Focal optogenetic suppression in macaque area MT biases direction discrimination and decision confidence, but only transiently
Abstract
Insights from causal manipulations of brain activity depend on targeting the spatial and temporal scales most relevant for behavior. Using a sensitive perceptual decision task in monkeys, we examined the effects of rapid, reversible inactivation on a spatial scale previously achieved only with electrical microstimulation. Inactivating groups of similarly tuned neurons in area MT produced systematic effects on choice and confidence. Behavioral effects were attenuated over the course of each session, suggesting compensatory adjustments in the downstream readout of MT over tens of minutes. Compensation also occurred on a sub-second time scale: behavior was largely unaffected when the visual stimulus (and concurrent suppression) lasted longer than 350 ms. These trends were similar for choice and confidence, consistent with the idea of a common mechanism underlying both measures. The findings demonstrate the utility of hyperpolarizing opsins for linking neural population activity at fine spatial and temporal scales to cognitive functions in primates.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.36523.001Introduction
To understand how neural activity gives rise to behavior, a powerful approach is to manipulate the activity of groups of neurons defined by particular functional or anatomical properties in the context of a suitable behavioral task. This strategy has grown in popularity over recent years with the advent of sophisticated tools for manipulating neural circuit function. However, recent perspectives have cautioned that so-called causal evidence is not always as decisive as it may appear (Jazayeri and Afraz, 2017), and by itself does not generate the level of understanding we wish to attain (Krakauer et al., 2017). These and other arguments serve to renew a longstanding dictum in systems neuroscience, namely the primary importance of developing a rigorous theoretical or conceptual framework for understanding the behavior of interest. Only then can clear hypotheses be articulated about the causal role of neural populations or circuits in generating the behavior.
The study of perceptual decision making has achieved a degree of progress toward this goal, in part by leveraging detailed knowledge of the neural representation of sensory evidence supporting the decision (reviewed by Cohen and Newsome, 2004; Romo and Salinas, 2001; Shadlen and Kiani, 2013). One well-studied behavioral task requires judgment of the net direction of motion in a noisy visual display designed to promote the integration of motion information across the display and over time. Neurons in motion-sensitive cortical areas, especially the middle temporal (MT) and medial superior temporal (MST) areas, are well suited to provide the evidence for the task, and theoretical considerations (Gold and Shadlen, 2001; Shadlen et al., 1996) point toward a simple and plausible computation: the subtraction of spike counts (or rates) between pools of neurons favoring each of the direction alternatives. This difference furnishes a quantity proportional to the log likelihood ratio favoring a given alternative, a statistical measure of the weight of evidence that can be accumulated over time to support optimal statistical decision making (Gold and Shadlen, 2002; Wald, 1947)
Building on the work of Newsome and colleagues (Celebrini and Newsome, 1995; Salzman et al., 1992), Ditterich et al. (2003) took advantage of the columnar organization of MT (Albright et al., 1984) to demonstrate, beyond simply a causal role for these neurons in the task, the specific differencing-and-integration mechanism that was previously hypothesized on theoretical grounds. Using electrical microstimulation (µStim) to activate neurons that were largely within a single direction column, they confirmed earlier reports showing that monkeys’ choices were biased toward the preferred direction of the activated neurons. More importantly, µStim increased the speed of decisions in favor of the preferred direction while slowing decisions made in favor of the opposite direction. The results supported a model in which the decision is formed by temporal integration of momentary evidence, defined as the difference in activity of pools of MT neurons tuned for the preferred and anti-preferred motion direction (Ditterich et al., 2003). More recently, we showed that decision confidence is affected by µStim in a way that is quantitatively commensurate with the effect on choices and well explained by a bounded evidence accumulation model (Fetsch et al., 2014a). Taken together, these findings suggest that a common process of evidence accumulation underlies all three behavioral measures of decision making in this task: choice, reaction time, and confidence.
Causal activation of neurons informs our understanding of the sufficiency, but not the necessity, of neural activity for driving behavior. Thus, reversible inactivation offers an important complement to stimulation, but conventional methods in primate neuroscience (e.g., muscimol or cooling) are lacking in both spatial and temporal resolution. The temporal component is crucial and often neglected. Indeed, the insights gained from the µStim studies described above depended on targeting the perturbation not only to the appropriate neurons but also the appropriate time frame within a trial (Seidemann et al., 1998). µStim itself has other drawbacks, including the possibility of antidromic activation, the generation of unwanted temporal and spatial patterns of inactivation (Butovas and Schwarz, 2003; Logothetis et al., 2010; Seidemann et al., 2002), and the difficulty of quantifying concurrent changes in neural activity due to voltage artifacts (but see Gnadt et al., 2003).
To address these limitations, we used an optogenetic approach to suppress MT activity during a motion discrimination task with post-decision wagering (PDW; Figure 1A). While some primate optogenetics studies set out to illuminate the largest possible volume of tissue (Acker et al., 2016; Gerits et al., 2012), we used very low light power levels to target relatively small clusters of excitatory neurons with similar selectivity for direction of motion. The rationale for this approach is that, in MT, such clusters constitute a critical functional unit for the conversion of a sensory representation into evidence for the decision. We found that optogenetic suppression was capable of inducing a choice bias against the neurons’ preferred direction, and a qualitatively similar change in the pattern of post-decision wagering, broadly consistent with a common mechanism underlying choice and confidence. We also found intriguing evidence for compensatory changes in the readout of MT activity, on both the sub-second and tens-of-minutes time scales. The results highlight the importance of spatial and temporal specificity in causal interventions, and they suggest a possible explanation for weak behavioral effects of optogenetic manipulations in some previous nonhuman primate studies.

Direction discrimination task with post-decision wagering (PDW).
(A) The monkey fixates on a central fixation point to initiate the trial. Two red choice targets are presented, followed by a random-dot motion (RDM) stimulus in the receptive field of the recorded neurons (left panel), shown for an experimenter-controlled variable duration (Figure 1—figure supplement 1). On a random half of trials, a ‘sure-bet’ target (Ts) is presented (blue spot in the lower panels). After a delay period, the monkey can report his choice by a leftward or rightward saccade to one of the red targets to obtain a large juice reward if he is correct, or, if available, choose the sure-bet target for a small but guaranteed reward. On half of trials, including both Ts-present and Ts-absent trials, the RDM stimulus was accompanied by red laser illumination (step-rampdown power profile) of a cluster of neurons expressing the light-sensitive chloride pump Jaws. (B) Proportion sure-bet choices as a function of motion strength (‘confidence function’) for all no-laser trials (N = 2 monkeys, 23 sessions, 9912 trials). Error bars in B and C indicate standard errors of the proportions and are often smaller than the data points. (C) Proportion of preferred-direction choices as a function of motion strength (‘choice function’) for all no-laser trials, separated by whether the sure-bet option was unavailable (dashed) or available but waived (solid).
-
Figure 1—source data 1
Data and Matlab code for reproducing all panels and figure supplements for Figure 1.
- https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.36523.004
Results
We examined the effects of optogenetic inactivation (hereafter, photosuppression) in extrastriate visual cortex (area MT) on perceptual choices and decision confidence. Two monkeys were trained to perform a random-dot motion (RDM) direction discrimination task with post-decision wagering (PDW; Figure 1A). The task was to decide whether the net direction of dot motion was to the left or to the right, and to indicate the choice with a saccade to a leftward or rightward target when prompted. In addition to the choice targets, a ‘sure-bet’ target was presented during the delay period on a random half of trials, allowing the monkey to receive a guaranteed but smaller reward and thereby indicate its lack of confidence in the binary left-right decision. As in previous studies (Fetsch et al., 2014a; Kiani and Shadlen, 2009; Zylberberg et al., 2016), monkeys chose the sure-bet most frequently when the motion was weak (Figure 1B) and of short duration (Figure 1—figure supplement 1B), and their accuracy was greater when the sure-bet was available but waived, versus when it was unavailable (Figure 1C; p<10−10, logistic regression). These behavioral observations, and quantitative analyses published previously (Fetsch et al., 2014a; Kiani and Shadlen, 2009), serve to validate the PDW assay as a measure of confidence—that is, a prediction of accuracy based on the state of a decision variable on a given trial, rather than a low-level estimate of trial difficulty or an index of lapses of attention.
Histological and physiological characterization of Jaws expression
At least 8 weeks before commencing experiments, area MT in one hemisphere of each animal was injected with an AAV vector to drive expression of the red light-sensitive chloride pump Jaws (cruxhalorhodopsin; Chuong et al., 2014) under the control of the CaMKIIα promoter. The two animals continue to participate in experiments and are unavailable for histology. Therefore, to test the overall efficacy of the AAV-CaMKIIα-Jaws vector (to our knowledge, not previously used in the macaque), we performed immunohistochemical analysis in a third animal following injections in the lateral intraparietal area (LIP). This analysis revealed robust Jaws-GFP expression in superficial and deep layers (Figure 2A), and a tropism for excitatory pyramidal cells (Figure 2B), as shown in previous studies using the CaMKIIα promoter (Han et al., 2009; Nassi et al., 2015). This tropism was not as strong as in previous work: 9% of Jaws-GFP-positive neurons (83 of 903) were double-labeled for the inhibitory marker parvalbumin (Figure 2B), giving an upper bound on selectivity of 91%, compared to >98% in macaque primary visual cortex (2 of 119 cells double-labeled for any of three different inhibitory markers; Nassi et al., 2015), and 100% in a study of the frontal eye field (0 of 78 cells double-labeled for GABA; Han et al., 2009). However, in addition to being conducted in different cortical areas, these studies used lentiviral vectors rather than AAV vectors. Thus, the effectiveness of promoter-based targeting of excitatory neurons likely depends on the viral vector and/or serotype (Nathanson et al., 2009; Gerits et al., 2015), and could also vary across brain areas. We emphasize that any conclusions from Figure 2 in relation to the results presented below should be rendered with caution, considering possible differences in cell-type distributions and laminar organization of MT versus LIP.

Transduction of neurons in macaque lateral intraparietal area (LIP) by AAV-CamKIIα-Jaws-KGC-GFP-ER2.
(A) Histological section imaged at 10X showing expression of Jaws (green) following a series of viral vector injections along a single injectrode penetration in area LIP (different monkey and brain area than was tested in experiments described below). Expression was strongest in layers II/III and V. Red: NeuN. Blue: DAPI. Scale bar = 1 mm. (B) 20X image of a nearby section stained for Jaws-GFP (green) and the inhibitory marker parvalbumin (PV, red). Scale bar = 500 µm. (C) Cell counts from the image in B, quantifying the small but nontrivial minority of neurons double-labeled for Jaws-GFP and PV. Approximate layer boundaries are indicated in gray.
In each experimental session, we advanced a custom optrode into area MT and began searching for candidate sites for testing the effects of photosuppression on behavior. If a suitable site was found (see Materials and methods), the monkey commenced the task described above, with low-power red illumination (633 nm, total power = 0.25–2.0 mW, irradiance = 2–16 mW/mm2 at a distance of 300 µm) delivered concurrently with visual stimulation (laser delayed by a 20 ms) on a random half of trials. Note that unlike previous studies, a red-shifted opsin was chosen not for its suitability for large-volume tissue illumination (Acker et al., 2016) but for its superior photocurrent at very low irradiance (Chuong et al., 2014). Optrode design, laser power, and site selection criteria were all intended to limit the light-induced suppression of activity to a relatively small cluster of MT neurons with consistent receptive field and tuning properties (i.e., preferred speed and direction of motion). We estimated tissue irradiance as a function of laser power and distance from the fiber tip using available online tools (https://web.stanford.edu/group/dlab/cgi-bin/graph/chart.php) as well as Monte Carlo simulations (Stujenske et al., 2015) (Figure 3—figure supplement 1). These estimates suggest that physiologically effective irradiance levels extended at most 300–500 µm from the fiber tip, which we confirmed qualitatively in pilot experiments using a fiber affixed to a linear electrode array (V-Probe, Plexon, Inc., Dallas, TX). After identifying a candidate site, we titrated the laser power to achieve an amount of suppression just shy of maximum (saturating) levels (Figure 3—figure supplement 2).
During the mapping procedure, we occasionally isolated single neurons and used this opportunity to better quantify the physiological effects of the light. Single-unit visual responses to high-coherence RDM stimuli—presented at the preferred direction and speed—were strongly suppressed on average (N = 26; Figure 3A), although a handful of neurons showed no effect or an increase in firing rate. Presumably this handful of cells includes those that did not express Jaws, as well as a small minority that were indirectly activated through polysynaptic mechanisms. Among putative Jaws-expressing neurons, the visually evoked response, after subtracting baseline activity, was suppressed by 101% (±13% SEM; N = 20; Figure 3B). Without baseline subtraction, the average firing rate during stimulus presentation was reduced by 72% ± 5%. In contrast, MU responses during the discrimination task were reduced by an average of 33% (±0.3% SEM, N = 20058 trials; Figure 3C). The fractional degree of suppression (Equation 4; Materials and methods), calculated from MU activity, varied substantially both within and across sessions (Figure 3A, inset; Figure 4A). We take this variability into consideration in the analyses to follow.

Photosupression of neural activity in area MT.
(A) Fractional change in firing rate of isolated single units (N = 26) in response to a high-coherence RDM stimulus and laser suppression, relative to RDM stimulus alone. Single-unit data were collected during a fixation task. The inset shows the fractional change in multi-unit activity recorded at the 23 sites tested in the discrimination task. (B) Average firing rate (peristimulus time histogram) of single units showing significant Jaws-mediated suppression (N = 20). Laser onset occurred 20 ms after stimulus onset, resulting in suppression that preceded the onset of visually-driven activity (i.e., firing rate driven below baseline, then recovered to near-baseline levels during visual stimulation). A post-suppression rebound of activity was observed after turning off the laser (and visual stimulus). All PSTHs depict spike counts in 1 ms bins convolved with a 40 ms causal boxcar filter and converted to spikes/s (C, D) Average multi-unit activity for N = 23 sites passing the selection criteria for the behavioral experiment (see Materials and methods), aligned to stimulus (dots) onset (C) and offset (D). The majority of sessions included a ramp-down of 140 ms in laser power starting 20 ms after stimulus offset, reducing the post-suppression burst seen when no ramp-down was used (inset). Shaded region shows ± SEM. Figure 3—figure supplement 1 shows an estimated spatial distribution of irradiance based on Monte Carlo simulations of light transmission in brain tissue, and Figure 3—figure supplement 2 illustrates an example of varying laser power at a fixed distance.
-
Figure 3—source data 1
Data and Matlab code for reproducing all panels and figure supplements for Figure 3.
- https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.36523.009

Behavioral effects of photosuppression depend on the magnitude of reduction in neural activity.
(A) Histogram of the fractional change in multi-unit firing rate (∆R) on laser trials, relative to the mean on no-laser trials for the corresponding session and trial type (Equation 4). (B) Behavioral effects, expressed as the horizontal shift in the choice function (Equation 1, red) and bell-shaped confidence function (Equation 3, blue), plotted as a function of ∆R. Each abscissa coordinate represents the mean of a sliding quantile, hence all the data in A are represented even though the abscissa range is different. Shaded error regions indicate ± SEM. Figure 4—figure supplement 2 shows the results of a corresponding control analysis of no-laser trials. (C) Choice functions for the set of laser trials (red) exhibiting strong suppression (∆R < −0.45, N = 3107 trials) compared to all no-laser trials (black, N = 9249 trials). (D) The corresponding confidence functions (proportion sure-bet choices as a function of motion strength) for the set of trials shown in C. For comparison, Figure 4—figure supplement 1 shows the effects of electrical microstimulation from previous experiments in monkey D, and Figure 4—figure supplement 3 shows the distribution of effects session by session, including all trials irrespective of ∆R and other influencing factors (see below).
-
Figure 4—source data 1
Data and Matlab code for reproducing all panels and figure supplements for Figure 4.
- https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.36523.014
Note that the rebound of spiking after abrupt laser offset (Figure 3B), a known byproduct of photosuppression, was mitigated by introducing a 140 ms ramp-down of laser power in the majority of behavioral sessions (Figure 3D; Chuong et al., 2014). We did not detect a difference in behavioral effects when the rebound was suppressed, compared to an earlier subset of sessions without the ramp-down (Figure 3D, inset), and therefore pooled all sessions for subsequent analyses.
Behavioral effects of photosuppression
To introduce the behavioral analyses presented below, we first revisit results from recent µStim experiments using the same behavioral task (Fetsch et al., 2014a). Microstimulation in areas MT and MST altered choice and confidence in a manner that largely mimics a change in the motion strength, with a directionality predicted by the preferred direction of the stimulated neurons (Figure 4—figure supplement 1). Although there were subtle changes in the slope of the choice function, and the height and width of the confidence function (see Discussion), here for simplicity we focus on the lateral shift of the functions in units of motion strength (% coherence; Equations 1 and 3, Materials and methods). The direction and size of these shifts in the previous study were well matched in the aggregate, and highly correlated across sessions, suggesting that MT/MST activity contributes to a common decision variable that governs both choice and confidence (Fetsch et al., 2014a; Kiani and Shadlen, 2009).
Under this interpretation, suppression of MT activity—provided it is targeted to a population of neurons that share a common tuning preference—should cause shifts in the opposite direction: fewer choices in the preferred direction, and a rightward shift of the bell-shaped confidence function. Moreover, one would expect behavioral effects to depend on the degree of suppression, which as noted above, was highly variable. For all laser trials in each session, we calculated the fractional change in multi-unit firing rate (∆R; Equation 4) relative to the mean of no-laser trials for the corresponding session and trial type (motion direction and coherence; Figure 4A). We then estimated the behavioral effect size (Equations 1 and 3) as a function of neuronal effect size, using a sliding window of trials pooled across sessions and sorted by ∆R. This analysis (Figure 4B) showed that photosuppression caused a modest but significant choice bias in the predicted direction, provided that the degree of suppression was greater than about 45% (Figure 4C; shift = −2.1% coh±0.6% SE; p=0.001). The relationship between ∆R and the effect on confidence was more complex (Figure 4B, blue trace), but the largest effects were also observed when suppression was strong (Figure 4D; shift = −2.9% ± 1.4%, p=0.03). We therefore limited most of the following analyses to trials showing clear effects on neural activity, using an arbitrary cutoff of 25% (∆R < −0.25, unless noted), rather than 45%, to increase statistical power.
To address potential contributions of neuronal response variability, independent of variability in the degree of suppression, we performed the following control analysis. Taking only no-laser trials, we randomly assigned 50% of them a fictitious category (“sham laser trials”), sorted these trials as above by their fractional difference (∆R) relative to the mean for a given session and trial type (Figure 4—figure supplement 2A), and performed the same sliding-window analysis of choice bias and shifts of the confidence function. We then repeated the procedure 100 times with different randomized trial assignments and averaged the results. The resulting traces (Figure 4—figure supplement 2B) showed a weak trend consistent with a covariation between response fluctuations and behavior (i.e., choice probability; Britten et al., 1996), but which was several fold smaller than in the original analysis that included actual photosuppression (Figure 4B) and not statistically significant (p=0.4, Equation 5, ). In direct comparison with the effects shown in Figure 4C–D, the behavioral shifts on a selected group of control trials with matching ∆R (i.e., less than -0.45) were significantly smaller (p<10−5 for choice and p = 0.02 for confidence). This result implies that the behavioral effects we observed under strong photosuppression cannot be explained away by normal response variability and its covariation with choice (and confidence).
Temporal factors influence behavioral effects: compensation on multiple time scales?
The result shown in Figure 4B implies that photosuppression was only effective at altering behavior for a subset of trials. Indeed, most individual sessions failed to show statistically reliable effects on behavior when analyzed in their entirety (Figure 4—figure supplement 3). However, while collecting the data we noticed a tendency for systematic biases on laser trials to appear early within a session, only to dissipate over the course of tens of minutes (100–500 trials; ~5 s per trial on average, including intertrial intervals). This can be seen in the example session shown in Figure 5. Over the first 500 trials, the monkey tended to choose the preferred direction less often on laser trials vs. no-laser trials, effectively shifting the psychometric curve to the right (Figure 5A; shift = −4.6%, p=0.14, Equation 1). In contrast, there was essentially no effect on choices in the remaining ~1000 trials of the session (Figure 5B; shift = −0.7% coh, p=0.75). The pattern was similar for the confidence assay: a rightward shift of the confidence function on early trials (Figure 5C; shift = −4.2%, p=0.34) but not later trials (Figure 5D, 1.2%, p=0.80). This trend was evident when pooling across all sessions with enough trials to measure it (minimum 800 trials per session, N = 10 sessions): rightward shifts early in the session (choice shift = −3.5% coh±1.1%, p=0.001, Figure 5A'; confidence shift = −3.8% ± 1.9% [Equation 3], p=0.05, Figure 5C') but not later (choice shift = −0.7%, p=0.26, Figure 5B'; confidence shift = 0.1%, p=0.93; Figure 5D'). Post-hoc tests using generalized linear models (Equation 5, see Materials and methods) showed that the difference between early and late trials was statistically significant for choice (p=0.01) and confidence (p=0.02, Bonferroni corrected).

Behavioral, but not neural, effects of photosuppression dissipated over time within a session.
(A, B) Choice functions for laser (red) and no-laser (black) trials for an example session, separated by trials early within the session (A, trials 1–500) versus later in the same session (B, trial number >500). Error bars show standard error of the proportions. (C, D) Corresponding confidence functions for the same groups of trials in A and B, respectively. The decrease in overall sure-bet proportion between C and D, indicating an increase in overall confidence over the course of a session, was a behavioral peculiarity of one monkey that was unrelated to photosuppression (i.e., occurred throughout training and in no-laser control sessions). (E,F) Average multi-unit activity showing a similar degree of suppression for early (E) and late (F) trials. Shaded regions indicate ± SEM. (G,H) Average firing rate on no-laser trials, early (G) vs. late (H) in the session, separated by signed motion coherence (positive = preferred direction, magenta; negative = null (antipreferred) direction, cyan). (A'–H') Same as A–H but for all sessions with >800 trials (N = 10 sessions, 4438 laser trials with ∆R < −0.25, 6489 no-laser trials).
-
Figure 5—source data 1
Data and Matlab code for reproducing all panels for Figure 5.
- https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.36523.016
Salzman et al. (1992) reported that the effects of µStim dissipated over the course of the behavioral sessions, which they interpreted as indicating neuronal fatigue, damage, or shifts in the position of the stimulating electrode. In our case, however, the decrease in effectiveness of photosuppression on behavior was not accompanied by an attenuation of its effect on neural responses. In the example session (Figure 5E vs. Figure 5F), the degree of suppression was slightly greater in early versus late trials (∆R = −0.80 ± 0.01 and −0.74 ± 0.01, respectively; p<0.05, bootstrap confidence interval, resampling trials with replacement), but this quantitative difference seems unlikely to explain the complete nullification of the behavioral effects (Figure 5B,D). Across all 10 sessions in the pooled dataset (Figure 5E' vs. Figure 5F') the trend was in the opposite direction, toward greater suppression on late trials (∆R early = −0.28 ± 0.008, late = −0.40 ± 0.004, p<0.05). Furthermore, we did not observe a qualitative change in the direction selectivity of the affected neurons between early and late trials (Figure 5G vs. Figure 5H; and Figure 5G' vs. Figure 5H'). At the conclusion of a subset of sessions, we re-measured direction tuning at multiple depths and found little evidence for optrode drift, tissue damage or response fatigue. Instead, the results suggest a compensatory change in the downstream readout of MT neural signals to reduce or nullify the effects of photosuppression on a time scale of 10–40 minutes.
To examine this putative compensation at a finer grain, we plotted the choice and confidence effects calculated in a sliding window of trials sorted by trial number after pooling across sessions (Figure 6A; N = 8057 trials from 23 sessions; see Materials and methods). Interestingly, the effect on confidence (blue trace) appears to dissipate more quickly than the effect on choice, and ‘overshoots’ zero for a period of time roughly between trial numbers 500–800 (~40–65 min), before settling back to zero later in the session (Figure 6A). Note that positive values on the ordinate indicate leftward shifts of the function, as observed previously with µStim, and are thus opposite of the expected effect from inactivation. Although the trend was not statistically significant (p=0.14; Equation 3 applied to trial numbers 550–750), it raises the possibility of a dissociation between choice and confidence on this particular subset of trials (see Discussion). Lastly, for comparison with previous work, we performed the same analysis on the µStim data from monkey D (Fetsch et al., 2014a). Unlike photosuppression, and contrary to the findings of Salzman et al. (1992), the biases in choice and confidence induced by µStim remained stable throughout the behavioral session (Figure 6—figure supplement 1A). This result implies that the capacity of cortical circuits to compensate for artificially induced activity patterns may be greater for photosuppression compared to µStim, although we cannot ascertain whether the key difference is the sign (suppression versus stimulation) or the modality (optogenetic versus electrical) of the perturbation.

Attenuation of behavioral effects of photosuppression on long and short time scales.
(A) Shifts of the choice (red) and confidence (blue) functions (± SEM) as a function of trial number (~8 min per 100 trials). Trials were pooled across sessions, conditioned on ∆R < −0.25 and duration <300 ms (N = 8057), and behavioral effects (Equations 1 and 3) were calculated over a sliding window of 1750 trials sorted by trial number. (B) Corresponding sliding-window analysis of trials sorted by stimulus/laser duration, after conditioning on ∆R < −0.25 and trial number <500. N = 7381 trials, window width = 1600 trials. Figure 6—figure supplement 1 shows the absence of such attenuation in µStim data collected previously. Figure 6—figure supplement 2 repeats these analyses after omitting one outlier session (see Materials and methods and Figure 4—figure supplement 3).
-
Figure 6—source data 1
Data and Matlab code for reproducing all panels and figure supplements for Figure 6.
- https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.36523.020
The presence of compensatory changes in readout across trials led us to wonder whether such changes could occur on a faster time scale as well. We addressed this question by grouping trials according to the experimenter-controlled stimulus (and laser) duration, which varied randomly across trials (range = 95–925 ms, truncated exponential distribution; Figure 1—figure supplement 1A). Remarkably, we found that the behavioral effects of photosuppression were largely restricted to trials with short and intermediate durations (duration <~350 ms; Figure 6B). Figure 7 depicts this trend pooled across all sessions, in the same format as Figure 5 and limited to the first 500 trials of each session. Short-duration trials exhibited light-induced shifts of −4.7% (p=0.0006, Figure 7A) and −5.7% (p=0.004, Figure 7C) for choice and confidence respectively, whereas long-duration trials showed no systematic biases in either measure (choice: −0.6%, p=0.50; confidence: 0.38%, p=0.84). The effect of duration was statistically significant (Equation 5, Materials and methods) for confidence (p=0.002, Bonferroni corrected) and marginally so for choice (p=0.04).

Compensation on a sub-second time scale.
Behavioral effects of photosuppression were present on trials with stimulus/laser epochs of short duration but not of longer durations (left and right columns, respectively), whereas the degree of suppression and direction selectivity were similar across durations. Format as in Figure 5. Data include trials with ∆R < −0.25 and trial number <500 (N = 4249 and 3132 trials for left and right columns, respectively). All durations were randomly interleaved.
-
Figure 7—source data 1
Data and Matlab code for reproducing all panels for Figure 7.
- https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.36523.022
As with the slower form of compensation, this sub-second attenuation of behavioral effects was not readily explained by differences in MT neural responses on short- versus long-duration trials (Figure 7E–H). We did observe weaker direction selectivity during the latter half of the stimulus presentation on long-duration trials (Figure 7H). It is conceivable that these late epochs contributed less to behavior, under the assumption that neural signals contribute in proportion to their sensitivity or informativeness (Britten et al., 1996; Gu et al., 2007; Purushothaman and Bradley, 2005). However, the relevant comparison here is between short- and long-duration trials, not early versus late within a trial. Unsurprisingly, neuronal sensitivity was greater on long-duration trials when including all spiking activity throughout the stimulus epoch (neuronal threshold = 27.2 ± 1.6% coh versus 37.5 ± 2.5% for short durations; see Materials and methods). Thus, without additional assumptions, the weaker direction selectivity at time points >300 ms (less separation of the traces on the right side of Figure 7H versus 7G) cannot explain the inability of photosuppression to affect behavior on long-duration trials. Indeed, any candidate explanation for this result must contend with the fact that activity early in the stimulus epoch was suppressed equally on short- and long-duration trials (Figure 7E vs. 7F), yet only the former showed behavioral effects. It seems improbable that downstream circuits could treat such early activity differently, because at any given time during a trial there would be no way to predict whether the stimulus (and laser) would terminate in the next moment or continue. Below we discuss possible explanations and implications of this unexpected finding.
Discussion
Focal optogenetic suppression
The results demonstrate that systematic behavioral effects of optogenetic manipulation in nonhuman primates are achievable even when this requires the targeting of neural populations on a small spatial scale, that of cortical columns or clusters of neurons with similar functional properties. This is the scale at which electrical microstimulation has proven most effective, but nonhuman primate neuroscience has lacked an inactivation counterpart with comparable spatial and temporal specificity, relying on pharmacological and cooling methods that persist for minutes to days and affect larger swaths of tissue (but see Afraz et al., 2015). There is justifiable excitement surrounding the development of optogenetic methods for targeting particular cell types or anatomical projections (El-Shamayleh et al., 2017; Klein et al., 2016; Stauffer et al., 2016; Ohayon, 2017), and many questions will require such an approach. Our study shows that another form of targeting—that of spatially or topographically organized populations—is viable in monkeys using off-the-shelf viral vectors combined with careful site selection and light delivery.
A common mechanism for choice and confidence
Decision confidence is defined as the subjective degree of belief that one has chosen the correct or more rewarding option among alternatives. In many natural situations, the decision maker does not receive immediate feedback on the accuracy of a choice, but instead must rely on confidence to guide subsequent decisions that depend on previous outcomes (Averbeck, 2015; van den Berg et al., 2016b). Confidence also has a powerful influence on learning and adapting to new environments. For instance, when negative feedback follows a choice made with high confidence, it implies that something about the world has changed, triggering a shift in behavioral strategy or an increase in learning rate (Purcell and Kiani, 2016; Yu and Dayan, 2005).
The neural basis of assigning confidence in a decision is not well understood. One class of hypotheses asserts that the processing of the evidence that supports a choice simultaneously furnishes an assessment of the reliability of the source of evidence, and hence a prediction about whether the choice will be correct (for reviews see Fetsch et al., 2014b; Kepecs and Mainen, 2012; Meyniel et al., 2015; Pouget et al., 2016; Yeung and Summerfield, 2012). In this vein, Kiani and Shadlen (2009) demonstrated how choice, reaction time, and confidence can arise through a common mechanism of evidence accumulation, and several studies have since provided additional support for this idea (Fetsch et al., 2014a; Kiani et al., 2014; van den Berg et al., 2016a; Zylberberg et al., 2016). Using the same task as in the present study, we found that the quantitative link between choice and confidence was preserved under MT/MST microstimulation (Fetsch et al., 2014a). Changes in both measures—and their specific relationship with motion strength and viewing duration—evinced a commensurate change in the strength of momentary evidence, and the improvement in discrimination sensitivity when confidence was high (i.e., Figure 1C) was retained, as predicted by a bounded evidence accumulation model.
The present study provides an inactivation counterpart to this approach. Photosuppression in MT biased the animal’s choices away from the preferred direction, and also altered confidence in a way that mimics a change in motion strength (i.e., a rightward shift of the bell-shaped curve describing sure-bet choices as a function of coherence). The effects were reasonably well matched as a function of the degree of suppression (Figure 4B), and they dissipated over a similar time course, both across (Figure 6A) and within trials (Figure 6B). These results are consistent with a role for MT neurons in providing a shared source of evidence supporting both aspects of a decision. Some features of the data may seem, at first glance, incompatible with this straightforward account. For instance, photosuppression appears to have exerted greater effects on confidence when the motion was in the antipreferred direction (negative coherence values, see Figures 4D, 5C’ and 7C), and this asymmetry was often accompanied by a subtle increase in the slope of the choice function (most evident in Figure 7A). Interestingly, these observations can be reconciled in a bounded accumulation framework, the interpretation being that photosuppression caused both a bias and an increase in discrimination sensitivity by improving the signal-to-noise, just as µStim appeared to reduce it (Fetsch et al., 2014a). Such a change would affect both the slope of the choice function and the width of the confidence function, the latter giving rise to the asymmetry. Our data set lacks sufficient power to conclusively support or reject this possibility, but if borne out by additional experiments, it would be within a family of previously observed alterations to signal and noise that jointly affect confidence and choice in a manner predicted by bounded accumulation (Zylberberg et al., 2016).
Other subtle anomalies are not as easily incorporated in this framework. The effect of photosuppression on confidence (Figure 6A, blue) dissipated more rapidly across trials than the effect on choices (Figure 6A, red), and even transiently shifted in the opposite direction (positive, or leftward). Effects at the shortest durations (Figure 6B) were greater for confidence than for choice (but see Figure 6—figure supplement 1 to compare to the size of such deviations observed with µStim). Lastly, the size of the effect on choices tended to be greater on trials without the sure-bet option available, compared to when it was available (data not shown), which was not the case for µStim (Fetsch et al., 2014b). None of these discrepancies reached statistical significance, but taken together, they suggest the possibility that the neural substrates of choice and confidence might be partly dissociable at the level of the representation of sensory evidence. It would be of interest to deploy manipulations that alter the correspondence between choice and confidence to a greater degree than what we observed here, while recording simultaneously from sensory and higher order areas. Recent studies in humans point toward several frontal lobe structures involved in generating and reporting the sense of confidence (Bang and Fleming, 2018; Fleming et al., 2015; Morales et al., 2018; Shekhar and Rahnev, 2018). Little is known about where and how such computations occur in the nonhuman primate brain, although the supplementary eye field seems to play a role (Middlebrooks and Sommer, 2012; So and Stuphorn, 2016).
Compensatory changes in the readout of sensory cortical activity
Behavioral effects of photosuppression were strongly attenuated over the course of the session (Figures 5 and 6A). This trend was not explained by differences in the physiological effectiveness of photosuppression, nor in the responsiveness or selectivity of the affected neurons. Rather, the results suggest a form of compensation or down-weighting of MT signals by downstream areas that are reading out this activity to form a decision. Because the decision in our task is rendered with a saccadic eye movement, the readout (and hence compensation) likely involves a network of areas including the lateral intraparietal area, frontal eye field, and superior colliculus, although probably by way of the thalamus rather than direct projections from MT.
What is the nature of the signal that triggers this compensation? In general, a pure choice bias will tend to increase the error rate, so a mechanism that detects and counteracts the bias could be beneficial in terms of maximizing reward. Downstream circuits could, in principle, learn to discount signals that were associated with a lower rate of reward, even though the perturbation was randomly interleaved across trials. However, in our experiments the reward rate was about 4% greater on laser trials compared to no-laser trials, owing to the small magnitude of the bias and the subtle increase in sensitivity. Thus, it is difficult to see how a reward-based associative mechanism could play a major role.
We think the key may lie in the faster form of compensation, revealed by the inability of photosuppression to influence behavior on long-duration trials (Figures 6B, 7B,D). This observation is surprising because the different durations were randomly interleaved, suggesting that the compensation must be induced by the suppression itself and initiated within a few tenths of a second. We do not know how this fast compensation comes about, but it is not a trivial consequence of the brain placing greater weight on sensory evidence arriving earlier within a trial (e.g., Kiani et al., 2008; Raposo et al., 2012), since early MT activity is suppressed to the same degree on long versus short trials (Figure 7E,F). Instead, the results motivate the hypothesis that the suppressed activity is sensed as anomalous or unreliable by downstream circuitry. That is, unlike µStim, the suppression of cortical tissue might have an effect that is analogous to diaschisis (Carrera and Tononi, 2014; Otchy et al., 2015), triggering a reconfiguration of functional connectivity in downstream networks. This reconfiguration could cause structures involved in decision making to consult other sources of evidence, either elsewhere in MT or from other visual areas (e.g., MST, V3a, or V4). Initially this happens only after the first few hundred ms within a trial, then gradually the readout becomes able to rely on these signals from the start of the trial, resulting in late trials showing no effect of suppression even when limiting the analysis to short durations (Figure 6A). This account is admittedly speculative, but it may suggest an opportunity for filling a major gap in our understanding, namely how information is dynamically routed from a multitude of possible sources (both sensory and mnemonic; Shadlen and Shohamy, 2016) to the circuits responsible for decision and action.
Conclusion
The approach taken in this study was motivated by a large body of research linking the functional properties of sensory neurons to the formation of a perceptual decision (e.g., Afraz et al., 2006; DeAngelis et al., 1998; Romo et al., 2000; Salzman et al., 1992). In nonhuman primates, the effectiveness of modern causal tools for manipulating behavior has been limited, although not without successes. One sensible response to this reality is to develop methods for illuminating larger volumes of tissue (Acker et al., 2016; Gerits et al., 2012). In some cases, however, a more nuanced understanding of the relationship between neural activity and behavior can yield important observations that would be obscured by the large-volume approach. Naturally, the ideal spatial scale depends not only on the anatomy but on the specific question being addressed. For researchers seeking to exploit the temporal control afforded by optogenetic inactivation, we suggest it is also worthwhile to give consideration to the spatial distribution of functional properties, including columnar and laminar segregation (Chandrasekaran et al., 2017; Chen et al., 2017; Self et al., 2013), as well as mesoscale connectivity patterns (Lewis and Van Essen, 2000). Relating these properties to behavior in the context of a well-characterized task can furnish insights that go beyond qualitative statements about causal involvement.
The nullification of behavioral effects of photosuppression on two distinct time scales (Figures 5–7) invites caution when interpreting inactivation experiments that fail to affect behavior. A key role for MT in visual motion discrimination is not in doubt (but see Liu and Pack, 2017), but for other areas and tasks that have not been extensively studied, or where the results are more equivocal, it may be difficult to rule out compensation by circuits that perform the same or similar function (e.g., de Lafuente et al., 2015). The present study suggests that this compensation can operate on a faster time scale than previously appreciated, further advocating the use of rigorous behavioral methods to identify the appropriate time scale for a perturbation. While these observations pose challenges to experimental neuroscience, they also attest to the brain’s capacity for functional recovery after a small insult. One might hope that the tools for investigating causality in systems neuroscience could help expose mechanisms that mediate such recovery so that they may be exploited for neurological rehabilitation.
Materials and methods
Behavioral task
Request a detailed protocolTwo adult male rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) performed a direction discrimination task with post-decision wagering (PDW, Figure 1A), as described previously (Fetsch et al., 2014a; Kiani and Shadlen, 2009; Zylberberg et al., 2016). In brief, animals were required to fixate a central target, after which two direction-choice targets appeared 9° to the left and right of the fixation point, followed by a dynamic random-dot motion (RDM) stimulus. Properties of the RDM (patch size, position, and dot speed) were set for each experimental session to match the aggregate receptive field (RF) and tuning properties of neurons throughout a 200–400 µm region near the optrode tip (see below for site selection criteria). The monkeys had previously performed only left-versus-right discriminations for several years and were resistant to training on up-versus-down, thus we bypassed sites with preferred directions >30° away from horizontal. Motion strength, or coherence (percent coherently moving dots), on each trial was sampled uniformly from the set {0, 3.2, 6.4, 12.8, 25.6, 51.2}, and stimulus duration was drawn from a truncated exponential distribution with range = 95–925 ms, mean = 350 ms, and median = 300 ms.
After motion offset, the monkey maintained fixation through a variable delay period during which a third target (the sure-bet target, Ts) was presented on a random half of trials. Ts differed in color and size from the direction choice targets, and was positioned 6° above the fixation point, perpendicular to the axis of motion and the direction-choice targets. Whether or not Ts was presented, the delay period ended with disappearance of the fixation point, which served as a ‘go’ cue for the monkey to saccade to one of the targets. If Ts was available, the monkey could choose it and receive a guaranteed reward (drop of water or juice), or waive it and make the higher-stakes direction choice; otherwise only the binary direction choice was available. Correct direction choices yielded a larger liquid reward than Ts choices, while errors resulted in a 4.5 s timeout. The ratio of sure-bet reward size to direction-choice reward size (mean = 0.51 for monkey D, 0.62 for monkey N) was set and periodically adjusted to encourage the animals to choose Ts approximately 60% of the time at the weakest motion strengths.
Surgery and injection of viral vector
View detailed protocolProcedures were in accordance with the Public Health Service Policy on Humane Care and Use of Laboratory Animals, and approved by Columbia University’s Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. Monkeys were surgically implanted with a head post and recording cylinder using aseptic technique. Cortical area MT (left hemisphere in monkey D, right in monkey N) was initially targeted using structural MRI scans and confirmed using established physiological criteria prior to injection of virus.
Viral vector injections were performed while the animals were awake and seated in a primate chair. A glass microinjection pipette (115 µm outer diameter; Thomas Recording GmbH, Giessen, Germany) was affixed to a tungsten microelectrode (75 µm shank diameter; FHC, Inc., Bowdoin, ME) with cyanoacrylate to create a custom ‘injectrode’ which was passed through a transdural guide tube and advanced into area MT using a hydraulic microdrive. The metal connector at the back of the pipette was coupled with flexible tubing to a Hamilton syringe loaded with 5–10 µl of virus (AAV8-CamKIIα-Jaws-KGC-GFP-ER2; titer = 5.9×1012 genomes/ml; UNC Vector Core, Chuong et al., 2014).
After locating a stretch of gray matter with neuronal responses consistent with the functional properties of MT, we advanced the injectrode to the deepest point of this stretch and began the first of a series of injections. Using a syringe pump (Harvard Apparatus, Holliston, MA), we injected 0.75–1.0 µl at a rate of 0.05 µl/min at each of several locations spaced 400–500 µm apart. Each injection was followed by a 10 min wait period before slowly (5 µm/s) retracting the injectrode to the next site. This process continued until reaching the shallowest point of the target region, resulting in 5–8 sites and a total of 4–7 µl injected along a given track. On subsequent days, the procedure was repeated in 1 (monkey N) or 3 (monkey D) neighboring grid holes (1 mm apart) for a total injected volume of 13 and 19 µl, respectively. We then waited at least 8 weeks before beginning experiments. The tissue remained viable and responsive to light for at least 12 months post-injection in monkey D and at least 9 months in monkey N. Some virus likely reached portions of nearby area MST, but all sites tested in the behavioral task were classified as residing in MT based on published criteria (Churchland et al., 2007; Komatsu and Wurtz, 1988; Tanaka et al., 1986), including RF size versus eccentricity, tuning for slower dot speeds (<20°/s), and position relative to the white matter ventral to the sulcus.
To examine viral vector-mediated opsin expression histologically, we injected a third animal (monkey E) following the same procedure and using the identical vector stock injected in monkeys N and D. Monkeys N and D remain actively contributing to other projects, thus precluding histological analysis of these animals. Monkey E received a total of 6.5 µl of vector delivered at seven injection sites spaced 400 µm apart along a single penetration. These injections were made in the lateral bank of the intraparietal sulcus, rather than in MT, due to constraints imposed by an existing recording chamber.
One month after the injections, monkey E was euthanized with an overdose of pentobarbital and perfused transcardially with 4% paraformaldehyde followed by a gradient of sucrose in phosphate buffer (10%, 20% and 30%). The brain was removed and cryoprotected in 30% sucrose. Coronal sections (50 μm) were cut on a sliding microtome and mounted onto slides. Transduced cells were first localized by inspecting native fluorescence signals in a series of sections spanning the intraparietal sulcus. Additional sections near the region of strongest expression were processed immunohistochemically using primary antibodies against GFP (Abcam 13970 RRID:AB_300798, 1:1000; Abcam, Cambridge, MA) and against the pan-neuronal marker NeuN (Millipore MAB377 RRID:AB_2298772; MilliporeSigma, Burlington, MA) or the inhibitory neuron marker parvalbumin (Swant PV235 RRID:AB_10000343 1:5000; Swant, Marly, Switzerland), and using secondary antibodies (Invitrogen Molecular Probes, Thermo Fisher Scientific, Waltham, MA): Alexa 594 (A21203 RRID:AB_141633, 1:400), Alexa 568 (A10042 RRID:AB_2534017, 1:400), Alexa 488 (custom, 1:400) and the nuclear stain DAPI (Invitrogen Molecular Probes D-21490, 1:5000) for visualization by epifluorescence microscopy. An upper bound on transduction selectivity for excitatory neurons was coarsely estimated by counting the number of GFP-positive, PV-positive and double-labeled somata in a representative section imaged at 20X (Figure 2B,C).
Optrode design, site selection and photosuppression protocol
Request a detailed protocolPhotosuppression was achieved using a custom optrode of similar design to the injectrode described above: a tungsten microelectrode (75 or 100 µm shank diameter,~1 MOhm; FHC) glued to an optical fiber (Thorlabs UM22-100, core+cladding diameter=110 µm; Thorlabs, Newton, NJ). The fiber tip was sharpened as follows to reduce tissue damage and generate a broader light cone (Dai et al., 2014; Hanks et al., 2015): After stripping the fiber jacket, the tip was immersed in 48% hydrofluoric acid to a depth of 0.5–1.0 mm, with the polyimide coating intact to enhance smoothness and reproducibility of the resulting tip (‘tube etching’; Lambelet et al., 1998; Stöckle et al., 1999). The immersion took place within the narrow end of a standard 100 µl pipette tip containing the acid and sealed with Parafilm. After 30–45 min, the acid was rinsed off and the polyimide coating mechanically stripped, leaving 6–10 mm of bare cladding and a conical tip with full angle of approximately 15°. The sharpened fiber was then passed through the guide tube along with an electrode and the two were glued together using a tiny amount of cyanoacrylate, with the electrode tip positioned 300–500 µm ahead of the fiber tip. The source end of the fiber patch cable was connected to a 633 nm diode laser (LuxX +633–100, Omicron-Laserage Laserprodukte GmbH, Rodgau, Germany) under analog and digital control.
Prior to each session, we used a handheld power meter (Thorlabs PM160) to calibrate the total light power as a function of analog input to the laser controller. Given the known values for the beam half-angle in air (20–30°) and separation between fiber and electrode tips (300–500 µm), we estimated the irradiance of neurons near the electrode tip using the brain tissue light transmission calculator provided by the Deisseroth Lab a Stanford University (https://web.stanford.edu/group/dlab/cgi-bin/graph/chart.php), as well as additional Monte Carlo simulations (Stujenske et al., 2015) (Figure 3—figure supplement 1). The goal (see below) was to use the minimum irradiance required to saturate the reduction in firing rates of neurons within a small region of interest (300–500 µm diameter), taking advantage of the fact that irradiance in tissue falls off exponentially with distance. Thus, total light power was nearly always kept below 2 mW (16 mW/mm2 at a distance of 300 µm) and was typically between 0.3 and 1.0 mW (1.2–4.0 mW/mm2) during the discrimination task (see Figure 3—figure supplement 2).
After advancing the optrode into area MT and pausing 20–40 min to allow the tissue to stabilize, we isolated single-unit (SU) or multi-unit (MU) activity using Plexon SortClient software (Plexon, Inc.). RF size, position, and selectivity for motion direction and speed were assessed using briefly flashed, 99% coherence RDM stimuli while the animal fixated a central target. Candidate sites were mapped extensively before each behavioral session to ensure consistency of tuning/RF parameters across at least 200 and preferably >300 µm of cortex. Responsiveness to red light was concurrently assessed by randomly interleaving laser trials (laser + RDM) with no-laser trials (RDM only), and sites were bypassed unless we observed a significant decrease in MU firing rate on laser versus no-laser trials (t-test, p<0.01) throughout the target area. A site was considered provisionally acceptable if (a) RF and tuning parameters remained relatively stable (∆ preferred direction <45°) for at least 200 µm, (b) direction selectivity was sufficiently strong, with at least two standard deviations separating preferred and null (antipreferred) direction MU responses, and (c) activity on laser trials was reduced by at least 10%. Once we encountered an acceptable site, we attempted to position the optrode at the optimal depth with respect to the above considerations, and commenced the discrimination task. Twenty-seven sites were provisionally accepted (17 in monkey D, 10 in monkey N), but 4 sites in monkey D were rejected post-hoc after failing to maintain robust suppression of MU activity throughout the session.
On a random half of discrimination trials, including Ts-present and Ts-absent trials, red light was delivered throughout the visual stimulus epoch. The laser power profile was a constant square pulse, and in a majority of sessions was terminated with a 140 ms linear ramp-down to reduce the post-suppression burst (Figure 3D and inset; Chuong et al., 2014). Laser onset began 20 ms after stimulus onset to partially account for visual response latency while ensuring that photosuppression began before the earliest feedforward visual inputs reached MT. Onset of the ramp-down began 20 ms after visual motion offset.
The fiber jacket and optrode shank were covered with opaque material such that no laser light was visible to the animal. Nevertheless, to rule out possible nonspecific effects of illumination, including tissue heating, we performed eight control sessions in which the optrode tip was positioned either near the surface of the brain (N = 2), or in the vicinity of the dorsal superior temporal sulcus but at least 2 mm outside the viral injection site (N = 6). No behavioral effects of illumination were observed in these control sessions.
Data analysis
Request a detailed protocolAll analyses were performed using custom routines in MATLAB (MathWorks, Natick, MA). We quantified the effect of photosuppression on behavioral choices using the logistic regression model:
Where Ppref is the probability of a preferred-direction choice, C is signed motion coherence (positive = preferred direction, negative = anti-preferred direction), and IL is an indicator variable for the presence/absence of laser illumination. The biasing effect of photosuppression is expressed in units of coherence by the ratio /, and its effect on discrimination sensitivity (slope) is captured by . The coefficients were fit by maximum likelihood estimation, with standard errors (SEs) estimated as the square roots of the diagonal elements of the inverse of the Hessian matrix. SEs were used to calculate t-statistics and corresponding p values to test the null hypothesis that a given . To quantify the difference in sensitivity when Ts was available versus unavailable (Figure 1C), we replaced IL in Equation 1 with an indicator variable for the presence/absence of Ts. For display purposes, smooth curves in Figure 4C, 5A,B and 7A,B were generated using the simpler model:
fitted separately to laser and no-laser trials. Unless otherwise indicated, plots and analyses of choices include both Ts-present (waived) and Ts-unavailable trials.
The effect of photosuppression on confidence was quantified by fitting the probability of a sure-bet choice (Psb) with the Gaussian function:
Where C is signed coherence, IL is the indicator variable for the laser, and A, µ, σ, and δ are free parameters. The parameter δ captures the photosuppression-induced lateral shift of the Gaussian in units of coherence.
For each laser trial, we estimated the degree of suppression of neural activity as follows. Multi-unit spike events were counted from 60 ms after RDM onset until 60 ms after RDM offset, then converted to spike rate (Rlaser) by dividing by the stimulus duration. The fractional change in spike rate (∆R) was then computed by comparing each Rlaser with the mean spike rate of a matching set of no-laser trials (), defined as being of the same session, motion direction, coherence, and quartile of stimulus duration:
Behavioral effects as a function of ∆R (e.g., Figure 4B) were computed by selecting laser trials with a given range of ∆R and comparing them to all no-laser trials across all sessions, using Equations 1 and 3 for choice and confidence, respectively. The traces in Figure 4B were mapped out by repeating this procedure for each of many sliding windows of trials sorted by ∆R, where the window width was 2100 trials (approximately ⅕ of laser trials, i.e. a sliding quintile) and the step size was 10 trials. Similarly, Figure 6A and B were constructed by sorting trials by the variable on the abscissa (conditioned on two other post-hoc variables; see below) and computing behavioral effects using a sliding quartile window.
Having observed variation in the size of behavioral effects as a function of three main explanatory variables—fractional change in spike rate on laser trials (∆R), trial number within session (T), and stimulus/laser duration (D)—we performed post-hoc statistical tests of these relationships using binomial generalized linear models (logistic regression, using the MATLAB function glmfit). The trends depicted in Figure 6 were qualitatively similar in the two monkeys, thus we pooled the data for these analyses. One session showed clear shifts in the opposite direction than predicted by the local MU activity (red data point in upper-right quadrant of Figure 4—figure supplement 3), yet these paradoxical effects were still sensitive to trial number and duration. Since our main focus was on the time course of these trends rather than on the size of behavioral effects overall, we manually reversed the direction preference associated with this individual session before pooling with the rest of the data. Excluding this session did not alter the qualitative trends shown in Figures 5–7 or the results of the main statistical tests.
Each post-hoc GLM consisted of three predictors, plus all 2-way interaction terms. The predictors were: signed motion coherence (C), the indicator variable for presence of the laser (IL), and an indicator variable defining a median split for one of the three variables mentioned above (∆R, D, or T). For example, the model to test the influence of stimulus duration D on photosuppression-induced choice effects was specified by:
Where ID = 1 if D < 300 ms (the median duration) and 0 otherwise. The model for ∆R substituted I∆R (assigned to 1 if ∆R < -0.35, the median ∆R) in place of ID, and for trial number the corresponding variable was IT (1 if T < 578, the median trial number). Since the relationship between trial number and the confidence effect (Figure 6A, blue) was found to be non-monotonic, we used a lower cutoff value of T < 300 to define IT for that test. For each model, the strength of the interaction between the given explanatory variable and the biasing effect of photosuppression is captured by and its associated p value.
A similar strategy was used for the effects on confidence (replacing Ppref with Psb in Equation 5 and its counterparts for ∆R and T), except that the models were fit separately for trials with C < 0 and C > 0. This piecewise-logistic approach achieved adequate fits because each side of the bell-shaped relationship between Psb and C is well approximated by a sigmoid, albeit of opposite slope. This yielded two coefficients and corresponding p values, and the test was deemed significant if either were less than 0.05 after Bonferroni correction (multiplication by 2). To better isolate the effects of trial number, all GLM analyses, as well as the sliding-window plot in Figure 6A, included only trials with ∆R < −0.25 and D < 300 ms. Similarly, the GLMs for duration (and Figures 6B, 7A–D) were limited to only trials with ∆R < −0.25 and T < 500.
To compare neuronal direction selectivity across different subsets of trials—e.g., short vs. long trials (Figure 7)—we used a standard approach to calculate neural thresholds (Britten et al., 1992). For each trial in a given session, the MU spike rate was normalized to the mean spike rate for 51.2% coherence motion in the preferred direction during that session. Normalized spike rates were then pooled across sessions, grouped according to desired criteria (duration, trial number within session, etc.), and the distributions of spike rates for preferred- vs. antipreferred direction of motion were compared for a given coherence level using ROC analysis. This quantified the ability of an ideal observer to discriminate motion direction based on the neural responses. The performance of the ideal observer (percent correct as a function of coherence) was fit with a cumulative Weibull distribution (Quick, 1974) and the α parameter (coherence level generating 82% correct) was taken as the neuronal threshold.
Data availability
Matlab code (m-files) and data (.mat files) have been provided for Figure 1 and Figures 3-7.
References
-
Columnar organization of directionally selective cells in visual area MT of the macaqueJournal of Neurophysiology 51:16–31.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1984.51.1.16
-
Theory of choice in bandit, information sampling and foraging tasksPLoS Computational Biology 11:e1004164.https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004164
-
The analysis of visual motion: a comparison of neuronal and psychophysical performanceThe Journal of Neuroscience 12:4745–4765.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.12-12-04745.1992
-
Spatiotemporal effects of microstimulation in rat neocortex: a parametric study using multielectrode recordingsJournal of Neurophysiology 90:3024–3039.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00245.2003
-
Microstimulation of extrastriate area MST influences performance on a direction discrimination taskJournal of Neurophysiology 73:437–448.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1995.73.2.437
-
Noninvasive optical inhibition with a red-shifted microbial rhodopsinNature Neuroscience 17:1123–1129.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3752
-
Responses of neurons in the medial superior temporal visual area to apparent motion stimuli in macaque monkeysJournal of Neurophysiology 97:272–282.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00941.2005
-
What electrical microstimulation has revealed about the neural basis of cognitionCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology 14:169–177.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2004.03.016
-
Representation of accumulating evidence for a decision in two parietal areasJournal of Neuroscience 35:4306–4318.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2451-14.2015
-
Microstimulation of visual cortex affects the speed of perceptual decisionsNature Neuroscience 6:891–898.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1094
-
Predicting the accuracy of a decision: a neural mechanism of confidenceCold Spring Harbor Symposia on Quantitative Biology 79:185–197.https://doi.org/10.1101/sqb.2014.79.024893
-
Action-specific disruption of perceptual confidencePsychological Science 26:89–98.https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614557697
-
Spectral cancellation of microstimulation artifact for simultaneous neural recording in situIEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering 50:1129–1135.https://doi.org/10.1109/TBME.2003.816077
-
Neural computations that underlie decisions about sensory stimuliTrends in Cognitive Sciences 5:10–16.https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01567-9
-
A functional link between area MSTd and heading perception based on vestibular signalsNature Neuroscience 10:1038–1047.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1935
-
A computational framework for the study of confidence in humans and animalsPhilosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 367:1322–1337.https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2012.0037
-
The effects of electrical microstimulation on cortical signal propagationNature Neuroscience 13:1283–1291.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2631
-
Domain-general and domain-specific patterns of activity supporting metacognition in human prefrontal cortexThe Journal of Neuroscience 38:3534–3546.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2360-17.2018
-
Confidence and certainty: distinct probabilistic quantities for different goalsNature Neuroscience 19:366–374.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4240
-
Neural population code for fine perceptual decisions in area MTNature Neuroscience 8:99–106.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1373
-
Multisensory decision-making in rats and humansJournal of Neuroscience 32:3726–3735.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4998-11.2012
-
Touch and go: decision-making mechanisms in somatosensationAnnual Review of Neuroscience 24:107–137.https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.107
-
Microstimulation in visual area MT: effects on direction discrimination performanceThe Journal of Neuroscience 12:2331–2355.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.12-06-02331.1992
-
A computational analysis of the relationship between neuronal and behavioral responses to visual motionThe Journal of Neuroscience 16:1486–1510.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-04-01486.1996
-
Distinguishing the roles of dorsolateral and anterior PFC in visual metacognitionThe Journal of Neuroscience 38:5078–5087.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3484-17.2018
-
Supplementary eye field encodes confidence in decisions under riskCerebral Cortex 26:764–782.https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhv025
-
High-quality near-field optical probes by tube etchingApplied Physics Letters 75:160–162.https://doi.org/10.1063/1.124305
-
Confidence is the bridge between multi-stage decisionsCurrent Biology 26:3157–3168.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2016.10.021
-
Metacognition in human decision-making: confidence and error monitoringPhilosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 367:1310–1321.https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2011.0416
Decision letter
-
David C Van EssenSenior Editor; Washington University in St. Louis, United States
-
Ranulfo RomoReviewing Editor; Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Mexico
In the interests of transparency, eLife includes the editorial decision letter and accompanying author responses. A lightly edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the most substantive concerns; minor comments are not usually included.
Thank you for submitting your article "Focal optogenetic suppression in macaque area MT biases direction discrimination and decision confidence, but only transiently" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by three peer reviewers, one of whom is a member of our Board of Reviewing Editors, and the evaluation has been overseen by David Van Essen as the Senior Editor. The following individual involved in review of your submission has agreed to reveal their identity: Richard Born (Reviewer #2).
The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.
The reviewers agree that this is an outstanding study that makes use of a powerful visual discrimination task in which choice and confidence decision mechanisms that can be linked to the directionality of the preferred response of MT neurons. The reviewers agree that your suppression optogenetic approach in the preferred direction related MT neurons and its effects on choice and confidence constitutes an important advance to linking neural activity and behavior. Having said this, there are some comments by reviewers #2 and 3 and to a lesser extent by reviewer #1 that must be considered to improve/strengthen your paper:
1) It is not clear whether choice and confidence relate to the same decision variable, at least for the photo suppression results provided here (reviewer #2).
2) It would be useful to know whether your weak effects obtained on both choice and confidence were due to the low light power used; the reviewers wonder whether you used higher light power in other sessions (reviewers #2 and 3). This could show that the effects are more comparable to those reported before with your electrical micro-stimulation method. This might also shed light on the spatial extent of suppression by light and its power dependence (reviewers #2 and 3).
3) Please comment on whether you tried up-down discriminations while inactivating right-left preferred columns as a control (reviewers #2 and 3).
4) The short and long temporal suppressive effects on decisions were pooled from sessions in the two monkeys; the reviewers wonder about the behavioral performances of each animal.
Reviewer #1:
This study is technically well executed. This research group previously proved that electrical micro-stimulation of MT increased the speed of decisions in favor of the preferred direction while slowing decisions in favor of the opposite directions. However and according to the authors, there are technical limitations using the micro-stimulation technique, and therefore the need of some other ways of perturbing clusters of neurons associated with behavior. Here, they used an optogenetic approach in which they suppressed small clusters of excitatory neurons with selectivity for visual motion. They found that optogenetic suppression of small clusters of MT neurons induced a weak choice bias against the neurons' preferred direction, consistent with a common mechanism underlying choice and confidence. These weak effects on choice and confidence were supposedly due to compensatory changes in the read out of MT activity. A more intriguing result was that these weak changes were present only during the first third of the perturbing session (first 500 trials). This was consistently observed across many sessions. Importantly, in all the recorded neuronal sessions, single and multi-unit activity decreased during the optogenetic test, showing the reliability of this suppression approach. As said above, the paper is technically well executed and the experiment is well designed. Of course, one major concern relies on the suppression effects of MT activity and therefore on the behavioral results. The experiment is fair since it shows the opposite of what a micro-stimulation effect was in this task. I understand the authors for using new experimental tools, and appreciate the finesse of the statistical analyses and the explanation they gave on the temporal suppression effects on behavior. I should say that this paper is one of the finest ones that I have seen using the optogenetic tool in behavior. But, honestly, I did not learn much of this study.
Reviewer #2:
The transfer of optogenetic (and other virally or genetically targeted) methods to the nonhuman primate is a big deal, and the authors have made several important contributions to this endeavor. First, and most importantly, they tested the new method in the context of a behavioral task that is extremely well characterized at the anatomical, physiological, behavioral and theoretical levels. Moreover, they used a slight variation of this task ("post-decision wagering") that buys them better interpretability of any effects produced. The effects they observed, while relatively modest in size by microstim criteria, are convincing and highly intriguing. They go well beyond demonstrating the efficacy of the method to reveal new and interesting results, particularly with respect to the apparent downstream compensation for inactivation at both short (within trial) and longer (across trials) time scales. A critical advance over the microstim experiments is that, here, the authors have a trial-by-trial measure of the efficacy of their inactivation. They also reveal what may be subtle differences (subject to limitations of the data, to which the authors make appropriate emphasis – subsection “Compensation on slow time scales”, last paragraph) in circuit mechanisms for choice vs. confidence. Overall, I think this is an outstanding paper that makes major methodological and conceptual contributions to our understanding of sensory decision making.
The biggest negative, and it is a relatively minor one, was the uncertainty in the spatial extent of the inactivation. In the main text, the authors supply us with only a rather vague description: "a relatively small cluster." They actually do much better in the second paragraph of the Materials and methods subsection “Optrode design, site selection and photosuppression protocol”. I suggest that some version of these lines – especially the point about confirming the lack of effect at 1 mm distances-be moved to (or repeated in) the main text. Obviously, the directional selectivity of the effects is also evidence for columnar specificity, but the authors only ever assessed the one (i.e. right-left) axis. It would be interesting to know if they ever tried the orthogonal axis (i.e. up-down when inactivating right-preferring neurons) as a negative control, or did some of the other specificity checks that have been previously performed for microstimulation: e.g. finding a lack of effect when moving the discriminand away from the recorded/inactivated receptive fields; increasing laser power to inactivate multiple direction columns to produce a flattening of the psychometric function. These are all obvious directions for future experiments and are absolutely not required for this study to have a large impact.
One other general concern: it appears that data from the two monkeys was pooled for all analyses. This is understandable. But I was not able to find a statement declaring a justification for this. Were the main results consistent between the two monkeys (except for the one-monkey peculiarity mentioned in the legend to Figure 5)?
Reviewer #3:
This paper uses optogenetic suppression of cortical activity to investigate how visual motion signals in area MT are read out during perceptual judgments. The experimental design is based on that used previously with electrical microstimulation – monkeys viewed a random dot visual motion display of variable coherence and reported their left/right judgment by making a saccade to the appropriate target and were also sometimes given a third opt-out option that gave a guaranteed smaller reward. The opt-out option provides a measure of confidence (higher opt-out rate implies lower decision confidence) and previous microstimulation results suggested that a common mechanism contributed to both confidence and choice. The major difference in this study is the use of focal optogenetic suppression that was orchestrated so that the causal effects were mostly limited to a population of MT neurons with similar directional tuning, which was confirmed with electrophysiological recordings. The ability to reversibly suppress neuronal activity with this spatial and temporal precision is a substantial and elegant advance over previous techniques.
The effects reported in the Results are subtle but clear – both choices and confidence are systematically altered by the optogenetic suppression. But unlike the case with electrical microstimulation, the causal effects are subject to some form of compensatory change, such that the effects on choice behavior are present only for the first 100 or so trials within each session and only for visual motion stimuli that are presented for shorter durations. The reasons for these compensatory changes are not known and lie beyond the scope of this paper, although the authors outline some reasonable explanations to be tested elsewhere. The main strength of the paper lies in its impressive demonstration of methodology for applying optogenetics to pose questions that cannot be addressed with older traditional techniques (e.g., stimulation, drug injection) that lack the necessary temporal and spatial precision. The results also point to some interesting distinctions between choice and confidence that are unexpected, and it seems that the authors themselves are somewhat unsure whether to fully embrace these effects, since they run counter to their previous interpretations.
Overall, this is a very well-written paper that presents novel results on a very interesting topic. My comments are mainly aimed at clarifying the results and interpretation.
The data in the paper provide a basis for possibly drawing novel distinctions between the mechanisms of choice versus confidence. The authors acknowledge that choice and confidence are not always coupled in their results, but nonetheless conclude that their data are consistent with a common mechanism for both (Abstract). As they point out, their analysis in Figure 6 shows that confidence and choice have different effects as a function of trial number. They also report that the shifts in the choice and confidence functions were not correlated, although this negative result may be hard to interpret. More to the point, when I look at the confidence curves in Figures 4D, 5A, etc., I see a very different pattern than the obvious shift that is observed during electrical stimulation (e.g., Figure 4—figure supplement 1B). Rather than a shift, it looks like optogenetic suppression results in a 1-sided (uni-directional) increase in confidence (i.e., a drop in opt-out choices for low signal strengths when the opposite motion direction is suppressed). This makes me wonder about the assumption that confidence is based on the same decision variable as the choice – that is, the difference between rightward and leftward motion signals. Is it possible that confidence is influenced by the total amount of rightward and leftward motion, and not just the magnitude of the difference? To be clear, I don't expect this issue can be settled with this data set. But there does seem to be an incongruity between the data presented and the conclusion about a 'common mechanism for choice and confidence' that should be resolved.
As part of the experimental design, very low light powers were used in order to target local clusters of MT neurons with similar selectivity. This explains why the effect sizes are modest and may not reach significance on individual sessions. For comparison with other studies, have you tried using higher light powers and illuminating larger volumes of tissue? I would expect to see stronger disruptions of performance, but for all directions of visual motion. Nonetheless, the disruptions would be specific for visual motion stimuli placed at the appropriate retinotopic locations. This might serve as a useful positive control and point of comparison with other studies.
The histology on the Jaws expression is an important aspect of the results. An obvious concern is that the histology was done in area LIP not area MT, and it is not obvious to me whether the expression patterns in area MT would be expected to closely follow those in LIP or whether they might end up being very different. Are there neuronal cell type similarities (or differences) between MT and LIP that should be pointed out when interpreting these results?
Unless I missed it, the paper presents the effects of photosuppression only for the trials that included the opt-out option. What effects do you find if the opt-out is not available? Does this substantially change the size of the effect?
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.36523.028Author response
The reviewers agree that this is an outstanding study that makes use of a powerful visual discrimination task in which choice and confidence decision mechanisms that can be linked to the directionality of the preferred response of MT neurons. The reviewers agree that your suppression optogenetic approach in the preferred direction related MT neurons and its effects on choice and confidence constitutes an important advance to linking neural activity and behavior. Having said this, there are some comments by reviewers #2 and 3 and to a lesser extent by reviewer #1 that must be considered to improve/strengthen your paper:
1) It is not clear whether choice and confidence relate to the same decision variable, at least for the photo suppression results provided here (reviewer #2).
We agree this question was left somewhat unresolved, and we have now attempted to address it more fully through modeling and changes to the text. These additions clarify what we can and cannot say about this issue with the current dataset. The upshot is that we have maintained, although softened, our conclusion that the results support a common decision variable for choice and confidence. For details please see response to reviewer 3 and Author response image 4.
2) It would be useful to know whether your weak effects obtained on both choice and confidence were due to the low light power used; the reviewers wonder whether you used higher light power in other sessions (reviewers #2 and 3). This could show that the effects are more comparable to those reported before with your electrical micro-stimulation method. This might also shed light on the spatial extent of suppression by light and its power dependence (reviewers #2 and 3).
We suspect there is a fundamental limit to the size of direction-specific biasing effects that can be caused by photosuppression, for three main reasons: (i) a nontrivial proportion of neurons in the injected region fail to express the opsin, a limitation of current viral vector technology; (ii) increasing the laser power much beyond the levels we used would suppress nearby columns with different direction preferences, thus diluting the direction specificity of the effect; and (iii) the magnitude of the change in firing rate with suppression is inherently limited by floor effects, more so than microstimulation is limited by ceiling effects. We did not have the opportunity to examine the question empirically, e.g., by systematically varying light power for a given behavioral session. We did modify the text (moving some from Materials and methods to Results) and added two figure supplements to emphasize pilot experiments addressing the spatial extent and power dependence of photosuppression, based on a suggestion from reviewer 2. See Author response image 2 and new figure supplements associated with main text Figure 3.
3) Please comment on whether you tried up-down discriminations while inactivating right-left preferred columns as a control (reviewers #2 and 3).
We thank the reviewers for this idea and we hope to pursue such experiments in the future. Here we were unable to test up-down discriminations, owing to a limitation in the behavioral capacities of the particular animals used in this study. This limitation is now mentioned in Materials and methods (subsection “Behavioral task”, first paragraph); see also response to reviewer 2 below.
4) The short and long temporal suppressive effects on decisions were pooled from sessions in the two monkeys; the reviewers wonder about the behavioral performances of each animal.
The effects of suppression on early/short trials (Figure 5A’+C’, Figure 7A+C) were highly significant in each animal individually, and the trends depicted in Figure 6 were qualitatively similar in both monkeys (Author response image 3), although with some minor differences. See response to reviewer 2 below for details.
Lastly, we corrected an error in the handling of data from one of the 23 inactivation sessions. This session showed clear behavioral effects on choice and confidence, but in the opposite direction than predicted by the recorded multiunit activity (see data point in the upper-right quadrant of Figure 4—figure supplement 3, now highlighted in red). Such ‘paradoxical’ effects occasionally happen with electrical µStim as well (e.g. Salzman et al., 1992; DeAngelis et al., 1998; Fetsch et al., 2014) and are believed to imply that a population of oppositely tuned neurons – nearby but not being picked up by the recording electrode – were affected by the manipulation and contributed more strongly to behavior than the neurons being recorded (Salzman et al., 1992).
Interestingly, the effects in this individual session were sensitive to trial number (Author response image 1A-D), duration (E-H), and degree of suppression (not shown), consistent with the patterns described for the full dataset. As the study’s emphasis shifted to the surprising compensatory effects, we decided to include this paradoxical site in these analyses. To do so required reversing the assignment of preferred and null directions, such that the sign of the behavioral shifts was negative (rightward); otherwise the compensation would have canceled out similar trends in other ‘non-paradoxical’ sessions. However, we mistakenly included the reversed session in the analyses of Figure 4, which has nothing to do with compensation. We have now remedied this by explaining the above reasoning in the Materials and methods (subsection “Data analysis”, last paragraph) and replacing Figure 4 with one that does not include the paradoxical session. We have also added a figure supplement (Figure 6—figure supplement 2) showing that the main results hold when the paradoxical session is omitted.

Behavioral effects of photosuppression in one ‘paradoxical’ session, separated by: early trials (A-B) versus late trials (C-D); and short durations (E-F) versus long durations (G-H).
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.36523.024Reviewer #1:
This study is technically well executed. This research group previously proved that electrical micro-stimulation of MT increased the speed of decisions in favor of the preferred direction while slowing decisions in favor of the opposite directions. However and according to the authors, there are technical limitations using the micro-stimulation technique, and therefore the need of some other ways of perturbing clusters of neurons associated with behavior. Here, they used an optogenetic approach in which they suppressed small clusters of excitatory neurons with selectivity for visual motion. They found that optogenetic suppression of small clusters of MT neurons induced a weak choice bias against the neurons' preferred direction, consistent with a common mechanism underlying choice and confidence. These weak effects on choice and confidence were supposedly due to compensatory changes in the read out of MT activity. A more intriguing result was that these weak changes were present only during the first third of the perturbing session (first 500 trials). This was consistently observed across many sessions. Importantly, in all the recorded neuronal sessions, single and multi-unit activity decreased during the optogenetic test, showing the reliability of this suppression approach. As said above, the paper is technically well executed and the experiment is well designed. Of course, one major concern relies on the suppression effects of MT activity and therefore on the behavioral results. The experiment is fair since it shows the opposite of what a micro-stimulation effect was in this task. I understand the authors for using new experimental tools, and appreciate the finesse of the statistical analyses and the explanation they gave on the temporal suppression effects on behavior. I should say that this paper is one of the finest ones that I have seen using the optogenetic tool in behavior. But, honestly, I did not learn much of this study.
We thank the reviewer for this concise summary of our findings and the positive assessment overall. We do not understand the major concern mentioned in the sentence beginning “Of course, one major concern relies on…” We assume the final remark reflects the reviewer’s acceptance of the conclusions drawn from previous studies, especially our electrical µStim study (Fetsch et al., 2014). Nevertheless, we feel it is not a foregone conclusion that inactivating a population of neurons would have clear, interpretable effects in the opposite direction as activating them. Or perhaps the reviewer feels this study does not represent a major technical advance. We would agree, in the sense that optogenetics in nonhuman primates still has yet to reach its full potential, but hope the reviewer would concede that this is nevertheless an important step.
Reviewer #2:
[…] The biggest negative, and it is a relatively minor one, was the uncertainty in the spatial extent of the inactivation. In the main text, the authors supply us with only a rather vague description: "a relatively small cluster." They actually do much better in the second paragraph of the Materials and methods subsection “Optrode design, site selection and photosuppression protocol”. I suggest that some version of these lines – especially the point about confirming the lack of effect at 1 mm distances-be moved to (or repeated in) the main text.
We agree that the issue of spatial extent was not given sufficient emphasis or clarity in the main text, and have now addressed this in several ways. First, as suggested, we reexamined the pilot sessions with 1 mm separation, but found that we had not tested high enough power levels, leaving it unclear whether the lack of an effect was because the light did not reach the neurons near the electrode tip or whether those neurons had simply failed to express the opsin. We do have some relevant data from the handful of V-probe + fiber sessions alluded to in the Materials and methods, now shown in Author response image 2, but we do not consider this direct estimate of light spread to be reliable enough (N=3 sessions) to warrant inclusion in the main text. We also provide a figure showing multiunit activity during a subset of mapping (fixation) blocks in which light power was systematically varied prior to beginning the behavioral task. This has been added to manuscript as Figure 3—figure supplement 2. Lastly, we performed new Monte Carlo simulations of light absorption/scattering in brain tissue (Figure 3—figure supplement 1), based on code made available by Stujenske et al. 2015. Each of these analyses provides support for our claim that photosuppression was typically limited to a region 300-500 µm across.

Qualitative estimate of the spatial extent of photosuppression, furnished by multiunit recordings during pilot mapping sessions using a 16-channel linear array (V-Probe, Plexon Inc.; N=3 sessions).
Left panel: The optical fiber was glued to the probe at a known position near one of the central contacts, which were spaced 150 µm apart. Right panel: Fractional change in firing rate (+/- SEM) as a function of distance from the fiber tip (N=3 sessions). Total power for these sessions was 0.6-0.9 mW, eliciting suppression that was limited to a distance of 300 µm (although these neurons were less light-sensitive than we typically observed).
Obviously, the directional selectivity of the effects is also evidence for columnar specificity, but the authors only ever assessed the one (i.e. right-left) axis. It would be interesting to know if they ever tried the orthogonal axis (i.e. up-down when inactivating right-preferring neurons) as a negative control, or did some of the other specificity checks that have been previously performed for microstimulation: e.g. finding a lack of effect when moving the discriminand away from the recorded/inactivated receptive fields; increasing laser power to inactivate multiple direction columns to produce a flattening of the psychometric function. These are all obvious directions for future experiments and are absolutely not required for this study to have a large impact.
These are all excellent suggestions and on the docket for future experiments. We do know that microstimulation of columns with preferred directions orthogonal to the discriminanda can affect choices in a systematic way (Salzman and Newsome, 1994; Groh et al., 1997; Nichols and Newsome 2002). These effects have been interpreted as favoring either a winner-take-all or vector-averaging readout mechanism, depending on the angular separation and the specifics of the task. Unfortunately we were unable to perform this type of experiment here because the particular monkeys we used were not trained to perform up-down discriminations, and in fact were unusually resistant to such training. We have clarified the text in Materials and methods that mentions this fact (subsection “Behavioral task”, first paragraph). We also did not try placing the dots stimulus outside of the receptive field. We know this eliminates the effect of microstimulation on choices (Salzman et al., 1992), but it would be interesting to look for effects on confidence. Regarding a high-power manipulation to flatten the psychometric function, please see response to reviewer 3 below.
One other general concern: it appears that data from the two monkeys was pooled for all analyses. This is understandable. But I was not able to find a statement declaring a justification for this. Were the main results consistent between the two monkeys (except for the one-monkey peculiarity mentioned in the legend to Figure 5)?
Yes, the main results were consistent between the two monkeys: the effects of suppression on early/short trials (Figure 5A’+C’, Figure 7A+C) were significant in each animal individually, and the time course of attenuation of these effects across trials and time within a trial were qualitatively similar, as shown in Author response image 3. All four trends were statistically significant in monkey D (blue and red traces in Author response image 3A-B), but not monkey N (Author response image 3C-D). In panel D the trends are there, but weaker (p = 0.14 for both). In panel C they are nonsignificant because the data exhibited non-monotonicity (e.g., blue trace for trial numbers ~950-1400; see inset), whereas the statistical approach we used assumes a monotonic relationship. We do not have an explanation for the transient ‘rebound’ of the confidence effect on late trials in monkey N (inset), but it is worth noting that only a minority of sessions (~7) lasted long enough to contribute to these late data points. Overall, we felt the results were similar enough to justify pooling the two monkeys, especially given the constraints on sample size owing to technical challenges involved in acquiring the data. But we agree this should have been mentioned in the text, and now do so (Materials and methods, subsection “Data analysis”, fourth paragraph).

Behavioral effects of photosuppression as a function of trial number within session (left column) and stimulus/laser duration (right column), as in main text Figure 6, plotted separately for the two monkeys.
Note that the abscissa range in panel C was chosen to facilitate comparison with panel A; the inset shows the full extent of trial numbers for monkey N.
Reviewer #3:
[…] The effects reported in the Results are subtle but clear – both choices and confidence are systematically altered by the optogenetic suppression. But unlike the case with electrical microstimulation, the causal effects are subject to some form of compensatory change, such that the effects on choice behavior are present only for the first 100 or so trials within each session and only for visual motion stimuli that are presented for shorter durations. The reasons for these compensatory changes are not known and lie beyond the scope of this paper, although the authors outline some reasonable explanations to be tested elsewhere. The main strength of the paper lies in its impressive demonstration of methodology for applying optogenetics to pose questions that cannot be addressed with older traditional techniques (e.g., stimulation, drug injection) that lack the necessary temporal and spatial precision. The results also point to some interesting distinctions between choice and confidence that are unexpected, and it seems that the authors themselves are somewhat unsure whether to fully embrace these effects, since they run counter to their previous interpretations.
Overall, this is a very well-written paper that presents novel results on a very interesting topic. My comments are mainly aimed at clarifying the results and interpretation.
We are grateful for these remarks, and agree there was some ambiguity in the interpretation relating to whether a common mechanism underlies choice and confidence. We have addressed this by fitting the data to a bounded evidence accumulation model (see below) and by improving the clarity with which we address this issue in the text.
The data in the paper provide a basis for possibly drawing novel distinctions between the mechanisms of choice versus confidence. The authors acknowledge that choice and confidence are not always coupled in their results, but nonetheless conclude that their data are consistent with a common mechanism for both (Abstract). As they point out, their analysis in Figure 6 shows that confidence and choice have different effects as a function of trial number. They also report that the shifts in the choice and confidence functions were not correlated, although this negative result may be hard to interpret. More to the point, when I look at the confidence curves in Figures 4D, 5A, etc., I see a very different pattern than the obvious shift that is observed during electrical stimulation (e.g., Figure 4—figure supplement 1B). Rather than a shift, it looks like optogenetic suppression results in a 1-sided (uni-directional) increase in confidence (i.e., a drop in opt-out choices for low signal strengths when the opposite motion direction is suppressed). This makes me wonder about the assumption that confidence is based on the same decision variable as the choice – that is, the difference between rightward and leftward motion signals. Is it possible that confidence is influenced by the total amount of rightward and leftward motion, and not just the magnitude of the difference? To be clear, I don't expect this issue can be settled with this data set. But there does seem to be an incongruity between the data presented and the conclusion about a 'common mechanism for choice and confidence' that should be resolved.
We thank the reviewer for these insightful comments. We hope the discussion below and the changes to the manuscript help resolve the incongruity between our conclusion of a common mechanism and the apparent discrepancies that the reviewer perceptively points out.
Regarding the asymmetry in the confidence function, we agree that the data seem to indicate a greater effect on confidence when the motion was in the null direction (negative coherence) compared to the preferred direction (positive coherence). We tried a couple different ways to quantify this observation (The first method was to calculate the odds ratio (OR) for the contingency table made up of the occurrence (or not) of a sure-bet choice and the presence/absence of the laser. We did this separately for each side of the confidence function (coherence > 3.2% versus < -3.2%) and computed 95% confidence intervals for the log-OR (method of Woolf). The 95% CI for positive coh was highly overlapping the abs(CI) for negative coh, meaning the asymmetry was not statistically significant. The second method was to use the width parameter of the fitted Gaussian, using a modification of Equation 3 with an indicator term applied to σ. A shift plus a decrease in width on laser trials would generate the asymmetry, but the width change was also not statistically significant.), and for both approaches it did not reach statistical significance. However, even if the asymmetry were significant, whether it entails a distinct decision variable (or mechanism) for confidence is unclear. First, we confess an inability to connect the dots between the observation and the interesting proposal the reviewer makes about confidence being influenced by the total amount of rightward and leftward motion. Trials with greater or lesser total motion could arise by chance, but were equally likely to occur for positive and negative coherence, and thus cannot straightforwardly explain the asymmetry (we did not systematically vary the total motion energy within a given coherence level). Still, we do not reject the possibility of such a mechanism, and it would be interesting to test other types of stimuli that independently vary the strength of evidence supporting each option (e.g., Zylberberg, Barttfeld, and Sigman, 2012; Odegaard et al., PNAS 2018).
It should be noted that the asymmetry could arise under a common mechanism, if photosuppression were to cause both a shift and a decrease in the width of the bell-shaped confidence function. In a bounded accumulation model (e.g., Fetsch et al., 2014), narrowing of the confidence curve is captured by an increase in the sensitivity parameter k, which converts the (experimenter-controlled) motion strength to the drift rate of the decision variable. Naturally, increasing k also entails an increase in the sensitivity, or slope, of the choice function, and indeed there is a hint of this in the data, especially in Figure 7A (although it too does not reach significance; p = 0.10, logistic regression).
To illustrate this point, we fit the model to the data shown in main text Figures 5A’+C’ and 7A+C. The fits are shown below in Author response image 4, showing that the model can account for the asymmetry in the confidence effect despite having a single decision variable that governs choice and confidence. Our sample size does not allow for rigorous model comparison, so we cannot make strong conclusions about whether and how photosuppression alters properties of sensory signal and noise. But the fitting exercise suggests that the observation of asymmetry is not impetus for rejecting the hypothesis of a common mechanism. We have now included this point in the Discussion (subsection “A common mechanism for choice and confidence”, third paragraph).

Fits to a bounded accumulation model (Fetsch et al., 2014) using the data shown in main text Figure 5A'+C' (left column) and Figure 7A+C (right column).
The model implements the biasing effect of photosuppression – on both choice and confidence – as an offset to the mean of the momentary evidence (i.e., motion strength). It also has free parameters that permit a change in the drift rate (sensitivity) and/or the sensory noise on laser trials. These parameters can account for more subtle effects including changes in the slope of the choice function, and the height and width of the confidence function. Importantly, however, the mechanism for establishing confidence still operates on the same decision variable as the one used for choice.
Regarding the discrepancies in time course (Figure 6), the current dataset cannot rule out that they occurred by chance. Using the standard errors of the shift parameters (β2 in Equation 1, and δ in Equation 3) – and conceding that the lateral shift is only one of several aspects of the data one could examine – none of the differences between the shifts in choice and confidence rise to the level of statistical significance (see reply to comment below about the ‘overshoot’).
Nevertheless, in light of the reviewer’s concern (which we share), we have walked back some of the emphasis in the manuscript on a common mechanism for choice and confidence. For instance, we removed a sentence relating to Figure 4B: "Note that the effects (or lack thereof) on choice and confidence were reasonably well matched across the full range of ∆R (Figure 4B), supporting the idea of a common decision mechanism linking the two measures," added phrases like “broadly consistent”, and rewrote a large part of the Discussion (subsection “A common mechanism for choice and confidence”) to include several of the considerations described above.
As part of the experimental design, very low light powers were used in order to target local clusters of MT neurons with similar selectivity. This explains why the effect sizes are modest and may not reach significance on individual sessions. For comparison with other studies, have you tried using higher light powers and illuminating larger volumes of tissue? I would expect to see stronger disruptions of performance, but for all directions of visual motion. Nonetheless, the disruptions would be specific for visual motion stimuli placed at the appropriate retinotopic locations. This might serve as a useful positive control and point of comparison with other studies.
We appreciate these suggestions, although in our view it is unclear whether low light power explains the small effect sizes. Increasing laser power might have suppressed the targeted column more strongly, but this would come at the cost of affecting nearby columns/clusters and thus diluting the directional specificity of the manipulation. It is conceivable that, if laser power were to be steadily increased, one might see larger bias effects kick in before being swamped by the overall disruption in performance (Murasugi et al., 1993). We suspect that this would not occur or be too subtle to detect, mainly due to floor effects; i.e., increasing the strength of suppression has diminishing returns as neurons approach zero spikes/s, whereas stimulation has a much higher ceiling. Also working against us here is the fact that current viral vector technology limits the percentage of neurons that express the transgene. Still, we hope to address these issues with future experiments that include more systematic variations in light power, and better reagents.
The histology on the Jaws expression is an important aspect of the results. An obvious concern is that the histology was done in area LIP not area MT, and it is not obvious to me whether the expression patterns in area MT would be expected to closely follow those in LIP or whether they might end up being very different. Are there neuronal cell type similarities (or differences) between MT and LIP that should be pointed out when interpreting these results?
We agree with this reviewer that presenting some histology will be useful to readers. We also agree with all three reviewers that there are limits to what one can infer from expression in a different brain area of a different monkey. The revised manuscript now clarifies these limitations. We are not aware of any studies that investigate the similarities or differences in cell types or laminar organization of MT and LIP, but if there is such a study we would gladly cite it.
Unless I missed it, the paper presents the effects of photosuppression only for the trials that included the opt-out option. What effects do you find if the opt-out is not available? Does this substantially change the size of the effect?
We apologize for leaving this ambiguous. In fact, to maximize statistical power, we pooled together trials with and without the sure-bet option for all graphs and analyses of choice data, with the exception of Figure 1C. We have now clarified this in the Materials and methods (subsection “Data analysis”, first paragraph). If we analyze these conditions separately, it turns out that the photosuppression effects were about twice as large on trials where the sure-bet option was unavailable compared to when it was available. We don’t know how seriously to take this result since the effect sizes were modest overall. One possible interpretation is that the sure-bet option absorbed some of the potential biasing effect – that is, some choices that would have been biased away from the preferred direction were instead rendered as opt-outs. But this would seem to predict an overall increase in opt-out choices on laser trials, which we did not observe (if anything, it was the opposite). We have added this point to the newly rewritten Discussion section regarding possible deviations from a straightforward model in which confidence and choice are yoked via a common decision variable (subsection “A common mechanism for choice and confidence”).
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.36523.029Article and author information
Author details
Funding
Howard Hughes Medical Institute
- Christopher R Fetsch
- Naomi N Odean
- Danique Jeurissen
- Michael N Shadlen
National Eye Institute (EY11378)
- Christopher R Fetsch
- Naomi N Odean
- Danique Jeurissen
- Michael N Shadlen
National Eye Institute (EY018849)
- Yasmine El-Shamayleh
- Gregory D Horwitz
Simons Foundation (Simons Collaboration on the Global Brain Postdoctoral Fellowship)
- Danique Jeurissen
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Acknowledgements
This research was supported by the Howard Hughes Medical Institute and the National Eye Institute. DJ is supported by a postdoctoral fellowship from the Simons Collaboration on the Global Brain. We thank Dr. Mehrdad Jazayeri (MIT) for contributions to the early development of the project, Dr. Ed Boyden (MIT) and his lab for developing and sharing the Jaws construct, Brian Madeira for technical support and animal care, and members of the Shadlen lab for discussions.
Ethics
Animal experimentation: This study was performed in strict accordance with the Public Health Service Policy on Humane Care and Use of Laboratory Animals. The animals were handled according to an approved institutional animal care and use committee (IACUC) protocol (#AAAN4900) of Columbia University. Surgery was performed under isoflurane anesthesia in aseptic conditions, and every effort was made to minimize suffering.
Senior Editor
- David C Van Essen, Washington University in St. Louis, United States
Reviewing Editor
- Ranulfo Romo, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Mexico
Version history
- Received: March 9, 2018
- Accepted: July 12, 2018
- Accepted Manuscript published: July 27, 2018 (version 1)
- Version of Record published: August 10, 2018 (version 2)
Copyright
© 2018, Fetsch et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 3,332
- Page views
-
- 616
- Downloads
-
- 26
- Citations
Article citation count generated by polling the highest count across the following sources: Crossref, PubMed Central, Scopus.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Neuroscience
Complex behaviors depend on the coordinated activity of neural ensembles in interconnected brain areas. The behavioral function of such coordination, often measured as co-fluctuations in neural activity across areas, is poorly understood. One hypothesis is that rapidly varying co-fluctuations may be a signature of moment-by-moment task-relevant influences of one area on another. We tested this possibility for error-corrective adaptation of birdsong, a form of motor learning which has been hypothesized to depend on the top-down influence of a higher-order area, LMAN (lateral magnocellular nucleus of the anterior nidopallium), in shaping moment-by-moment output from a primary motor area, RA (robust nucleus of the arcopallium). In paired recordings of LMAN and RA in singing birds, we discovered a neural signature of a top-down influence of LMAN on RA, quantified as an LMAN-leading co-fluctuation in activity between these areas. During learning, this co-fluctuation strengthened in a premotor temporal window linked to the specific movement, sequential context, and acoustic modification associated with learning. Moreover, transient perturbation of LMAN activity specifically within this premotor window caused rapid occlusion of pitch modifications, consistent with LMAN conveying a temporally localized motor-biasing signal. Combined, our results reveal a dynamic top-down influence of LMAN on RA that varies on the rapid timescale of individual movements and is flexibly linked to contexts associated with learning. This finding indicates that inter-area co-fluctuations can be a signature of dynamic top-down influences that support complex behavior and its adaptation.
-
- Genetics and Genomics
- Neuroscience
The Maillard reaction, a chemical reaction between amino acids and sugars, is exploited to produce flavorful food ubiquitously, from the baking industry to our everyday lives. However, the Maillard reaction also occurs in all cells, from prokaryotes to eukaryotes, forming Advanced Glycation End-products (AGEs). AGEs are a heterogeneous group of compounds resulting from the irreversible reaction between biomolecules and α-dicarbonyls (α-DCs), including methylglyoxal (MGO), an unavoidable byproduct of anaerobic glycolysis and lipid peroxidation. We previously demonstrated that Caenorhabditis elegans mutants lacking the glod-4 glyoxalase enzyme displayed enhanced accumulation of α-DCs, reduced lifespan, increased neuronal damage, and touch hypersensitivity. Here, we demonstrate that glod-4 mutation increased food intake and identify that MGO-derived hydroimidazolone, MG-H1, is a mediator of the observed increase in food intake. RNAseq analysis in glod-4 knockdown worms identified upregulation of several neurotransmitters and feeding genes. Suppressor screening of the overfeeding phenotype identified the tdc-1-tyramine-tyra-2/ser-2 signaling as an essential pathway mediating AGEs (MG-H1) induced feeding in glod-4 mutants. We also identified the elt-3 GATA transcription factor as an essential upstream regulator for increased feeding upon accumulation of AGEs by partially controlling the expression of tdc-1 gene. Further, the lack of either tdc-1 or tyra-2/ser-2 receptors suppresses the reduced lifespan and rescues neuronal damage observed in glod-4 mutants. Thus, in C. elegans, we identified an elt-3 regulated tyramine-dependent pathway mediating the toxic effects of MG-H1 AGE. Understanding this signaling pathway may help understand hedonistic overfeeding behavior observed due to modern AGEs-rich diets.