Extrinsic and intrinsic dynamics in movement intermittency
Abstract
What determines how we move in the world? Motor neuroscience often focusses either on intrinsic rhythmical properties of motor circuits or extrinsic sensorimotor feedback loops. Here we show that the interplay of both intrinsic and extrinsic dynamics is required to explain the intermittency observed in continuous tracking movements. Using spatiotemporal perturbations in humans, we demonstrate that apparently discrete submovements made 2–3 times per second reflect constructive interference between motor errors and continuous feedback corrections that are filtered by intrinsic circuitry in the motor system. Local field potentials in monkey motor cortex revealed characteristic signatures of a Kalman filter, giving rise to both low-frequency cortical cycles during movement, and delta oscillations during sleep. We interpret these results within the framework of optimal feedback control, and suggest that the intrinsic rhythmicity of motor cortical networks reflects an internal model of external dynamics, which is used for state estimation during feedback-guided movement.
Editorial note: This article has been through an editorial process in which the authors decide how to respond to the issues raised during peer review. The Reviewing Editor's assessment is that all the issues have been addressed (see decision letter).
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.40145.001Introduction
Many visually-guided movements are characterized by intermittent speed fluctuations. For example, during the tracking of slowly-moving target, humans make around 2–3 submovements per second. Although first described over a century ago (Woodworth, 1899; Craik, 1947; Vince, 1948) the cause of movement intermittency remains debated. Submovements often disappear in the absence of vision (Miall et al., 1993a) and are influenced by feedback delays (Miall, 1996), suggesting that their timing depends on extrinsic properties of visuomotor feedback loops. However, some rhythmicity persists in the absence of feedback (Doeringer and Hogan, 1998), and it has been suggested that an internal refractory period, clock or oscillator parses complex movements into discrete, isochronal segments (Viviani and Flash, 1995; Loram et al., 2006; Hogan and Sternad, 2012; Russell and Sternad, 2001). Cyclical dynamics within motor cortical networks with a time period of 300–500 ms may reflect the neural correlates of such an intrinsic oscillator (Churchland et al., 2012; Hall et al., 2014). During continuous tracking, each submovement is phase-locked to a single cortical cycle, giving rise to low-frequency coherence between cortical oscillations and movement speed (Jerbi et al., 2007; Hall et al., 2014; Pereira et al., 2017).
It has been proposed that the intrinsic dynamics of recurrently-connected cortical networks act as an ‘engine of movement’, responsible for internal generation and timing of the descending motor command (Churchland et al., 2012). However, another possibility is that low-frequency dynamics observed in motor cortex arise from sensorimotor feedback loops through the external environment. On the one hand, cortical cycles appear conserved across a wide range of behaviors and even share a common structure with delta oscillations during sleep (Hall et al., 2014), consistent with a purely intrinsic origin. On the other hand, the influence of feedback delays on submovement timing suggests an extrinsic contribution to movement intermittency. Therefore, we examined the effect of delay perturbations during visuomotor tracking in humans and monkeys, to dissociate both delay-independent (intrinsic) and delay-dependent (extrinsic) components of movement kinematics and cortical dynamics.
We interpret our findings using stochastic optimal control theory, which has emerged as an influential approach to understanding movement (Todorov and Jordan, 2002; Scott, 2004). Given noisy, delayed sensory measurements, an optimal feedback controller (OFC) continually estimates the current motor state using an internal model of external dynamics. We show that this can provide a computational framework for understanding both extrinsic and intrinsic contributions to intermittency, accounting for many puzzling features of submovements and providing a parsimonious explanation for conserved cyclical dynamics in motor cortex networks during behavior and sleep.
Results
Overview
Our results are organized as follows. First, we describe behavioral results with human subjects, examining the effects of delay perturbations on movement intermittency and feedback responses during an isometric visuomotor tracking task. Second, we introduce a simple computational model to illustrate how principles of optimal feedback control, and in particular state estimation, can explain the key features of our data. Finally, we examine local field potentials recorded from the motor cortex of monkeys performing a similar task, to show that cyclical neural trajectories are consistent with the implementation of state estimation circuitry.
Submovement frequencies are affected by feedback delays
Our first experiment aimed to characterize the dependence of submovement frequencies on feedback delays. Human subjects generated bimanual, isometric, index finger forces to track targets that moved in slow 2D circular trajectories with constant speed (Figure 1A). We measured intermittency in the angular velocity of the cursor (Figure 1B,C) using spectral analysis over a 10 s window beginning 5 s after the trial start. Under unperturbed feedback conditions, power spectra generally exhibited a principal peak at around 2 Hz (Figure 1D). This frequency was only slightly affected by target speed (Figure 1—figure supplement 1), consistent with previous reports (Miall, 1996) and perhaps suggestive of an intrinsic oscillator or clock determining submovement timing.
-
Figure 1—source data 1
- https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.40145.006
However, submovement frequencies were markedly altered when visual feedback of the cursor was delayed relative to finger forces. With delays of 100 and 200 ms, the frequency of the primary peak reduced to around 1.4 and 1 Hz respectively (Figure 1D, Figure 1—figure supplements 1 and 2), demonstrating that submovement timing in fact depended on extrinsic feedback properties. Interestingly, a further peak appeared at approximately three times the frequency of the primary peak, and with increased delays, of 300 and 400 ms, a fifth harmonic was observed. The time-periods of the first, third and fifth harmonics were linearly related to extrinsic delay times, with gradients of 1.89 ± 0.20, 0.59 ± 0.04 and 0.33 ± 0.11 respectively (Figure 1E, Table 1).
These results are consistent with a feedback controller responding to broad-spectrum (stochastic) tracking errors introduced by noise in the motor output, for which the response is delayed by time, . In signal-processing terms, subtracting a delayed version from the original signal is known as comb filtering (Figure 1E). Although comb filters subtracting in either feedforward or feedback directions have qualitatively similar behavior, we illustrate only the feedforward architecture in Figure 1E, as we will later show this to match better the experimental data. For motor noise components with a time period, , delayed corrections accurately reflect current errors, resulting in regularly spaced notches in the amplitude response of the system (Figure 1G) and attenuation in the resultant cursor movement through destructive interference. By contrast, for motor noise with a time-period, , delayed corrections are exactly out-of-phase with the current error. Thus, corrective movements exacerbate these components through constructive interference, leading to spectral peaks at frequencies:
Submovement frequencies in our data approximately matched this model, assuming the total feedback delay comprised the experimental manipulation added to a constant physiological response latency of around 300 ms (Table 1), comparable to visual reaction times.
Submovements occur at frequencies of constructive interference between motor errors and delayed corrections
According to this extrinsic interpretation, intermittency arises not from active internal generation of discrete submovement events, but as a byproduct of continuous, linear feedback control with inherent time delays. Submovement frequencies need not be present in the smooth target movement, nor do they arise from controller non-linearities. Instead these frequencies reflect components of broad-band motor noise that are exacerbated by constructive interference with delayed feedback corrections. To seek further evidence that intermittency arises from such constructive interference, we performed a second experiment in which artificial errors were generated by spatial perturbation of the cursor. Within individual trials, a sinusoidal displacement was added to the cursor position in a direction aligned to target motion and at a frequency between 1 and 5 Hz. Perturbation amplitudes were scaled to have equivalent peak angular velocities (equal to the angular velocity of the target). Our hypothesis was that artificial errors at submovement frequencies would be harder to track (because of constructive interference) than perturbations at frequencies absent from the velocity spectrum.
Figure 2A shows example tracking behavior with a 2 Hz perturbation. Note that the peak angular velocity of force responses (black line, calculated from the subject’s finger forces) occurred around the same time as the peak angular velocity of the perturbation (green line). As a result, the angular velocity of the cursor (yellow line, reflecting the combination of the subject’s forces with the perturbation) exhibited pronounced oscillations that were larger than the perturbation. Figure 2B shows performance in the same task when visual feedback was delayed by 200 ms. In this condition, peaks in force velocity coincided with perturbation troughs, attenuating the disturbance to cursor velocity. Figure 2C,D and Figure 2—figure supplement 1 overlay cursor velocity spectra in the presence of each perturbation frequency (with feedback delays of 0 and 200 ms), again calculated over a 10 s window beginning 5 s after the trial start. As previously, in the absence of feedback delay, the frequency of submovements was around 2 Hz. Correspondingly, perturbations at 2 Hz induced a large peak in the cursor velocity spectrum, indicating that the artificial error was not effectively tracked. By contrast, with a feedback delay of 200 ms the cursor velocity spectrum with a 2 Hz perturbation was attenuated. The largest spectral peaks were instead associated with 1 and 3 Hz perturbations, matching the frequencies of submovements in this delay condition.
-
Figure 2—source data 1
- https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.40145.012
Figure 2E shows the amplitude response of cursor movements at each frequency for both delay conditions. Unlike a power spectrum, the cursor amplitude response measures only cursor movements that are phase-locked to the perturbation (normalized by the perturbation amplitude), and therefore estimates the overall transfer function of the closed-loop control system. Cursor amplitude responses greater than unity at 2 Hz (with no delay), and at 1 and 3 Hz (with 200 ms delay) indicate exacerbation of intermittencies introduced by artificial errors at submovement frequencies. Analysis of variance (ANOVA) with two factors (delay time and perturbation frequency) revealed a highly significant interaction (n = 8 subjects, F4,70=110.2, p<0.0001), confirming the interdependence of feedback delays and frequencies of constructive/destructive interference.
Intrinsic dynamics in the visuomotor feedback loop
Although submovement frequencies depended on extrinsic feedback delays, examination of the velocity spectra in Figure 1D suggests that intermittency peaks were embedded within a broad, delay-independent low-pass envelope. This envelope could simply reflect the power spectrum of tracking errors (i.e. motor noise is dominated by low-frequency components). However, an additional possibility is that the gain of the feedback controller varies across frequencies (e.g. low-frequency noise components generate larger feedback corrections). To explore the latter directly, we examined subjects’ force responses to our artificial perturbations.
Figure 2F,G and Figure 2—figure supplement 2 show power spectra of the angular velocity derived from subjects’ forces, under feedback delays of 0 and 200 ms. Note that this analysis differs from Figure 2C,D in that we now consider only the forces generated by the subjects, rather than the resultant cursor movement (which combines these forces with the perturbation). Figure 2H shows the corresponding force amplitude response for each perturbation frequency. The force amplitude response measures only force responses that are phase-locked to the perturbation (normalized by the perturbation amplitude) and is related to the transfer function within the feedback loop. Unlike the cursor amplitude responses described previously, force amplitude responses were largely independent of extrinsic delay. However, as with the velocity spectra in Figure 1D, feedback gains were also attenuated at higher frequencies. A two-factor ANOVA confirmed a significant main effect of frequency (n = 8 subjects, F4,70=36.3, p<0.0001) but not delay time (F1,70=3.1, p=0.08), and only a weakly significant interaction (F4,70=2.9, p=0.03). In other words, feedback corrections to artificial errors revealed a delay-independent filter matching the attenuation of submovement peaks at higher frequencies.
Interestingly, the phase delay of force responses was also influenced by perturbation frequency (Figure 2I). Effectively, corrections to low-frequency perturbations occurred slightly earlier than those to higher frequencies, indicating a predictive component to feedback responses. As with the amplitude response, there was a significant effect of frequency (F4,70=9.5, p<0.0001) but not extrinsic delay (F1,70 =2.6, p=0.12) on this phase delay, and no significant interaction (F4,70=0.7, p=0.6).
We next considered whether high-frequency attenuation of feedback responses was a property of motor pathways, for example reflecting filtering by the musculoskeletal system. However, it is well-known that the frequencies of feedforward movements can readily exceed submovement frequencies observed during feedback-guided behavior (Kunesch et al., 1989). We confirmed this by asking subjects to produce force fluctuations of a defined amplitude, but without providing a moving target to track. Instead we used auditory cues (a metronome) to indicate the required movement frequency. In this case, subjects could generate force fluctuations up to 5 Hz with little attenuation (Figure 2J,K and Figure 2—figure supplement 3). Therefore we concluded that filtering of corrective responses was not an inherent property of feedforward motor pathways but instead reflected intrinsic dynamics in the visuomotor feedback loop.
Intrinsic dynamics and optimal state estimation
The visual system can perceive relatively high frequencies (up to flicker-fusion frequencies above 10 Hz). However, while feedforward movements can in some cases approach these frequencies, discrepancies while tracking slowly-moving objects in the physical world are unlikely to change quickly. The limbs and real-world targets will tend (to a first approximation) to move with a constant velocity unless acted upon by a force. Moreover, even for isometric tasks, the drift in force production is dominated by low-frequency components (Baweja et al., 2009; Slifkin and Newell, 2000), possibly consistent with neural integration in the descending motor pathway (Shadmehr, 2017). Given inherent uncertainties in sensation, an optimal state estimator should attribute high-frequency errors to sensory noise (which is unconstrained by Newtonian and/or neuromuscular dynamics).
Formally, the task of distinguishing the true state of the world from uncertain, delayed measurements can be achieved by a Kalman filter, which continuously integrates new evidence with updated estimates of the current state, evolving according to a model of the external dynamics (Figure 3A). For simplicity we used Newtonian dynamics, although similar results would likely be obtained for other second-order state transition models. We assumed the 1D position of the body (cursor) relative to the target should evolve with constant velocity unless acted upon by accelerative forces, leading to the state transition model:
where and are the relative position and velocity of the cursor at time-step , is the interval between time-steps, and the process noise . Visual feedback, , was assumed to comprise a noisy measurement of relative position:
with measurement noise .
Optimal estimates of relative position and velocity, and are given by a steady-state Kalman filter of the form:
The innovation gains and depend only on the ratio of (accelerative) process to (position) measurement noise, , which in turn determines the cut-off frequency above which measurements are filtered . Figure 3B,C shows the amplitude response for position and velocity estimates produced by the Kalman filter. Since these are out of phase with each other, their cross-spectral density (which captures the amplitude and phase-difference between frequency components common to both signals) will generally be complex. Broadband input therefore results in an imaginary component to this cross-spectrum with a characteristic low-frequency resonance peak determined by the state estimator dynamics (Figure 3D).
Feedback delays can be accommodated by projecting the state estimate forward in time:
The phase delay of the optimal position estimate of the current state, , falls towards zero at low frequencies, consistent with a predictive component when interpreting low-frequency errors (Figure 3E).
Incorporating intrinsic and extrinsic dynamics in a model of movement intermittency
To illustrate how such a steady-state Kalman filter can account for the main features of our human behavioral data, we incorporated it within a simple 1D feedback controller (Figure 3F; see Materials and methods for details). We included an internal feedback loop to cancel the sensory consequences of motor commands, known as a Smith Predictor (Abe and Yamanaka, 2003; Miall et al., 1993b). This prevents corrections from reverberating around the external feedback loop, such that the resultant closed-loop behavior is formally equivalent to the simpler feedforward comb filter shown in Figure 3G. This rearrangement provides a useful intuition about our behavioral results. Tracking errors (due to motor noise) drive feedback corrections that are delayed, corrupted (by sensory noise) and filtered (by intrinsic dynamics). The power spectrum of the resultant movements reflects constructive/destructive interference between feedback corrections and the original tracking error.
The Smith Predictor model readily accounted for the main features of our human data, including the cursor amplitude response to perturbations (Figure 4A–E), and the low-pass filtering (Figure 4F–H) and phase delay (Figure 4I) of force responses. Moreover, because of frequency-dependent phase delays introduced by state estimation, the model suggested that precise frequencies of submovement peaks should deviate slightly from those calculated using a constant physiological response latency. Effectively, a predictive state estimator responding more quickly to low-frequency errors behaves like a feedback controller with a reduced loop delay. This effect was confirmed in our behavioral data by calculating (with Equation (1)) the intrinsic delay time corresponding to each spectral peak under all feedback delay conditions in our first experiment (arrows in Figure 1D). Rather than being a constant, this intrinsic delay time was positively correlated with frequency (n = 11 spectral peaks, R = 0.78, p=0.0046) and matched well the frequency-dependent phase delay predicted by our model (Figure 4J).
Finally, overall tracking performance (as measured by the root mean squared positional error over time) matched well with subjects’ actual performance across conditions in our second experiment (Figure 4K). Note that, irrespective of delay, the lowest frequency perturbation was associated with the greatest positional error, since perturbations had equal peak-to-peak velocity and were therefore larger in amplitude at low frequencies. However, performance was most affected by the 1 Hz perturbation with a 200 ms delay, corresponding to a frequency of constructive interference.
Emergence of delay-specific predictive control during individual trials
While not simulating the full complexity of upper-limb control, our model was intended to illustrate the interplay between intrinsic and extrinsic dynamics during tracking. More sophisticated models would likely exhibit qualitatively similar behavior, so long as they also incorporated extrinsic, delay-dependent feedback and intrinsic, delay-independent dynamics. However, in order to generate stable tracking behavior, the Smith Predictor architecture requires accurate compensation for external delays within an internal feedback loop. For this to be a plausible model to explain our data, the controller would need to adapt quickly to new extrinsic delay conditions that varied from trial to trial in our experiment. Therefore we were interested in whether we could observe such rapid adaptation of control strategies within individual trials. Moreover, we asked whether this adaptation was delay-specific as expected for a Smith Predictor, or could perhaps be explained by a simpler delay-independent feedback controller.
Figure 5A,B compares schematics of two linear feedback controllers with and without the internal Smith Predictor loop. Both incorporate intrinsic dynamics, and as a result of extrinsic feedback delays exacerbate motor noise at frequencies given by Equation (1). However, the comb filter rearrangements in Figure 5C,D show how the two architectures predict different relationships between the feedback gain resulting from this intrinsic dynamics, , and the closed-loop force amplitude response to perturbations, . We asked how each model explained the experimental data by inferring the intrinsic dynamics that would be required to generate our observed force amplitude responses under both architectures.
Figure 5E shows the intrinsic gain that a simple feedback controller would need to explain the amplitude responses observed during 5 s sections of experimental data taken from the start, middle and end of each trial. While the general pattern was one of low-pass filtering, the intrinsic dynamics inferred for each delay condition diverged progressively through the trial. Therefore we can conclude that the control strategy used by subjects was indeed adapting during a single trial, and that this adaptation was delay-specific. Interestingly, intrinsic feedback gains inferred using the Smith Predictor model (Figure 5F) became progressively more similar as the trial progressed. Therefore the adaptation process could parsimoniously be interpreted as the emergence of an appropriately-calibrated Smith Predictor with delay-independent intrinsic dynamics, as predicted by our optimal control model. The time-course of this adaptation (Figure 5G) was associated with a reduction in both low-frequency phase delays (Figure 5H) and the average lag of the cursor behind the target (Figure 5I), showing that subjects quickly learned to compensate for feedback delays within individual trials.
Movement intermittency in a non-human primate tracking task
The amplitude and phase responses to perturbations during human visuomotor tracking provided evidence for intrinsic low-frequency dynamics in feedback corrections, which we have interpreted in the framework of optimal state estimation. The schematic on the right of Figure 3A suggests how a simple Kalman filter could be implemented by neural circuitry, with two neural populations (representing position and velocity) evolving according to Equation (4) and exhibiting a resonant cross-spectral peak (Figure 3D). To seek further evidence for the neural implementation of such a filter we turned to intracortical recordings in non-human primates. We were interested in whether cyclical motor cortex trajectories could reflect the delay-independent dynamics of the two interacting neural populations described above, and thereby account for filtering of feedback responses during visuomotor tracking.
We analyzed local field potential (LFP) recordings from monkey primary motor cortex (M1) during a center-out isometric wrist torque task, which we have used previously to characterize both submovement kinematics and population dynamics (Hall et al., 2014). Figure 6 shows example tracking behavior (Figure 6A), radial cursor velocity (Figure 6B) and multichannel LFPs (Figure 6C) as monkeys moved to peripheral targets under two feedback delay conditions. Movement intermittency was apparent as regular submovement peaks in the radial cursor velocity. Moreover, LFPs exhibited low-frequency oscillations during movement, with a variety of phase-shifts present on different channels. Principal component analysis (PCA) yielded two orthogonal components of the cortical cycle (Figure 6E), and the close coupling with submovements was revealed by overlaying the cursor velocity profile onto, in this case, the second principal component (PC) (Figure 6E).
Intrinsic cortical dynamics are unaffected by feedback delays
As with humans, in the absence of feedback delay the cursor velocity (after removing task-locked components, see Materials and methods) was dominated by a single spectral peak (Figure 7A,E; top red traces). A broad peak at approximately the same frequency was also observed in average LFP power spectra (Figure 7B,F). Additionally, we used coherence analysis to confirm consistent phase-coupling between LFPs and cursor velocity (Figure 7C,G). Finally, we calculated imaginary coherence spectra between pairs of LFPs (see Materials and methods). The imaginary component of coherence indicates frequencies at which there is consistent out-of-phase coupling in the LFP cross-spectrum. This effectively separates locally-varying oscillatory components from in-phase background signals (e.g. due to volume conduction from distant sources), and revealed more clearly the LFP rhythmicity (Figure 7D,H). Note that for no feedback delay, all spectra contain a single peak at around 2–3 Hz.
An obvious interpretation of these results would be that oscillatory activity in the motor system drives submovements in a feedforward manner. In this case, we would expect the frequency of the cortical oscillation to reliably reflect the intermittency observed in behavior. With increasing feedback delays, submovement peaks in monkeys (Figure 7A,E; lower traces) exhibited a pattern similar to that seen with human subjects. The fundamental frequency was reduced, while odd harmonics grew more pronounced as they came below about 4 Hz. Moreover, coherence spectra between cursor velocity and LFP (Figure 7C,G) revealed delay-dependent peaks at both fundamental and harmonic frequencies. Surprisingly however, the power spectrum of the LFP (Figure 7B,F) was unaffected by feedback delay, with a single broad peak in the delta band persisting throughout. Moreover, imaginary coherence spectra between pairs of LFPs were also unchanged (Figure 7D,H). These results are incompatible with the hypothesis that motor cortical oscillations drive movement intermittency directly, and instead demonstrate a dissociation between delay-dependent submovements and delay-independent cortical dynamics.
We next identified submovements from peaks in the radial cursor speed, in order to examine the temporal profile of their associated LFPs. Submovement-triggered averages (SmTAs) of LFPs exhibited multiphasic potentials around the time of movement, as well as a second feature following submovements with a latency that depended on extrinsic delay (Figure 8A, Figure 8—figure supplement 1). This feature was revealed more clearly by reducing the dimensionality of the LFPs with PCA (Figure 8B). Note that if submovements reflect interference between stochastic motor errors and feedback corrections, a submovement in the positive direction can arise from two underlying causes. First, it may be a positive correction to a preceding negative error. In this case, cortical activity associated with the feedback correction should occur around time zero. Second, the submovement may itself be a positive error which is followed by a negative correction, and the associated cortical activity will hence be delayed by the feedback latency. Since the SmTA pools submovements arising from both causes, this accounts for two features with opposite polarity separated by the feedback delay. Note also that SmTAs of cursor velocity similarly overlay (negative) tracking errors preceding (positive) feedback corrections, and (negative) feedback corrections following (positive) tracking errors, evident as symmetrical troughs on either side of the central submovement peak (Figure 8C).
Importantly however, LFP oscillations around the time of submovements appeared largely unaffected by delay. To visualize this, we projected the SmTAs of multichannel LFPs onto the same PC plane. For all delay conditions, LFPs traced a single cycle with the same directional of rotation and comparable angular velocity (Figure 8D). The period of these cycles (approx. 300 ms) matched the frequency of imaginary coherence between LFPs (approx. 3 Hz). This is as expected, since signals with a consistent phase difference will be orthogonalized by PCA and appear as cyclical trajectories in the PC plane. In other words, although the precise frequency of submovements depended on extrinsic delays in visual feedback, the constant frequency of associated LFP cycles revealed delay-independent intrinsic dynamics within motor cortex. Note also that the resonant frequency of these dynamics matched the delay-independent filtering of feedback responses observed in our human experiments.
Modelling submovement-related LFP cycles and delta oscillations in sleep
These various observations could be understood using the same computational model that explained our human behavioral data (Figure 9). For simplicity, we simulated two out-of-phase components within the LFP by using the total synaptic input to each of the two neural populations in the state estimator. We also added common low-frequency background noise to represent volume conduction from distant sources. The simulated LFPs exhibited a broad, delay-independent spectral peak arising from the dynamics of the recurrent network (Figure 7J). By contrast, the resultant cursor velocity comprised the summation of motor noise and (delayed) feedback corrections, and therefore contained sharper, delay-dependent spectral peaks, due to constructive/destructive interference (Figure 7I). Note however, that coherence was nonetheless observed between LFPs and cursor velocity (Figure 7K). Time-domain SmTAs of the simulated data also reproduced features of the experimental recordings, including delay-dependent peaks/troughs reflecting extrinsic feedback delays (Figure 8E–G). Meanwhile, the coupling of simulated neural populations, according to conserved intrinsic dynamics, resulted in consistent LFP cycles around the time of movement (Figure 8H), and an imaginary cross-spectrum with a single delay-independent resonant peak (Figure 7L).
Finally, we examined whether the model could also account for cortical oscillations in the absence of behavior. Previously we have described a common dynamical structure within both cortical cycles during movement and low-frequency oscillations during sleep and sedation (Hall et al., 2014). In particular, K-complex events under ketamine sedation (Figure 10A), thought to reflect transitions between down- and up-states of the cortex, are associated with brief bursts of delta oscillation (Figure 10B) (Amzica and Steriade, 1997). The relative phases of multichannel LFPs aligned to these events matches those seen during submovements (Figure 10D,E). As a result, when projected onto the PC plane, LFPs trace similar cycles during both K-complexes (Figure 10C) and submovements (Figure 10F). We modelled the sedated condition by disconnecting motor and sensory connections between the feedback controller and the external world; instead providing a pulsatile input to the state estimator simulating a down- to up-state transition (Figure 10G). Effectively, transient excitation of the state estimator elicited an impulse response reflecting its intrinsic dynamics. The simulated LFPs generated a burst of delta-frequency oscillation around the K-complex (Figure 10H) which resembled submovement-related activity (Figure 10J,K). Projecting this activity onto the same PC plane revealed consistent cycles during simulated K-complexes (Figure 10I) and submovements (Figure 10L). Thus it appears that our computational model, incorporating the intrinsic dynamics of motor cortical networks, could also account for the conserved structure of low-frequency LFPs during movement and delta oscillations in sleep.
Discussion
Submovement kinematics are influenced by both extrinsic and intrinsic dynamics
Previous theories of intermittency have focused on either extrinsic or intrinsic explanations for the regularity of submovements, but little consensus has emerged over this fundamental feature of movement. There is good evidence for a common low-frequency oscillatory structure to motor cortex activity across multiple behavioral states (Churchland et al., 2012; Hall et al., 2014; Russo et al., 2018) but also an influence of feedback delays on submovement timing (Miall, 1996). Experimentally manipulating visual feedback with artificial time delays and spatial perturbations allowed us to dissociate both contributions to submovement kinematics. We found that precise frequencies of submovement peaks were determined by delays in the extrinsic feedback loop, but that these were embedded within a delay-independent envelope reflecting intrinsic filtering of feedback corrections. This dissociation of extrinsic and intrinsic dynamics was also evident in intracortically-recorded LFPs during tracking movements. Both delay-dependent feedback corrections and delay-independent cycles were observed in submovement-triggered averages of LFPs. Moreover, while coherence between LFPs and cursor movement exhibited delay-dependent spectral peaks, the imaginary coherence between multichannel LFPs revealed a consistent dynamical structure across behaviors.
Modelling isometric visuomotor tracking
We were able to explain these apparently contradictory results by using a continuous feedback control model, which incorporated optimal state estimation based on a second-order internal model of the external dynamics. Previously, intermittency has been implemented in optimal feedback control models by explicitly including a refractory period between submovements (Gawthrop et al., 2011; Sakaguchi et al., 2015), but theoretical justification for such an additions is lacking. In our model, submovements instead arose from constructive interference between motor errors and continuous, delayed feedback corrections. Optimal state estimation used a steady-state Kalman filter to separate process (motor) noise from measurement (sensory) noise. One free parameter was tuned to achieve correspondence between simulated and experimental data, namely the ratio of process to measurement noise, which determined the intrinsic resonance frequency around 2–3 Hz. It would be interesting in future to vary these noise characteristics experimentally (e.g. by artificially degrading visual acuity, or by extensively training subjects to produce faster or more accurate movements) and examine the effect on perturbation responses. One possible outcome would be a change to the observed resonance, although this seems to contradict the ubiquity of 2–3 Hz cortical dynamics. Alternatively, there may be other computational advantages to maintaining a consistent cortical rhythm. For example, it is notable that 2–3 Hz intrinsic dynamics matched the frequency of the primary submovement peak under unperturbed external feedback conditions, thus accentuating the fundamental submovement frequency around 2 Hz, while suppressing higher harmonics. This may be beneficial in allowing other aspects of the visuomotor machinery to be synchronized to a single rhythm, for example eye movements, which are influenced by hand movement during tracking tasks (Koken and Erkelens, 1992).
One puzzling feature of our results was that force amplitude responses to cursor perturbations were uniformly less than unity, which initially appears suboptimal for rejecting even slow perturbations. We first considered that proprioceptive information (which is in conflict with vision during cursor perturbations) might cause subjects to underestimate the true displacement of the cursor. However, sub-unity amplitude responses were also observed in separate experiments (not shown) when sinusoidal displacements were added to the target position. In this situation there was no discrepancy between vision and proprioception, yet subjects consistently undershot corrections to all but the lowest frequency perturbations (even in the absence of any delay). An alternative explanation is that subjects avoided making corrections requiring large changes to the motor command. This can be formalized by a cost function that is minimized by proportional-integral (PI) control, which has been used in the past to model human movement (Kleinman, 1974). It is more common in optimal control models to use cost functions that penalize the absolute motor command, leading to proportional feedback policies (Todorov and Jordan, 2002), under the assumption that this minimizes signal-dependent noise in muscles (Jones et al., 2002). However, the trajectory variability observed in our isometric tracking task appeared more correlated with large changes in finger forces, rather than the absolute force magnitude (Figure 1—figure supplement 3). Derivative-dependent motor noise was also evident as increased variability at high frequencies in our feedforward task (Figure 2—figure supplement 3). Since submovements result from constructive interference between tracking errors and feedback corrections, derivative-dependent motor noise also provides a counterintuitive, but necessary, explanation for why the amplitude of submovements increases with target speed (Figure 1—figure supplement 2). Increased intermittency cannot be a direct consequence of faster target motion, since the frequency content of this motion is nevertheless low by comparison to submovements. Rather, faster tracking requires a larger change in the motor command, leading to increased broad-band motor noise which, after constructive interference with feedback corrections, results in more pronounced peaks at submovement frequencies.
State estimation by motor cortical population dynamics
PCA of multichannel LFPs in monkey motor cortex revealed two underlying components, which we interpret as arising from distinct but coupled neural populations. The cyclical movement-related dynamics of these components resembled those described for M1 firing rates (Churchland et al., 2012), which have previously been implicated in feedforward generation of movement. Specifically, it was proposed that preparatory activity first develops along ‘output-null’ dimensions of the neural state space before, at movement onset, evolving via intrinsic dynamics into orthogonal ‘output-potent’ dimensions that drive muscles (Churchland et al., 2010). However, this purely feedforward view cannot account for our isometric tracking data, since manipulation of feedback delays dissociated delay-dependent submovements from delay-independent rotational dynamics. Instead we interpret these intrinsic dynamics as implementing a state estimator during continuous feedback control, driven by noise in motor and sensory signals. We used Newtonian dynamics to construct a simple two-dimensional state transition model based on both the cursor-target discrepancy and its first derivative. While this undoubtedly neglects the true complexity of muscle and limb biomechanics, simulations based on this plausible first approximation reproduced both the amplitude response and phase-delay to sinusoidal cursor perturbations in humans, and the population dynamics of LFP cycles in the monkey. We suggest that for discrete, fast movements to static targets, transient cursor-target discrepancies effectively provide impulse excitation to the state estimator, generating a rotational cycle in the neural space. Note that this account also offers a natural explanation of why preparatory and movement-related activity lies along distinct state-space dimensions, since the static discrepancy present during preparation is encoded differently to the changing discrepancy that exists during movement. At the same time, the lawful relationship between discrepancy and its derivative couples these dimensions within the state estimator and is evident as consistent rotational dynamics across different tasks and behavioral states.
It may seem unusual to ascribe the role of state estimation to M1, when this function is usually attributed to parietal (Mulliken et al., 2008) and premotor areas (where rotational dynamics have also been reported, albeit at a lower frequency [Churchland et al., 2012; Hall et al., 2014]). We suggest that the computations involved in optimal tracking behaviors are likely distributed across multiple cortical areas including (but not limited to) M1, with local circuitry reflecting multiple dynamical models of the various sensory and efference copy signals that must be integrated for accurate control. These could include the estimation of the position of moving stimuli based on noisy visual inputs (Kwon et al., 2015), as well the optimal integration of visual and somatosensory information, which may have different temporal delays (Crevecoeur et al., 2016).
An alternative explanation for consistent rotational dynamics has recently been proposed by Russo et al. (2018), based on the behavior of recurrent neural networks trained to produce different feed-forward muscle patterns whilst minimizing ‘tangling’ between neural trajectories. It is interesting to compare this with our OFC-based interpretation, since both are motivated by the problem of maintaining accurate behavior in the presence of noise. Minimizing tangling leads to network architectures that are robust to intrinsic noise in individual neurons, while OFC focusses on optimizing movements in the face of unreliable motor commands and noisy sensory signals. Given this conceptual link, it is perhaps unsurprising if recurrent neural network approaches learn implementations of computational architectures such as Kalman filters that minimize the influence of noise on behavior. In the future, it may be productive to incorporate sensory feedback into recurrent neural network models of movement, as well as including intrinsic sources of neural noise in optimal control models. The convergence of these frameworks may further help to reveal how computational principles are implemented in the human motor system.
Materials and methods
Subjects
Based on pilot studies, we decided in advance to use a sample size of eight subjects in each experiment. In total, we recruited 11 adult subjects at the Institute of Neuroscience, Newcastle University. Eight subjects (three females; age 23–33; one left-handed) participated in both Experiment 1 (feedback delay) and Experiment 2 (feedback delay and spatial perturbation). Eight subjects (three females; age 23–33; all right-handed) participated in Experiment 3 (feedforward task); 6 of these subjects also participated in experiments 1 and 2. Eight subjects (three females; age 23–33; all right-handed) participated in the experiment shown in Figure 1—figure supplement 2; 7 out of these subjects also participated in Experiment 3. All experiments were approved by the local ethics committee at Newcastle University and performed after informed consent, which was given in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.
Human tracking task
Request a detailed protocolSubjects tracked a (red) target on a computer monitor by exerting bimanual, isometric, index finger forces on two sensors (FSG15N1A; Honeywell). The target underwent uniform, slow, circular motion with a pseudorandom order of clockwise and anticlockwise directions across trials. Finger forces were sampled at 50 samples/s (USB-6343; National Instruments) and mapped to (yellow) cursor position, by projecting onto two diagonal screen axes. In addition, a feedback delay () was interposed between force and cursor movement. The feedback delay was kept constant throughout the duration of each trial (lasting 20 s). We express screen coordinates in terms of the radius of target motion, . Tracking the target rotation thus required the generation of sinusoidal motion in the range of −100% to +100%, corresponding to finger forces of 0 to 3.26N, with a 90° phase-shift between each hand. At the end of each trial, subjects were given a numerical score from 0 to 1000, indicating how accurately they had tracked the target. Subjects were instructed to attempt to maximize this score, which was calculated as:
where and are the 2D positions of the cursor and target respectively, and . Apart from the experiment shown in Figure 1—figure supplement 2, all experiments used a frequency of target rotation, = 0.2 rotations per second.
Experiment 1 used five delay conditions (= 0, 100, 200, 300, or 400 ms). Subjects performed a total of 70 trials, comprising 14 of each condition, presented in pseudorandom order.
For Experiment 2, spatial perturbations were added to the cursor position, as well as time delays. The perturbations were equivalent to sinusoidal modulation of the target angular velocity, but were instead added to the cursor. Expressed in polar coordinates relative to the center of the screen, the target and cursor positions were thus given by:
where is the angular velocity of the target around the centre of the screen, is the angular frequency of the perturbation, and is the unperturbed cursor position calculated from the subject’s forces at time .
Although the 2D cursor position was not constrained to follow the target trajectory, we did not analyze off-trajectory deviations. For simplicity, kinematic analyses were based on the time-varying angular velocity of the cursor subtended at the center of the screen:
For spatial perturbation experiments, we also calculated the angular velocity of the unperturbed cursor position subtended at the center of the screen:
Note that since , the perturbation effectively adds a sinusoidal component to the angular velocity of the cursor:
Six different spatial perturbations (= 0, 1, 2, 3, 4, 5 Hz) were combined with two feedback delays (= 0, 200 ms) yielding 12 conditions. Subjects performed a total of 144 trials, comprising 12 trials per condition, presented in pseudorandom order.
Human feedforward task
Request a detailed protocolIn Experiment 3, we used a unimanual isometric task in which subjects were asked to make sinusoidal forces with their right index finger. Subjects received visual feedback of the cursor, but no target was shown. Instead, subjects were shown two amplitude boundaries to move between, and the frequency of movement was cued with auditory beeps at frequencies of 1, 2, 3, 4 and 5 Hz. Subjects performed a total of 15 trials, comprising three 20 s trials per frequency condition.
Monkey experiments
Subjects
We used two purpose-bred female rhesus macaques (monkey S: 6 years old, 6.6 kg; monkey U: 6 years old, 8.8 kg). Animal experiments were approved by the local Animal Welfare Ethical Review Board and performed under appropriate UK Home Office licenses in accordance with the Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act 1986 (2013 revision).
Monkey isometric tracking task
Request a detailed protocolMonkeys moved a 2D computer cursor by generating isometric flexion-extension (vertical) and radial-ulnar (horizontal) torques at the wrist, measured by a 6-axis force/torque transducer (Nano25; ATI Industrial Automation). Centre-out targets were presented at 8 peripheral positions in a pseudorandom order. Targets were positioned at 70% of the distance to the screen edge (100% corresponding to torque of 0.67 Nm). The diameter of the target and cursor ranged between 14 and 36%. A successful trial required maintaining an overlap between cursor and peripheral target for 0.6 s, after which the monkeys returned the cursor to the center of the screen to receive a food reward. Visual feedback of the cursor was delayed by = 0, 200, 400, 600 ms throughout separate blocks of 50–70 trials each. Monkey S performed the task with the right hand. Monkey U initially used the right hand and was then retrained for a second period of data collection with the left hand.
LFP recording
Request a detailed protocolLFPs were recorded using custom arrays of 12 moveable 50 µm diameter tungsten microwires (impedance ~200 kΩ at 1 kHz) chronically implanted in the contralateral wrist area of M1 under sevoflurane anesthesia with postoperative analgesics and antibiotics. Head-free recordings were made using unity-gain headstages followed by wide-band amplification and sampling at 24.4 kilosamples/s (System 3; Tucker-Davis Technologies). LFPs were digitally low-pass filtered at 200 Hz and recorded at 488 samples/second.
Analysis of kinematics and neural data was performed on recordings over eight sessions comprising of 56 task blocks in Monkey S (no delay: 24 blocks; 200 ms delay: 13; 400 ms delay: 13; 600 ms delay: 6), and 89 sessions comprising of 356 task blocks in Monkey U (no delay: 89; 200 ms delay: 89; 400 ms delay: 89; 600 ms delay: 89). Each task block comprised 50 (monkey S) or 70 trials (monkey U).
Human data analysis
Request a detailed protocolSpectral analysis used fast Fourier transforms (FFTs) performed on non-overlapping 512 sample-point windows (approx. 10 s) taken from the middle of each trial. Submovement peaks in the power spectra were measured after smoothing with a seven-point moving-average.
For perturbation experiments, we additionally defined two complex transfer functions and :
Cursor and force amplitude responses to perturbations were calculated as the magnitude of the corresponding transfer functions, and the intrinsic phase delay of force responses was given by:
Additionally, tracking performance was quantified off-line using the root-mean-squared Euclidean distance between cursor and target.
Monkey data analysis
Request a detailed protocolWe differentiated the magnitude of the absolute 2D torque (expressed as a percentage of the distance to the edge of the screen) to obtain the radial cursor velocity. LFP channels were subjected to visual inspection to reject noisy channels prior to mean-subtraction. For time-domain analysis, LFPs and cursor velocities were low-pass filtered at 10 Hz. Submovements were defined as a peak radial cursor speed exceeding 100 %/s. For frequency-domain analysis, we took unfiltered sections of 1024 sample points from each trial (approx. 1.5 s before to 0.5 s after the end of the peripheral hold period). We subtracted the trial-averaged profile from each section before concatenating to yield long data sections without any consistent low-frequency components related to the periodicity of the task. FFTs were calculated with overlapping Hanning windows (214 sample points ≈ 34 s; 75% overlap), from which we derived the following spectra:
Cursor power:
LFP power:
LFP-cursor coherence:
LFP-LFP imaginary coherence:
where and represent Fourier coefficients at frequency and window from LFP channel and cursor velocity respectively. All spectra were smoothed with a 16-point Hanning window. In addition, LFP power and LFP-cursor coherence were averaged across all LFP channels, while LFP-LFP imaginary coherence was averaged over all pairs of LFPs.
Modelling
Request a detailed protocolAlthough both human and monkey tasks involved 2D isometric control, for simplicity we modelled only a 1D controller and assumed a one-to-one mapping from control signal, to position, . We neglected target motion and designed the controller to minimize the influence of stochastic motor errors using delayed, noisy feedback of position. We set the model time step = 0.01 s, intrinsic feedback delay = 0.26 s, and the ratio of process/measurement noise = 250 s−2 unless otherwise stated. Steady-state Kalman gains were calculated using the function kalman in Matlab, and the resultant discrete time dynamic system (Equation (4)) was implemented by two integrating neuronal populations representing and , receiving a synaptic input on each time-step equal to:
Two LFP components were simulated by normalizing and to unity variance, before adding background common noise with a spectrum.
The motor command was generated on each time step using the Smith Predictor architecture shown in Figure 3. Based on our observation that trajectory variability was maximal at times when force output was changing (Figure 1—figure supplement 3 ), we used a linear quadratic regulator (LQR) control framework to minimize a quadratic cost function, , incorporating the rate of change in motor command, :
For a state transition matrix in the form:
is minimized by a state feedback policy of the form:
which can be integrated to yield a PI controller:
We found the proportional and integral gains and using the function lqr in Matlab with and . In the full model, this controller acted on the optimal estimate of position, , after incorporating the delay feedback loop of the Smith Predictor. Note that the transfer function of a PI controller inside the fast feedback loop of the Smith Predictor is given by Abe and Yamanaka (2003):
which equals 1 for but tends to at higher frequencies. Therefore this effectively reduces the response amplitude to perturbations. The full transfer function of the intrinsic dynamics, including time-delay is given by:
where is the transfer function of the Kalman filter relating delayed position measurement to optimal position estimate.
Data and software availability
Request a detailed protocolDatasets from all human and monkey experiments, analysis code and model associated with this work are available on Dryad doi:10.5061/dryad.53sq7kn.
Data availability
Datasets from the human and monkey experiments, together with sample analysis code and modelling associated with this work are available on Dryad doi:10.5061/dryad.53sq7kn.
-
Dryad Digital RepositoryData from: Extrinsic and Intrinsic Dynamics in Movement Intermittency.https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.53sq7kn
References
-
Smith predictor control and internal model control - a tutorialSICE 2003 Annual Conference 2:1383–1387.
-
Theory of the human operator in control systems; the operator as an engineering systemThe British Journal of Psychology. General Section 38:56–61.
-
Dynamic multisensory integration: somatosensory speed trumps visual accuracy during feedback controlThe Journal of Neuroscience 36:8598–8611.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0184-16.2016
-
Intermittency in preplanned elbow movements persists in the absence of visual feedbackJournal of Neurophysiology 80:1787–1799.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1998.80.4.1787
-
Intermittent control: a computational theory of human controlBiological Cybernetics 104:31–51.https://doi.org/10.1007/s00422-010-0416-4
-
Dynamic primitives of motor behaviorBiological Cybernetics 106:727–739.https://doi.org/10.1007/s00422-012-0527-1
-
Sources of signal-dependent noise during isometric force productionJournal of Neurophysiology 88:1533–1544.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.2002.88.3.1533
-
Towards modeling human information processing and control in economic systems: an approach based on manned vehicle systems analysisAnnals of Economic and Social Measurement 3:117–134.
-
Influences of hand movements on eye movements in tracking tasks in manExperimental Brain Research 88:657–664.https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00228195
-
Invariant temporal characteristics of manipulative hand movementsExperimental Brain Research 78:539–546.https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00230241
-
Intermittency in human manual tracking tasksJournal of Motor Behavior 25:53–63.https://doi.org/10.1080/00222895.1993.9941639
-
Is the cerebellum a smith predictor?Journal of Motor Behavior 25:203–216.https://doi.org/10.1080/00222895.1993.9942050
-
Task-dependent changes in visual feedback control: a frequency analysis of human manual trackingJournal of Motor Behavior 28:125–135.https://doi.org/10.1080/00222895.1996.9941739
-
Sinusoidal visuomotor tracking: intermittent servo-control or coupled oscillations?Journal of Motor Behavior 33:329–349.https://doi.org/10.1080/00222890109601918
-
Optimal feedback control and the neural basis of volitional motor controlNature Reviews Neuroscience 5:532–545.https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1427
-
Distinct neural circuits for control of movement vs. holding stillJournal of Neurophysiology 117:1431–1460.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00840.2016
-
Variability and noise in continuous force productionJournal of Motor Behavior 32:141–150.https://doi.org/10.1080/00222890009601366
-
Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordinationNature Neuroscience 5:1226–1235.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn963
-
The intermittency of control movements and the psychological refractory period1British Journal of Psychology. General Section 38:149–157.https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1948.tb01150.x
-
Minimum-jerk, two-thirds power law, and isochrony: converging approaches to movement planningJournal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 21:32–53.https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.21.1.32
-
Accuracy of voluntary movementThe Psychological Review: Monograph Supplements 3:1–114.https://doi.org/10.1037/h0092992
Article and author information
Author details
Funding
Wellcome (106149)
- Andrew Jackson
Indonesia Endowment Fund for Education (S-2648/LPDP.3/2014)
- Damar Susilaradeya
Medical Research Council (K501396)
- Thomas M Hall
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Acknowledgements
We thank Jenifer Tulip and Norman Charlton for technical assistance. This work was supported by the Indonesia Endowment Fund for Education (S-2648/LPDP.3/2014), the Medical Research Council (K501396) and the Wellcome Trust (106149).
Ethics
Human subjects: All experiments were approved by the local ethics committee at Newcastle University (000023/2008) and performed after informed consent, which was given in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.
Animal experimentation: Animal experiments were approved by the local Animal Welfare Ethical Review Board and performed under appropriate UK Home Office licenses (PPL 60/4410) in accordance with the Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act 1986. Surgeries were performed under sevoflurane anesthesia with postoperative analgesics and antibiotics, and every effort was made to reduce suffering.
Copyright
© 2019, Susilaradeya et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 3,149
- views
-
- 210
- downloads
-
- 30
- citations
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Neuroscience
Sour taste, which is elicited by low pH, may serve to help animals distinguish appetitive from potentially harmful food sources. In all species studied to date, the attractiveness of oral acids is contingent on concentration. Many carboxylic acids are attractive at ecologically relevant concentrations but become aversive beyond some maximal concentration. Recent work found that Drosophila ionotropic receptors IR25a and IR76b expressed by sweet-responsive gustatory receptor neurons (GRNs) in the labellum, a peripheral gustatory organ, mediate appetitive feeding behaviors toward dilute carboxylic acids. Here, we disclose the existence of pharyngeal sensors in Drosophila melanogaster that detect ingested carboxylic acids and are also involved in the appetitive responses to carboxylic acids. These pharyngeal sensors rely on IR51b, IR94a, and IR94h, together with IR25a and IR76b, to drive responses to carboxylic acids. We then demonstrate that optogenetic activation of either Ir94a+ or Ir94h+ GRNs promotes an appetitive feeding response, confirming their contributions to appetitive feeding behavior. Our discovery of internal pharyngeal sour taste receptors opens up new avenues for investigating the internal sensation of tastants in insects.
-
- Neuroscience
Granule cells of the cerebellum make up to 175,000 excitatory synapses on a single Purkinje cell, encoding the wide variety of information from the mossy fibre inputs into the cerebellar cortex. The granule cell axon is made of an ascending portion and a long parallel fibre extending at right angles, an architecture suggesting that synapses formed by the two segments of the axon could encode different information. There are controversial indications that ascending axon (AA) and parallel fibre (PF) synapse properties and modalities of plasticity are different. We tested the hypothesis that AA and PF synapses encode different information, and that the association of these distinct inputs to Purkinje cells might be relevant to the circuit and trigger plasticity, similar to the coincident activation of PF and climbing fibre inputs. Here, by recording synaptic currents in Purkinje cells from either proximal or distal granule cells (mostly AA and PF synapses, respectively), we describe a new form of associative plasticity between these two distinct granule cell inputs. We show for the first time that synchronous AA and PF repetitive train stimulation, with inhibition intact, triggers long-term potentiation (LTP) at AA synapses specifically. Furthermore, the timing of the presentation of the two inputs controls the outcome of plasticity and induction requires NMDAR and mGluR1 activation. The long length of the PFs allows us to preferentially activate the two inputs independently, and despite a lack of morphological reconstruction of the connections, these observations reinforce the suggestion that AA and PF synapses have different coding capabilities and plasticity that is associative, enabling effective association of information transmitted via granule cells.