Functionally defined white matter of the macaque monkey brain reveals a dorso-ventral attention network
Abstract
Classical studies of attention have identified areas of parietal and frontal cortex as sources of attentional control. Recently, a ventral region in the macaque temporal cortex, the posterior infero-temporal dorsal area PITd, has been suggested as a third attentional control area. This raises the question of whether and how spatially distant areas coordinate a joint focus of attention. Here we tested the hypothesis that parieto-frontal attention areas and PITd are directly interconnected. By combining functional MRI with ex-vivo high-resolution diffusion MRI, we found that PITd and dorsal attention areas are all directly connected through three specific fascicles. These results ascribe a new function, the communication of attention signals, to two known fiber-bundles, highlight the importance of vertical interactions across the two visual streams, and imply that the control of endogenous attention, hitherto thought to reside in macaque dorsal cortical areas, is exerted by a dorso-ventral network.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.40520.001Introduction
Attention is the neuro-cognitive function that selects currently relevant pieces of information at the expense of irrelevant ones (Carrasco, 2011; Chelazzi et al., 2011). Attentional selection results from the interplay between exogenous factors, like intrinsic object saliency, and endogenous processes, like the relevance of an object for the task at hand. Endogenous attention is thought to be controlled by a fronto-parietal network (Corbetta et al., 2008; Kastner and Ungerleider, 2000). In the macaque monkey brain, the lateral intraparietal area (LIP; Bisley and Goldberg, 2010; Gottlieb et al., 1998) and the frontal eye field (FEF; Thompson and Bichot, 2005) exhibit key properties of attentional control and are thought to form the core of the endogenous attention network (Stemmann and Freiwald, 2016).
The fronto-parietal theory of attentional control is supported by recent whole-brain functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies in the macaque monkey using a wide range of attention tasks (Caspari et al., 2015; Patel et al., 2015; Stemmann and Freiwald, 2016). One of these studies (Stemmann and Freiwald, 2016), found that an area in the lower bank of the Superior Temporal Sulcus (STS), the dorsal posterior infero-temporal area, PITd (Felleman and Van Essen, 1991; Saleem and Logothetis, 2007) was even more strongly attention-modulated than LIP, was engaged by multiple attention tasks, and was not modulated by the task-relevant feature dimension. Thus PITd was shown to exhibit key properties of an attentional control area (Figure 1A–B; Stemmann and Freiwald, 2016). These results raised the possibility that the network for endogenous control of attention may not be confined to fronto-parietal (dorsal) structures, but also includes a ventral region in the temporal lobe.
A ventral attention network has been previously proposed in humans (Corbetta et al., 2008; Geng and Vossel, 2013). However, its functional properties are profoundly different from those of macaque area PITd. First, human dorsal and ventral attention networks have a different functional profile, with the first primarily supporting endogenous attention, and the latter primarily supporting exogenous attention (Corbetta et al., 2008), along with context updating (Geng and Vossel, 2013) and social cognition (Carter and Huettel, 2013; Mars et al., 2013; Schwiedrzik et al., 2015). On the contrary ventral area PITd shared similar functional properties with dorsal areas LIP and FEF (Stemmann and Freiwald, 2016). Second, the most prominent part of the human ventral attention network, the temporo-parietal-junction (TPJ), is suppressed during endogenous attention, and activated during target detection and shifts of attention, particularly when stimuli are salient or unexpected (Corbetta et al., 2008; Corbetta and Shulman, 2002), whereas PITd was strongly activated during prolonged sustained attention (Stemmann and Freiwald, 2016). Third, macaque PITd possesses a putative human homolog, phPITd, an area located far ventrally to TPJ (Glasser et al., 2016; Kolster et al., 2010). Thus the discovery of attention area PITd located ventrally to the ventral attention network raises important questions about attentional control mechanisms and their support circuitry.
In the brain imaging literature, the term network is often used loosely to describe a set of co-activated brain regions without explicit reference to their physical connections (Petersen and Sporns, 2015). The fact that multiple brain areas exhibit similar functional profiles across visual and cognitive conditions, like PITd, LIP and FEF did in (Stemmann and Freiwald, 2016), could be for a number of fundamentally different reasons. Co-activation might be initiated by a common driver outside the brain, like the visual stimulus, it could result from a common source inside the brain, with functional nodes only indirectly connected, or from connections that directly link the areas of interest. Only the characterization of the connectivity pattern between functionally identified areas establishes true network status. Understanding the structure of connectivity between putative attention control areas would thus be critical to establish whether they form an attention control network and help to answer the question of how they might coordinate a common focus of attention.
Long-range inter-areal connectivity is supported by the white matter. Uncovering the white matter tracts supporting endpoint-to-endpoint connections is important to know for at least two reasons. First, fascicles respond to behavior by adapting density, shape, and molecular composition in a manner that corresponds to individual abilities in health and disease (Fields, 2008; Jbabdi et al., 2015; Pestilli, 2018; Rokem et al., 2017; Thomason and Thompson, 2011; Wandell, 2016), and thus take an active role in shaping network function. To fully understand brain functionality, we thus need to combine knowledge about brain function and structure. Second, connectivity is crucial to elucidating how neurons and neural networks process information. The main organizing principle of the visual system is its separation into two major posterior-anterior information-processing streams: a dorsal path for spatial processing, and a ventral path for feature and object processing (Kravitz et al., 2013; Milner and Goodale, 1995). Each pathway may contain multiple, largely parallel routes (Kravitz et al., 2013), a conclusion that seems to be confirmed by the existence of dorsal and ventral attention areas. In this view, the study of connectivity between parietal and temporal attention nodes can serve as a model system for a general understanding of information flow between the dorsal and ventral visual pathways and between the parietal and temporal lobes at large.
Here we sought to, first, determine connections between attention areas PITd, LIP, and FEF, and, second, to delineate the long association fascicles of this postulated endogenous attention control network. Previous anatomical studies have demonstrated strong and direct connections between parietal area LIP and frontal area FEF (Blatt et al., 1990; Lewis and Van Essen, 2000; Petrides and Pandya, 2006) mainly via the Superior Longitudinal Fasciculus (SLF). However, function and anatomy of PITd have, so far, received little attention. More specifically, to date only a single classical anatomical study has targeted PITd (Distler et al., 1993), but due to the study’s paucity of data and lack of functional characterization, the relationship to attention networks remains unclear. We thus asked whether PITd connects to LIP, whether it connects to the FEF, and which fiber pathways these cortical areas use to exchange attentional information (Figure 1D). The general anatomy of macaque white matter organization suggests different candidate pathways: PITd may connect to LIP through the Vertical Occipital Fasciculus (VOF), a fascicle that has been shown to have some ventral terminations in the temporal cortex (Takemura et al., 2017). Alternatively, PITd and LIP might connect through a more anterior fascicle, the vertical branch of the Inferior Longitudinal Fasciculus (vILF). Similarly, it would be possible for PITd and the FEF to connect via one of two major pathways: fibers might run via the Arcuate Fasciculus (AF), as areas in the caudal superior temporal regions do (Petrides and Pandya, 2006). Alternatively, fibers connecting PITd and FEF could course via the Extreme Capsule (EmC), as the middle superior temporal region and some anterior patches of the inferior temporal sulcus do (Petrides and Pandya, 1988; Petrides and Pandya, 2006).
To test these hypotheses, we developed a new approach that combines in-vivo fMRI with high-resolution ex-vivo diffusion imaging (dMRI) and ensemble probabilistic tractography. This experimental approach was specifically designed to facilitate the comparison of dMRI with classical tracer studies, and overcome some of the limitations of dMRI and tractography. For example, this approach reduces artifacts and distortions, improves robustness to crossing fibers, and advances anatomical accuracy. This technique allows for the study of the connectivity of functionally specific brain areas, the exploration of fine-grain structural connectivity, and the description of the pathways connecting the functional areas. These three aspects are key to the understanding of brain functional networks, and the lack of a method combining them all has prevented, so far, the full characterization of the macaque attention network. High-resolution dMRI, combined with functionally-defined tractography, also facilitates the comparison between human and non-human cognitive networks, helps define which circuitry in the macaque attention system can serve as an accurate model for humans, and bridges the gap between traditional tracer studies and dMRI in humans.
Results
Ventral cortical area PITd directly connects to the fronto-parietal attention network
To unravel whether PITd communicates with attention areas LIP and FEF in parietal and frontal cortex, we combined fMRI localization of attention-modulated areas PITd, LIP, and FEF with ex-vivo dMRI (Figure 1A–C). We acquired high angular and spatial resolution dMRI, and preprocessed diffusion images (Material and methods). We first estimated the voxel-wise microstructure of the brain tissue, computed Fractional Anisotropy (FA), Principal Diffusion Direction (PDD) maps (Material and methods), and identified the position of putative connections (Pajevic and Pierpaoli, 1999; Takemura et al., 2017; Thiebaut de Schotten et al., 2012; Yeatman et al., 2013). Second, we performed ROI-to-ROI probabilistic tractography to create a macrostructural model of brain pathways, and, third, we compared tracking results with previous histological studies.
Attentional activation was defined through a demanding attention task requiring motion discrimination and spatial selection (Figure 1A; see also Stemmann and Freiwald, 2016). Monkeys were cued to pay attention to one of two random dots surfaces both to detect and discriminate the occurrence of a coherent motion event (see Material and methods). To isolate cortical areas modulated specifically by endogenous attention and define LIP and PITd, we contrasted blocks with two spatial attention conditions: attend contralateral and attend ipsilateral (Figure 1B, Figure 1—figure supplement 1, and Supplementary file 1). These spatial attention conditions were dissociated from saccade planning, and largely separated from motion selectivity (Figure 1B, dashed lines). FEF was defined by means of a guided saccade task where monkeys were required, in different blocks, to either maintain fixation on a central target, or make saccades (Figure 1B, see also Stemmann and Freiwald, 2016).
We first established the position of putative connections. In high-resolution FA and PDD maps, the core of several major tracts within the white matter was clearly identifiable as a bright area where fibers run in the same direction (Figure 2A). Crucially, we found a major superior-inferior diffusion direction (blue) in the lateral temporal white matter close to PITd (Figure 2B). This is compatible with a direct vertical connection between PITd and LIP. The tract was identifiable from PDD maps in multiple slices between the two ROIs (Figure 2C) and consistently in all subjects (Figure 2—figure supplement 1).
To confirm the existence of a direct connection between LIP and PITd, we performed probabilistic tractography and defined streamlines (i.e. putative fiber trajectories reconstructed by the tractography algorithm) between the two ROIs (see Material and methods). We found a white matter tract connecting the two areas (Figure 3B). We observed the core of the tract at a consistent position for all subjects and hemispheres (Figure 3—figure supplement 1, yellow streamlines), thus validating the hypothesis of a direct vertical connection (Figure 1D, dark and light pink dashed lines).
Using a similar approach, we asked whether PITd connects to prefrontal attention and oculo-motor area FEF (Schall and Hanes, 1993; Thompson and Bichot, 2005).We performed probabilistic tractography between PITd and FEF and successfully identified a white matter tract directly connecting PITd and FEF (Figure 3C). We observed the core of the tract for all subjects and hemispheres (Figure 3—figure supplement 1; cyan streamlines). The findings support the hypothesis (Figure 1D, dark and light green dashed lines) that a direct connection between PITd and FEF exists.
To quantify the physical properties of the two white matter tracts, we calculated the number of streamlines, the length, the density (number of streamlines per mm3), and the volume (number of voxels touched by the streamlines in mm3), of the connections (see Material and methods). Across subjects, LIP-PITd connections comprised an estimated 777 ± 300 streamlines and PITd-FEF connections 188.5 ± 77.8 streamlines. The fronto-temporal tract was on average longer than the temporo-parietal connection (Figure 3D, left panel), it had a lower density and similar volume (Figure 3D, middle panels). Next, we used the Linear Fascicle Evaluation (LiFE, Caiafa and Pestilli, 2017; Pestilli et al., 2014) to assess the statistical evidence (strength of evidence, see Material and methods) supporting the existence of the fascicles. Briefly, we used a virtual lesion method (Honey and Sporns, 2008; Pestilli et al., 2014; Takemura et al., 2016a) to compare the prediction accuracy on the diffusion signals between two connectome models, that is two comprehensive maps of neural connections in the brain. The first connectome model contains all streamlines (unlesioned), and the second connectome model contains all streamlines except the tract of interest (lesioned; see also Material and methods). The strength of evidence values demonstrate statistically that the measured data has signal consistent with the reported tracts (Figure 3D, right panel). In sum, the visible evidence on PDD maps (Figure 2), the quantitative descriptions of the streamlines (Figure 3A–C), the statistical evidence computed through LiFE (Figure 3D), all agree in supporting direct PITd-LIP and PITd-FEF wiring.
vILF and EmC connect dorsal and ventral attention areas
Tractography is ideally suited to determine not only whether any two areas are connected to each other, but also which routes these connections take, a piece of knowledge typically not available in classical tracer studies (Schmahmann and Pandya, 2006). Thus next, we characterized the white matter tract anatomy of the macaque attention network. First, we tested whether the connections between functionally defined areas LIP and FEF conform to the previously established finding, that direct LIP-FEF connections run through a component of the Superior Longitudinal Fasciculus (SLF; Figure 1D dark and light blue dashed lines; Blatt et al., 1990; Lewis and Van Essen, 2000; Petrides and Pandya, 2006), a notion obtained without specifically targeting attentional areas. Second, we tested the hypothesis that direct PITd-LIP connections are part of the vILF and distinct of the Vertical Occipital Fasciculus (VOF, Figure 1D dark and light pink lines), a fascicle that has been shown to have some ventral terminations in the temporal cortex and may therefore subserve PITd (Takemura et al., 2017). Third, we tested the hypothesis that the PITd-FEF pathway runs through the ILF and EmC, and is distinct from the Arcuate Fasciculus (AF; Figure 1D dark and light green lines; Petrides and Pandya, 2006). We tested these hypotheses in two ways. Firstly, we showed and quantified the spatial relationships between our functionally-defined bundles and major brain tracts. Using standard inclusion and exclusion anatomical planes (Figure 4—figure supplement 1; see also Material and methods) we defined the VOF, vILF, ILF, EmC, AF, SLFII and SLFIII, relying on anatomical information from previous tracer studies (for example, Petrides and Pandya, 2006; Schmahmann and Pandya, 2006; Schmahmann et al., 2007) as well as from dMRI studies (Croxson et al., 2005; Eichert et al., 2018; Mars et al., 2016; Takemura et al., 2017; Thiebaut de Schotten et al., 2011; Thiebaut de Schotten et al., 2012). We then established a direct comparison between our results and previous autoradiography studies (Material and methods), a histological approach able to show fiber path (Schmahmann and Pandya, 2006).
Probabilistic tractography between FEF and LIP demonstrated a direct pathway between these two dorsal attention areas (Figure 4A and Figure 3—figure supplement 1; orange). This fronto-parietal connection had an average length comparable to the PITd-FEF tract but showed higher density and volume (Figure 3—figure supplement 2). These results confirm numerous tracer studies reporting a strong connection between LIP and FEF. We next investigated whether the LIP-FEF connection may be part of SLF II or SLFIII, as defined in Thiebaut de Schotten et al. (2011) (see also Figure 4—figure supplement 1A). Consistent with previous autoradiography studies (Schmahmann and Pandya, 2006), SLFIII emerges from the inferior parietal lobule, courses horizontally through the white matter of the parietal and frontal operculum and reaches the region of the arcuate sulcus (Figure 4A, dark blue traces). Similarly, SLFII links the posterior parietal cortex to frontal lobe regions by running horizontally through the white matter dorsal to SLFIII (Figure 4A, light blue traces). The FEF-LIP tract is part of the most dorso-medial region of SLFIII and the most ventro-medial region of SLFII (Figure 4B, orange traces) thus lying at the intersection between SLFIII and SLFII and overlapping almost equally with both tracts (Figure 4C). These results complement previous studies by adding functional specificity and by localizing more precisely the anatomical position of FEF-LIP bundle.
To determine whether the PITd-LIP tract runs through the vILF or the VOF, we overlaid these two tracts and demonstrated their spatial separation (Figure 4E). LIP-PITd tract is located anterior to VOF, runs mainly vertically, but it is also elongated from posterior to anterior (Figure 4F, yellow streamlines) and it almost completely overlap with the vILF (Figure 4G). The VOF also runs vertically but lacks the anterior-posterior elongation and overall has more posterior endpoints which mostly serve occipital visual areas. We did not find any streamline running through the VOF (Figure 4E), and the only overlap between this tract and PITd-LIP connection occurs at the level of VOF termination in the temporal lobe (Figure 4F–G). Importantly, vILF doesn’t seem to be easier to obtain when compared to VOF (Figure 4H). A control analyses, where both PITd-LIP tract and the VOF were functionally defined, confirmed the separation between the two bundles (Figure 4—figure supplement 3). Interestingly, when we functionally parceled areas nearby PITd to test for some degree of specificity in the way PITd connects to LIP, we found that PITd showed the highest degree of overlap with vILF (Figure 4—figure supplement 4A–C). We then directly compared this result with autoradiography studies and established that PITd-LIP connection is part of the vILF (Figure 4F, see also section 105 and 129 of Schmahmann and Pandya, 2006). Tracer studies have shown that fibers of the vILF terminated ventrally in areas V4t, MT, MST, and FST in the fundus of the STS and dorsally in the lower bank of the IPS (Blatt et al., 1990; Schmahmann and Pandya, 2006). Here we show that the vILF also contains a bundle connecting the two functionally defined attention areas PITd and LIP, and provide anatomical and functional evidence for the proposed distinction between VOF and vILF (Takemura et al., 2017).
To test whether PITd-FEF connection may be part of the horizontal limb of ILF and then enter the frontal lobe through the ventral component of the EmC, we defined the ILF as in Takemura et al. (2017) (see also Figure 4—figure supplement 1C), EmC as in Mars et al. (2016), the AF as in Eichert et al. (2018). ILF courses horizontally along the anterior-posterior axis of the temporal lobe within the white matter of the infero-temporal region and parahippocampal gyrus (Figure 4I, violet streamlines). ILF continues anteriorly within the temporal lobe up to the anterior end of the lateral geniculate nucleus, whereas FEF-PITd tract defined through functional ROI tracking departs from the ILF (Figure 4I, cyan streamlines) and joins the EmC (Figure 4I, dark green streamlines). A direct comparison of this results with autoradiography studies suggests FEF-PITd bundle leaves the seeded temporal ROI by travelling in the horizontal limb of ILF (Figure 4L, left panel) and then enters the frontal lobe through the ventral component of the EmC (Figure 4L, right panel). We also tested whether the tract overlapped with AF (Figure 4M). We did not find any streamline running through the AF (Figure 4I–L, light green), and the only overlap between this tract and PITd-FEF connection occurs at the level of the termination in the frontal lobe. Importantly, EmC and ILF don’t seem to be easier to obtain when compared to the alternative hypothesis AF (Figure 4N). Interestingly, we functionally parceled areas nearby PITd to test for some degree of specificity in the way PITd connect to FEF (Figure 4—figure supplement 4D–F). We found that PITd showed a similar degree of overlap with ILF/EmC as neighboring areas in the lower bank of the STS, and slightly higher overlap when compared to areas in the STS fundus, which are specialized for motion processing (see also Discussion). Thus, here we show that attention-modulated area PITd connects to the FEF through the ILF and the EmC, rather than through the AF. This wiring resembles that of middle superior temporal regions connecting to the prearcuate gyrus (Petrides and Pandya, 1988; Petrides and Pandya, 2006).
Collectively, these results thus establish that the vILF, but not the VOF, connects attention-modulated PITd with LIP, and that the EmC, but not the AF, connects PITd to FEF.
Discussion
Endogenous attentional control in both humans and macaque monkeys is thought to be exerted by a fronto-parietal network (Corbetta et al., 2008; Gottlieb et al., 1998; Kastner and Ungerleider, 2000; Thompson and Bichot, 2005). Here we show that, in the macaque brain, classical attentional control areas LIP, in posterior parietal cortex, and FEF, in prefrontal cortex, are connected to PITd, an area in temporal cortex only recently implied in endogenous attention (Stemmann and Freiwald, 2016). We thus provide new evidence that PITd might be an endogenous attention control area, that attentional control may not be limited to dorsal fronto-parietal structures in the macaque brain, and that these areas form an attention network through direct connections (Figure 5 and Figure 5—video 1). The fine anatomical characterization of the bundles connecting the three areas constitutes the first connectivity model of the macaque endogenous attention and should inform future theories of attentional processing.
PITd is a visual area located in the lower bank of the STS, it contains neurons that are shape- and color-selective (Conway and Tsao, 2009; Hikosaka, 1998) and have circumscribed receptive fields slightly larger than those of V4 (Hikosaka, 1998). Its anatomical connectivity and its role in cognition have been little explored. While LIP-FEF connectivity has been established through tracer studies (Blatt et al., 1990; Lewis and Van Essen, 2000; Petrides and Pandya, 2006), connectivity of either area with the attention-modulated part of PITd has hitherto remained unknown. Even general connectivity with PITd, anatomically defined, was not fully established. One previous anatomical study investigated cortical connections of inferior temporal areas (Distler et al., 1993) and, through a single tracer injection in PITd, provided some evidence for connectivity between PITd and LIP. Due to the study’s paucity of data and lack of relating connectivity to functional characterization, the relationship to attentional control networks remained unclear. Similarly, tracer studies targeting LIP and FEF showed variable results in terms of existence and strength of connections with PITd (Barbas and Mesulam, 1981; Barbas, 1988; Blatt et al., 1990; Bullier et al., 1996; Distler et al., 1993; Schall et al., 1995). These inconsistencies are likely the result of differences in tracing methods, injection sites, anatomical definitions, and possibly even nomenclature (Felleman and Van Essen, 1991; Kötter, 2004; Stephan et al., 2001). We resolved these difficulties by combining functional mapping with dMRI. The part of PITd that is strongly modulated by attention in ways similar to those of LIP and FEF, is directly connected to these two classical attentional control regions. The comparative tractography we conducted between PITd and nearby functional areas (Figure 4—figure supplement 4) confirms the existence of tract-specific connections from PITd and demonstrates that, structurally, this connectivity is part of a larger-scale pattern of connectivity that PITd shares with its immediate neighborhood. Yet, functionally, PITd differs fundamentally from nearby STS areas in not being motion selective, while being very strongly attention modulated during attentive motion discrimination. Thus, functional specificity of PITd does not arise as a simple consequence of its connectivity with LIP and FEF. It is noteworthy that this connectivity scheme is different from the nearby face-processing network (Grimaldi et al., 2016; Moeller et al., 2008), which encompasses two face areas right next to attention-modulated area PITd (Stemmann and Freiwald, 2016). In the face-processing network, tight connectivity between face areas helps to ensure domain specificity (for faces) (Grimaldi et al., 2016; Moeller et al., 2008). Yet, for attention control networks shared connectivity fulfills the need to exert control over multiple areas and gather visual information from across multiple visual domains (Fecteau and Munoz, 2006). In particular, PITd’s position within the ventral visual pathway is likely crucial to collect attention-relevant information about properties of objects (Conway and Tsao, 2009; Hikosaka, 1998), including faces (Tsao et al., 2006) and gaze (Marciniak et al., 2014), possibly providing information on object value (Ghazizadeh et al., 2018). Through the described connections, PITd might thus make important contributions to core functionality of endogenous attention and to a wide range of attention-related functions.
In humans, attention has been proposed to be controlled by two networks with different functions, located in the dorsal and ventral part of the brain (Corbetta et al., 2008). The most prominent part of the ventral attention network is the temporo-parietal-junction (TPJ). TPJ is suppressed during endogenous attention, and activated during target detection and shifts of attention, particularly when stimuli are salient or unexpected (Corbetta et al., 2008; Corbetta and Shulman, 2002). It is thus thought to be functionally, but also anatomically, distinct from the dorsal fronto-parietal network (Corbetta et al., 2008). An area homologous to TPJ in the macaque has been hypothesized, but not yet found (Patel et al., 2015). Macaque PITd is activated during sustained attention (Stemmann and Freiwald, 2016), does not activate during attentional shift (Caspari et al., 2015), and, as we show here, is structurally connected to the dorsal fronto-parietal network. PITd thus bears neither functional nor structural similarity to human TPJ. One of the macaque candidate regions previously suggested for the putative correspondence to the human TPJ is the dorsal bank of STS and the adjacent superior temporal gyrus. Lesions of this region cause neglect-like (or spatial extinction-like) syndromes (Luh et al., 1986; Scalaidhe et al., 1995; Watson et al., 1994), which led to a suggestion of a possible homology to the (right hemisphere) superior temporal gyrus (STG) in humans, one of the major lesion foci in neglect patients (Karnath, 2001). Alternatively, macaque TPO/STP and neighboring parietal area seven were hypothesized to correspond to human TPJ. These dorsal STS areas show, similarly to areas LIP and FEF, memory delay activity during saccade preparation (Kagan et al., 2010), and exhibit task-specific changes after reversible LIP inactivation (Wilke et al., 2012). Furthermore, recent work on the social brain also suggested that a potential precursor to human TPJ may reside in macaque posterior dorsal STS (Schwiedrzik et al., 2015) or in area 7a (Sliwa and Freiwald, 2017). Furthermore, macaque PITd possesses a putative human homolog, phPITd, an area located far ventrally to TPJ (Glasser et al., 2016; Kolster et al., 2010). Human phPITd occupies a similar positions in retinotopic cortex as macaque PITd, and, like PITd, is sensitive to two-dimensional shapes and is insensitive to motion (Kolster et al., 2010). Our results predict, under a homology argument of attention network organization, this area as the ventral node of the human endogenous attention network.
PITd connects to LIP and FEF through specific white matter tracts. Knowing these tracts is important for a full characterization of endogenous attentional control circuits, their vulnerabilities, and developmental potential. For example, the properties of specific fiber-bundles have been found to correlate with attentive behaviors (Chechlacz et al., 2015; Chica et al., 2018; Parlatini et al., 2017), and their disconnection results in attentional deficits like spatial neglect (Carter et al., 2017; Lunven and Bartolomeo, 2017), it is therefore of great importance to characterize their structure and path. Traditional tracer studies typically focus on the analysis of the origins and terminations of white matter fibers, without characterizing the white matter pathways between the endpoints. Our experimental approach, by capitalizing on the macroscopic bird-eye view of dMRI (Jbabdi et al., 2015; Rokem et al., 2017) combined with behaviorally guided fMRI (Stemmann and Freiwald, 2016), allowed us also to describe the cortico-cortical pathways of the endogenous control attention. We established that the vertical branch of the ILF, but not the VOF, connects the two posterior attention areas PITd with LIP, and that the ILF and the EmC, but not the AF, connects PITd to the dorsal prefrontal area FEF. Our results thus imply a role in attentional control for the vILF and the ILF/EmC. (Figure 5 and Figure 5—video 1).
We have used quality data, combined multimodal data, and used advanced quantification and statistical evaluation methods (see Material and methods), yet we fully acknowledge that tractography has limitations and is challenged by the trade-off between specificity and sensitivity (Thomas et al., 2014). Recent studies have demonstrated limitations of dMRI-based tractography in three important way: (1) in the ability to solve crossing fibers (Maier-Hein et al., 2017; Roebroeck et al., 2008), (2) in the dependency on tractography algorithms or parameter settings (Bastiani et al., 2012; Chamberland et al., 2014; Domin et al., 2014; Kunimatsu et al., 2004; Parizel et al., 2007; Takemura et al., 2016a; Taoka et al., 2009; Thomas et al., 2014), and (3) in the accuracy for estimating fiber projections into cortical gray matter (Reveley et al., 2015). Our results set the foundation for future investigations using other modalities to infer the connectivity of attentional control circuitry in macaque monkeys and other species, like human subjects.
In humans, two distinct vertical fiber bundles that connect the dorsal and ventral visual streams have been described, the VOF and the posterior Arcuate Fasciculus (pAF). The VOF supports a variety of perceptual functions, like skilled reading and spatial orienting (Takemura et al., 2016b; Yeatman et al., 2014; Yeatman et al., 2013), the pAF, located anterior to the VOF (Catani and Thiebaut de Schotten, 2008; Thiebaut de Schotten et al., 2014; Weiner et al., 2017), is thought to support language (Catani and Thiebaut de Schotten, 2008; Thiebaut de Schotten et al., 2014) and action-to-object coordination (Budisavljevic et al., 2018). The existence of two separated vertical tracts in the posterior part of the human brain resembles the connectivity profile of the monkey described here and suggest a possible resolution of current cross-species inconsistencies in the literature. Because of its anatomical location, its cortical endpoints, and its separation from the VOF, human pAF may be a candidate tract for the exchange of attentional information between dorsal and ventral attention areas in the posterior part of the human brain, thus being functionally and anatomically similar to the vILF. The fronto-temporal connection between macaque PITd and FEF via the EmC we found in this study, may also have a human homolog. Human EmC is important for language (Catani and Bambini, 2014; Friederici and Gierhan, 2013) and may also carry attentional information (Umarova et al., 2010). Umarova and colleagues found that human EmC connects ventral frontal cortex and insula to the TPJ, namely the core nodes of the human exogenous attention network (Corbetta et al., 2008; Corbetta and Shulman, 2002). A direct connection via EmC between phPITd and FEF would suggest a division of function between phPITd and TPJ, but similar frontal lobe connectivity. Alternatively, the functional difference between PITd and TPJ may be paralleled by a difference in structural connectivity: the AF may be a good candidate to exchange endogenous attention information between human frontal and temporal lobes. Future work examining both structure and function in the human brain should be able clarify the structural similarity between macaque and human attention networks and thus reveal putative constraints by white-matter fascicles on the evolution of functional networks. Determining the mesoscopic correspondence between structure and function across species is a fundamental challenge for neuroscience (Zhang et al., 2013), a challenge that cannot be met by pure functional and pure anatomical measurements alone (Pollen and Hofmann, 2008). The first fine-scale functional characterization of the white matter tracts supporting endogenous attention described here sets up a new framework to understand the evolution of function in white matter bundles, and to determine the relationship between brain structure and function during evolution.
The main organizing principle of the visual system is its separation into two major posterior-anterior information-processing streams: a dorsal path for spatial processing, and a ventral path for feature and object processing (Kravitz et al., 2013; Milner and Goodale, 1995). Separation of information into different streams, however, appears to be at odds with the requirements of visual attention which generally needs to integrate spatial and featural information (Baylis et al., 2001; Milner, 2017; Treisman and Gelade, 1980). One possible solution to this binding problem, is the development of shape-selectivity in the dorsal stream (Theys et al., 2015). Alternatively, vertical connections between the dorsal and ventral attention areas as described here, provide a structural substrate for attentional integration that capitalizes on the full functional repertoire of both streams. This vertical connectivity may thus be a general theme, a second organizing principle of the visual system in humans (Kay and Yeatman, 2017; Pestilli et al., 2014) and macaques (Nelissen et al., 2011), extending from attention to other functions requiring vertical integration like language (Cloutman, 2013) or skilled grasping (van Polanen and Davare, 2015).
Taken together, our work advances the understanding of the architecture of attentional control by suggesting a three-node cortical network, with nodes in three different cortical lobes as the structural substrate of this cognitive function. The description of direct structural connections between these nodes we provide here, now allows investigations of the ‘effective connectivity’ between attention areas and the causal impact of these interactions. The findings reported here have important consequences for the understanding of the mechanisms and functional capabilities of endogenous attention, for cognitive theories of endogenous attention (Itti et al., 2005), for the evolution of attention systems, and for the functional organization of the primate brain.
Materials and methods
Animal model and sample preparation
Request a detailed protocolAll animal procedures met the National Institutes of Health Guide for Care and Use of Laboratory Animals, and were approved by the local Institutional Animal Care and Use Committees of the Rockefeller University (protocol number 15849 hr). The brain of four adult male Rhesus macaque monkeys (Macaca mulatta, M1-4) (~10–15 y old,~9 kg) were used in this study. Animals were euthanized first by sedation using ketamine and dexdomitor, followed by deep anesthetization using pentobarbital and phenytoin, and then were perfused transcardially with 4% (wt/vol) paraformaldehyde in phosphate buffer. The brains were extracted and immersed for 24 hr in the same fixation solution. Gadolinium- diethylenetriamine penta-acetic acid (Gd-DTPA) (Magnevist, Berlex Laboratories) was added at 0.1% vol to the fixation solution. After a 24 hr fixation, the brain was soaked for 5–9 weeks at 4°C in phosphate-buffered Saline (PBS) mixed with 0.1% Gd-DTPA to reduce the longitudinal relaxation time (T1) (D'Arceuil et al., 2007; Wedeen et al., 2008). The brains were transferred from the Gd-DTPA– doped PBS into a sealable container built with a plexiglass tube (60 mm i.d.) for scanning. The container was filled with liquid Fomblin (Solvay Solexis) to match closely the susceptibility of the brain tissue. A custom-made brain mold held the brain securely in space, with the anterior and posterior commissure aligned to the bore (Figure 1C).
Diffusion MRI data acquisition
Request a detailed protocolA 7-Tesla Biospec 70/30 USR Avance III MRI scanner (Bruker BioSpin) equipped with 20 i.d. gradients with a strength of 450 mT/m and a rise time of 100 μs was used for imaging. Acquisition parameters were set as in Thomas et al. (2014). In brief, diffusion data were acquired with a 3D spin-echo diffusion-weighted EPI sequence. b-value was set at 4,025 s/mm2 (Thomas et al., 2014), which has been demonstrated to be sufficient to model multiple fiber populations in ex vivo specimens (Dyrby et al., 2007). Two diffusion-weighting gradient tables of 60 directions each was used, and ten additional image volumes were collected with b = 0 s/mm2. Echo time was reduced to TE = 36.5 ms by acquiring the EPI train in 16 segments, and the repeat time was TR = 500 ms. The 3D image matrix size was 320 × 320 × 256, resulting in a near-isotropic spatial resolution of 250 × 250 × 254 μm. The entire DWI acquisition took ∼74 hr. We also acquired two phase maps and two sets of reversed gradient EPI images with b = 0 s/mm2, one each before and one after the main DTI scan. Reversed gradient were used for distortion correction.
Preprocessing, distortion correction, and Tissue Segmentation
Request a detailed protocolThe diffusion weighted images were corrected for motion, pulsation and eddy-current artifacts before image processing using TORTOISE (Pierpaoli et al., 2010), despite the amount of misregistration in the data was negligible on visual inspection. More specifically, we performed a two-step correction: DIFF_PREP - software by using a structural image as target, performed image resampling, motion, eddy current distortion, and EPI distortion correction, while DR-BUDDI- software by using pairs of diffusion data sets acquired with opposite phase encoding (blip-up blip-down acquisitions), performed additional EPI distortion correction. Images were additionally denoised by means of non-local mean algorithm (sigma = 0; dipy toolbox, https://github.com/nipy/dipy). Before applying statistical evaluation, voxels with extremely low signal were removed.
Functional and anatomical MRI: Tissue segmentation
Request a detailed protocolAttentional and visual regions of interests (ROIs) were defined functionally and anatomically. Two of the monkeys (M1 and M2) previously underwent whole brain functional imaging while performing a demanding attentional task. Experimental procedures are described in details elsewhere (Stemmann and Freiwald, 2016). In brief, the animals were cued to attend either the left or the right hemifield. They had to detect and discriminate the direction of motion of random dot stimuli at the cued location while ignoring a similar stimulus displayed in the opposite hemifield by making a saccade towards one of eight possible targets. Attentional ROIs were defined by contrasting blocks where the monkeys attended the ipsilateral hemifield versus blocks where the monkeys attended the contralateral hemifield (Figure 1A–B). The two attentional conditions were dissociated from saccade planning, since saccades to any of the eight different targets were generated equally frequently in the two blocks. Additional ROIs were defined by five localizer experiments, including retinotopic, motion and saccade mapping (Supplementary file 1). Functional ROIs maps used for tracking were created by projecting the volumetric functional ROIs at the intersection between gray and white matter on a T1 image (Figure 1—figure supplement 1 and see also below).
To compare functional tracts with standard anatomical fascicles we defined inclusion and exclusion waypoints ROIs (Figure 4—figure supplement 1). The waypoint ROIs were defined in locations that isolate the central portion of the tract where the fibers are coherently bundled together and before they begin diverging towards cortex. Specifically, we defined the second and third branch of Superior Longitudinal Fascicle (SLFII and SLFIII) as in Thiebaut de Schotten et al. (2011), the Inferior Longitudinal Fasciculus (ILF), the Extreme Capsule (EmC) as in Mars et al. (2016), the Arcuate Fasciculus (AF) as in Eichert et al. (2018), the vertical branch of the ILF (vILF) as in Schmahmann and Pandya (2006) and, the Ventral Occipital Fasciculus (VOF) as in Takemura et al., 2017. As an additional control, the VOF was defined also through ROI-to-ROI tracking. A previous study showed that most of the endpoints of the VOF were in V4d and V4v (Takemura et al., 2017). We functionally defined V4 through retinotopic mapping (Supplementary file 1; see also Stemmann and Freiwald, 2016), and used V4d and V4v as seed regions (Figure 1—figure supplement 1; Figure 4—figure supplement 3).
To compare the connectivity profile of PITd with that of nearby areas, we functionally and anatomically parceled areas nearby PITd and analyzed the connectivity of those regions with LIP and FEF. More specifically, we defined MST, MT, and FST by means of a motion localizer, V4t by means of attentional modulation, PL by using a face localizer, and TEa anatomically (Figure 4—figure supplement 4A,D; see also Stemmann and Freiwald, 2016). Then, we tracked the connections between these areas and functionally defined target attention areas LIP and FEF (see below).
The white and gray matter border was defined using a B0 image. An initial segmentation was performed using an automated procedure in MRtrix (erode/threshold, [Tournier et al., 2012] and refined manually [Yushkevich et al., 2006]; http://www.itksnap.org/pmwiki/pmwiki.php).
Functional ROIs maps used for tracking were created by projecting the volumetric functional ROIs at the intersection between gray and white matter on a T1 image (Figure 1—figure supplement 1). ROIs and the T1 were then aligned to the high-resolution B0 diffusion image (Figure 2A) using the Advanced Normalization Toolbox (Ants, Avants et al., 2011).ROI projections were then smoothed and inflated to find their intersection with white matter tracts (FSL, Jenkinson et al., 2012). This method has been previously used in human (Pestilli et al., 2014; Takemura et al., 2016b; Yeatman et al., 2014). Attentional and visual ROIs were functionally mapped in two animals (M1 and M2) and directly used for tracking. Since M1 and M2 underwent both fMRI and electrophysiology investigations, M3 and M4 diffusion imaging data were acquired to control for the possibility that electrophysiological recordings near the functional ROIs of the two mapped brains may have introduced discontinuities in the white matter. For M3 and M4, attentional and visual ROIs from monkeys M1 and M2 were warped to the ex-vivo brains ROIs and the B0 of M1 and M2 were aligned to the high-resolution B0 diffusion image of M3 and M4 using the Advanced Normalization Toolbox (Ants). Both sets of ROIs (from M1 and M2) were tested and gave consistent results (Figure 3—figure supplement 3). Results mostly report data from M1 ROIs.
Diffusion MRI data analyses
Request a detailed protocolOur experimental approach was specifically designed to facilitate the comparison of dMRI with classical tracer studies and overcome some of the limitations of dMRI and tractography. We acquired data ex-vivo to eliminate experimental confounds and artifacts such as motion, noise, cardiac pulsation, and EPI distortion. We acquired high angular and spatial resolution dMRI, which –along with our relatively small ROIs - reduced the inherent coarseness of dMRI tractography (Thomas et al., 2014). We used a fine grained white matter mask to reduce the potential of tractography to introduce fiber continuity where there is none (Basser and Pajevic, 2000). We used probabilistic CSD tractography (MRtrix 0.2 - RRID:SCR_006971 (Tournier et al., 2012), see below) to improve the robustness of the tractography results, especially to crossing fibers (Tournier et al., 2012). We used ensemble tractography, that is a combination of candidate streamlines obtained by systematically varying tracking parameters (Takemura et al., 2016a; see also below) to be able to describe tracts that differ in length and curvature, to improve anatomical accuracy, and to reduce variations in the extent and strength of derived pathways, shown to be dependent upon parameters of the tractography algorithm (Jones, 2010; Thomas et al., 2014). We tracked each bundle multiple times to have an estimate of variability of the probabilistic procedure (Figure 3—figure supplement 2A). As a final step, we used a model-based approach to the selection of fibers which removes most false positive tracts (Pestilli et al., 2014). In brief, we estimated how much each fascicle in the candidate connectome contributes towards predicting the diffusion signal and used a virtual lesion method (Honey and Sporns, 2008; Pestilli et al., 2014) to characterize the strength of evidence supporting the fascicles of interest (Earth Mover’s Distance, EMD [Pestilli et al., 2014; Rubner et al., 2000]; see also below).
Tractography
Request a detailed protocolTracking of potential streamlines was performed using MRtrix 0.2 (Tournier et al., 2012). The white matter volume was used as seed region for connectome generation. The white matter volume and two ROIs of interests were used as seed region for single tracts of interest. We used constrained spherical deconvolution (CSD; Tournier et al., 2007) and probabilistic ensemble tractography (Takemura et al., 2016a) to reduce the possibility that tractography estimates would miss a real fascicle. Specifically, we used four curvature thresholds (minimum radius of curvature 1, 2, 3, 4) and four values of maximum number of harmonics (Lmax = 2, 4, 6, 8). We set other parameters as: step size 0.2 mm; maximum length 200 mm; minimum length 5 mm. For each tract and connectome, we generated 200,000 streamlines. We then merged the tracts and the connectomes.
We used two ROI-based methods to track: ROI-to-ROI and waypoints tracking. For each tract, the set of potential streamlines was refined by removing outliers on the basis of length and distance from the core portion of the tract (Yeatman et al., 2013). For each tract, the set of potential streamlines was refined by removing outliers. Specifically, for tracts defined with cortical ROIs we removed streamlines with length ≥4 SD longer than the mean streamline length in the tract, and position ≥4 SD away from the mean position of the tract. For tracts defined with waypoint ROIs we removed streamlines with length ≥3 SD longer than the mean streamline length in the tract, and position ≥3.5 SD away from the mean position of the tract (Yeatman et al., 2012).
To evaluate the statistical evidence supporting the existence of the fascicle, we used a Linear Fascicle Evaluation method (LiFE, Pestilli et al., 2014). We estimated how much each fascicle in the candidate connectome contributes towards predicting the diffusion signal and assigned a weight to each streamline. We then eliminated fascicles with zero weight to create an optimized connectome and optimized tracts. We used a virtual lesion method (Honey and Sporns, 2008; Pestilli et al., 2014) to characterize the strength of evidence supporting the fascicles of interest. We generated ‘lesioned’ connectome model by excluding the fascicle from the optimized connectome. We compared the prediction accuracy of lesioned model with that of optimized connectome (‘unlesioned’ model). We used the Earth Mover’s Distance (EMD; Pestilli et al., 2014; Rubner et al., 2000) to estimate the strength of evidence.
To get a quantitative description of the tracts of interest we measured the number of streamlines, average tract length, tract volume (number of voxels touched by a streamline * voxel volume) and tract density (2 * number of streamlines / (number of voxels in both ROIs * voxel volume)). To measure how difficult the tracking process was for the different anatomical tracts being compared, we calculated tract density (Figure 4D,H,N), a measure that takes into account the number of streamlines obtained and corrects it for the ROI size. To quantify the similarity between attentional tracts and anatomical pathways, and therefore test our alternative hypotheses, we calculated the percentage of tract overlap as overlapping volume in the two tracts (unique streamline coordinates) divided by the total volume of the attention tract. To analyze the connectivity fingerprint of PITd and nearby functionally defined areas, we calculated the percentage of overlap of each single tract with traditional anatomical pathways (Figure 4—figure supplement 4, panels B,C,E,F). Tracts were overlaid on a non-diffusion weighted (b = 0) image and directly compared with matching anatomical slices obtained through autoradiography studies (Schmahmann and Pandya, 2006).
Data availability
All data generated or analysed during this study are included in the manuscript and supporting files. Source data for Figures 2, 3, 4, 5, have been made available via the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/8ks5t/).
-
Open Science FrameworkFunctionally defined white matter of the macaque monkey brain reveals a dorso-ventral attention network - Data.https://doi.org/10.17605/osf.io/8ks5t
References
-
Organization of afferent input to subdivisions of area 8 in the rhesus monkeyThe Journal of Comparative Neurology 200:407–431.https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.902000309
-
Anatomic organization of basoventral and mediodorsal visual recipient prefrontal regions in the rhesus monkeyThe Journal of Comparative Neurology 276:313–342.https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.902760302
-
Statistical artifacts in diffusion tensor MRI (DT-MRI) caused by background noiseMagnetic Resonance in Medicine 44:41–50.https://doi.org/10.1002/1522-2594(200007)44:1<41::AID-MRM8>3.0.CO;2-O
-
Attention, intention, and priority in the parietal lobeAnnual Review of Neuroscience 33:1–21.https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-060909-152823
-
Visual receptive field organization and cortico-cortical connections of the lateral intraparietal area (area LIP) in the macaqueThe Journal of Comparative Neurology 299:421–445.https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.902990404
-
Functional streams in occipito-frontal connections in the monkeyBehavioural Brain Research 76:89–97.https://doi.org/10.1016/0166-4328(95)00182-4
-
Multidimensional encoding of brain connectomesScientific Reports 7:11491.https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-09250-w
-
Visual attention: the past 25 yearsVision Research 51:1484–1525.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2011.04.012
-
A nexus model of the temporal-parietal junctionTrends in Cognitive Sciences 17:328–336.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.05.007
-
Covert shifts of spatial attention in the macaque monkeyJournal of Neuroscience 35:7695–7714.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4383-14.2015
-
A model for social communication and language evolution and development (SCALED)Current Opinion in Neurobiology 28:165–171.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2014.07.018
-
Real-time multi-peak tractography for instantaneous connectivity displayFrontiers in Neuroinformatics 8:59.https://doi.org/10.3389/fninf.2014.00059
-
Neural basis of visual selective attentionWiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science 2:392–407.https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.117
-
White matter microstructure of attentional networks predicts attention and consciousness functional interactionsBrain Structure and Function 223:653–668.https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-017-1511-2
-
Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brainNature Reviews Neuroscience 3:201–215.https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn755
-
Cortical connections of inferior temporal area TEO in macaque monkeysThe Journal of Comparative Neurology 334:125–150.https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.903340111
-
Salience, relevance, and firing: a priority map for target selectionTrends in Cognitive Sciences 10:382–390.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.06.011
-
White matter in learning, cognition and psychiatric disordersTrends in Neurosciences 31:361–370.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2008.04.001
-
The language networkCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology 23:250–254.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2012.10.002
-
Re-evaluating the role of TPJ in attentional control: contextual updating?Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 37:2608–2620.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.08.010
-
Dynamical consequences of lesions in cortical networksHuman Brain Mapping 29:802–809.https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20579
-
Measuring macroscopic brain connections in vivoNature Neuroscience 18:1546–1555.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4134
-
Precision and accuracy in diffusion tensor magnetic resonance imagingTopics in Magnetic Resonance Imaging 21:87–99.https://doi.org/10.1097/RMR.0b013e31821e56ac
-
New insights into the functions of the superior temporal cortexNature Reviews Neuroscience 2:568–576.https://doi.org/10.1038/35086057
-
Mechanisms of visual attention in the human cortexAnnual Review of Neuroscience 23:315–341.https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.315
-
The retinotopic organization of the human middle temporal area MT/V5 and its cortical neighborsJournal of Neuroscience 30:9801–9820.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2069-10.2010
-
The ventral visual pathway: an expanded neural framework for the processing of object qualityTrends in Cognitive Sciences 17:26–49.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.10.011
-
The optimal trackability threshold of fractional anisotropy for diffusion tensor tractography of the corticospinal tractMagnetic Resonance in Medical Sciences 3:11–17.https://doi.org/10.2463/mrms.3.11
-
Attention and spatial cognition: Neural and anatomical substrates of visual neglectAnnals of Physical and Rehabilitation Medicine 60:124–129.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rehab.2016.01.004
-
The extreme capsule fiber complex in humans and macaque monkeys: a comparative diffusion MRI tractography studyBrain Structure and Function 221:4059–4071.https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-015-1146-0
-
BookThe Visual Brain in ActionUKMilner A. D, Perrett D. I, Johnston R. S, Benson R. S, Jordan P. J, editors. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
-
How do the two visual streams interact with each other?Experimental Brain Research 235:1297–1308.https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-017-4917-4
-
Action observation circuits in the macaque monkey cortexJournal of Neuroscience 31:3743–3756.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4803-10.2011
-
Influence of user-defined parameters on diffusion tensor tractography of the corticospinal tractThe Neuroradiology Journal 20:139–147.https://doi.org/10.1177/197140090702000202
-
Evaluation and statistical inference for human connectomesNature Methods 11:1058–1063.https://doi.org/10.1038/nmeth.3098
-
Human white matter and knowledge representationPLOS Biology 16:e2005758.https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.2005758
-
Association fiber pathways to the frontal cortex from the superior temporal region in the rhesus monkeyThe Journal of Comparative Neurology 273:52–66.https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.902730106
-
Efferent association pathways originating in the caudal prefrontal cortex in the macaque monkeyThe Journal of Comparative Neurology 498:227–251.https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.21048
-
ConferenceTORTOISE: an integrated software package for processing of diffusion MRI dataISMRM 18th Annual Meeting.
-
Beyond neuroanatomy: novel approaches to studying brain evolutionBrain, Behavior and Evolution 72:145–158.https://doi.org/10.1159/000151474
-
The earth mover’s distance as a metric for image retrievalInternational Journal of Computer Vision 40:99–121.
-
BookA Combined MRI and Histology Atlas of the Rhesus Monkey Brain Instereotaxic CoordinatesLondon: Academic.
-
Effects of superior temporal polysensory area lesions on eye movements in the macaque monkeyJournal of Neurophysiology 73:1–19.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1995.73.1.1
-
Attentive motion discrimination recruits an area in inferotemporal cortexThe Journal of Neuroscience 36:11918–11928.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1888-16.2016
-
Advanced database methodology for the collation of connectivity data on the macaque brain (CoCoMac)Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences 356:1159–1186.https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2001.0908
-
Ensemble tractographyPLOS Computational Biology 12:e1004692.https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004692
-
A major human white matter pathway between dorsal and ventral visual cortexCerebral Cortex 26:2205–2214.https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhv064
-
Occipital white matter tracts in human and macaqueCerebral Cortex 27:3346–3359.https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhx070
-
Fractional anisotropy--threshold dependence in tract-based diffusion tensor analysis: evaluation of the uncinate fasciculus in Alzheimer diseaseAmerican Journal of Neuroradiology 30:1700–1703.https://doi.org/10.3174/ajnr.A1698
-
Shape representations in the primate dorsal visual streamFrontiers in Computational Neuroscience 9:43.https://doi.org/10.3389/fncom.2015.00043
-
A lateralized brain network for visuospatial attentionNature Neuroscience 14:1245–1246.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2905
-
Learning to read improves the structure of the arcuate fasciculusCerebral Cortex 24:989–995.https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhs383
-
Diffusion imaging, white matter, and psychopathologyAnnual Review of Clinical Psychology 7:63–85.https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032210-104507
-
A visual salience map in the primate frontal eye fieldProgress in Brain Research 147:251–262.https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(04)47019-8
-
MRtrix: Diffusion tractography in crossing fiber regionsInternational Journal of Imaging Systems and Technology 22:53–66.https://doi.org/10.1002/ima.22005
-
A feature-integration theory of attentionCognitive Psychology 12:97–136.https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(80)90005-5
-
Clarifying human white matterAnnual Review of Neuroscience 39:103–128.https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-070815-013815
-
Posterior neocortical systems subserving awareness and neglect. Neglect associated with superior temporal sulcus but not area 7 lesionsArchives of Neurology 51:1014–1021.
-
Diffusion tensor imaging reveals evolution of primate brain architecturesBrain Structure and Function 218:1429–1450.https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-012-0468-4
Article and author information
Author details
Funding
Leon Levy Foundation
- Ilaria Sani
New York Stem Cell Foundation
- Winrich A Freiwald
National Science Foundation (BCS-1057006)
- Winrich A Freiwald
National Institute of Mental Health (ULTTR001108)
- Franco Pestilli
Indiana Clinical and Translational Sciences Institute (Passthrough)
- Franco Pestilli
Microsoft Research (Azure Credits Award)
- Franco Pestilli
Indiana University (Areas of Emergent Research initiative Learning: Brains-Machines-Children)
- Franco Pestilli
National Institutes of Health (1U54MH091657)
- Franco Pestilli
National Science Foundation (IIS-1636893)
- Franco Pestilli
National Science Foundation (BCS-1734853)
- Franco Pestilli
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Acknowledgements
We thank Eric Aronowitz and Henning U Voss for setting up diffusion MRI sequences; Margaret Fabiszak for help with sample preparation; James M Petrillo for his help in designing and fabricating the custom-made brain mold; Yifat Prut, Sofia Landi, Farid Aboharb for comments on a previous version of the manuscript. The authors acknowledge the Indiana University Pervasive Technology Institute for providing HPC resources that have contributed to the research results reported within this paper (URL: https://pti.iu.edu/). This work was supported by Leon Levy Fellowship to IS. The New York Stem Cell Foundation and, the NSF BCS-1057006 to WAF; NSF IIS-1636893, NSF BCS-1734853, NIH NIMH ULTTR001108, Indiana Clinical and Translational Institute (Passthrough), Microsoft Research Award - grant number Azure Credits Award, the Indiana University -grant number Areas of Emergent Research initiative ‘Learning: Brains, Machines, Children’, NIH 1U54MH091657, and Pervasive Technology Institute to FP. WAF is a New York Stem Cell Foundation - Robertson Investigator. This paper’s content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NIH or the National Science Foundation.
Ethics
Animal experimentation: Our Protocols protocol has been approved by The Rockefeller University, Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. In vivo imaging procedures were performed at the Center for Advanced Imaging of Bremen University. They conformed to the National Institutes of Health Guide for Use and Care of Laboratory Animals, regulations for the welfare of experimental animals issued by the federal government of Germany, and stipulations of local Bremen authorities
Copyright
© 2019, Sani et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 3,354
- views
-
- 505
- downloads
-
- 46
- citations
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Neuroscience
Sour taste, which is elicited by low pH, may serve to help animals distinguish appetitive from potentially harmful food sources. In all species studied to date, the attractiveness of oral acids is contingent on concentration. Many carboxylic acids are attractive at ecologically relevant concentrations but become aversive beyond some maximal concentration. Recent work found that Drosophila ionotropic receptors IR25a and IR76b expressed by sweet-responsive gustatory receptor neurons (GRNs) in the labellum, a peripheral gustatory organ, mediate appetitive feeding behaviors toward dilute carboxylic acids. Here, we disclose the existence of pharyngeal sensors in Drosophila melanogaster that detect ingested carboxylic acids and are also involved in the appetitive responses to carboxylic acids. These pharyngeal sensors rely on IR51b, IR94a, and IR94h, together with IR25a and IR76b, to drive responses to carboxylic acids. We then demonstrate that optogenetic activation of either Ir94a+ or Ir94h+ GRNs promotes an appetitive feeding response, confirming their contributions to appetitive feeding behavior. Our discovery of internal pharyngeal sour taste receptors opens up new avenues for investigating the internal sensation of tastants in insects.
-
- Neuroscience
Time estimation is an essential prerequisite underlying various cognitive functions. Previous studies identified ‘sequential firing’ and ‘activity ramps’ as the primary neuron activity patterns in the medial frontal cortex (mPFC) that could convey information regarding time. However, the relationship between these patterns and the timing behavior has not been fully understood. In this study, we utilized in vivo calcium imaging of mPFC in rats performing a timing task. We observed cells that showed selective activation at trial start, end, or during the timing interval. By aligning long-term time-lapse datasets, we discovered that sequential patterns of time coding were stable over weeks, while cells coding for trial start or end showed constant dynamism. Furthermore, with a novel behavior design that allowed the animal to determine individual trial interval, we were able to demonstrate that real-time adjustment in the sequence procession speed closely tracked the trial-to-trial interval variations. And errors in the rats’ timing behavior can be primarily attributed to the premature ending of the time sequence. Together, our data suggest that sequential activity maybe a stable neural substrate that represents time under physiological conditions. Furthermore, our results imply the existence of a unique cell type in the mPFC that participates in the time-related sequences. Future characterization of this cell type could provide important insights in the neural mechanism of timing and related cognitive functions.