores in NOV (red) and FAM (black) mazes (mean ± SE) versus lap number. Gray box highlights the comparison of NOV versus FAM within the initial laps (1-2). Black horizontal lines indicate the significance of unpaired comparisons (two lap smoothing) between the given FAM laps and the initial laps 1–2 in the FAM mazes; thick lines: p<0.05, thin lines: n.s. Similarly, red horizontal lines are for NOV mazes. Note that (B) excludes all first maze epochs experienced by the animal on that day (see text and Figure 5—figure supplements 1 and 3). *p<0.05; **p<0.01.
In contrast, the subthreshold Vm spatial correlation scores were significantly lower in novel than familiar mazes (NOV: 0.58 ± 0.12, FAM: 0.69 ± 0.03, p=0.003, Figure 5B, middle left). Moreover, the Vm correlation scores were lower in the initial laps (p=0.003, Figure 5B, middle right), then significantly increased across laps within a novel but not familiar epoch, reaching close to the familiar values in later laps (Figure 5B, middle right). This progressive stabilization of spatial tuning with experience is an intracellular signature of the establishment of a long-term memory trace.
Spatial tuning of subthreshold Vm stabilizes across the entire environment
We then asked where this progressive stabilization was occurring. Extracellular studies have shown that CA1 place field spiking stabilizes with experience (Wilson and McNaughton, 1993; Frank et al., 2004; Leutgeb et al., 2004; Cacucci et al., 2007). Is the stabilization of spatial tuning we observed in CA1 occurring for its inputs across the whole environment?
We addressed this question by analyzing the subthreshold Vm outside of the peak subthreshold Vm region (‘out-field’). As with the overall spatial correlation scores, the out-field scores started low in the initial laps (p=0.008) of novel environments then increased in the later laps, and in familiar mazes were generally well-correlated across laps (Figure 5B, bottom). Furthermore, for the subset of epochs in which cells were silent, the spatial correlation of the subthreshold Vm also increased during novel exploration and was high in familiar environments (Figure 5—figure supplement 1C). These results indicate that the increase in spatial tuning was not limited to the place field region, and that the spatial tuning of the rest of the inputs also stabilizes. This is consistent with stabilization of the entire network with experience, including excitatory inputs from hippocampal area CA3 and the entorhinal cortex (EC), each of which contain neurons that display spatially tuned firing (e.g., place fields in CA3 and grid cells in EC) that must itself be established in novel environments (Leutgeb et al., 2004; Barry et al., 2012).
While animals ran at similar speeds in novel and familiar environments, the spatial profile of running speed was somewhat less consistent during novel exploration (p=0.025; Figure 5—figure supplement 1D). However, the lower Vm correlation scores in novel mazes were not attributable to differences in running behavior, as speed correlation scores did not predict Vm correlation scores (p=0.28; Figure 5—figure supplement 1E; see Materials and methods).
We also measured the shape of the subthreshold Vm hills. A backward shift of the hill with experience (i.e., a shift in location in the direction opposite to the animal’s motion) (Blum and Abbott, 1996; Mehta et al., 1997) and a negative skew (i.e., a slower rise in the direction of the animal’s motion) (Mehta et al., 2000; 2002; Ekstrom et al., 2001; Harvey et al., 2009) are thought to result from spike timing-dependent plasticity (Markram et al., 1997; Bi and Poo, 1998). Signs of both were observed in the subthreshold Vm hills underlying the AP place field (Figure 5—figure supplement 4), providing additional evidence of experience-dependent plasticity.
Complex spikes are not necessary for place field formation in novel environments
Finally, we examined the role of CSs in place field formation and plasticity during novel environment exploration. CSs (also called plateau potentials in Bittner et al., 2015) are detected in somatic recordings as large amplitude, plateau-like Vm depolarizations with accompanying spiking (Figure 6A–B, expanded traces), and are distinct (Epsztein et al., 2011) from extracellularly defined complex spikes (Ranck, 1973). CSs occurred in novel and familiar mazes and had similar properties in both (e.g., duration, number of APs, Figure 6—figure supplement 1A–C). CSs spontaneously occurred at low rates in both novel (14 CSs total in 10 of 190 total laps from nine maze epochs with ≥1 AP) and familiar (17 CSs in 11/228 laps from 26 epochs) mazes, were present in less than half of all cells (Figure 6—figure supplement 1D, left), and had occurrence rates that increased similarly with increasing subthreshold Vm level (Figure 6—figure supplement 1E).

Complex spikes transiently boost spatial responses but are not required for place field formation in novel mazes.
(A)-(B) Example novel maze recordings that contained ≥1 CS in the maze. Overall epoch 2-D AP rate map and subthreshold Vm (above). Individual laps are shown as three traces: Vm (top), AP rate (middle), and subthreshold Vm (bottom), with the location and trace of the first CSs in each maze epoch highlighted. AP rate and subthreshold Vm lap activity is normalized to the peak activity value in each lap. In no cases were CSs evoked by the current injections (asterisks) in (A)-(B). In (A), the place field had already formed before the first CS occurs. In (B), the CS occurs in the lap in which the place field first appears (same epoch as shown in Figure 3C). Note that there were no CSs in the novel maze place field recordings in Figure 2B or Figure 3D. (C) Peak AP rate (top) and subthreshold Vm (bottom) for individual laps aligned (lap = 0) to the lap containing the first CS in the maze epoch for those epochs with a CS (green) or containing the largest peak subthreshold Vm for those epochs without a CS (gray), then pooled across NOV and FAM maze epochs. Peak activity determined from the region (±3 spatial bins) surrounding the CS or peak subthreshold Vm location. Mean ± SE. Green horizontal lines indicate the significance of paired comparisons (two lap smoothing) between the given laps and the mean of the pre-CS (pre-lap 0) laps; thick lines: p<0.05, thin lines: n.s. Similarly, gray horizontal lines are for epochs without a CS. (D) Same as (C) for AP rate (top) and subthreshold Vm (bottom) spatial correlation scores (Pearson’s r).
Even though CSs were rare events, previous work suggests that they play a significant role in the formation of place fields in novel mazes, as has been shown for familiar environments. Specifically, in previous work using a familiar treadmill-based virtual environment, CSs were always present in the first lap of spontaneously appearing place fields in cells that were silent in previous laps, and artificially evoked CSs were capable of immediately creating place fields in previously silent cells (Bittner et al., 2015). Therefore, we checked whether the initial appearance of place fields in novel mazes, which by definition are new place fields, was accompanied by CSs. Of the nine novel epoch place fields, four displayed stable activity in the place field region prior to any CSs (which then occurred later in the place field in 3/4 cases, e.g., Figure 6A), two had ≥1 CS in the initial lap of the emergent field (e.g., Figures 3C and 6B), and the remaining 3 were in epochs that did not have any CSs (e.g., Figures 2B and 3D did not contain CSs). Of these 9 place fields, 7 occurred in mazes that the animal had never previously been exposed to, and 2 occurred in mazes previously experienced 1 or 2 times (versus ≥19 exposures for familiar epochs, Table 1). Thus, CSs were not required for place field formation in novel environments. Moreover, CSs in familiar mazes did not necessarily stabilize place fields (Figure 6—figure supplement 2).
Details of the individual cells and maze epochs. The ‘P’ and ‘N’ for epochs in mazes which the animal explored in both directions denote the two directions (i.e. moving in the direction of positive and negative changes in position value). The ‘#” indicates epochs that were excluded from analysis because their place fields appear to have been attributable to experimenter manipulations (e.g. inadvertent triggering of a CS during a current step used to probe series and input resistance and evoked spiking response), and thus their place field properties could not be assumed to be those of spontaneously occurring place fields. These epochs were however included in the analysis of Figure 6C–D since one can still assess the effect of any spontaneous CSs they displayed. The ‘##” marks an epoch in which the AP threshold could not be measured because all APs in the epoch contained a shoulder (see Materials and methods).
Cell # | Epoch # | Maze | # Prev. Maze Sessions | Rec. dur. (s) | # Laps | Subth. vm baseline (mV) | Subth. vm peak (mV) | AP rate peak (Hz) | AP rate mean (Hz) | AP threshold (mV) | Input R. (MOhm) | AP rate Infield-Outfield ratio | Place field (0=No, 1=Yes) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 | 1 | 'O' | ≥39 | 106.5 | 7 | −61.3 | 1.4 | 0.00 | 0.00 | N/A | 147.5 | N/A | N/A |
1 | 2 | '8' | 0 | 282.7 | 17 | −60.3 | 2.4 | 0.00 | 0.00 | N/A | 197.0 | N/A | N/A |
2 | 1 | 'O' | ≥39 | 99.7 | 5 | −56.9 | 1.2 | 0.00 | 0.00 | N/A | 88.8 | N/A | N/A |
2 | 3P | 'l' | ≥19 | 209.0 | 3 | −54.0 | 2.8 | 0.00 | 0.00 | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A |
2 | 3N | 'l' | ≥19 | 2 | −53.7 | 2.1 | 0.00 | 0.00 | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | |
3 | 1 | 'O' | ≥39 | 171.5 | 6 | −56.5 | 1.9 | 2.32 | 0.59 | −48.5 | 77.7 | 6.1 | 1 |
3 | 2 | '8' | 1 | 124.3 | 5 | −55.4 | 1.7 | 4.14 | 1.68 | −49.4 | 195.3 | 1.7 | 0 |
4 | 2 | '8' | 0 | 807.7 | 13 | −52.8 | 1.1 | 1.35 | 0.44 | −48.1 | 58.1 | 2.1 | 0 |
5 | 1 | 'O' | ≥39 | 115.5 | 5 | −62.6 | 1.3 | 0.00 | 0.00 | N/A | 120.3 | N/A | N/A |
6 | 1 | 'O' | ≥39 | 103.1 | 8 | −57.2 | 2.0 | 0.00 | 0.00 | N/A | 83.6 | N/A | N/A |
6 | 2P | 'L' | 0 | 587.7 | 12 | −53.3 | 2.3 | 2.67 | 0.78 | −40.4 | 23.9 | 4.5 | 1 |
6 | 2N | 'L' | 0 | 12 | −53.6 | 1.7 | 0.89 | 0.38 | −40.4 | 23.9 | 7.1 | 1 | |
7 | 1 | 'O' | ≥39 | 96.6 | 8 | −66.1 | 1.4 | 0.00 | 0.00 | N/A | 83.4 | N/A | N/A |
7 | 2 | '8' | 1 | 147.7 | 9 | −66.4 | 1.9 | 0.00 | 0.00 | N/A | 86.8 | N/A | N/A |
7 | 3P | 'l' | ≥19 | 124.4 | 5 | −66.6 | 3.0 | 0.00 | 0.00 | N/A | 69.0 | N/A | N/A |
7 | 3N | 'l' | ≥19 | 5 | −65.6 | 2.0 | 0.00 | 0.00 | N/A | 69.0 | N/A | N/A | |
8 | 1 | 'O' | ≥39 | 174.9 | 6 | −54.7 | 1.6 | 0.80 | 0.07 | N/A | N/A | 32.6 | 0 |
9 | 2 | '8' | 0 | 121.0 | 6 | −59.4 | 1.1 | 0.00 | 0.00 | N/A | 68.9 | N/A | N/A |
10 | 1 | 'O' | ≥39 | 143.9 | 5 | −57.0 | 2.0 | 6.58 | 1.36 | N/A | 128.0 | 3.9 | 1 |
11 | 1 | 'O' | ≥39 | 57.5 | 6 | −57.8 | 1.1 | 0.00 | 0.00 | N/A | 117.3 | N/A | N/A |
11 | 2# | '8' | 0 | 1058.3 | 9 | −55.7 | 7.9 | 12.05 | 0.89 | −42.2 | 112.7 | 35.5 | 1 |
12 | 1 | 'O' | ≥39 | 248.1 | 16 | −55.3 | 2.2 | 2.89 | 1.51 | −47.3 | 83.8 | 14.5 | 1 |
12 | 2P | 'l' | ≥19 | 67.5 | 4 | −55.9 | 3.9 | 9.52 | 2.57 | −46.0 | 73.6 | 3.7 | 1 |
12 | 2N | 'l' | ≥19 | 3 | −55.6 | 3.4 | 14.08 | 4.14 | −46.0 | 73.6 | 3.3 | 1 | |
13 | 1 | '8' | 0 | 1012.1 | 27 | −57.5 | 3.3 | 11.26 | 2.02 | −52.8 | 145.1 | 6.7 | 1 |
14 | 1 | 'O' | ≥39 | 224.8 | 13 | −54.9 | 1.7 | 2.10 | 0.80 | N/A | 69.0 | 1.5 | 0 |
14 | 2 | '8' | 1 | 367.1 | 21 | −55.6 | 3.6 | 8.46 | 0.75 | −48.0 | 55.4 | 25.7 | 1 |
14 | 3 | 'O' | ≥39 | 250.2 | 19 | −55.4 | 1.7 | 1.69 | 0.45 | −48.0 | 45.1 | 3.3 | 1 |
14 | 4 | '8' | 2 | 428.4 | 25 | −55.0 | 2.3 | 1.49 | 0.22 | −43.8 | 55.3 | 6.6 | 1 |
14 | 5P | 'l' | ≥19 | 203.5 | 9 | −53.9 | 2.2 | 0.22 | 0.02 | −45.3 | 100.9 | 20.0 | 0 |
14 | 5N | 'l' | ≥19 | 9 | −53.9 | 1.7 | 2.31 | 0.22 | −45.3 | 100.9 | 34.3 | 1 | |
15 | 1 | 'O' | ≥39 | 37.6 | 4 | −60.5 | 1.5 | 0.00 | 0.00 | N/A | 40.4 | N/A | N/A |
15 | 2 | '8' | 0 | 505.6 | 26 | −54.2 | 3.8 | 7.56 | 1.00 | −47.5 | 49.9 | 15.3 | 1 |
15 | 3 | 'O' | ≥39 | 199.5 | 18 | −47.8 | 1.9 | 3.02 | 1.08 | −43.7 | 57.4 | 3.0 | 1 |
16 | 1 | 'O' | ≥39 | 99.7 | 6 | −58.8 | 2.5 | 0.50 | 0.04 | N/A | 59.8 | 29.4 | 0 |
16 | 2# | '8' | 1 | 302.6 | 20 | −57.0 | 3.2 | 8.86 | 2.48 | −50.4 | 56.8 | 3.2 | 1 |
16 | 3 | 'O' | ≥39 | 198.2 | 8 | −58.8 | 3.9 | 3.79 | 0.45 | −51.5 | 49.9 | 13.9 | 1 |
16 | 4# | '8' | 2 | 271.3 | 13 | −54.3 | 4.6 | 14.51 | 4.06 | −50.7 | 39.9 | 5.0 | 1 |
16 | 5 | 'O' | ≥39 | 306.0 | 7 | −57.4 | 3.0 | 2.67 | 0.55 | −50.6 | N/A | 5.5 | 1 |
16 | 6# | '8' | 3 | 421.1 | 17 | −61.5 | 2.9 | 3.45 | 0.42 | N/A | N/A | 23.3 | 1 |
17 | 1P | 'l' | ≥19 | 604.2 | 9 | −52.5 | 6.6 | 7.99 | 1.53 | −47.0 | 42.6 | 7.5 | 1 |
17 | 1N | 'l' | ≥19 | 10 | −52.3 | 3.6 | 21.46 | 4.64 | −47.0 | 42.6 | 11.4 | 1 | |
18 | 1 | 'O' | ≥39 | 47.3 | 4 | −61.2 | 1.7 | 0.00 | 0.00 | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A |
19 | 1 | 'O' | ≥39 | 191.9 | 4 | −56.7 | 2.6 | 0.48 | 0.03 | N/A | N/A | Inf | 0 |
19 | 2 | '8' | 0 | 221.2 | 3 | −64.3 | 1.6 | 0.00 | 0.00 | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A |
20 | 1 | 'O' | ≥39 | 154.7 | 9 | −61.6 | 1.2 | 0.00 | 0.00 | N/A | 140.6 | N/A | N/A |
20 | 2 | '8' | ≥19 | 65.2 | 3 | −63.8 | 3.0 | 0.00 | 0.00 | N/A | 196.1 | N/A | N/A |
21 | 1 | 'O' | ≥39 | 169.6 | 10 | −54.9 | 0.8 | 0.54 | 0.04 | N/A | 169.4 | 79.0 | 0 |
21 | 2 | '8' | ≥19 | 60.1 | 3 | −53.1 | 1.0 | 0.00 | 0.00 | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A |
22 | 1 | 'O' | ≥39 | 149.2 | 9 | −57.2 | 1.6 | 0.23 | 0.01 | −44.6 | 76.8 | Inf | 0 |
22 | 2 | '8' | ≥19 | 137.9 | 5 | −55.6 | 1.7 | 0.00 | 0.00 | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A |
23 | 1 | 'O' | ≥39 | 233.1 | 5 | −63.1 | 3.0 | 0.00 | 0.00 | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A |
23 | 2 | '8' | ≥19 | 206.1 | 4 | −47.4 | 2.3 | 7.49 | 2.47 | −44.3 | 183.5 | 3.1 | 1 |
24 | 1 | 'O' | ≥39 | 295.6 | 8 | −48.2 | 1.6 | 3.96 | 1.41 | −46.4 | 159.3 | 2.7 | 0 |
24 | 2 | '8' | ≥19 | 384.7 | 5 | −49.6 | 5.8 | 14.50 | 2.73 | N/A | N/A | 5.4 | 1 |
25 | 1 | 'O' | ≥39 | 175.8 | 9 | −58.1 | 2.4 | 1.17 | 0.07 | −43.2 | 85.6 | Inf | 0 |
25 | 3P | 'L' | 0 | 1214.3 | 10 | −56.4 | 1.2 | 0.09 | 0.01 | −46.0 | 80.8 | Inf | 0 |
25 | 3N | 'L' | 0 | 11 | −56.5 | 3.4 | 1.90 | 0.46 | −46.0 | 80.8 | 31.5 | 0 | |
26 | 1 | 'O' | ≥39 | 53.2 | 3 | −60.6 | 2.5 | 0.00 | 0.00 | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A |
27 | 1 | 'O' | ≥39 | 77.9 | 4 | −52.0 | 8.4 | 12.61 | 3.61 | −44.9 | 77.4 | 3.6 | 1 |
27 | 2 | '8' | ≥19 | 399.4 | 4 | −53.5 | 8.2 | 14.84 | 3.06 | −44.9 | N/A | 4.0 | 1 |
27 | 3 | 'O' | ≥39 | 105.2 | 8 | −56.4 | 3.8 | 4.26 | 0.81 | −46.6 | 67.1 | 4.3 | 1 |
27 | 4 | '8' | ≥19 | 112.8 | 6 | −57.8 | 3.9 | 0.57 | 0.03 | N/A | N/A | Inf | 0 |
28 | 2 | '8' | ≥19 | 217.2 | 4 | −55.4 | 2.5 | 0.76 | 0.06 | −41.4 | 145.7 | 23.3 | 1 |
29 | 1 | 'O' | ≥39 | 135.3 | 7 | −59.4 | 1.7 | 0.00 | 0.00 | N/A | 75.4 | N/A | N/A |
30 | 1 | 'O' | ≥39 | 141.7 | 6 | −60.9 | 1.2 | 0.00 | 0.00 | N/A | 43.4 | N/A | N/A |
31 | 2 | '8' | ≥19 | 82.4 | 3 | −56.0 | 1.5 | 0.00 | 0.00 | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A |
32 | 2 | '8' | ≥19 | 159.9 | 5 | −55.2 | 2.5 | 4.66 | 0.82 | −48.3 | 124.4 | 4.6 | 1 |
32 | 3P | 'L' | 0 | 378.1 | 14 | −53.8 | 3.0 | 11.95 | 3.29 | N/A## | 145.1 | 3.1 | 1 |
32 | 3N | 'L' | 0 | 14 | −53.7 | 2.7 | 5.43 | 1.55 | N/A## | 145.1 | 3.1 | 1 |
In addition to a specific role in forming new place fields in familiar environments (Bittner et al., 2015), CSs have been shown to trigger synaptic plasticity in general (Takahashi and Magee, 2009), so we examined the possibility that they contribute to the experience-dependent changes in activity that we observed. We checked whether CSs may contribute to the stabilization of activity by boosting subsequent activity and spatial correlation scores. We aligned each lap’s activity to the lap containing the first CS in each epoch (whether novel or familiar). For comparison to epochs that did not contain CSs, we aligned laps in those epochs to the lap with the largest peak subthreshold Vm (since CSs tend to occur at the most depolarized Vm values, Figure 6—figure supplement 1E). We found a transient, but significant, increase in AP rate in the first lap after the first CS (Figure 6C, top), but not in subsequent peak subthreshold Vm activity (Figure 6C, bottom). Regarding spatial tuning, we found a transient increase in AP rate and subthreshold Vm spatial correlation scores after the first CS, though a similar result for the subthreshold Vm spatial correlation was found following the largest peak Vm for maze epochs without CSs (Figure 6D). Therefore, individual CSs appear to contribute an amount to plasticity and spatial learning that varies in magnitude – from small (i.e., transient increases in spiking and spatial correlation) to potentially large (i.e., occurring in the first lap of new place fields).
Discussion
Using intracellular recording, we have investigated the mechanisms underlying how hippocampal spatial representations form and stabilize with experience. We began by testing a model of this process that was based on previous experimental and theoretical work (Figure 1). In summary (Figure 7), we found that the inputs driving place field spiking did indeed increase in amplitude during the exploration of a novel environment (Figure 3). Furthermore, this growth could occur before spiking occurred, suggesting a role for plasticity mechanisms that do not depend on postsynaptic spiking (Golding et al., 2002; Dudman et al., 2007). However, these increases in input amplitude were not sustained after additional experience in the environment, suggesting the need for alternate mechanisms of establishing stable place field activity in familiar environments. We then found an experience-dependent change that was maintained with familiarization – an increase in the similarity of the spatial tuning of inputs across the entire extent of the environment (Figure 5). This is a signature of the long-term stabilization of the hippocampal representation and provides a foundation for reliable spatial firing in familiar environments.

Summary of intracellular features underlying the formation of hippocampal CA1 place cell representations.
Schematic based on findings from this study (compare to Figure 1). During novel exploration (left), the subthreshold Vm depolarization underlying place fields grows in amplitude. This increase in amplitude is not sustained as the environment becomes more familiar (right). However, the spatial tuning of inputs, which is initially lower in all locations of novel mazes, stabilizes during novel exploration, both inside and outside of the place field. The higher AP rate in novel environments is due to the larger amplitude subthreshold depolarizations, as no differences were observed in the baseline Vm or somatic excitability (e.g., AP threshold). CSs occur in a fraction of novel maze epochs and the appearance of place fields in novel environments does not require CSs. Thus, stable representations of familiar mazes are supported by the emergence of less strongly modulated, but more repeatable, spatially tuned subthreshold inputs.
We found that complex spikes (CSs), which can induce synaptic plasticity (Takahashi and Magee, 2009) and are sufficient to trigger place field formation in familiar environments (Bittner et al., 2015), were not required for the appearance of individual place fields during novel exploration (e.g., Figures 2B, 3D and 6A). However, temporary increases in firing rate were seen in the laps subsequent to CS occurrence (Figure 6C). This suggests that CS-based plasticity could contribute to the long-term stabilization of place fields, perhaps in a similar fashion to other large, calcium-mediated events (Sheffield and Dombeck, 2015). This would be consistent with experiments in which NMDAR-dependent plasticity is not necessary for the formation of place fields in novel environments (McHugh et al., 1996; Kentros et al., 1998), but rather for their persistence across days (Kentros et al., 1998). Note that CSs could be more potent triggers for place field formation in proximal cue-based environments (Bittner et al., 2015) versus the distal cue-based environments of our study.
While inputs and spiking were boosted during novel exploration, we found no differences in the baseline Vm and somatic excitability between novel and familiar environments (Figure 4A–D and Figure 4—figure supplement 1). However, across different cells recorded in novel environments, firing rates were inversely correlated with AP threshold (Figure 4E), in agreement with previous work showing that cells with higher excitability are more likely to display place field activity during novel exploration (Epsztein et al., 2011; Rich et al., 2014).
Altogether, our findings support the following model of hippocampal spatial learning: (1) initially, small inhomogeneities in spatial inputs and excitability present during the first experience in an environment are amplified to produce robust place field activity without the need for plasticity; (2) plasticity during repeated traversals of the environment progressively refines and stabilizes this representation for long-term storage; (3) after learning, the inputs are no longer amplified, but the stability of these less strongly modulated inputs yields reliable spatially tuned firing. That is, the role of plasticity is to capture the initial representation and stabilize it before the network changes again, perhaps as a result of learning in other contexts in intervening periods. Plasticity would presumably stabilize CA1 place fields that happened to receive sufficiently strong, consistent, and/or matched (e.g., both EC and CA3) inputs (Dudman et al., 2007; Sheffield and Dombeck, 2015; Bittner et al., 2015; Basu et al., 2016). The initial expansion of activity (i.e., greater numbers of more active cells) could provide a diversity of synaptic and cellular patterns from which a sparser representation of space can be selected for long-term stabilization. The amplification of spiking activity could also serve to drive plasticity in downstream targets (Lisman, 1997; Harris et al., 2001; Xu et al., 2012) or send a general novelty signal (Duncan et al., 2012; Larkin et al., 2014) to the rest of the brain, which in turn may drive neuromodulatory feedback to the hippocampus and promote learning (Lisman and Grace, 2005). Afterwards, the comparatively low amplitude of inputs and firing rate in familiar environments may function to limit changes to the representation, further contributing to its stability. Additional changes, such as the creation of new fields via CSs (Bittner et al., 2015) at behaviorally important locations (Hollup et al., 2001; Dupret et al., 2010; Monaco et al., 2014), would presumably occur within a now stable spatial context. Variability of the low amplitude, but spatially reliable, modulation of inputs across the environment could explain why a given place cell is not always active in the same environment, but that when it fires, it does so in the same location (Ziv et al., 2013; Rubin et al., 2015).
What might underlie the amplification of inputs during novel spatial exploration? To begin with, there could be increased activity in input areas. However, the evidence to date indicates no difference in the average firing rates of excitatory neurons in CA3 (Karlsson and Frank, 2008) or EC (Barry et al., 2012) in novel compared to familiar environments. Whether there are differences in the activity of other excitatory inputs to CA1, such as the nucleus reuniens (Herkenham, 1978; Vertes, 2015), frontal cortex (Rajasethupathy et al., 2015), or the amygdala (Sheth et al., 2008) is currently unknown.
There are also multiple means by which the same excitatory input activity could result in larger amplitude depolarizations. Differences in the relative timing of inputs within and/or between, for example, CA3 and EC (Dudman et al., 2007; Penley et al., 2013; Bittner et al., 2015) could boost integration. The larger subthreshold theta oscillations we observed could reflect such altered timing, but could also simply be due to the increased peak Vm values in novel mazes combined with the observed relationship between intracellular theta power and Vm (Kamondi et al., 1998; Lee et al., 2012) (which could be caused by an increased driving force with respect to the inhibitory reversal potential). In addition, altered timing alone would not necessarily lead to the larger peak subthreshold responses we measured, as they involved the mean Vm over many theta cycles.
Several types of changes in excitability could increase the size of the subthreshold response to a given input. First, a more depolarized somatic baseline Vm can greatly enhance the subthreshold response of CA1 pyramidal neurons to spatial inputs (Lee et al., 2012, 2014). However, as mentioned above, we found no evidence for differences in baseline Vm (or input resistance) that could account for the larger peak subthreshold Vm responses in novel mazes. While we did not detect differences in somatic excitability, changes in inhibition (Lovett-Barron et al., 2012; Royer et al., 2012; Milstein et al., 2015) or intrinsic conductances (Johnston et al., 2003; Johnston and Narayanan, 2008; Giessel and Sabatini, 2010) in dendrites could alter the response to synaptic inputs. Neuromodulators such as acetylcholine (Hasselmo, 2006; Teles-Grilo Ruivo and Mellor, 2013) and dopamine (Li et al., 2003; Kentros et al., 2004; McNamara et al., 2014; Takeuchi et al., 2016) could underlie a novelty-specific enhancement of inputs. For instance, neuromodulators have been shown to affect the gain of receptive field responses (Fu et al., 2014). An analogous mechanism could be at work here, though with a difference; the gain change shown in recent work occurs when comparing active and quiescent behavior (Chiappe et al., 2010; Maimon et al., 2010; Niell and Stryker, 2010; Bennett et al., 2013; Polack et al., 2013; Fu et al., 2014; Schneider et al., 2014). In our case, the difference in responsiveness would occur between two similar active behaviors – running at similar speeds in environments differentiated only by the degree of familiarity. On the other hand, unlike what could be expected from an overall change in gain, we did not observe a difference in the amplitude of the second highest subthreshold Vm peaks in novel compared to familiar environments.
Instead of an amplification of inputs in novel environments, alternate mechanisms such as habituation or synaptic depression (Xu et al., 1998; Kemp and Manahan-Vaughan, 2007) could decrease response magnitudes across days (Lim et al., 2015). In such models (Lim et al., 2015), the larger response to novel versus familiar items occurs in a bottom-up fashion and thus could occur without a novelty-specific signal. In any case, the increasing lap-to-lap similarity of the subthreshold Vm indicates that some form of plasticity underlies the formation of stable representations of familiar environments.
The correlation between spike rate and AP threshold in novel but not familiar environments (Figure 4E) is consistent with a model in which initial differences in intrinsic excitability bias which cells will be originally recruited into a given memory representation, with changes in excitability over time resulting in the allocation of different sets of cells to subsequent experiences (Han et al., 2007; Zhou et al., 2009; Epsztein et al., 2011; Cai et al., 2016; Rashid et al., 2016). In particular, the lack of correlation between firing rate and threshold in familiar environments suggests that, after the stabilization of a representation, inputs determine activity irrespective of excitability levels. Furthermore, the correlation between firing rate and baseline Vm in familiar environments (Figure 4F) suggests that learning leads to the emergence of a cell assembly (Hebb, 1949) representing each maze, resulting in a spatially uniform subthreshold depolarizing bias for all place cells active in a given environment. In this case, the lower amplitude of the Vm hills under place fields in familiar mazes may not represent weaker inputs. Rather, the inputs driving place fields may have remained strengthened, but the out-of-field inputs may also have strengthened with familiarization, thus reducing the amplitude of the hill relative to the baseline. Meanwhile, the higher firing rate of inhibitory neurons in CA1 in familiar environments (Wilson and McNaughton, 1993; Frank et al., 2004; Nitz and McNaughton, 2004) could reduce the Vm everywhere, keeping the average cell’s baseline Vm similar to that in novel environments, thereby masking the presence of these strengthened synapses.
Materials and methods
Virtual Reality software and behavioral setup
Request a detailed protocolOur custom virtual reality software was developed at the HHMI Janelia Research Campus as part of Janelia’s open-source virtual reality software platform (Jovian). Our software suite, named ‘MouseoVeR’, is written in C++ and built from a number of open-source software components (Boost, Bullet, osgBullet, osgWorks, OpenSceneGraph, Collada, OpenGL, and Qt) that work together to generate a system allowing users to create configurable visual environments. The MouseoVeR code used in this study is available at https://github.com/JaneliaSciComp/CohenBolstadLee_eLife2017. Virtual maze environments were created using the open-source animation software Blender (www.blender.org) and rendered by MouseoVeR. Blender environments were rendered using three virtual camera objects located at a single point in space. One camera faced forwards and captured a field of view (FOV) of 75°. The two other cameras were identical but rotated ±75° to create a total azimuthal FOV of 225°. For display, the three images were rear-projected onto a 30 in diameter, ~40% translucent cylindrical screen to create a final image encompassing a total FOV of 20° below, 60° above, and ±112.5° lateral with respect to the location of head fixation (Figure 2A, top). The cylindrical screen surrounded the spherical treadmill – a large and hollow lightweight polystyrene sphere (16 in diameter, 65 g) resting on a bed of ten individually air-cushioned ping-pong balls in an acrylic frame (http://www.flintbox.com/public/project/26501/). To track the motion of the treadmill, two cameras separated by 90° were positioned at the equator and focused on 4 mm2 regions under infrared light (modified from FlyFizz, https://openwiki.janelia.org/wiki/display/flyfizz/Home; Seelig et al., 2010). The cameras captured 30 × 30 pixel images of the treadmill surface at 4 kHz. Motion of the treadmill was computed from the accumulated differences in the images over time. A brief description of the MouseoVeR processing loop is as follows. In each iteration of the rendering loop, MouseoVeR communicates with the treadmill’s data server to retrieve the updated motion values since the last request. The values coming from the data server are defined as Euler angles in the server’s coordinate space. For calibration, we created mappings between 180° rotations of the treadmill along each of its three rotational axes (roll, pitch, and yaw) in real-world space and the data server’s coordinate space. For converting real into virtual behavior, we ignored any yaw rotations and extracted from pitch and roll values a rotational direction as well as a magnitude of rotation (which was converted into an arc length using the treadmill radius). Manually (i.e., animal-) controlled heading direction was derived from the treadmill’s pitch-to-roll ratio by continuously sampling, smoothing (~500 ms), and converting it into a °/s turning rate. The motion vector was sent to the physics engine to compute collisions with virtual objects (e.g., maze walls), and the resulting virtual movement was rendered for display at a synchronized projector frame rate of 30 Hz.
Behavioral training
Request a detailed protocolEight-to-twelve week-old male C57BL/6NCrl (Charles River Laboratories, Wilmington, MA) mice were used for all experiments. All procedures were performed in accordance with the Janelia Research Campus Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee guidelines on animal welfare. Prior to behavioral training, a stainless steel head plate – with a large central recording well to access the hippocampus bilaterally – was attached to the skull surface using light-cured adhesives (Optibond All-in-One, Kerr; Charisma, Heraeus Kulzer, South Bend, IN) and dental acrylic. Sites for future hippocampal CA1 craniotomies (bregma: −1.6 to −2.0 mm AP, 1.2 to 1.8 mm ML) were marked with a fine-tipped cautery pen (Medline Inc., Northfield, IL). Mice were allowed to recover for at least five days before behavioral training started. During recovery, the mice were given food and water ad libitum, and a saucer running wheel was placed in the home cage.
The day before starting behavioral training, mice were placed on water-restriction (1.0 ml/day). From this day onwards, the saucer wheel was removed from the home cage overnight. Body weight and overall health were checked each day to ensure the mice remained healthy over the course of the experiment (Guo et al., 2014). On day 1, mice were acclimated to the experimental setup and head fixation procedure over several short sessions (increasing from ~1 to 15 min). The head was centered and fixed atop the treadmill, with the eyes ~20 mm from the surface. This created a comfortable ~20° angle below horizontal between the skull surface and the neck-to-tail body line. A tube (2.0 mm O.D. thick-walled glass with silicone-coated tip) was positioned near the mouth for delivery of artificially sweetened water rewards (acesulfame potassium, 4 mM, Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO;~2 µl liquid drop per reward). Initially, mice earned rewards by simply licking the reward tube, and then later, by producing forward motions on the treadmill of increasing distances. Mice were shown the initial virtual maze, but motion in the maze was disabled and their view was restricted to that of the start location.
On day 2, mice were exposed to the initial virtual maze for a total of ~1 hr over three training sessions (~10, 20, and 30 min durations). Four 1-D virtual mazes were used in the study (Figure 2A, middle). Two mazes were closed paths: an oval- (~175 cm length) and a figure-8-shaped track (~240 cm length). Two mazes were bidirectional: an approximately straight track (~160 cm in each direction) and an L-shaped track (~135 cm in each direction). Mice were trained to explore the virtual mazes by navigating in accordance with the geometry of the maze (e.g., heading to the right when the path veers to the right). Rewards were earned as the mice completed laps around the virtual environments. For closed path mazes, a single location was selected as the primary reward zone where two rewards were always delivered after completing a lap. In addition, ~1 reward was delivered per lap in a random location for additional motivation. For bidirectional mazes, the primary reward zone for each direction was located at the corresponding end of the track where either 1 or 2 rewards were dispensed. In addition, ~1 reward was delivered at a random location per direction. If a mouse consumed less than 1.0 ml of liquid across all training sessions in a day, a supplement of water was provided to ensure at least 1.0 ml total was received each day. In the early stages of training, head direction was partially automated by MouseoVeR to ensure that the mice could successfully navigate all regions of the mazes. As performance and skill on the spherical treadmill improved, we reduced the contribution of the automated heading component and correspondingly increased the manually controlled turning component. This increased the contribution of the subject’s running behavior to the control of head direction.
On days 3–5, mice continued to explore the same virtual maze over 2–3 training sessions per day, for a total of ~1 hr of in-maze time per day. On day 6, mice were exposed to a second, unique virtual maze, in addition to the initial maze, which was now familiar. For days 6–10, mice were trained to explore and become familiar with the second maze and the transitions between the two mazes. On day 11, after a minimum of 5 days of training in each of the virtual mazes, mice were considered ready for experimental recordings. From day 11 onwards, each mouse explored familiar and novel mazes during recording on 3–4 days, interleaved with additional training sessions or rest days.
Electrophysiology
Request a detailed protocolOn recording days, trained mice were anesthetized with isoflurane (~1.5%,~0.8 l/min flow rate) and placed in a modified stereotaxic frame that clamps the head plate. If this was the first recording day from that hemisphere, a craniotomy (~1 mm2) was performed over dorsal CA1. Dura removal was performed with the assistance of collagenase (1 mg/ml in 1x PBS with 1.5 mM Ca, Collagenase type I, Sigma-Aldrich). Collagenase was applied for ~10–15 min, followed by application of bovine serum albumin solution (1 mg/ml in 1x PBS, Sigma Aldrich) for ~5 min, and finally rinsed with saline. A glass recording pipette (~2 MΩ) filled with saline was lowered into the brain and used to monitor the extracellular local field potential and unit activity in order to accurately map the depth of the dorsal CA1 pyramidal cell layer. Once the depth of the pyramidal layer was determined, the craniotomy was cleaned and rinsed with saline. To protect the brain during the recovery period, we created a sealed environment by filling a well surrounding the craniotomy with saline that was closed off with a cap lined with silicone (Kwik-Cast sealant, WPI, Sarasota, FL). All animals were mobile within a few minutes after being placed in their home cages. After a minimum of 1 hr of recovery time, mice were placed on the treadmill. Blind in vivo whole-cell recordings were obtained from the right or left dorsal CA1 pyramidal cell layer (Lee et al., 2009, 2014) using recording pipettes (5–7 MΩ) filled with an intracellular solution containing (in mM) K-gluconate 135, HEPES 10, Na2-phosphocreatine 10, KCl 4, MgATP 4, and Na3GTP 0.3 (pH adjusted to 7.2 with KOH) as well as biocytin (0.2%). After a gigaseal was obtained, the seal was held for up to ~4 min with no positive pressure to let the network recover before breaking into the membrane. After achieving the whole-cell configuration, the VR display was turned on and the mice were free to explore the mazes for sweetened water rewards as on training days. However, on recording days, in addition to the previously explored familiar mazes, mice could be exposed to a novel, previously unencountered, maze. Current-clamp measurements of Vm (amplifier low-pass filter set to 5 kHz) were sampled at 25 kHz. The animal’s behavior in the virtual maze was sampled at 30 Hz (the projector frame rate). All data analyzed is from recording periods with no holding current or no more than −20 pA of negative holding current applied to the pipette. Recordings were not corrected for the liquid junction potential. All neurons included in this study had the electrophysiological characteristics of somatic CA1 pyramidal whole-cell recordings (Lee et al., 2009; Epsztein et al., 2011; Lee et al., 2012, 2014). To minimize the number of animals used, we attempted to maximize the number of recordings per animal. Due to recording from multiple cells within a single animal across a window of ~5–10 days, we did not attempt to recover the histology of the recorded cells.
Data analysis
Request a detailed protocolAll analysis was done using custom-written programs in Matlab. All novel versus familiar maze epoch comparisons as well as other comparisons (e.g., initial versus late lap amplitudes within epochs) were done using non-parametric paired (Wilcoxon signed-rank) and unpaired (Mann-Whitney U) tests assuming unequal variances, unless otherwise noted. Correspondingly, all numerical values in the text are reported as the median ± standard error of the median, unless otherwise noted, and boxplots show the 25–75%-iles and the median (horizontal black line). (Standard error of the median was computed as follows. From the sample population containing n samples, we drew n samples with replacement to create a sample group and then computed the median of the sample group. This was repeated 10,000 times to obtain 10,000 sample group medians. Finally, we computed the standard deviation of the sample group medians to obtain the standard error.) Pearson’s linear correlation coefficient was used to assess correlations between features, with significance determined with respect to the hypothesis that there was no correlation. All P-values reported are 2-sided. A value of p<0.05 was defined as statistically significant. Only temporally adjacent novel and familiar mazes were included in the analysis of NOV-FAM maze pairs. For example, if the order of presentation of mazes during the recording protocol was (1) FAM, (2) NOV, (3) FAM’, and (4) FAM2, then 2 NOV-FAM maze pairs were considered for analysis: FAM-NOV and NOV-FAM’. Analysis was limited to data collected during periods when the animal’s speed was ≥5 cm/s unless otherwise noted. The choice of dividing each epoch into early and late periods between laps 5 and 6 was based on previous extracellular studies showing that a substantial amount of the change in place field spiking activity during an epoch occurs within the first five laps (Mehta et al., 2000; Ekstrom et al., 2001).
Thirty-two cells were recorded from 15 mice navigating in virtual mazes. A maze was considered novel if it was the first day that the animal had encountered that maze (with the exception of 3 cases in which the animal had encountered the maze once on a previous day). For all familiar maze epochs, the animal had previously explored the maze a minimum of 19 times over the course of at least 5 days. Cells were active (where ‘active’ was defined as follows: peak AP firing rate as a function of location averaged over the maze epoch ≥0.5 Hz and spiking in ≥2 laps) in 47% of epochs in familiar mazes (21/45 epochs, 15/29 cells) and 69% of epochs in novel mazes (9/13 epochs, 8/12 cells). If active epochs were instead defined as those with an overall mean AP firing rate >0.1 Hz, the results were similar (NOV: 9/13, FAM: 18/45). The ~1.5 fold larger fraction of active epochs in novel environments agrees with the ~1.4 fold increased fraction of active dorsal CA1 pyramidal cells in rats exploring real-world novel versus familiar mazes (Karlsson and Frank, 2008).
Subthreshold membrane potential and AP firing rate
Request a detailed protocolThe locations in the virtual mazes were linearized by collapsing them onto a long curve that went around the track, giving a 1-D representation of animal location. For display purposes, the beginning (position = 0 cm) and end (position =~135–240 cm) of the curve, which represent the same location, was chosen based on where the cellular activity was approximately the lowest across all time periods in the maze epoch (overall epoch average). Movement from zero to the end represents a full lap in the maze. The subthreshold Vm trace was estimated from the raw Vm trace as previously described (Epsztein et al., 2011; Lee et al., 2012). Briefly, all APs and any parts of the raw Vm trace directly attributable to the somatic spikes themselves (e.g., after-depolarizations) were removed, as well as the entirety of the slow, large, putatively calcium-based depolarization that often follows a burst of APs (which thus includes removing the entirety of each CS). The remaining trace was then linearly interpolated across the resulting gaps, yielding the subthreshold Vm. The overall epoch mean AP firing rate and subthreshold Vm as a function of the animal’s virtual location were determined every 4 cm (‘spatial bin’) along the track, here using 12 cm-wide boxcar smoothing. The peak AP rate and peak subthreshold Vm were defined as the maximum spatially binned values. The baseline Vm (Figure 4B) was computed as the mean of the subthreshold Vm values for the 10% of spatial bins with lowest subthreshold Vm. Similar operations were performed for individual laps to derive the AP rate, subthreshold Vm, and peak and baseline Vm values for each lap. For each maze epoch, only directions in which the animal sampled each location on at least two different laps during the recording were considered for analysis. For bidirectional mazes, cellular activity in the direction that contained the largest overall epoch peak subthreshold Vm response was selected for comparisons between NOV-FAM maze pairs. For analysis of place fields or activity across laps within an epoch, each direction was included independently. Some laps were eliminated from analysis because of holding current outside of the −20 to 0 pA range, or because of within-lap continuous baseline shifts (e.g., possibly due to residual dialysis even after waiting ~4 min after seal formation before breaking in, or from sudden brain movements relative to the pipette, etc.). The original absolute lap numbers were used for analysis when intervening laps were eliminated.
Determination of the place field region and amplitude of the subthreshold Vm hill
Request a detailed protocolPlace fields were determined for each epoch and direction as follows. From the epoch average firing rate map, the candidate place field was defined as the region containing the peak AP firing rate and contiguous bins with rate greater than or equal to the baseline rate plus 20% of the difference between the peak and baseline rate (where the baseline is the mean rate in the 10% of bins with lowest rate, which is generally 0 Hz). Then, candidate place fields with peak rate ≥0.5 Hz, at least 2 laps of spiking in-field, and an in-field/out-field average firing rate ratio ≥3 were classified as place fields. Unless noted as referring to the place field, the field was determined from the overall mean subthreshold Vm as a function of position in the epoch as follows. From this subthreshold function, the baseline Vm (Figure 4B) was determined as was the spatial bin with peak subthreshold Vm. The set of contiguous position bins around this peak bin where the mean subthreshold Vm was greater than or equal to the baseline Vm plus 30% of the difference between the peak and baseline Vm was defined as the inside of the field. The amplitude of the subthreshold hill was computed by subtracting the baseline Vm from the peak value of the mean subthreshold Vm. Similar operations were performed for each lap to derive within-lap baseline (Figure 4—figure supplement 1B) and peak (Figure 3—figure supplement 1A,right and 1C) Vm values.
Determination of the spatial response amplitude of AP firing rate and subthreshold Vm for experience-dependent analysis
Request a detailed protocolThe in-field peak spatial activity in each lap within a maze epoch (Figure 3A and J) was determined as follows. For each maze epoch the peak AP rate and subthreshold Vm value in each lap were determined from the region surrounding (±3 spatial bins) the location of the overall epoch peak AP rate and subthreshold Vm, respectively. Individual lap peak values from this in-field region were then averaged across a specific set of laps (e.g., laps 1–2 for the ‘initial laps’ in Figure 3A), or averaged across all laps to obtain a mean value for the maze epoch (Figure 3G–H). Individual lap peak values were also determined irrespective of location (‘lap peak’), then collapsed across laps to obtain a mean value for the maze epoch (Figure 3—figure supplement 1B–C). For peak subthreshold Vm activity in each lap within a maze epoch with respect to the place field (Figure 3B, E–F and I), we took the peak subthreshold Vm value in each lap inside the place field region determined from the overall epoch AP activity. Individual lap peak values from this place field region were then averaged across a specific set of laps (e.g., laps 1–2 for the ‘initial laps’ in Figure 3B), or averaged across all laps to obtain a mean value for the maze epoch (Figure 3I). In the case that one of the epochs in the maze pair (e.g. in Figure 3I) was active but did not have a place field or was silent, values for that epoch were computed with respect to the region surrounding (±3 spatial bins) the location of the overall epoch peak subthreshold Vm.
To assess the effect of CSs on peak spatial activity within a maze epoch (Figure 6C), for all cells recorded in a FAM or a NOV maze, and for maze epochs that contained a CS, we first determined the location (lap number and spatial bin) of the first CS in the epoch. Then for each lap, we determined the peak AP rate and subthreshold Vm in the region surrounding (±3 spatial bins) the CS location. These lap peak activity values were then aligned to the lap (lap = 0) that contained the first CS. Similarly, for comparison with maze epochs that did not contain a CS, we determined the lap number and spatial bin containing the peak individual lap subthreshold Vm value. Then for each lap, we found the peak activity values within the region surrounding this subthreshold Vm value.
Spatial correlation of the AP firing rate, subthreshold Vm, and speed profile for experience-dependent analysis
Request a detailed protocolFor Figure 5, Figure 5—figure supplements 1A–C and and 2, for maze epochs in which there were at least 4 laps of activity, the stability of spatial tuning of the AP firing rate and, separately, subthreshold Vm was determined for each lap as follows. A lap correlation score was determined by computing the Pearson’s correlation coefficient between the activity across each location (i.e., each spatial bin in the maze) within the lap with the overall epoch activity. A mean maze correlation score was then determined by taking the mean of the individual lap scores. For bidirectional mazes, the direction with the largest amplitude overall epoch peak subthreshold Vm response was selected for comparison.
To assess the effect of CSs on spatial tuning within a maze epoch (Figure 6D), for all cells recorded in a FAM or a NOV maze, and for maze epochs that contained a CS, we first determined the lap number of the first CS in the epoch. For each lap, the spatial correlation scores were computed as described above and aligned to the lap (lap = 0) that contained the first CS. Similarly, for comparison with maze epochs that did not contain a CS, we determined the lap number containing the peak individual lap subthreshold Vm value and lap correlation scores were then aligned to that lap.
To assess the animal’s speed profile for each lap (Figure 5—figure supplement 1D), the spatial correlation was taken between each lap’s speed profile and the average speed profile for the overall epoch. For Figure 5—figure supplement 1E, for every pair of laps in a novel epoch, a spatial correlation between the subthreshold Vm of the 2 laps and between the speed profile of the 2 laps was computed. Then the correlation of these values across all pairs of laps in an epoch was computed. The distribution (i.e., median) of the correlation values (one per epoch) across all epochs was then compared to 0. In a related analysis, the relationship between the instantaneous speed of the animal and the instantaneous subthreshold Vm value was determined as follows. For each novel and familiar epoch, the correlation between instantaneous speed and corresponding subthreshold Vm was determined for all contiguous timestamps associated with speed >5 cm/s (which is the same speed threshold as is used in all the analysis). The distribution of correlation values (one per epoch) across all epochs was then compared to 0.
Spectral analysis and standard deviation of the subthreshold Vm
Request a detailed protocolFor Figure 3—figure supplement 2, each maze epoch (and each lap) was separated into periods when the animal was inside versus outside the field for that epoch. In the case of the theta (5–10 Hz) band, the subthreshold Vm trace was first reduced to a sampling rate of 500 Hz via decimation. Short-time Fourier transform (STFT) methods were applied to compute the average power in the 5–10 Hz band. Note that ~0.25 s of data were excluded before and after each of the crossing times between the inside and outside of the field to eliminate contamination due to the temporal extent of the STFT window. In the case of the gamma (25–100 Hz) band, a 25–160 Hz band-pass and 60 Hz notch filter were applied to the subthreshold Vm trace, the sampling rate was reduced to 2 kHz, and the STFT of the resulting trace was taken. Then, the periods 60 ms before to 60 ms after each AP and intracellular complex spike burst were removed, as well as 60 ms of data before and after each crossing time between the inside and outside of the field, and the power in the 25–100 Hz band was averaged over the remaining periods inside the field. For the standard deviation of the subthreshold Vm trace, a 1–160 Hz band-pass and a 60 Hz notch filter were applied, and periods 17 ms before to 17 ms after each AP and complex spike burst as well as crossing times were removed. Then, the standard deviation over the remaining periods inside the field was computed.
Determination of spontaneous AP threshold
Request a detailed protocolThe spontaneous AP threshold (Figure 4A) for a given maze epoch was determined as previously described (Epsztein et al., 2011). Briefly, for each AP, we set the threshold to be the Vm value at which the dV/dt crossed 10 V/s (or 0.33 × the peak dV/dt of that AP, whichever was lower, in order to handle the slower APs that occurred later within bursts and CSs) on its way to the AP peak Vm. Note that the threshold for individual APs varies with the level of depolarization before the AP (Figure 4—figure supplement 1A), and therefore we determined the threshold from a subset of APs that did not have APs immediately preceding them (i.e., isolated spikes and first spikes of bursts), that occurred during less depolarized periods, that did not possess a dV/dt shoulder (Epsztein et al., 2010), and that were not a part of a CS.
Determination of input resistance
Request a detailed protocolDuring maze exploration, we injected two 100-ms-long hyperpolarizing current steps of −0.2 nA separated by 500 ms every 20 s through the recording pipette (Figure 4C, left). To determine the mean input resistance (RN) within a maze epoch (Figure 4C), we first eliminated any Vm responses to the current steps that were masked by large spontaneous fluctuations, averaged the remaining responses during the epoch, and then applied a previously described procedure (Crochet and Petersen, 2006) to the average response. The specific steps used were as previously described (Epsztein et al., 2011).
Analysis of evoked number of APs
Request a detailed protocolDuring maze exploration, we injected one 100-ms-long depolarizing current step of +0.1–0.2 nA every 20 s through the recording pipette (Figure 4C, left). For each maze epoch, a single stimulus intensity was chosen to evoke APs, and the mean number of APs evoked per stimulus was normalized to the median value across all maze epochs in which the same stimulus intensity was used. For the comparison of evoked activity (Figure 4D), we only included data if the amplitude of the current step was the same for the FAM and NOV epochs of the maze pair.
Shape of subthreshold Vm hill under place field
Request a detailed protocolFor Figure 5—figure supplement 4, the overall epoch AP rate and subthreshold Vm per lap as a function of position were determined every 2 cm along the track using 12 cm-wide boxcar smoothing for each epoch with a clear place field. From the AP rate function, the baseline was determined (mean of the AP rate values for the 10% of spatial bins with lowest AP rate, generally 0 Hz) as was the spatial bin with peak AP rate. The set of contiguous position bins around this peak bin where the mean AP rate was greater than or equal to the baseline plus 30% of the difference between the peak and baseline was defined as the inside of the AP rate field. The subthreshold Vm from each lap was taken for positions starting one-fourth of the width of the AP rate field before the start of the (AP rate) field to one-fourth of the field width after the end of the field, then averaged across the desired set of laps (e.g., laps 1–2 for the ‘initial’ period). Each such curve was interpolated at 60 evenly spaced position values, normalized in amplitude (0 = minimum subthreshold Vm value, 1 = maximum) and normalized with respect to the position along the field (0 to 1 for the inside of the field, −0.25 to 1.25 including the regions on each side of the field, and flipped if necessary so that the animal’s running direction was from −0.25 to 1.25), then averaged across the different epoch’s fields (mean ± SE).
Complex spike occurrence
Request a detailed protocolClassification of events as complex spikes (CSs) was done as previously described (Epsztein et al., 2011). Note that for the purposes of determining the location at which a CS occurred, we set the time of occurrence to be the time of the peak of the first AP in the CS. To assess the degree of association between novel place field formation and CSs, we considered all CSs whether or not they occurred when the animal was moving above or below the threshold speed (5 cm/s) and whether or not they occurred spontaneously or were triggered (i.e., evoked inadvertently by the 100-ms-long depolarizing current steps). In all other analyses involving CSs, we only included CSs that occurred spontaneously and when the animal was moving above the threshold speed.
References
-
Synaptic modifications in cultured hippocampal neurons: dependence on spike timing, synaptic strength, and postsynaptic cell typeJournal of Neuroscience 18:10464–10472.
-
Conjunctive input processing drives feature selectivity in hippocampal CA1 neuronsNature Neuroscience 18:1133–1142.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4062
-
A model of spatial map formation in the Hippocampus of the ratNeural Computation 8:85–93.https://doi.org/10.1162/neco.1996.8.1.85
-
Experience-dependent increase in CA1 place cell spatial information, but not spatial reproducibility, is dependent on the autophosphorylation of the alpha-isoform of the calcium/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase IIJournal of Neuroscience 27:7854–7859.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1704-07.2007
-
Walking modulates speed sensitivity in Drosophila motion visionCurrent Biology 20:1470–1475.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2010.06.072
-
Correlating whisker behavior with membrane potential in barrel cortex of awake miceNature Neuroscience 9:608–610.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1690
-
The reorganization and reactivation of hippocampal maps predict spatial memory performanceNature Neuroscience 13:995–1002.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2599
-
Hippocampal plasticity across multiple days of exposure to novel environmentsJournal of Neuroscience 24:7681–7689.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1958-04.2004
-
The role of acetylcholine in learning and memoryCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology 16:710–715.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2006.09.002
-
The connections of the nucleus reuniens thalami: evidence for a direct thalamo-hippocampal pathway in the ratThe Journal of Comparative Neurology 177:589–609.https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.901770405
-
First occurrence of hippocampal spatial firing in a new environmentExperimental Neurology 62:282–297.https://doi.org/10.1016/0014-4886(78)90058-4
-
Accumulation of hippocampal place fields at the goal location in an annular watermaze taskJournal of Neuroscience 21:1635–1644.
-
Rats are able to navigate in virtual environmentsJournal of Experimental Biology 208:561–569.https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.01371
-
Active dendrites, potassium channels and synaptic plasticityPhilosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 358:667–674.https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2002.1248
-
Active dendrites: colorful wings of the mysterious butterfliesTrends in Neurosciences 31:309–316.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2008.03.004
-
Electrophysiology of hippocampal neurons. II. After-potentials and repetitive firingJournal of Neurophysiology 24:243–259.
-
Network dynamics underlying the formation of sparse, informative representations in the HippocampusJournal of Neuroscience 28:14271–14281.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4261-08.2008
-
Hippocampal long-term depression: master or minion in declarative memory processes?Trends in Neurosciences 30:111–118.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2007.01.002
-
Head-anchored whole-cell recordings in freely moving ratsNature Protocols 4:385–392.https://doi.org/10.1038/nprot.2009.5
-
Inferring learning rules from distributions of firing rates in cortical neuronsNature Neuroscience 18:1804–1810.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4158
-
Bursts as a unit of neural information: making unreliable synapses reliableTrends in Neurosciences 20:38–43.https://doi.org/10.1016/S0166-2236(96)10070-9
-
Regulation of neuronal input transformations by tunable dendritic inhibitionNature Neuroscience 15:423–430.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3024
-
Active flight increases the gain of visual motion processing in DrosophilaNature Neuroscience 13:393–399.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2492
-
Dopaminergic neurons promote hippocampal reactivation and spatial memory persistenceNature Neuroscience 17:1658–1660.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3843
-
Attentive scanning behavior drives one-trial potentiation of hippocampal place fieldsNature Neuroscience 17:725–731.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3687
-
Synaptic plasticity and learning: selective impairment of learning rats and blockade of long-term potentiation in vivo by the N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor antagonist AP5Journal of Neuroscience 9:3040–3057.
-
The effects of changes in the environment on the spatial firing of hippocampal complex-spike cellsJournal of Neuroscience 7:1951–1968.
-
Differential modulation of CA1 and dentate gyrus interneurons during exploration of novel environmentsJournal of Neurophysiology 91:863–872.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00614.2003
-
Hippocampal place units in the freely moving rat: why they fire where they fireExperimental Brain Research 31:573–590.https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00239813
-
Novel space alters theta and gamma synchrony across the longitudinal axis of the hippocampusFrontiers in Systems Neuroscience 7:20.https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2013.00020
-
Cellular mechanisms of brain state-dependent gain modulation in visual cortexNature Neuroscience 16:1331–1339.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3464
-
Hebbian analysis of the transformation of medial entorhinal grid-cell inputs to hippocampal place fieldsJournal of Neurophysiology 103:3167–3183.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00932.2009
-
Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesionsJournal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 20:11–21.https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.20.1.11
-
Cholinergic modulation of hippocampal network functionFrontiers in Synaptic Neuroscience 5:2.https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsyn.2013.00002
-
Hippocampal pyramidal cells: significance of dendritic ionic conductances for neuronal function and epileptogenesisJournal of Neurophysiology 42:476–496.
-
CREB regulates excitability and the allocation of memory to subsets of neurons in the amygdalaNature Neuroscience 12:1438–1443.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2405
-
Long-term dynamics of CA1 hippocampal place codesNature Neuroscience 16:264–266.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3329
Decision letter
-
Howard EichenbaumReviewing Editor; Boston University, United States
In the interests of transparency, eLife includes the editorial decision letter and accompanying author responses. A lightly edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the most substantive concerns; minor comments are not usually included.
Thank you for submitting your article "Experience-dependent shaping of hippocampal CA1 intracellular activity in novel and familiar environments" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by three peer reviewers, and the evaluation has been overseen by a Reviewing Editor and Andrew King as the Senior Editor. The following individual involved in review of your submission has agreed to reveal his identity: Thomas J. McHugh (Reviewer #3).
The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.
Summary:
The present study uses intracellular recordings to investigate how environmental novelty is represented by hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons and what role plasticity plays in place cell formation and stabilization. The authors used a virtual reality (VR) setup to enable head-fixed mice running on a polystyrene sphere to switch between novel (NOV) and familiar (FAM) environments. The experimental design is clever and nicely controls for many behavioral variables. Overall, the study examines a clearly important and interesting question, and it is novel in a sense that this is the first of its kind to use intracellular recordings to study the effect of environmental novelty/familiarity on hippocampal CA1 place field formation and stabilization. Although the reviewers agree that there is considerable potential value in these findings, several major concerns were raised that will need to be addressed in any revision.
Essential changes:
1) These in vivo intracellular recordings are obviously challenging and state-of-the-art, since they would – ideally – require recordings from the same cells over different conditions (e.g. FAM>NOV>FAM, as in Figure 2C) to collect data with robust, paired design and to control for animal-to-animal variability. Unfortunately, only a subset of the recordings in the study meet this criterion (n=11), and the authors had to include other cells with less complete recordings and with variable condition exposures. In addition, most of the recordings seem to be quite short (on average ~4 min/condition) and often contain only few laps (the laps/epoch reported is 10 +/- 7, e.g. Figure 4A and related supplementary figure). Obviously, statistical interpretability of any intracellular electrophysiological properties and experience-dependent changes during such short trials are in question. For example, place fields are not being defined in terms of statistical significance so this issue is not being factored in or controlled for. Furthermore, when analyzing stabilization of spatial tuning, the authors had to remove some epochs from FAM conditions (Figure 4) to show significant effects. Together, the data pools together cells with quite variable condition exposures/durations/quality and therefore it is quite difficult to assess at the moment what statistical power the dataset really possesses and the strength of conclusions that could be drawn from it. These issues need to be addressed.
2) A concern was expressed about the authors' place field definition, which is based on subthreshold membrane potential (Vm). It is unclear how the authors can argue that a cell without spatially-tuned spiking activity could count as a bona fide place cell. This is a critical issue that should be carefully revisited, by focusing on cells with AP-based place fields to provide more informative insights into experience- and plasticity-dependent mechanisms of place field formation and stabilization and to better relate the results presented here to the existing literature. Since there is only a handful of cells the authors could really focus on here (i.e. cells with recordings in both FAM and NOV and with bona fide place fields at least in one condition), it would be important to also provide a table with recording duration, lap number, condition exposure, and physiological properties (e.g. sub/suprathreshold place fields) for each cell, which would allow a better assessment of the dataset.
3) An implicit assumption of the experimental design is that the animal can reliably discriminate between different VR conditions used to define NOV and FAM conditions. The key issue of the paper – novelty – is unfortunately not supported by any behavioral measures. The mice did not change their running speed or behavior (pausing) between the novel and familiar tracks. The authors note that the mice did show a change in the spatial profile of running (Figure 4—figure supplement 1C), however it is unclear what this measure captures. Typically, mice show a significant change in running speed and distance traveled in a novel environment. While the physiological data seem to argue convincingly for a network response to novelty, it would be best if there was some measure of behavior that the authors could use as verification the animals are truly treating the virtual space as new. This is important also because the interpretation of experience-dependent changes in physiological properties of CA1 pyramidal cells would be quite complicated if context representations are largely overlapping in these VRs conditions and if partial or partial/rate remapping dominates the networks dynamics. In this case, one can argue, for example, that for cells with recording in only one (e.g. NOV) condition, place fields may have already formed during preceding FAM exposure(s) and then may or may not undergo some remapping in NOV. In general, the authors have to be clearer in their analysis on how many times they observe de novo place field formation.
4) What is the role of plasticity in creating new place cell representations? As a measure of plasticity, the authors use the in-field subthreshold membrane potential (Vm) 'hill', defined as the peak value of the subthreshold Vm minus the baseline Vm. One main finding of the study is that this subthreshold membrane potential hill in NOV continues to increase with the number of completed laps, while in FAM it does not. This effect seems quite robust, with ~1.7-fold increase in the subthreshold Vm hill leading to a 3-fold increase in firing rate. Overall the authors interpret this result in support of a model in which plasticity amplifies already existing place fields with repeated exposure to a novel environment until presumably novelty is reduced: "The increase in amplitude is consistent with a strengthening of inputs to place fields via Hebbian, spike timing-dependent, or other (Golding et al., 2002) mechanisms of synaptic plasticity, and indicates rapid learning in CA1 in novel environments." The authors' point is that inputs to place fields are strengthened, but the data show that this increase is not restricted to inputs within the place field, and this increase occurs irrespective of the peak Vm location. That is the lap peak Vm change appears comparable or even larger than the in-field Vm change (Figure 2E-F), arguing against a field-specific plasticity mechanism. This may require more detailed analysis, again, by focusing on place cells with bona fide place fields. As mentioned above, defining place fields based on subthreshold Vm profile, instead of spiking, is different from conventional spike-based definition of place cells. Moreover, from this subthreshold definition of place fields it follows that the same (non-Hebbian?) experience-dependent plasticity mechanism would operate on both silent and active cells. Isn't it equally possible that instead of synaptic plasticity, these initial and transient changes in Vm during the first few minutes of a NOV exposure result mostly from non-specific neuromodulatory influences?
5) The authors argue in the last paragraph of the subsection “Spatial tuning of subthreshold Vm stabilizes more rapidly inside than outside the place field” (Figure 4—figure supplement 4) that the shape of the Vm hill shifts in a direction opposite to the motion of the animal through the place field, similar to what was observed with extracellular recordings in Mehta et al. 1997. The observation in the Mehta paper was not confined to novel tracks, however, at least in CA1, and seemed to reset daily, while the effect here, while not quantified, seems to only occur during the first exploration. This inconsistency should be addressed.
6) Because of the relatively short duration of the recordings, and because the only measure of novelty is the number of laps (and time) elapsed, which is arguably quite arbitrary (early vs. late), it is quite difficult to assess what exactly such short exposure to an environment means and what plasticity mechanism could be operational during this short time. Rather than asking about some arbitrarily defined 'initial' vs. 'late', it would be more informative, for example, to ask how changes in cellular dynamics correspond to behavioral dynamics (e.g. whether the peak amplitude is significantly different for laps before and after the mouse has learned its way through the novel environment – when it begins to reliably lick at the reward location).
7) The authors make some quite strong statements based on the data in Figure 4B, but it is unclear that the data support such strong conclusions. The data in FAM environment are quite noisy to start with and, in addition, the authors need to exclude the first FAM epochs experienced by the animal on a given day: "All epochs were included, with the following exception. In our experiments, for each recording, the animal was generally first exposed to a familiar maze followed by a novel or other familiar maze. In many cases (10/32 cells), this first familiar maze recording was also the first exposure to any maze for the animal on that day. We suspected that spatial correlations could be disrupted in these first familiar epochs due to nonspecific behavioral factors, thus partially masking differences from novel epochs." This data selection need to be justified, since it treats NOV and FAM conditions differently and may introduce some artificial bias. For example, if there is a condition-independent, time-dependent stabilization during the initial laps in both NOV and FAM, such treatment may mitigate this effect for FAM but not for NOV, and exaggerate the experience-dependent changes in NOV. Importantly, the statistics in Figure 4B are difficult to interpret: the Vm stabilization for NOV-FAM and inside-outside the place field should not be analyzed with independent comparisons, but with 2-way ANOVA of context (NOV-FAM) and place field location (inside-outside). Can the authors identify significant effects for context, location and/or interaction this way? Alternatively, the authors could perform bootstrapping by repeatedly selecting regions outside the place fields on the track with equal width to the 'place field'.
8) Similarly, the complex spike (CS) part requires some more scrutiny. The authors state: "…4/7 epochs contained {greater than or equal to}1 CS in the initial lap of the emergent field (Figure 6A), while 3/7 displayed stable activity in the place field region prior to any CSs (Figure 6B) or did not have any CSs (for example, the novel maze place cell in Figure 2B did not contain a CS). Thus, CSs were not required for place field formation in novel environments…". So, it appears that >50% of place field were indeed formed following a CS, and in other cases place fields were already present prior to a CS – as appears to be the case in Figure 2B and Figure 6B. How often were place fields actually formed de novo without a CS, which might argue against the role of CS in place cells formation. Again, how strong is the evidence for de novo place field formation in NOV? In other words, could it be the case that for place fields already present in NOV, these fields were formed during preceding FAM exposures? A more detailed analysis of the subset of cells with recordings in both FAM-NOV(-FAM) could perhaps be informative here.
9) Relatedly, the statement that "Moreover, CSs in familiar mazes did not necessarily stabilize place fields" should be qualified, since there seem to be no statistics supporting this claim other than the single example cells in Figure 6C.
10) There is a confusing argument about where the stabilization of Vm occurred: in CA1 itself, or upstream in CA3 and Entorhinal cortex? It is unclear how an observation of different rates of correlation increase can be interpreted unambiguously as localizing the key plasticity steps to CA1.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.23040.021Author response
Essential changes:
1) These in vivo intracellular recordings are obviously challenging and state-of-the-art, since they would – ideally – require recordings from the same cells over different conditions (e.g. FAM>NOV>FAM, as in Figure 2C) to collect data with robust, paired design and to control for animal-to-animal variability. Unfortunately, only a subset of the recordings in the study meet this criterion (n=11), and the authors had to include other cells with less complete recordings and with variable condition exposures. In addition, most of the recordings seem to be quite short (on average ~4 min/condition) and often contain only few laps (the laps/epoch reported is 10 +/- 7, e.g. Figure 4A and related supplementary figure). Obviously, statistical interpretability of any intracellular electrophysiological properties and experience-dependent changes during such short trials are in question. For example, place fields are not being defined in terms of statistical significance so this issue is not being factored in or controlled for. Furthermore, when analyzing stabilization of spatial tuning, the authors had to remove some epochs from FAM conditions (Figure 4) to show significant effects. Together, the data pools together cells with quite variable condition exposures/durations/quality and therefore it is quite difficult to assess at the moment what statistical power the dataset really possesses and the strength of conclusions that could be drawn from it. These issues need to be addressed.
2) A concern was expressed about the authors' place field definition, which is based on subthreshold membrane potential (Vm). It is unclear how the authors can argue that a cell without spatially-tuned spiking activity could count as a bona fide place cell. This is a critical issue that should be carefully revisited, by focusing on cells with AP-based place fields to provide more informative insights into experience- and plasticity-dependent mechanisms of place field formation and stabilization and to better relate the results presented here to the existing literature. Since there is only a handful of cells the authors could really focus on here (i.e. cells with recordings in both FAM and NOV and with bona fide place fields at least in one condition), it would be important to also provide a table with recording duration, lap number, condition exposure, and physiological properties (e.g. sub/suprathreshold place fields) for each cell, which would allow a better assessment of the dataset.
We have combined our responses to points 1 and 2 below. The reviewers raise questions related to the criteria for including cells in the analysis, especially regarding cells with (spiking) place fields versus active cells (that may not show spatially tuned spiking) or silent cells.
As the reviewers note, in several of our analyses on the relative magnitude of subthreshold Vm peaks, we included both active and silent cells and defined the field based on peaks in the average subthreshold Vm as a function of location. We believe these results using the subthreshold Vm-defined fields of all cells are of value because they are unbiased regarding possible underlying mechanisms, such as plasticity mechanisms that do not require postsynaptic spiking (e.g. Golding et al., 2002; cited in the original manuscript for this possibility), and especially in light of our finding that the subthreshold Vm generally shows spatial tuning everywhere (whether the cell is active or silent). However, we fully agree with the reviewers’ point that these analyses should also be done with respect to spatially tuned spiking, and thus we defined place fields based on spiking and also analyzed the subthreshold Vm with respect to these place fields. The results agree with the original findings: growth of the subthreshold Vm within NOV and not within FAM epochs, and larger values in NOV compared to FAM. In addition, we further investigated the within-epoch growth of the subthreshold Vm and the results lead to a modified interpretation of the changes during NOV exploration. The details are as follows.
We classified cells as place cells (i.e. cells having a place field) by the following criteria for each epoch and direction: from the epoch average firing rate map, the candidate place field was defined as the region (“in-field”) containing the peak AP firing rate and any contiguous bins with rate above 20% of the way from the baseline to peak rate (where the baseline is the mean rate in the 10% of bins with lowest rate, which is generally 0 Hz); then candidate fields with peak rate ≥0.5 Hz, at least 2 laps of spiking in-field, and an in-field/out-field average firing rate ratio ≥3 were classified as place fields. As requested, we analyzed the NOV versus FAM peak subthreshold Vm data using only NOV-FAM pairs in which the cell had a place field in at least one maze epoch of each maze pair (i.e. place-place, place-active, and place-silent pairs). As before, the peak Vm responses were larger in NOV versus FAM epochs (NOV: 2.5 ± 0.4, FAM: 1.6 ± 0.1, n=10, P=0.004). We have added this result to the text and figures (Figure 3I), and the place field definition to the Methods.
Next, as the reviewers requested, we specifically assessed the relationship of spiking activity to the within-epoch growth of the subthreshold Vm hill inside the spiking place fields of NOV place cells. For this analysis we added a new figure (Figure 3), which includes examples (Figure 3C-D) that illustrate the analysis. First, we compared the in-field peak subthreshold Vm for the laps before any in-field spiking activity (“Pre-Active” laps) versus the laps starting from the first lap of in-field spiking activity (“Post” laps). (The Pre-Active laps in the Figure 3C-D examples are illustrated with shades of blue, and the Post laps by green and shades of red.) As expected, the peak subthreshold Vm increased sharply (though non-significantly, since only a subset of place fields first appeared after lap 1) for these cases in which in-field spiking activity first appeared after lap 1. Then, as a key test, we compared the in-field peak subthreshold Vm in the first lap with in-field spiking (“First-Active” lap, which is shown in green in the Figure 3C-D examples) versus all subsequent laps (“Post’” laps, which is colored with shades of red in Figure 3C-D). This included cases where the field was already present in the first lap of the epoch. We found no significant change (First-Active: 4.0 ± 0.9 mV, Post’: 4.1 ± 0.5, n=9, P=1.0; Figure 3E, right). Together, these results indicate that much of the growth of the Vm hill within NOV epochs occurs at the transition from silent to spiking activity, with no consistent increase after the first active lap. That is, here we did not find evidence of postsynaptic spiking-based strengthening of inputs. Rather, the results are consistent with plasticity mechanisms that do not require postsynaptic spiking (e.g. Golding et al., 2002). This result is a clarification of what is occurring during field formation in NOV environments, and we have revised the interpretation of what these findings may mean for plasticity mechanisms in the Results and Discussion sections.
Furthermore, as the reviewers suggested, we now provide a table (Table 1) containing descriptive statistics for all cells included in the study.
Regarding the analysis of spatial tuning in Figure 4 and the removal of some epochs (those that were the first epoch of the day), we address this in the response to point 7.
3) An implicit assumption of the experimental design is that the animal can reliably discriminate between different VR conditions used to define NOV and FAM conditions. The key issue of the paper – novelty – is unfortunately not supported by any behavioral measures. The mice did not change their running speed or behavior (pausing) between the novel and familiar tracks. The authors note that the mice did show a change in the spatial profile of running (Figure 4—figure supplement 1C), however it is unclear what this measure captures. Typically, mice show a significant change in running speed and distance traveled in a novel environment. While the physiological data seem to argue convincingly for a network response to novelty, it would be best if there was some measure of behavior that the authors could use as verification the animals are truly treating the virtual space as new. This is important also because the interpretation of experience-dependent changes in physiological properties of CA1 pyramidal cells would be quite complicated if context representations are largely overlapping in these VRs conditions and if partial or partial/rate remapping dominates the networks dynamics. In this case, one can argue, for example, that for cells with recording in only one (e.g. NOV) condition, place fields may have already formed during preceding FAM exposure(s) and then may or may not undergo some remapping in NOV. In general, the authors have to be clearer in their analysis on how many times they observe de novo place field formation.
The reviewers ask for evidence that animals distinguished between mazes that were novel versus familiar. We address this point in two ways: an analysis of predictive licking behavior, and an analysis of measures that potentially reflect (global) remapping.
We assessed the licking behavior of the animal as a measure that could be associated with experience in the mazes, and found clear evidence of learning in the virtual mazes. In all mazes (and each direction for the subset of bidirectional mazes) there was a single, fixed reward zone (RDZ) where rewards were always delivered to the subject. We found significantly more predictive licking behavior (licking immediately before reward delivery in the RDZ) in FAM versus NOV mazes (“Pre-RDZ”) (Figure 2E). We also found an increase in predictive licking from the initial laps (1-2) to the late laps (6-end) in all epochs (Figure 2—figure supplement 2A). These results indicate that the animal learned the location of the RDZ with experience and licked accordingly, and thus that the animals could actively discriminate between NOV and FAM environments. This has been added to both the text and the figures (as described).
Regarding the reviewers’ question of remapping, we looked for evidence that the spatial tuning of neural activity did or did not differ between the NOV and FAM VR environments. In freely moving animals, cells can display rate remapping by firing in corresponding locations of different mazes situated within the same room. But our VR mazes were geometrically distinct arenas and thus there were no clear geometrically corresponding locations. Therefore, to attempt a comparable analysis in VR, we used the most salient common reference point, the primary reward zone, to define corresponding locations. Previous VR studies have shown that a substantial fraction of CA1 cells fire at a fixed distance from reward zones (Ravassard et al. 2013). If this was true across our different VR mazes, it would suggest that the spatially tuned activity in NOV mazes was not truly novel activity. Therefore, we correlated the spatial distribution of spiking activity (and, separately, the subthreshold Vm) aligned to the reward zone for each recorded pair of NOV and FAM mazes. We did not find any evidence (P>0.05 for all comparisons) of corresponding activity across mazes. That is, there was no evidence of rate remapping-like processes, and instead the results were suggestive of global remapping in the novel environments. This analysis is now included in a new figure (Figure 2—figure supplement 1).
We address the reviewers’ question about de novo place fields in our response to point 8.
4) What is the role of plasticity in creating new place cell representations? As a measure of plasticity, the authors use the in-field subthreshold membrane potential (Vm) 'hill', defined as the peak value of the subthreshold Vm minus the baseline Vm. One main finding of the study is that this subthreshold membrane potential hill in NOV continues to increase with the number of completed laps, while in FAM it does not. This effect seems quite robust, with ~1.7-fold increase in the subthreshold Vm hill leading to a 3-fold increase in firing rate. Overall the authors interpret this result in support of a model in which plasticity amplifies already existing place fields with repeated exposure to a novel environment until presumably novelty is reduced: "The increase in amplitude is consistent with a strengthening of inputs to place fields via Hebbian, spike timing-dependent, or other (Golding et al., 2002) mechanisms of synaptic plasticity, and indicates rapid learning in CA1 in novel environments." The authors' point is that inputs to place fields are strengthened, but the data show that this increase is not restricted to inputs within the place field, and this increase occurs irrespective of the peak Vm location. That is the lap peak Vmchange appears comparable or even larger than the in-field Vm change (Figure 2E-F), arguing against a field-specific plasticity mechanism. This may require more detailed analysis, again, by focusing on place cells with bona fide place fields. As mentioned above, defining place fields based on subthreshold Vm profile, instead of spiking, is different from conventional spike-based definition of place cells. Moreover, from this subthreshold definition of place fields it follows that the same (non-Hebbian?) experience-dependent plasticity mechanism would operate on both silent and active cells. Isn't it equally possible that instead of synaptic plasticity, these initial and transient changes in Vmduring the first few minutes of a NOV exposure result mostly from non-specific neuromodulatory influences?
We agree with the reviewers that it’s critical to analyze what happens to the subthreshold Vm within the spiking place field of place cells. This has been addressed in our response to points 1 and 2 above.
Regarding the increase in the “lap peak” Vm – which averages the peak Vm per lap irrespective of location – within NOV epochs, we included this analysis as a control for variability of the subthreshold Vm field since it was not known in advance how much the peak might jitter or systematically shift (e.g. due to learning) in location. We found that the direction and significance of the results were the same whether using the lap peak values or the field-based values. The magnitude of the values was larger using the lap peak version because it first takes the peak Vm per lap then averages, while the field-based peak Vm first averages (and thus smooths) the Vm over laps then takes the peak. We have added a comment about this (i.e. the calculation of lap peak values as a control for possible jitter) to the revised text and have also moved the corresponding figures to the figure supplements.
5) The authors argue in the last paragraph of the subsection “Spatial tuning of subthreshold Vm stabilizes more rapidly inside than outside the place field” (Figure 4—figure supplement 4) that the shape of the Vm hill shifts in a direction opposite to the motion of the animal through the place field, similar to what was observed with extracellular recordings in Mehta et al. 1997. The observation in the Mehta paper was not confined to novel tracks, however, at least in CA1, and seemed to reset daily, while the effect here, while not quantified, seems to only occur during the first exploration. This inconsistency should be addressed.
To clarify the issue, we have revised this figure (now Figure 5—figure supplement 4) to also show the location of the field’s center of mass (CoM), and have overlaid the early and late field shapes to better allow comparison. The CoM does indeed shift in the direction opposite to the motion of the animal within both NOV and FAM epochs, consistent with the previous findings of Mehta et al., 1997.
6) Because of the relatively short duration of the recordings, and because the only measure of novelty is the number of laps (and time) elapsed, which is arguably quite arbitrary (early vs. late), it is quite difficult to assess what exactly such short exposure to an environment means and what plasticity mechanism could be operational during this short time. Rather than asking about some arbitrarily defined 'initial' vs. 'late', it would be more informative, for example, to ask how changes in cellular dynamics correspond to behavioral dynamics (e.g. whether the peak amplitude is significantly different for laps before and after the mouse has learned its way through the novel environment – when it begins to reliably lick at the reward location).
In previous studies, it has been shown that a significant amount of the change in spiking with experience (e.g. Mehta et al., 2000; Ekstrom et al., 2001) occurs within the first 5 laps of an epoch, which is the reason we chose laps 1-5 as “early” experience and laps 6-end as “late” values. We have added this point to the text. Furthermore, behavioral measures, such as the newly added predictive licking rate described in the response to point 3 (newly added Figure 2—figure supplement 2A), provide evidence of learning at the behavioral level within the first 5 laps. Together, this provides support for focusing on changes in neural activity between the initial laps and laps 6-end, and for considering plasticity mechanisms that act on these timescales to underlie rapid hippocampal learning.
7) The authors make some quite strong statements based on the data in Figure 4B, but it is unclear that the data support such strong conclusions. The data in FAM environment are quite noisy to start with and, in addition, the authors need to exclude the first FAM epochs experienced by the animal on a given day: "All epochs were included, with the following exception. In our experiments, for each recording, the animal was generally first exposed to a familiar maze followed by a novel or other familiar maze. In many cases (10/32 cells), this first familiar maze recording was also the first exposure to any maze for the animal on that day. We suspected that spatial correlations could be disrupted in these first familiar epochs due to nonspecific behavioral factors, thus partially masking differences from novel epochs." This data selection need to be justified, since it treats NOV and FAM conditions differently and may introduce some artificial bias. For example, if there is a condition-independent, time-dependent stabilization during the initial laps in both NOV and FAM, such treatment may mitigate this effect for FAM but not for NOV, and exaggerate the experience-dependent changes in NOV. Importantly, the statistics in Figure 4B are difficult to interpret: the Vm stabilization for NOV-FAM and inside-outside the place field should not be analyzed with independent comparisons, but with 2-way ANOVA of context (NOV-FAM) and place field location (inside-outside). Can the authors identify significant effects for context, location and/or interaction this way? Alternatively, the authors could perform bootstrapping by repeatedly selecting regions outside the place fields on the track with equal width to the 'place field'.
The reviewers ask for justification for our exclusion of the first maze experienced by the animal on a given day in the spatial tuning analysis (previously Figure 4B, now Figure 5B).
First, the reviewers raise the possibility that there could be changes in neural activity in all first-mazes-of-the-day whether NOV or FAM, and that if first FAM mazes were excluded, these changes could be mistakenly attributed to the NOV condition specifically. In the original manuscript we wrote “With these first familiar epochs excluded … we observed that …” This was written incorrectly, as we excluded all first mazes of the day for the analysis in Figure 5B (previously Figure 4B), not only the first FAM mazes. (It turned out to be the case that no first mazes of the day were NOV mazes, which still addresses the reviewers’ point that no such NOV mazes should be included if the corresponding FAM mazes are excluded.) We apologize for the mistaken sentence and resulting misunderstanding, and have corrected the text.
Second, an analysis of licking shows a difference in behavior between the first mazes of the day in FAM environments and all other FAM epochs. In particular, although predictive licking appeared intact in these first FAM mazes, the mice licked significantly less after the reward had been delivered (presented in the newly added Figure 2—figure supplement 2B). This is consistent with our initial view that there may have been a reduced level of task engagement in the first mazes of the day that could affect spatial tuning non-specifically. We feel that this may be an important point to consider for future studies using virtual reality and have also added a comment about this in the text for researchers to consider in their future projects.
Note that when no epochs are excluded from the analysis (now Figure 5—figure supplement 1B), most of the main findings of Figure 5B are also present. Specifically, the spatial tuning in the first laps was significantly lower in NOV compared to FAM epochs, the spatial tuning showed a significant experience-dependent increase in NOV epochs, and the spatial tuning started high and remained so during FAM epochs.
As the reviewers suggested, we also performed statistical analyses on spatial correlation scores using the two-way RMANOVA. First, for all maze epochs we performed a two-way RMANOVA for context (NOV vs. FAM) and location (all locations vs. Out-field – note that we do not perform correlations on data solely from the In-field region as the number of spatial bins is often too small for such a comparison; instead, we compare activity for all locations with Out-field locations that excludes the In-field bins). This revealed a significant effect of location (F(1,38)=20.00, P<0.001). Excluding first epochs of the day, we observed a significant effect of location (F(1,31)=18.98, P<0.001) and context (F(1,31)10.65, P=0.003). Next, for all epochs we performed a two-way RMANOVA for context (NOV vs. FAM) and experience (initial laps 1-2 vs. late laps 6-end). This revealed a significant effect of experience (F(1,26)=8.75, P=0.007). Excluding first epochs of the day, we observed a significant effect of experience (F(1,21)=21.91, P<0.001) and context (F(1,21)=2.54, P=0.011). These results justify a more detailed look at the effect of location and experience in the NOV and FAM epochs separately, such as that provided in Figure 5B.
In conclusion, we believe the data clearly show that the spatial tuning begins lower in NOV than FAM mazes, and that the tuning grows with experience within NOV. Regarding our interpretation of these spatial tuning results, please refer to our response to point 10.
8) Similarly, the complex spike (CS) part requires some more scrutiny. The authors state: "…4/7 epochs contained {greater than or equal to}1 CS in the initial lap of the emergent field (Figure 6A), while 3/7 displayed stable activity in the place field region prior to any CSs (Figure 6B) or did not have any CSs (for example, the novel maze place cell in Figure 2B did not contain a CS). Thus, CSs were not required for place field formation in novel environments…". So, it appears that >50% of place field were indeed formed following a CS, and in other cases place fields were already present prior to a CS – as appears to be the case in Figure 2B and Figure 6B. How often were place fields actually formed de novo without a CS, which might argue against the role of CS in place cells formation. Again, how strong is the evidence for de novo place field formation in NOV? In other words, could it be the case that for place fields already present in NOV, these fields were formed during preceding FAM exposures? A more detailed analysis of the subset of cells with recordings in both FAM-NOV(-FAM) could perhaps be informative here.
Regarding de novo place field formation, one line of evidence in favor of NOV place fields as being de novo fields is the absence of evidence for rate remapping (discussed in the response to point 3). In particular, this would argue against the idea that the field was originally formed in a preceding FAM environment and re-expressed in the NOV environment.
We also redid the lap-by-lap analysis on CSs and novel place field formation now using the place field definition added in response to points 1 and 2. First, we note that during this analysis, we found 2 cells (which together contained 4 novel epochs included in the original manuscript) in which experimenter manipulations appeared to have triggered place field formation. In particular, one cell, which had a high rate of spontaneous CSs, also had some CSs triggered by our short current steps, and these triggered CSs appear to have generated place fields in those locations in subsequent laps (as in Bittner et al., 2015). Including or excluding these epochs did not alter the significance of the results regarding growth of the subthreshold Vm hill in NOV or the higher peak subthreshold Vm in NOV vs. FAM (though it did change the precise values, as marked in the revised manuscript). But, we removed these epochs from our NOV group in the revised manuscript because their place fields do not clearly represent spontaneous novel place fields. Their exclusion did however result in the following changes (which have been incorporated into the revised text and figures): it reduced the difference in CS occurrence in NOV vs. FAM epochs to below significance, and it removed the lack of increase in out-field Vm spatial correlation within novel mazes. Of relevance for the current point 8, it also reduced the number of cases in which spontaneous (not triggered) CSs occurred in the first lap of a novel place field (because one of the now-excluded cells had a high propensity for spontaneous CSs, including CSs that spontaneously occurred in the first lap of place fields). The resulting numbers of novel epoch place fields and their relationship to CSs has been added to the revised text:
“Of the 9 novel epoch place fields, 4 displayed stable activity in the place field region prior to any CSs (which then occurred later in the place field in 3/4 cases, e.g. Figure 6A), 2 had ≥1 CS in the initial lap of the emergent field (e.g. Figure 3C and 6B), and the remaining 3 were in epochs that did not have any CSs (e.g. Figure 2B and 3D did not contain CSs). Of these 9 place fields, 7 occurred in mazes that the animal had never previously been exposed to, and 2 occurred in mazes previously experienced 1 or 2 times (versus ≥19 exposures for familiar epochs, Table 1). Thus, CSs were not required for place field formation in novel environments.”
That is, only 2 of 9 novel epoch place fields had a CS in the first lap in which spiking in the field appeared. Also, of the 3 place fields in novel epochs without a CS, there was a CS in a preceding FAM epoch in 1 case. Therefore, these results support the hypothesis that cells can form new place field activity in novel environments without CSs.
9) Relatedly, the statement that "Moreover, CSs in familiar mazes did not necessarily stabilize place fields" should be qualified, since there seem to be no statistics supporting this claim other than the single example cells in Figure 6C.
In Figure 6D, in which the spatial tuning scores of all epochs that contained CSs are aligned to the lap containing the CS, we did not observe a CS-specific effect on the subsequent tuning of activity in the maze. That is, the tuning was similarly, transiently increased following control laps which had a large Vm depolarization but no CSs. This suggests that individual CSs usually do not have a large effect on subsequent activity. Looking at individual FAM epochs, for the 6 FAM epochs with at least one CS, spiking activity was stable throughout the entire epoch in 5/6 cases. And of the 5 stable epochs, spiking activity was already present in the field location in the laps prior to the CS in 4/5 cases. In the 5th case, the CS occurred in the first active lap. For the one FAM epoch (shown in Figure 6C in the original manuscript, now in Figure 6—figure supplement 2) with unstable spiking activity, the 2 CSs in the 9th lap did not lead to the stabilization of spiking activity in the field. We noted this as a counterexample to stabilization of fields by CSs, especially since one of the CSs was of particularly long duration, and such long-duration, single CSs have been associated with spontaneous place field formation (Bittner et al., 2015). Since it is an isolated example, we have moved it to the figure supplement, where we have also included the above numbers for all FAM epochs with CSs in the legend. Note that the number of FAM cases for which it was possible to assess whether CSs could stabilize a field (or create a stable field) was 2 (since in the other 4 cases the CS occurred in fields that were already stable), and in 1/2 cases the CS did not do so.
10) There is a confusing argument about where the stabilization of Vm occurred: in CA1 itself, or upstream in CA3 and Entorhinal cortex? It is unclear how an observation of different rates of correlation increase can be interpreted unambiguously as localizing the key plasticity steps to CA1.
As mentioned briefly in the response to point 8, the exclusion of NOV epochs with likely non-spontaneous place field activity had the consequence of changing the original result (a lack of increase of in out-field Vm spatial correlation with experience in NOV epochs) to an out-field correlation increase that generally matches the NOV increase in spatial correlation for the entire environment (in-field + out-field). This removes the issue of differential rates of correlation increase and what this might mean for the locus of plasticity. As a result, we have modified the text accordingly.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.23040.022Article and author information
Author details
Funding
Howard Hughes Medical Institute
- Jeremy D Cohen
- Mark Bolstad
- Albert K Lee
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Acknowledgements
We thank Min Ji Kim for technical assistance; Joshua Dudman, Adam Hantman, Michael Tadross, Nelson Spruston, Jeffrey Magee, Aaron Milstein, Simon Peron, and members of the Lee Lab for helpful discussions and comments on the manuscript; Steve Bassin and Jason Osborne for mechanical engineering and machining; Vivek Jayaraman, Hannah Haberkern, Christopher Bruns, and Sean Murphy for contributions to the development of the virtual reality software; and Magnus Karlsson and Lakshmi Ramasamy for electrical engineering. This work was supported by the Howard Hughes Medical Institute.
Ethics
Animal experimentation: All procedures were performed in accordance with the Janelia Research Campus Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee guidelines on animal welfare (protocol #14-116).
Reviewing Editor
- Howard Eichenbaum, Boston University, United States
Version history
- Received: November 6, 2016
- Accepted: April 6, 2017
- Version of Record published: July 25, 2017 (version 1)
Copyright
© 2017, Cohen et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 3,848
- Page views
-
- 735
- Downloads
-
- 61
- Citations
Article citation count generated by polling the highest count across the following sources: Scopus, Crossref, PubMed Central.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Genetics and Genomics
- Neuroscience
A hexanucleotide repeat expansion in C9ORF72 is the most common genetic cause of amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS) and frontotemporal dementia (FTD). A hallmark of ALS/FTD pathology is the presence of dipeptide repeat (DPR) proteins, produced from both sense GGGGCC (poly-GA, poly-GP, poly-GR) and antisense CCCCGG (poly-PR, poly-PG, poly-PA) transcripts. Translation of sense DPRs, such as poly-GA and poly-GR, depends on non-canonical (non-AUG) initiation codons. Here, we provide evidence for canonical AUG-dependent translation of two antisense DPRs, poly-PR and poly-PG. A single AUG is required for synthesis of poly-PR, one of the most toxic DPRs. Unexpectedly, we found redundancy between three AUG codons necessary for poly-PG translation. Further, the eukaryotic translation initiation factor 2D (EIF2D), which was previously implicated in sense DPR synthesis, is not required for AUG-dependent poly-PR or poly-PG translation, suggesting that distinct translation initiation factors control DPR synthesis from sense and antisense transcripts. Our findings on DPR synthesis from the C9ORF72 locus may be broadly applicable to many other nucleotide repeat expansion disorders.
-
- Cell Biology
- Neuroscience
The amyloid beta (Aβ) plaques found in Alzheimer’s disease (AD) patients’ brains contain collagens and are embedded extracellularly. Several collagens have been proposed to influence Aβ aggregate formation, yet their role in clearance is unknown. To investigate the potential role of collagens in forming and clearance of extracellular aggregates in vivo, we created a transgenic Caenorhabditis elegans strain that expresses and secretes human Aβ1-42. This secreted Aβ forms aggregates in two distinct places within the extracellular matrix. In a screen for extracellular human Aβ aggregation regulators, we identified different collagens to ameliorate or potentiate Aβ aggregation. We show that a disintegrin and metalloprotease a disintegrin and metalloprotease 2 (ADM-2), an ortholog of ADAM9, reduces the load of extracellular Aβ aggregates. ADM-2 is required and sufficient to remove the extracellular Aβ aggregates. Thus, we provide in vivo evidence of collagens essential for aggregate formation and metalloprotease participating in extracellular Aβ aggregate removal.