Inference of nonlinear receptive field subunits with spike-triggered clustering
Abstract
Responses of sensory neurons are often modeled using a weighted combination of rectified linear subunits. Since these subunits often cannot be measured directly, a flexible method is needed to infer their properties from the responses of downstream neurons. We present a method for maximum likelihood estimation of subunits by soft-clustering spike-triggered stimuli, and demonstrate its effectiveness in visual neurons. For parasol retinal ganglion cells in macaque retina, estimated subunits partitioned the receptive field into compact regions, likely representing aggregated bipolar cell inputs. Joint clustering revealed shared subunits between neighboring cells, producing a parsimonious population model. Closed-loop validation, using stimuli lying in the null space of the linear receptive field, revealed stronger nonlinearities in OFF cells than ON cells. Responses to natural images, jittered to emulate fixational eye movements, were accurately predicted by the subunit model. Finally, the generality of the approach was demonstrated in macaque V1 neurons.
Introduction
The functional properties of sensory neurons are often probed by presenting stimuli and then inferring the rules by which inputs from presynaptic neurons are combined to produce receptive field selectivity. The most common simplifying assumption in these models is that the combination rule is linear, but it is well-known that many aspects of neural response are highly nonlinear. For example, retinal ganglion cells (RGCs) are known to be driven by nonlinear 'subunits' (Hochstein and Shapley, 1976), which reflect signals from presynaptic bipolar cells that are rectified at the synapse onto RGCs (Demb et al., 1999; Demb et al., 2001; Borghuis et al., 2013). This subunit computation is fundamentally nonlinear because hyperpolarization of one bipolar cell input does not cancel depolarization of another. The subunit architecture endows RGCs with sensitivity to finer spatial detail than would arise from a linear receptive field (Hochstein and Shapley, 1976; Demb et al., 2001;; Baccus et al., 2008; Crook et al., 2008; Schwartz et al., 2012) and has been implicated in the processing of visual features like object motion and looming (Olveczky et al., 2007; Münch et al., 2009). As another example, complex cells in primary visual cortex are thought to perform subunit computations on simple cell inputs, producing invariance to the spatial location of a stimulus. Indeed, subunits appear to be a common computational motif in the brain, and inferring their functional properties from neural recordings requires the development of effective estimation methods.
To this end, simple techniques have been used to reveal the presence and typical sizes of subunits (Hochstein and Shapley, 1976), and more elaborate techniques have been used in specific experimental settings (Emerson et al., 1992; Paninski, 2003; Sharpee et al., 2004; Rust et al., 2005; Schwartz et al., 2006; Pillow and Simoncelli, 2006; Rajan and Bialek, 2013; Park et al., 2013; Theis et al., 2013; Kaardal et al., 2013; Freeman et al., 2015; Liu et al., 2017; Maheswaranathan et al., 2018; Shi et al., 2019). However, no widely accepted general computational tools exist for inferring the structure of nonlinear subunits providing inputs to a neuron, such as their individual sizes, shapes, and spatial arrangement. Such a tool must be robust, efficient, and applicable, possibly with simple modifications, in a variety of different experimental contexts.
Here, we present a novel method for estimating the properties of subunits providing inputs to a recorded neuron. The method was derived as a maximum likelihood procedure for estimating subunit model parameters and took the form of a spike-triggered clustering algorithm. As such, it provided a natural generalization of spike-triggered averaging, which is widely used to estimate linear receptive fields because of its conceptual simplicity, robustness, and straightforward interpretation. In populations of recorded parasol RGCs in macaque retina, the new method revealed a gradual partitioning of receptive fields with a hierarchical organization of spatially localized and regularly spaced subunits, consistent with the input from a mosaic of bipolar cells, and generalized naturally and parsimoniously to a population of cells. Unlike other approaches, the technique optimized a simple and explicit model of neural response, required no assumptions about the spatio-temporal properties of subunits, and was validated on an extensive data set. The structure of the model permitted prior information, such as spatial locality, to be incorporated naturally in cases of limited data. A novel closed-loop 'null' stimulus revealed that the subunit model was substantially more accurate than a linear model in explaining RGC responses, a finding that also extended to natural stimuli. Responses to null stimuli were stronger in OFF parasol cells than ON parasol cells, consistent with their stronger nonlinear responses (Demb et al., 2001; Chichilnisky and Kalmar, 2002; Turner and Rieke, 2016). Application of the estimation method to complex cells in primary visual cortex revealed subunits with expected spatiotemporal structure, demonstrating that the approach generalizes to other experimental contexts and neural circuits.
Results
The goal is to develop a method to estimate the nonlinear subunits of recorded neurons, examine the spatial and temporal properties of the estimated subunits, and verify their predictions for a wide range of stimuli.
Subunit response model and parameter estimation
The method is developed as an optimal estimation procedure for a generic subunit model in which responses are described as an alternating cascade of two linear-nonlinear (LN) stages (Figure 1A). In the first LN stage, visual inputs over space and recent time are weighted and summed by linear subunit filters, followed by an exponential nonlinearity. In the second LN stage, the subunit outputs are weighted by non-negative scalars, summed, and passed through a final output nonlinearity. This yields the neuron's firing rate, which drives a Poisson spike generator.
To predict neural responses using the model, the model parameters (subunits, weights, and nonlinearity) must be estimated from recorded data. The estimation problem is difficult because the negative log-likelihood is not a convex function of the parameters. An efficient solution is presented with two steps (see Materials and methods): (1) obtain a maximum likelihood estimate of the subunits by identifying clusters in the space of spike-triggered stimuli (Figure 1B), (2) estimate the parameters of the output nonlinearity using standard optimization methods. This spike-triggered clustering solution offers a simple and robust generalization of the commonly used spike-triggered averaging method for receptive field estimation (see Chichilnisky, 2001). Intuitively, clustering is effective because stimuli that produce spikes will typically fall into distinct categories based on which subunit(s) they activate. Simulations with a RGC model show that this procedure yields accurate estimates of model parameters when sufficient data are available (Figure 1—figure supplement 1).
The remaining model parameter, the number of subunits, is discrete and is therefore typically selected by evaluating model predictions on held-out data (i.e. cross-validation) (Figure 2C). However, in some cases, it is desirable to either compare the results obtained with different numbers of estimated subunits (in order to study the strongest sources of nonlinearity; Figure 2A,B, 4, 5–8), or to use a fixed number of subunits (to compare variants of the algorithm; Figure 3).
In what follows, results are presented using the core estimation approach, followed by several modifications appropriate for analyzing data from different experimental conditions. Specifically, consider two situations with data limitations: short recording durations, and biased stimulus statistics. For short durations, the core method is modified to include regularization that assumes localized spatial structure of subunits (see Materials and methods, Figures 3,4). For biased stimuli (e.g. natural scenes), subunit filters are estimated using responses from white noise, then the output nonlinearities are fine-tuned (Figures 6,7).
Estimated subunits emerge hierarchically and are spatially localized and non-overlapping
To test the subunit model and estimation procedure on primate RGCs, light responses were obtained using large-scale multielectrode recordings from isolated macaque retina (Chichilnisky and Kalmar, 2002; Litke et al., 2004; Frechette et al., 2005). Spiking responses of hundreds of RGCs to a spatiotemporal white noise stimulus were used to classify distinct cell types, by examining the properties of the spike-triggered average (STA) stimulus (Frechette et al., 2005; Field et al., 2007). Complete populations of ON and OFF parasol RGCs covering the recorded region were examined.
Fitting a subunit model reliably to these data was aided by decoupling the spatial and temporal properties of subunits. Specifically, although the fitting approach in principle permits estimation of full spatiotemporal subunit filters, the high dimensionality of these filters requires substantial data (and thus, long recordings). The dimension of the parameter space was reduced by assuming that subunit responses are spatio-temporally separable, and that all subunits have the same temporal filter (consistent with known properties of retinal bipolar cells that are thought to form the RGC subunits, see Enroth-Cugell et al., 1983 and Cowan et al., 2016). The common temporal filter was estimated from the STA (see Materials and methods). The stimulus was then convolved with this time course to produce an instantaneous spatial stimulus associated with each spike time. This temporally prefiltered spatial stimulus was then used to fit a model with purely spatial subunits.
The core spike triggered clustering method (no regularization) was applied to study the spatial properties of subunits with increasing numbers of subunits (N). As expected, setting N = 1 yielded a single subunit with a receptive field essentially identical to the STA, i.e. the spatial filter of a LN model. Interestingly, a model with two subunits partitioned this receptive field into spatially localized regions (Figure 2A, second row). Fitting the model with additional subunits (Figure 2A, subsequent rows) typically caused one of the subunits from the preceding fit to be partitioned further, while other subunits were largely unchanged. In principle this procedure could be repeated many times to capture all subunits in the receptive field. However, the number of model parameters increases with N, and thus the estimation accuracy decreases, with estimated subunits becoming noisier and exhibiting larger overlap (Figure 2A, last row). The partitioning observation suggests the possibility of an estimation approach in which the hierarchical introduction of subunits is built in, with the potential for greater efficiency (see Materials and methods). Explicitly incorporating the hierarchical assumption produces a similar collection of subunits at each level (Figure 2—figure supplement 1), reinforcing the idea of subunit partitioning as the number of subunits increases. However, for clarity and brevity, the hierarchical estimation approach is not used for the remainder of this paper.
An optimal number of subunits was chosen for each cell to maximize the cross-validated likelihood (i.e. the likelihood measured on a held out test set - Figure 2B). The typical optimum for OFF parasol cells in the recorded data was 4–6 subunits (Figure 2C). This optimal value is governed by the true number of subunits, the resolution of the stimulus (which determines the dimensionality of the parameter space, see Figure 3), and the amount of data (i.e. number of spikes). Since the ON parasol cells had a smaller optimum number of subunits (one or two subunits provided the optimum fit for 48% of ON parasol cells, while this held for only 3% of OFF parasol cells) and exhibited much smaller increases in likelihood of the subunit model compared to a LN model (not shown), subsequent analysis focuses only on the OFF parasol cells. Note, however, that a model with multiple subunits frequently explained ON parasol data more accurately than a model with one subunit (three or more subunits for 52% of cells), implying that ON parasol cells do have spatial nonlinearities.
The estimated subunits were larger and fewer in number than expected from the size and number of bipolar cells providing input to parasol RGCs at the eccentricities recorded (Jacoby et al., 2000; Schwartz and Rieke, 2011; Tsukamoto and Omi, 2015). However, two other features of estimated subunits suggested a relationship to the collection of underlying bipolar cells contributing to the receptive field. First, estimated subunits were compact in space as shown in Figure 2A. This was quantified by comparing each subunit with the collection of subunits derived by randomly permuting the filter values at each pixel location across different subunits of the same cell. Spatial locality for estimated and permuted subunits was then evaluated by the mean-squared error (MSE) of 2D Gaussian fits. Compared to the permuted subunits, the estimated subunits had substantially lower MSE (Figure 2D,E). Second, the subunits 'tiled' the RGC receptive field, in that they covered it with minimal gaps and modest overlap. This was quantified by noting that on average, the neighboring subunits for a given cell were separated by ∼ 1.5 times their width, with relatively little variation (Figure 2F).
Given these observations, a natural interpretation of subunits observed with coarse stimulation is that they correspond to aggregates of neighboring bipolar cells. To test this intuition, the algorithm was applied to white noise responses generated from a simulated RGC with a realistic size and number of subunits in the receptive field (Jacoby et al., 2000; Schwartz and Rieke, 2011). For data simulated using coarse pixelated stimuli matched to those used in the experiments, the estimated subunits were fewer and larger than the bipolar cells, with each subunit representing an aggregate of several bipolar cells (Figure 1—figure supplement 1). The recovered subunits also exhibited spatial locality and tiling, as in the data.
Regularization for spatially localized subunit estimation
To estimate subunits with a resolution approaching that of the underlying bipolar cell lattice would require much higher resolution stimuli. But higher resolution stimuli typically require more data. Specifically, to obtain a constant quality estimate of subunits as the stimulus resolution/dimensionality is increased requires a proportional increase in the number of spikes (e.g. see Dayan and Abbott, 2001). Furthermore, higher resolution stimuli typically produce lower firing rates, because the effective stimulus contrast is lower. To illustrate the problem, the impact of increased resolution and smaller duration of data were examined separately. Higher resolution stimuli led to worse subunit estimates, even when all the recorded data (97 min) were used (Figure 3A, top row). Specifically, of the five estimated subunits, one resembled the receptive field, and the others were apparently dominated by noise and had low weights; thus, the algorithm effectively estimated a LN model. For coarse resolution stimuli, using only 10% of the recorded data led to noisy subunit estimates (Figure 3B, top row) compared to the estimates obtained with 81% of the data (24 min, Figure 3B, last row). These observations suggest that it would be useful to modify the core clustering algorithm by incorporating prior knowledge about subunit structure, and thus potentially obtain more accurate estimation with limited data.
To accomplish this, a regularizer was introduced to encourage the spatial locality of estimated subunits (Figure 2). Previous studies (Liu et al., 2017; Maheswaranathan et al., 2018; McFarland et al., 2013) have used L1 regularization (a common means of inducing sparsity), but L1 is agnostic to spatial locality, and indeed is invariant to spatial rearrangement. Thus, a novel locally normalized L1 (LNL1) regularizer was developed that penalizes large weights, but only if all of the neighboring weights are small: for , a value smaller than typical non-zero weights which are on the order of unity. This penalty is designed to have a relatively modest influence on subunit size compared to L1, which induces a strong preference for smaller subunits. The LNL1 penalty was incorporated into the estimation procedure by introducing a proximal operator in the clustering loop (see Materials and methods). In this case, the appropriate projection operator is an extension of the soft-thresholding operator for the L1 regularizer, where the threshold for each pixel depends on the strength of neighboring pixels. The regularization strength is chosen to maximize cross-validated likelihood (see Materials and methods).
Introducing the LNL1 prior improved subunit estimates for both of the limited data situations (high resolution, or small duration) presented above. Since the optimal number of subunits can vary for different regularization methods, the subunits estimated from different regularization methods were compared after fixing the total number of subunits to the most likely optimum across cells (Figure 2). In the case of the coarse resolution data, both L1 and LNL1 priors produced spatially localized subunits (Figure 3B, middle rows) whose locations matched the location of the subunits estimated using more data without regularization (Figure 3B, last row). This suggests that the proposed prior leads to efficient estimates without introducing significant biases. Relative to L1 regularization, LNL1 regularization yielded fewer apparent noise pixels in the background (Figure 3B, middle rows) and a larger optimum number of subunits (not shown). This improvement in spatial locality is reflected in more accurate response prediction with LNL1 regularization (Figure 3C).
In the case of high dimensional stimuli (Figure 3A, top row), both L1 and LNL1 priors were successful in estimating compact subunits (Figure 3A, middle rows) and matched the location of subunits obtained with a coarser stimulus (Figure 3A, bottom row). The estimated subunits tiled the receptive field, as would be expected from an underlying mosaic of bipolar cells. The LNL1 prior also led to subunit estimates with fewer spurious pixels compared to the L1 prior. Note that although these spurious pixels can also be suppressed by using a larger weight on the L1 prior, this also yielded smaller subunit size, which produced less accurate response predictions, while LNL1 had a much smaller effect on estimated subunit size (not shown). Hence, in both the conditions of limited data examined, the novel LNL1 prior yielded spatially localized subunits, fewer spurious pixels, and a net improvement in response prediction accuracy.
Parsimonious modeling of population responses using shared subunits
To fully understand visual processing in retina it is necessary to understand how a population of RGCs coordinate to encode the visual stimulus in their joint activity. The core spike triggered clustering method extends naturally to joint identification of subunits in populations of neurons. Since neighboring OFF parasol cells have partially overlapping receptive fields (Gauthier et al., 2009), and sample a mosaic of bipolar cells, neighboring cells would be expected to receive input from common subunits. Indeed, in mosaics (lattices) of recorded OFF parasol cells, estimated subunits of different cells were frequently closer than the typical nearest-neighbor subunit distances obtained from single-cell data. For example, in one data set, the fraction of subunits from different cells closer than 1SD of a Gaussian fit was 14%, substantially higher than the predicted value of 9% based on the separation of subunits within a cell (Figure 2F). Examination of the mosaic suggests that these closely-spaced subunits correspond to single subunits shared by neighboring RGCs (Figure 4A), that is, that they reflect a shared biological substrate. If so, then joint estimation of subunits from the pooled data of neighboring RGCs may more correctly reveal their collective spatial structure.
This hypothesis was tested by fitting a common 'bank' of subunits to simultaneously predict the responses for multiple nearby cells. Specifically, the firing rate for the neuron was modeled as: . Here is the stimulus, are the filters for the common population of subunits, is the output nonlinearity for the neuron,and is the non-negative weight of the subunit to the neuron, which is 0 if they are not connected. The model parameters were estimated by maximizing the sum of log-likelihood across cells, again in two steps. For the first step, the output nonlinearities were ignored for each cell and a common set of subunit filters was found that clustered the spike-triggered stimuli for all the cells simultaneously. Specifically, on each iteration, a) the relative weights for different subunits were computed for each spike-triggered stimulus and b) the cluster centers were updated using the weighted sum of spike-triggered stimuli across different cells (see Materials and methods). Since the subunit filters could span the receptive fields of all the cells, leading to higher dimensionality, the spatial locality (LNL1) prior was used to efficiently estimate the subunits (Figure 3). In the second step of the algorithm, the non-linearities were then estimated for each cell independently (similar to Figure 1).
To quantify the advantages of joint subunit estimation, the method was applied to a group of four nearby OFF parasol cells. As a baseline, models with different numbers of subunits were estimated for each cell separately, and the combination of 15 subunits that maximized the cross-validated log-likelihood when summed over all four cells was chosen (Figure 4B). Then, a joint model with 12 subunits was fitted. Similar test log-likelihood was observed using the joint model, which effectively reduced model complexity by three subunits (Figure 4E, horizontal line). Examination of the subunit locations revealed that pairs of overlapping subunits from neighboring cells in separate fits were replaced with one shared subunit in the joint model (Figure 4C). Also, a joint model with 15 subunits showed a higher prediction accuracy than a model in which the 15 subunits were estimated separately (Figure 4E, vertical line). In this case, sharing of subunits leads to larger number of subunits per cell (Figure 4D).
This trend was studied for different number of subunits, by comparing the total test negative log-likelihood associated with the joint model (Figure 4E, green) and the best combination of subunits from separate fits to each cell based on validation data (Figure 4E, blue). Joint fitting provided more accurate predictions than separate fits, when using a large total number of subunits (>12). For a smaller number of subunits, a horizontal shift in the observed likelihood curves revealed the reduction in the number of subunits that was obtained with joint estimation, without a reduction in prediction accuracy. In sum, jointly estimating a common collection of subunits for nearby cells resulted in a more parsimonious explanation of population responses.
Subunit model explains spatial nonlinearities revealed by null stimulus
To more directly expose the advantage of a subunit model over commonly used LN models of RGC light response, experiments were performed using a visual stimulus that elicits no response in a LN model (or other model that begins with linear integration of the visual stimulus over space, for example Pillow et al., 2008). An example of such a stimulus is a contrast-reversing grating stimulus centered on the RF (Hochstein and Shapley, 1976; Demb et al., 1999). However, to avoid recruiting additional nonlinearities, a stimulus was developed that has spatio-temporal statistics very similar to the high-entropy white noise stimulus used for model characterization. This 'null' stimulus was obtained by manipulating white noise so as to eliminate responses of the linear RF.
Specifically, the computation performed by a hypothetical LN neuron can be represented in a stimulus space in which each axis represents stimulus contrast at a single pixel location (Figure 5A). In this representation, the LN response is determined by the projection of the stimulus vector onto the direction associated with the weights in the neuron's spatial RF. Thus, a null stimulus orthogonal to the RF can be computed by subtracting a vector with the same RF projection from the original stimulus (Figure 5B). In addition to the orthogonality constraint, two other constraints are imposed in constructing the stimulus: (i) the range of pixel values is bounded for the stimulus display, and (ii) the variance of each pixel over time is held constant (as in white noise), to prevent gain changes in photoreceptors (see Materials and methods). Given these constraints, the resulting stimulus is the closest stimulus to the original white noise stimulus that yields zero response in a LN neuron. Note that although this approach is described purely in the spatial domain, it generalizes to the full spatiotemporal RF, and the procedure based on the spatial RF alone will produce a null stimulus for a spatio-temporal linear filter that is space-time separable (see Materials and methods).
The ability to silence RGC responses with null stimuli was tested as follows. A 10 s movie of white noise was projected into the intersection of the spatial null spaces corresponding to all cells in a region, of a single type (e.g. OFF parasol). Responses were recorded to 30 repeated presentations of both the white noise and the null stimulus. RGC firing rates showed a substantial modulation for both white noise and the null stimulus that was highly reproducible across repeats (Figure 5C,D), inconsistent with linear integration by the RGC over its RF. Note that the null stimulus modulated OFF parasol cells more strongly than ON parasol cells, consistent with previous results obtained with white noise and natural scene stimuli (Demb et al., 2001; Chichilnisky and Kalmar, 2002; Turner and Rieke, 2016); henceforth, analysis is focused on OFF parasol cells.
The subunit model substantially captured response nonlinearities revealed by the null stimulus. Because the biased statistics of the null stimulus induce errors in the estimated subunits, subunit filters were first estimated from responses to white noise (step 1 of model estimation, without regularization, Figure 1), and response nonlinearities were then estimated from responses to the null stimulus (step 2 of model estimation, Figure 1). As expected, the single subunit (LN) model failed to capture responses to the held-out null stimulus (Figure 6A, second row). As the number of subunits was increased, model performance progressively increased (Figure 6A, subsequent rows). Prediction accuracy, defined as the correlation between the recorded and predicted firing rate, was evaluated across entire populations of OFF parasol cells in three recordings (Figure 6B). The single subunit model failed, with prediction accuracy near 0 (mean accuracy : 0.02, 0.12, 0.03 for three retinas). The prediction accuracy gradually increased with the addition of more subunits, saturating at roughly 4–6 subunits (Figure 6B, red). In contrast, the addition of subunits showed marginal improvements for white noise stimuli of the same duration (Figure 6B, black), despite being measurable with longer stimuli (Figure 2B). Thus, the response nonlinearities captured by the subunit model are more efficiently exposed by the null stimulus.
Subunits enhance response prediction for naturalistic stimuli
A fuller understanding of the biological role of spatial nonlinearities requires examining how they influence responses elicited by natural stimuli. For this purpose, segments of images from the Van Hateren database were used, with a new image presented every second to emulate saccadic eye movements. Between simulated saccades, the image was jittered according to the eye movement trajectories recorded during fixation by awake macaque monkeys (ZM Hafed and RJ Krauzlis, personal communication, van Hateren and van der Schaaf, 1998; Heitman et al., 2016). Because the biased statistics of natural stimuli make it infeasible to directly estimate subunits, subunit filters and time courses were first estimated using white noise stimuli (1st step, without regularization Figure 1). Then, the non-linearities and subunit magnitudes were fitted using natural stimuli (2nd step, Figure 1) (see Materials and methods, similar to above). Subsequently, the ability of the subunit model to capture responses to natural stimuli was measured.
Responses to natural stimuli showed a distinct structure, with strong periods of firing or silence immediately following saccades and less frequent firing elicited by image jitter (Heitman et al., 2016, Figure 7A black raster). This structure was progressively better captured by the model as the number of subunits increased, as shown by the rasters for an OFF parasol cell in Figure 7A. This finding was replicated in OFF parasol populations in three retinas, and quantified by the correlation between the recorded and predicted PSTH (Figure 7B). However, some of the firing events could not be captured even with a large number of subunits, suggesting that models with additional mechanisms such as gain control (Heeger, 1992; Carandini and Heeger, 2012) or nonlinear interactions between subunits (Kuo et al., 2016; Manookin et al., 2018) may be required to describe natural scene responses more completely.
Interestingly, further examination revealed that addition of subunits produced greater prediction improvements for responses elicited by image jitter compared to those elicited immediately following simulated saccades (< 250 ms) (Figure 7B). This could be explained by the fact that changes in luminance are usually more spatially uniform at saccades, resulting in more linear signaling by RGCs. Indeed, after temporally filtering the visual stimulus, there were fewer stimulus samples in the null space of the receptive field (90 degrees) during saccades than between saccades (Figure 7C), and the subunit model provided a more accurate explanation of responses to stimuli in the null space (Figure 6). Thus, these results indicate that spatial non-linearities contribute to explaining responses to natural stimuli, especially those elicited by jittering images.
Application to simple and complex cells in primary visual cortex
Identification of subunits using spike-triggered stimulus clustering ideally would generalize to other neural systems with similar cascades of linear and non-linear operations. As a test of this generality, spike triggered clustering (Figure 1) was applied to obtain estimates of space-time subunit filters from responses of V1 simple and complex cells to flickering bars (Rust et al., 2005). The estimated subunits showed oriented spatio-temporal structure similar to that observed with other methods that imposed more restrictions (Rust et al., 2005; Vintch et al., 2015) (because of this similarity, additional priors on the filters were not explored). Increasing the number of subunits typically caused one of the subunits from the preceding fits to be partitioned into two, while other subunits were largely unchanged (Figure 8); that is, a hierarchical structure similar to that observed in RGCs (Figure 2). The number of cross-validated subunits for the complex cell (N = 8) was greater than for the simple cell (N = 4), consistent with previous work (Rust et al., 2005). All subunits for a given cell had similar structure in space and time, but were approximately translated relative to each other, consistent with the similar frequency spectrum (Figure 8A,B, bottom row). For the complex cell, this is consistent with the hypothesis that it receives inputs from multiple simple cells at different locations (Hubel and Wiesel, 1962; Adelson and Bergen, 1985; Vintch et al., 2015). Thus, the method presented here extends to V1 data, producing subunit estimates that are not constrained by assumptions of orthogonality (Rust et al., 2005) or convolutional structure (Vintch et al., 2015), yet broadly resembling the results of previous work (see Discussion and Figure 10 for a detailed comparison).
Comparison to other methods
Comparisons with recent studies reveal advantages of the spike-triggered clustering approach in terms of efficiency of estimation, and interpretation of estimated subunits. A relevant comparison is the recently published subspace-based approach, spike-triggered non-negative matrix factorization (SNMF) (Liu et al., 2017; Jia et al., 2018). This method was applied to salamander RGCs, and estimated subunits were shown to be well-matched to independent physiological measurements of bipolar cell receptive fields, an impressive validation. This approach works by collecting the spike triggered stimuli in a matrix and then factorizing the matrix. Because this method differs in a variety of ways from the spike-triggered clustering approach, only the core aspects of the two methods were compared (see Discussion). Both methods were applied without any regularization, and were run without perturbing the parameters after convergence (contrary to the suggestion in Liu et al., 2017). The number of subunits used was equal to the true number of subunits in the simulated cell. In these conditions, recovery of subunits revealed two trends. First, for a realistic amount of data, the SNMF approach often produced mixtures of underlying subunits, while the spike-triggered clustering approach produced unique and separate subunits (see Figure 9). Second, the spatial structure of extracted subunit was substantially more variable using SNMF across different initializations. These results indicate that, at least in these simulated conditions, the spike-triggered clustering method produces more consistent and accurate estimates of subunit spatial structure than SNMF. Note, however, that choices of initialization and regularization can alter the efficiency of both procedures. A summary of other differences with SNMF is presented in Discussion.
Subunit behaviors have also been captured using convolutional models that rely on the simplifying assumption that all subunit filters are spatially shifted copies of one another (Eickenberg et al., 2012; Vintch et al., 2015; Wu et al., 2015). To perform the comparison, RGC responses were analyzed with a modification of the spike-triggered clustering approach that enforces convolutional subunit structure, with model parameters fitted by gradient descent. The method was applied with locally normalized L1 regularization, shown above to be effective for retinal data (Figure 10A,B). As expected, the convolutional model was substantially more efficient, exhibiting lower prediction error for limited training data. However, for longer durations of training data (within the range of typical experiments), the unconstrained model exhibited lower error (Figure 10A). The spatial location and resulting layout of subunits obtained from the two methods were similar (Figure 10B). However, spike-triggered clustering captured the deviations from a purely convolutional structure, leading to improved performance. Similar results were obtained using spike-triggered clustering without LNL1 regularization (not shown). Similar results were also obtained using data from a V1 complex cell (Figure 10C,D). Note that the nearly identical power spectra (Figure 8) of the subunits estimated using spike-triggered clustering suggested nearly convolutional structure, without explicitly enforcing this constraint. Note that further improvement in both approaches could potentially be achieved with modifications. For example in the V1 neurons, replacing the exponential nonlinearity with a quadratic nonlinearity (as in Vintch et al., 2015) provided more accurate fits for both models, but the performance of spike-triggered clustering was still superior when more data were available (Figure 10—figure supplement 1).
Discussion
We developed an efficient modeling and estimation approach to study a ubiquitous motif of neural computation in sensory circuits: summation of responses of rectified subunits. The approach allowed efficient subunit estimation by clustering of spike-triggered stimuli, and the inclusion of spatial localization regularizers to additionally constrain the fitting under conditions of limited data. The model and fitting procedure extended naturally to populations of cells, allowing joint fitting of shared subunits. The effectiveness of the method was demonstrated by its ability to capture subunit computations in macaque parasol RGCs, specifically in stimulus conditions in which linear models completely fail, as well in V1 neurons. Below, we discuss the properties of the approach compared to others, and the implications of the application to retinal data.
Modeling linear-nonlinear cascades
The subunit model is a natural generalization of linear-nonlinear (LN) cascade models, and the estimation procedure is a natural generalization of the spike-triggered estimation procedures that facilitated the widespread use of LN models. In the present model framework, a maximum-likelihood estimate of model parameters leads to a spike-triggered clustering algorithm for obtaining the early linear filters that represent model subunits. These linear components are the key, high-dimensional entities that define stimulus selectivity, and, in the absence of a robust computational framework, would be difficult to estimate.
The closest prior work in retina is spike-triggered non-negative matrix factorization (SNMF) (Liu et al., 2017; Jia et al., 2018). In addition to the greater efficiency of spike-triggered clustering (Figure 9), there are several technical differences worth noting. First, the assumptions of the SNMF algorithm are not directly matched to the structure of the subunit response model: the present model assumes nonnegative subunit outputs, while the SNMF factorization method assumes that the subunit filters are nonnegative. This assumption of nonnegative filters is inconsistent with suppressive surrounds previously reported in retinal bipolar cells (Dacey et al., 2000; Fahey and Burkhardt, 2003; Turner et al., 2018), and limits the application of the method to other systems such as V1 (Figure 8). In contrast, the spike-triggered clustering method makes no assumptions about the structure of the subunit filers. Second, the SNMF method requires a sequence of additional steps to relate the estimated matrix factors to the parameters of an LNLN model. In contrast, in the present work the clustering of spike-triggered stimuli is shown to be equivalent to optimizing the likelihood of a LNLN model, eliminating extra steps in relating model parameters to biology. Third, the comparison to bipolar cell receptive fields using SNMF relied on a regularization parameter that trades off sparseness and reconstruction error. This regularization can influence the size of the estimated subunits, and thus the comparison to bipolar receptive fields. In contrast, the present method includes a novel regularizer that encourages spatial contiguity of receptive fields, while exerting minimal influence on the size of estimated subunits, and the strength of this regularizer is chosen using a cross-validation procedure.
Another study (Freeman et al., 2015) presented an approach to estimating subunits from OFF midget RGCs with visual inputs presented at single-cone resolution, and was used to obtain a successful cellular resolution dissection of subunits. However, this method assumed that subunits receive inputs from non-overlapping subsets of cones, which is likely to be incorrect for other cell types, including parasol RGCs. Moreover, the cone to subunit assignments were learned using a greedy procedure, which was effective for subunits composed of 1–2 cone inputs, but is more likely to converge to incorrect local minima in more general conditions.
Recent studies fitted a large class of LNLN models to simulated RGC responses (McFarland et al., 2013), to recorded ON-OFF mouse RGC responses (Shi et al., 2019), and to flickering bar responses in salamander RGCs (Maheswaranathan et al., 2018) using standard gradient descent procedures. These approaches have the advantage of flexibility and simplicity. In contrast, this work assumes a specific subunit nonlinearity that leads to a specialized efficient optimization algorithm, and applies it to recorded data from primate RGCs. The choice of an exponential subunit non-linearity enables likelihood maximization for Gaussian stimuli, which leads to more accurate estimates with limited data, and reduces the computational cost of each iteration during fitting because the loss depends only on the collection of stimuli that elicit a spike (Ramirez and Paninski, 2014). Additionally, the soft-clustering algorithm requires far fewer iterations than stochastic gradient descent algorithms used in fitting deep neural networks (Kingma and Ba, 2014; Duchi et al., 2011). Depending on the experimental conditions (e.g. type of stimuli, duration of recording, number of subunits to be estimated), the present approach may provide more accurate estimates of subunits, with less data, and more quickly.
Other recent studies applied convolutional and recurrent neural network models, fitted to natural scene responses, to mimic a striking variety of retinal response properties (McIntosh et al., 2016; Batty et al., 2016). The general structure of the model – convolutions and rectifying nonlinearities – resembles the subunit model used here. However, the number of layers and the interactions between channels do not have a direct correspondence to the known retinal architecture, and to date the specific properties of the inferred subunits, and their contributions to the behaviors of the model, have not been elucidated. The complexity of the underlying models suggest that a comparison to the biology may prove difficult.
In the context of V1 neurons, the present approach has advantages and disadvantages compared to previous methods. Spike-triggered covariance (STC) methods (Rust et al., 2005) estimate an orthogonal basis for the stimulus subspace captured by the subunits. However, the underlying subunits are unlikely to be orthogonal and are not guaranteed to align with the basis elements. The method also requires substantial data to obtain accurate estimates. To reduce the data requirements, other methods (Eickenberg et al., 2012; Vintch et al., 2015; Wu et al., 2015) imposed an assumption of convolutional structure on the subunits. This approach is efficient, because it effectively fits only one subunit filter that is then applied convolutionally. However, as shown above using recorded data from RGCs and V1 complex cells, the convolutional restriction may not be necessary when enough data are available, and under these conditions, the present method can more accurately reveal subunit structure in receptive fields that is not precisely convolutional (Figure 10).
A generalization of the STC approach (Sharpee et al., 2004; Paninski, 2003; Eickenberg et al., 2012) involves finding a subspace of stimulus space which is most informative about a neuron's response. This approach also yields a basis for the space spanned by subunits, but the particular axes of the space may or may not correspond to the subunits. Unlike the method presented here, this approach requires minimal assumptions about stimulus statistics, making it very flexible. However, methods based on information-theoretic measures typically require substantially more data than are available in experimental recordings (Paninski, 2003).
Although constraints on the structure of the subunit model (e.g. convolutional), and the estimation algorithm (e.g. spike-triggered covariance or clustering), both influence the efficiency of subunit estimation, the choice of priors used to estimate the subunits efficiently with limited data (i.e. regularization) is also important. In the present work, a regularizer was developed that imposes spatial contiguity, with minimal impact on the size of estimated subunits. A variety of other regularization schemes have been previously proposed for spatial contiguity for the estimation of V1 subunits (Park and Pillow, 2011). These could be incorporated in the estimation of the present model using a corresponding projection operator.
As suggested by the results, another algorithmic modification that could improve the subunit estimation is the hierarchical variant of the clustering approach (Figure 2—figure supplement 1). By splitting one of the subunits into two subunits at each step, models with different number of subunits can be estimated with greater efficiency, and preliminary exploration indicated that this may offer improvements in data efficiency and speed. However, potential tradeoffs emerging from the additional assumptions will require further study.
Revealing the impact of non-linear computations using null stimuli
The null stimulus methodology revealed the failure of LN models, and isolated the nonlinear behaviors arising from receptive field subunits. Previous studies have developed specialized stimuli to examine different aspects of the cascaded linear-nonlinear model. Contrast reversing gratings (CRGs) have been the most commonly used stimuli for identifying the existence of a spatial nonlinearity (Hochstein and Shapley, 1976; Demb et al., 2001). Recent studies (Schwartz et al., 2012; Turner and Rieke, 2016) extended this logic to more naturalistic stimuli, with changes in responses to translations and rotations of texture stimuli revealing the degree of spatial nonlinearity. Another (Bölinger and Gollisch, 2012) developed a closed-loop experimental method to measure subunit nonlinearities, by tracing the intensities of oppositely signed stimuli on two halves of the receptive field to measure iso-response curves. More recent work (Freeman et al., 2015) developed stimuli in closed loop that targeted pairs of distinct cones in the receptive field, and used these to identify linear and nonlinear summation in RGCs depending on whether the cones provided input to the same or different subunits.
The null stimulus approach introduced here offers a generalization of the preceding methods. Null stimuli can be generated starting from any desired stimulus ensemble (e.g. natural images), and the method does not assume any particular receptive field structure (e.g. radial symmetry). Construction of the null stimuli does, however, require fitting a linear (or LN) model to the data in closed loop. Projecting a diverse set of stimuli (e.g. white noise) onto the null space yields a stimulus ensemble with rich spatio-temporal structure that generates diverse neural responses. By construction, the null stimulus is the closest stimulus to the original stimulus that is orthogonal to the receptive field, and thus minimally alters other statistical properties of the stimulus that affect neural response. This property is useful for studying the effect of spatial nonlinearities in the context of specific stimuli, such as natural scenes. Whereas contrast reversing gratings do not reveal stronger nonlinearities in OFF parasol cells compared to ON parasol cells in responses with natural stimuli (Turner and Rieke, 2016), the null stimulus captures these differences (Figure 6), highlighting the advantages of tailored stimulation with rich spatio-temporal responses.
Further applications and extensions
The assumptions behind the proposed subunit model enable efficient fitting and interpretability, but also lead to certain limitations that could potentially be overcome. First, the choice of an exponential subunit nonlinearity improves efficiency by allowing the maximum expected likelihood approximation. However, other nonlinearities that allow this approximation, such as a rectified quadratic form (Bölinger and Gollisch, 2012), could be explored. Second, in fitting RGCs, the assumption of space-time separable subunits significantly reduces the number of parameters that need to be estimated, but could be relaxed with more data. The space-time separability assumption can be verified by checking if the spatio-temporal STA had rank one. In the present data (not shown), the STA has rank one when only pixels corresponding to the receptive field center were considered but had higher rank when the surround was also taken into consideration (Enroth-Cugell and Pinto, 1970; Cowan et al., 2016). Thus, replacing the space-time separability assumption may be desirable. For example, a modification to the procedure that enforces subunits with rank two (not shown) could capture the center-surround structure of bipolar cell receptive fields (Dacey et al., 2000; Turner et al., 2018).
The methods developed here may also prove useful in tackling additional challenging problems in neural circuitry and nonlinear coding, in the retina and beyond. First, applying the model to high-resolution visual stimulation of the retina could be used to reveal how individual cones connect to bipolar cells, the likely cellular substrate of subunits, in multiple RGC types (see Freeman et al., 2015). Second, the incorporation of noise in the model, along with estimation of shared subunits (Figure 4), could help to probe the origin of noise correlations in the firing of nearby RGCs (see Rieke and Chichilnisky, 2014). Modeling the nonlinear interaction between subunits (Kuo et al., 2016; Manookin et al., 2018) could also improve prediction accuracy, especially for natural scenes. Third, extending the subunit model to include gain control, another ubiquitous nonlinear computation in the retina and throughout the visual system, could provide a more complete description of the responses in diverse stimulus conditions (Heeger, 1992; Carandini and Heeger, 2012). In addition to these potential scientific advances, a more accurate functional subunit model may be critical for the development of artificial retinas for treating blindness. Finally, the successful application of the method to V1 data (Figure 8) suggests that the method could be effective in capturing computations in other neural circuits that share the nonlinear subunit motif.
Materials and methods
Recordings
Request a detailed protocolDetailed preparation and recording methods are described elsewhere (Litke et al., 2004; Frechette et al., 2005; Chichilnisky and Kalmar, 2002). Briefly, eyes were enucleated from seven terminally anesthetized macaque monkeys (Macaca sp.) used by other experimenters in accordance with institutional guidelines for the care and use of animals. Immediately after enucleation, the anterior portion of the eye and the vitreous were removed in room light. The eye was stored in darkness in oxygenated Ames' solution (Sigma, St Louis, MO) at 33°C, pH 7.4. Segments of isolated or RPE-attached peripheral retina (6–15 mm temporal equivalent eccentricity [Chichilnisky and Kalmar, 2002], approximately 3 mm x 3 mm) were placed flat, RGC side down, on a planar array of extracellular microelectrodes. The array consisted of 512 electrodes in an isosceles triangular lattice, with 60 µm inter-electrode spacing in each row, covering a rectangular region measuring 1800 µm x 900 µm. While recording, the retina was perfused with Ames' solution (35°C for isolated recordings and 33°C for RPE-attached recordings), bubbled with 95% O2% and 5% CO2, pH 7.4. Voltage signals on each electrode were bandpass filtered, amplified, and digitized at 20 kHz.
A custom spike-sorting algorithm was used to identify spikes from different cells (Litke et al., 2004). Briefly, candidate spike events were detected using a threshold on each electrode, and voltage waveforms on the electrode and nearby electrodes in the 5 ms period surrounding the time of the spike were extracted. Candidate neurons were identified by clustering the waveforms using a Gaussian mixture model. Candidate neurons were retained only if the assigned spikes exhibited a 1 ms refractory period and totaled more than 100 in 30 min of recording. Duplicate spike trains were identified by temporal cross-correlation and removed. Manual analysis was used to further select cells with a stable firing rate over the course of the experiment, and with a spatially localized receptive field.
Visual stimuli
Request a detailed protocolVisual stimuli were delivered using the optically reduced image of a CRT monitor refreshing at 120 Hz and focused on the photoreceptor outer segments. The optical path passed through the mostly transparent electrode array and the retina. The relative emission spectrum of each display primary was measured with a spectroradiometer (PR-701, PhotoResearch) after passing through the optical elements between the display and the retina. The total power of each display primary was measured with a calibrated photodiode (UDT Instruments). The mean photoisomerization rates for the L, M, and S cones were estimated by computing the inner product of the display primary power spectra with the spectral sensitivity of each cone type, and multiplying by the effective collecting area of primate cones (0.6 μm2) (Angueyra and Rieke, 2013; Schnapf et al., 1990). During white noise and null stimulus, the mean background illumination level resulted in photoisomerization rates of (800–2200, 800–2200, 400–900) for the (L, M, S) cones. The pixel size was either 41.6 microns (eight monitor pixels on a side) or 20.8 microns (four monitor pixels on a side). New black binary white noise frames were drawn at a rate of 60 Hz (Figure 2) or 30 Hz (Figures 5,6). The pixel contrast (difference between the maximum and minimum intensities divided by the sum) was either 48% or 96% for each display primary, with mean luminance at 50%.
The details of natural stimuli used are presented in Heitman et al. (2016). Briefly, the stimuli consisted of images from the Van Hateren database, shown for one second each, jittered according to eye movement trajectories recorded during fixation by awake macaque monkeys (Z.M. Hafed and R.J. Krauzlis, personal communication), (Heitman et al., 2016; van Hateren and van der Schaaf, 1998). Image intensities produced mean photoisomerization rates of (1900, 1800, 700) for the (L, M, S) cones, respectively. The pixel width was 10.4 microns (two monitor pixels on a side), and image frames refreshed at 120 Hz. The training data, consisting of 59 groups of 60 distinct natural scenes, was interleaved with identical repeats of testing data, consisting of 30 distinct natural scenes.
Subunit estimation
Request a detailed protocolThe light responses of RGCs were modeled using a cascade of two linear-nonlinear (LN) stages, followed by a Poisson spike generator. Specifically, the instantaneous stimulus-conditional firing rate was modeled as where is the visual stimulus at time (expressed as a vector), are the spatial subunit filters (also vectors), are non-negative weights on different subunits, and , is the output nonlinearity.
The parameters are estimated by minimizing their negative log-likelihood given observed spiking responses to visual stimuli , in two steps. In the first step, the parameters are estimated using a clustering procedure while ignoring the nonlinearity (). In the second step, are estimated using gradient descent, with the held fixed (up to a scale factor).
The clustering procedure is derived by minimizing an approximate convex upper bound of the negative log-likelihood based on current parameter estimates . The convex upper-bound is derived as follows:
where and does not depend on parameters . For simplicity, we present the equations for a standard Gaussian stimulus (modifications for arbitrary Gaussian stimuli are straightforward). Since the first term in the log-likelihood (Equation 1) does not depend on recorded responses (), it is replaced with its expected value across stimuli (Equation 2). Because the projections of the input distribution onto filters are assumed to be Gaussian, this expectation can be computed using the moment generating function of a Gaussian distribution (Equation 3). The second term only depends on the stimuli for which the cell generated spikes (), reducing computational cost. Finally, to optimize the resulting non-convex function, a convex upper bound was minimized at each step. Specifically, the second term is replaced by a first-order Taylor approximation (Equation 4) (the first term is already convex). This upper bound is separable across parameters of different subunits, as can be seen by re-arranging the summation over time and subunits (Equation 5). Therefore, the parameters for each subunit can be updated separately by minimizing the corresponding term in Equation 5.
The parameters for each subunit are optimized in three steps. First, the subunit activations, are updated based on the parameters from the previous iteration. Then, setting to zero the partial derivatives of Equation 5 with respect to and produces a system of two equations, which are rearranged to yield the remaining two steps:
Update subunit activations (cluster weights) for each stimulus : .
Update linear subunit filters (cluster centers) : .
Update relative weights for different subunits : .
Hence, applying the three steps repeatedly results in a clustering algorithm that iteratively minimizes a convex upper bound on the approximate negative log-likelihood function. Since the upper bound is tight at the current parameter estimates, the successive minimization of the convex upper bound reduces the approximate negative log-likelihood monotonically, leading to guaranteed convergence. However, due to non-convexity of the approximate negative log-likelihood function, the estimated parameters might not correspond to a global minimum. In the limit of large recording duration, the approximation in Equation 2 is exact.
The memory requirement for each step of the algorithm is proportional to the number of spike triggered stimuli, which could be large for fine resolution stimuli and/or high firing rates. Computational runtime depends linearly on the number of spikes, the stimulus dimensions, the number of subunits, and the number of steps for convergence of the clustering procedure. For most of the results presented in this paper, each fit converged in ∼ 100 steps and required a few minutes on a single-core computer. Additional speed improvements would be possible by parallelizing each step of clustering across subunits, making runtime independent of the number of subunits. The resulting subunits can be interpreted as a decomposition of the spike triggered average (STA), since the sum of estimated subunits (), weighted by their expected contribution to the response (), is proportional to the spike-triggered average:
where (6) comes form (3) (at convergence), (7) comes from (2) (at convergence), and (8) arises because the sum to one. Step (6) may be interpreted as replacing the average spike rate, multiplied by the contribution of each subunit. Step (7) may be interpreted as a weighted average of spike-triggered stimuli, where the weighting assigns responsibility for each spike across the subunits. The above formulae ignore the output non-linearity, justified by the observation that it is approximately linear after fitting.
Incorporating priors
Request a detailed protocolThe log-likelihood can be augmented with a regularization term, to incorporate prior knowledge about the structure of subunits, thereby improving estimation in the case of limited data. Regularization is incorporated into the algorithm by projecting the subunit kernels () with a proximal operator (), derived from the regularization function, after each update of the cluster centers (Figure 11). For a regularization term of the form , the proximal operator is defined as . Regularization was applied for estimating subunits for both single cell (Figure 3) as well as population (Figure 4) data and in each case, the regularization strength was chosen by cross-validation (see below).
For -norm regularization (section 3), , where is the value of th pixel, and the proximal operator is a soft-thresholding operator . For the locally normalized -norm regularization, the proximal operator of the Taylor approximation was used at each step. The Taylor approximation is a weighted -norm with the weight for each pixel determined by the parameter value of the neighboring pixels. Hence, the proximal operator for the th pixel is , where for black (smaller than typical non-zero values in , which are on the order of unity). Simulations were used to verify that this regularizer induces a preference for spatially contiguous subunits, while being relatively insensitive to subunit area (compared with regularization).
The entire procedure is summarized in Figure 11, with simplified notation illustrating the four key steps.
Subunit estimation using hierarchical clustering
Request a detailed protocolAs shown in Figure 2, a hierarchical organization of subunits was observed in fits to RGC data, with one subunit broken down into two subunits each time the total number of subunits was increased by one. This hierarchical structure can be enforced explicitly to make the estimation procedure more efficient.
Since the softmax weights (between 0 and 1) can be interpreted as the ‘activation probability’ of subunit , hierarchical clustering is performed by estimating two 'child’ subunits with factorized activation probability : , where is the conditional probability of child given the activation of parent subunit with . The factorization is accurate if the activation of the parent subunit is the sum of activation of the child subunits ().
The child subunits were initialized by adding independent random noise to parent subunits () and equally dividing the subunit weight into two (). Hierarchical clustering was performed by iteratively updating the cluster assignments () and cluster centers () for the child subunits using the factored as outlined above.
The parent subunit for splitting was chosen greedily, to yield maximum increase in log-likelihood at each step. Hierarchical subunit estimation provides a computationally efficient way to estimate the entire solution path with different . The estimated subunits obtained with this procedure are shown in Figure 2—figure supplement 1.
Population model
Request a detailed protocolIn Section 4, a method to estimate a common bank of subunits jointly using multiple nearby cells is described. For each cell , the firing rate is given by where are the cell-specific subunit weights. The model parameters were estimated by maximizing the summation of log-likelihood across cells.
Similar to the single-cell case, the estimation was performed in two steps: 1) Ignoring the output nonlinearity , estimate by clustering, and 2 ) Estimate , and the magnitude of for each cell independently using gradient descent, with the vector direction of fixed. Clustering in the first step can be interpreted as finding a common set of centroids to cluster the spike-triggered stimuli for multiple cells simultaneously. The following steps are repeated iteratively:
Update subunit activations (cluster weights) for each stimulus and cell:
Update linear subunit filters (cluster centers) using population responses:
Update subunit weights for each cell:
As with subunit estimation using single-cell responses, prior knowledge about subunit structure can be incorporated with a proximal operator () in the clustering loop. For RGC data, locally normalized L1 regularization was used (Figure 4).
Application to neural responses
Request a detailed protocolThe subunit estimation algorithm was applied to a rectangular segment of stimulus covering the receptive field (along with a one stimulus pixel boundary around it) and to the RGC spike counts over time (8.33 ms bin size). The binary white noise stimulus was temporally filtered and the resulting normally distributed stimulus (zero mean) was used for subunit estimation (). The receptive field was estimated as a contiguous set of pixels black in the STA with magnitude larger than 2.5σ, where σ is the standard deviation of the background noise in the STA. The time course for filtering (125 ms length) was estimated by averaging the time course of significant pixels in the STA. The data were partitioned into three groups: testing data consisted of a contiguous segment of recording (last 10%), and the rest of the recording was randomly partitioned for training (90%) and validation (10%) data.
Models were fit with different numbers of subunits (1-12) and regularization values . Selection of hyperparameters ( and ) was performed by averaging the performance of multiple fits with different training/validation partitions and random initializations (see Figure captions for details). For a given number of subunits, the regularization value was chosen by optimizing the performance on validation data. Similarly, the most accurate model was chosen by optimizing over the number of subunits as well as regularization strength. When data with repeated presentations of the same stimulus were available (e.g. Figures 6,7 ), the prediction accuracy was evaluated by measuring the correlation between peri-stimulus time histogram (PSTH) of recorded data and the predicted spikes for the same number of repetitions. The PSTH was computed by averaging the discretized responses over repeats and gaussian smoothing with standard deviation 16.67 ms.
For null stimuli, responses from 30 repeats of a 10 s long stimulus were divided into training (first 5 s) and testing (last 5 s) data. Since the training data were limited, the subunit filters and weights (phase 1) were first estimated from responses to a long white noise stimulus and only the subunit scales and output nonlinearity (phase 2) were estimated using the null stimulus.
For natural scenes stimuli, the spatial kernel for subunits was first estimated from responses to the white noise stimulus (phase 1), and the scales of subunits and output nonlinearity were then fit using responses to a natural stimulus (phase 2). This procedure was used because subunits estimated directly from natural scenes did not show any improvement in prediction accuracy, and all the subunits were essentially identical to the RF, presumably because of the strong spatial correlations in natural images.
V1 neurons were fit to previously published responses to flickering bars (see Rust et al., 2005 for details). In contrast to the retinal data, the two dimensions of the stimulus were time and one dimension of space (orthogonal to the preferred orientation of the cell). The estimation procedure (without regularization) was applied for different numbers of subunits. For successive numbers of subunits ( and ), the hierarchical relationship was evaluated by greedily matching filters across the two fits using the inner product of their spatio-temporal filters and splitting the remaining subunit in the -subunit fit into the two remaining subunits in the -subunit fit. The maximum number of subunits was chosen by cross validation as described above.
Simulated RGC model
Request a detailed protocolTo test the estimation procedure in conditions where the answer is known, the procedure was applied to responses generated from a simulated RGC (Appendix I). In the simulation, each RGC received exponentiated inputs from bipolar cells, which in turn linearly summed inputs from photoreceptors. The spatial dimensions of the simulation and the number of cones and bipolar cells were approximately matched to parasol RF centers at the eccentricities of the recorded data (Schwartz and Rieke, 2011; Jacoby et al., 2000). Below, all the measurements are presented in grid units (g.u.), with 1 g.u. = 1 micron resulting in a biologically plausible simulation. The first layer consisted of 64 photoreceptors arranged on a jittered hexagonal lattice with nearest-neighbor distance 5 g.u., oriented at 60° with respect to the stimulus grid. The location of each cone was independently jittered with a radially symmetric Gaussian with standard deviation black 0.35 g.u. Stimuli were pooled by individual photoreceptors with a spatio-temporally separable filter, with time course derived from a typical parasol cell and a Gaussian spatial filter (standard deviation = black 1 g.u.).
The second layer of the cascade consisted of 12 model bipolar cells, each summing the inputs from a spatially localized set of photoreceptors, followed by an exponential nonlinearity. The photoreceptors were assigned to individual bipolars by k-means clustering (12 clusters) of their locations. Finally, the RGC firing rate is computed by a positively weighted summation of bipolar activations. The bipolar weights were drawn from a Gaussian distribution with standard deviation equal to 10% of the mean. The choice of photoreceptors and bipolar cell weights give a mean firing rate of ≈19 spikes/s.
First, the subunit model was fitted to 24 min of responses to a white noise stimulus, such that the receptive field was roughly covered by 6 × 6 stimulus pixels (Figure S1B), as in the recorded experimental data (Figure 2B). Subsequently, the subunit model was fitted to 6 hr of stimuli that excites each cone individually (Figure S1C). In this case, subunits are represented as weights on individual photoreceptors. In a retinal preparation, individual cone activation is possible using a fine resolution white noise such that the locations of individual photoreceptors can be identified (see Freeman et al., 2015).
Null stimulus
Request a detailed protocolNull stimuli were constructed to experimentally verify the nonlinear influence of estimated subunits on light response. Below, a general approach is first presented to generate a spatio-temporal null stimulus which can be used for any neural system. Subsequently, it is shown that if the STA is space-time separable, the spatio-temporal null stimulus can be derived with a simpler and faster method that only uses spatial information . Since this separability assumption is approximately accurate for RGCs, the spatial null stimulus is used for the results presented in main paper (Figures 5,6).
The purpose of spatio-temporal nulling is to find the closest stimulus sequence such that convolution with a specific spatio-temporal linear filter gives 0 for all time points. For simplicity, the algorithm is described only for a single cell (the extension to multiple cells is straightforward). Let represent the spatio-temporal linear filter, estimated by computing the STA on white noise responses. The orthogonality constraint for a null stimulus is written as:
The constraints for successive frames are not independent, but they become so when transformed to the temporal frequency domain. Writing and for the Fourier transform of and respectively, the constraints may be rewritten as:
Hence for each temporal frequency, the spatial content can be projected onto the subspace orthogonal to the estimated spatial receptive field. Specifically, for a given temporal frequency , given a vectorized stimulus frame and a matrix with columns corresponding to the vectorized receptive field, the null frame is obtained by solving the following optimization problem:
The solution may be written in closed form:
For the results presented in this paper, binary white noise stimuli () are projected onto the null space. However, in general, any visual stimulus can be used instead of white noise. The receptive field (linear filter) for each cell was estimated by computing a space-time separable approximation of the spike-triggered average (STA), with the support limited to the pixels with value significantly different from background noise (absolute value of pixel value > 2.5 σ, with σ a robust estimate of standard deviation of background noise).
In addition to the orthogonality constraint, two additional constraints are imposed: a) each pixel must be in the range of monitor intensities (−0.5 to 0.5) and b) the variance of each pixel over time must be preserved, to avoid the possibility of contrast adaption in photoreceptors (Clark et al., 2013, but see Rieke, 2001). These two constraints were incorporated using Dykstra’s algorithm (Boyle and Dykstra, 1986), which iteratively projects the estimated null stimulus into the subspace corresponding to the individual constraints until convergence. Setting the contrast of initial white noise black () to a value lower than 100% was necessary to satisfy these additional constraints. Finally, the pixel values of the resulting null stimuli were discretized to 8-bit integers, for display on a monitor with limited dynamic range.
For a space-time separable (rank one) STA (as is approximately true for RGCs), spatio-temporal nulling gives the same solution as spatial nulling. To see this, assume , a space-time separable linear filter. Hence, the null constraint can be rewritten as
where . In the frequency domain this is written as
Since has limited support in time, it has infinite support in frequency. Hence, implying spatial-only nulling .
Data availability
Data has been deposited with Dryad at: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.dncjsxkvk. Code available on Github: https://github.com/Chichilnisky-Lab/shah-elife-2020 (copy archived at https://github.com/elifesciences-publications/shah-elife-2020).
-
Dryad Digital RepositoryData from: Inference of nonlinear receptive field subunits with spike-triggered clustering.https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.dncjsxkvk
References
-
Spatiotemporal energy models for the perception of motionJournal of the Optical Society of America 2:284–299.https://doi.org/10.1364/JOSAA.2.000284
-
Origin and effect of phototransduction noise in primate cone photoreceptorsNature Neuroscience 16:1692–1700.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3534
-
A retinal circuit that computes object motionJournal of Neuroscience 28:6807–6817.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4206-07.2008
-
Two-Photon imaging of nonlinear glutamate release dynamics at bipolar cell synapses in the mouse retinaJournal of Neuroscience 33:10972–10985.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1241-13.2013
-
Advances in Order Restricted Statistical Inference28–47, A method for finding projections onto the intersection of convex sets in hilbert spaces, Advances in Order Restricted Statistical Inference, Springer, 10.1007/978-1-4613-9940-7_3.
-
Normalization as a canonical neural computationNature Reviews Neuroscience 13:51–62.https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3136
-
A simple white noise analysis of neuronal light responsesNetwork: Computation in Neural Systems 12:199–213.https://doi.org/10.1080/713663221
-
Functional asymmetries in ON and OFF ganglion cells of primate retinaThe Journal of Neuroscience 22:2737–2747.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-07-02737.2002
-
Dynamical adaptation in photoreceptorsPLOS Computational Biology 9:e1003289.https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003289
-
Y-Cell receptive field and collicular projection of parasol ganglion cells in macaque monkey retinaJournal of Neuroscience 28:11277–11291.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2982-08.2008
-
BookTheoretical Neuroscience: Computational and Mathematical Modeling of Neural SystemsMIT press.
-
Functional circuitry of the retinal ganglion cell's nonlinear receptive fieldThe Journal of Neuroscience 19:9756–9767.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-22-09756.1999
-
Adaptive subgradient methods for online learning and stochastic optimizationJournal of Machine Learning Research 12:2121–2159.
-
Spatio-temporal interactions in cat retinal ganglion cells showing linear spatial summationThe Journal of Physiology 341:279–307.https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1983.sp014806
-
Spatial properties and functional organization of small bistratified ganglion cells in primate retinaJournal of Neuroscience 27:13261–13272.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3437-07.2007
-
Fidelity of the ensemble code for visual motion in primate retinaJournal of Neurophysiology 94:119–135.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.01175.2004
-
Normalization of cell responses in cat striate cortexVisual Neuroscience 9:181–197.https://doi.org/10.1017/S0952523800009640
-
Linear and nonlinear spatial subunits in Y cat retinal ganglion cellsThe Journal of Physiology 262:265–284.https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1976.sp011595
-
Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortexThe Journal of Physiology 160:106–154.https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1962.sp006837
-
Diffuse bipolar cells provide input to OFF parasol ganglion cells in the macaque retinaThe Journal of Comparative Neurology 416:6–18.https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(20000103)416:1<6::AID-CNE2>3.0.CO;2-X
-
Identifying functional bases for multidimensional neural computationsNeural Computation 25:1870–1890.https://doi.org/10.1162/NECO_a_00465
-
What does the eye tell the brain?: development of a system for the large-scale recording of retinal output activityIEEE Transactions on Nuclear Science 51:1434–1440.https://doi.org/10.1109/TNS.2004.832706
-
Inferring hidden structure in multilayered neural circuitsPLOS Computational Biology 14:e1006291.https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1006291
-
Inferring nonlinear neuronal computation based on physiologically plausible inputsPLOS Computational Biology 9:e1003143.https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003143
-
ConferenceDeep learning models of the retinal response to natural scenesAdvances in Neural Information Processing Systems. pp. 1369–1377.
-
Approach sensitivity in the retina processed by a multifunctional neural circuitNature Neuroscience 12:1308–1316.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2389
-
ConferenceConvergence properties of some spike-triggered analysis techniquesAdvances in Neural Information Processing Systems.. pp. 189–196.
-
ConferenceSpectral methods for neural characterization using generalized quadratic models.Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems. pp. 2454–2462.
-
Receptive field inference with localized priorsPLOS Computational Biology 7:e1002219.https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002219
-
Fast inference in generalized linear models via expected log-likelihoodsJournal of Computational Neuroscience 36:215–234.https://doi.org/10.1007/s10827-013-0466-4
-
Temporal contrast adaptation in salamander bipolar cellsThe Journal of Neuroscience 21:9445–9454.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-23-09445.2001
-
The New Visual Neurosciences145–162, Correlated activity in the retina, The New Visual Neurosciences, 12, MIT Press.
-
Visual transduction in cones of the monkey Macaca fascicularisThe Journal of Physiology 427:681–713.https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1990.sp018193
-
The spatial structure of a nonlinear receptive fieldNature Neuroscience 15:1572–1580.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3225
-
Nonlinear spatial encoding by retinal ganglion cells: when 1 + 1 ≠ 2The Journal of General Physiology 138:283–290.https://doi.org/10.1085/jgp.201110629
-
Beyond GLMs: a generative mixture modeling approach to neural system identificationPLOS Computational Biology 9:e1003356.https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003356
-
Independent component filters of natural images compared with simple cells in primary visual cortexProceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences 265:359–366.https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.1998.0303
-
A convolutional subunit model for neuronal responses in macaque V1Journal of Neuroscience 35:14829–14841.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2815-13.2015
-
ConferenceConvolutional spike-triggered covariance analysis for neural subunit modelsAdvances in Neural Information Processing Systems.. pp. 793–801.
Article and author information
Author details
Funding
National Science Foundation (NSF IGERT Grant 0801700)
- Nora Jane Brackbill
National Eye Institute (NIH NEI R01-EY021271)
- EJ Chichilnisky
Howard Hughes Medical Institute
- Eero Simoncelli
Pew Charitable Trusts
- Alexander Sher
National Eye Institute (NIH NEI F31EY027166)
- Colleen Rhoades
National Science Foundation (NSF GRFP DGE-114747)
- Colleen Rhoades
National Eye Institute (NIH NEI P30-EY019005)
- E J Chichilnisky
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Ethics
Animal experimentation: Eyes were removed from terminally anesthetized macaque monkeys (Macaca mulatta, Macaca fascicularis) used by other laboratories in the course of their experiments, in accordance with the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee guidelines. All of the animals were handled according to approved institutional animal care and use committee (IACUC) protocols (#28860) of the Stanford University. The protocol was approved by the Administrative Panel on Laboratory Animal Care of the Stanford University (Assurance Number: A3213-01).
Copyright
© 2020, Shah et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 1,702
- views
-
- 266
- downloads
-
- 36
- citations
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Computational and Systems Biology
- Neuroscience
Hypothalamic kisspeptin (Kiss1) neurons are vital for pubertal development and reproduction. Arcuate nucleus Kiss1 (Kiss1ARH) neurons are responsible for the pulsatile release of gonadotropin-releasing hormone (GnRH). In females, the behavior of Kiss1ARH neurons, expressing Kiss1, neurokinin B (NKB), and dynorphin (Dyn), varies throughout the ovarian cycle. Studies indicate that 17β-estradiol (E2) reduces peptide expression but increases Slc17a6 (Vglut2) mRNA and glutamate neurotransmission in these neurons, suggesting a shift from peptidergic to glutamatergic signaling. To investigate this shift, we combined transcriptomics, electrophysiology, and mathematical modeling. Our results demonstrate that E2 treatment upregulates the mRNA expression of voltage-activated calcium channels, elevating the whole-cell calcium current that contributes to high-frequency burst firing. Additionally, E2 treatment decreased the mRNA levels of canonical transient receptor potential (TPRC) 5 and G protein-coupled K+ (GIRK) channels. When Trpc5 channels in Kiss1ARH neurons were deleted using CRISPR/SaCas9, the slow excitatory postsynaptic potential was eliminated. Our data enabled us to formulate a biophysically realistic mathematical model of Kiss1ARH neurons, suggesting that E2 modifies ionic conductances in these neurons, enabling the transition from high-frequency synchronous firing through NKB-driven activation of TRPC5 channels to a short bursting mode facilitating glutamate release. In a low E2 milieu, synchronous firing of Kiss1ARH neurons drives pulsatile release of GnRH, while the transition to burst firing with high, preovulatory levels of E2 would facilitate the GnRH surge through its glutamatergic synaptic connection to preoptic Kiss1 neurons.
-
- Neuroscience
Specialized chemosensory signals elicit innate social behaviors in individuals of several vertebrate species, a process that is mediated via the accessory olfactory system (AOS). The AOS comprising the peripheral sensory vomeronasal organ has evolved elaborate molecular and cellular mechanisms to detect chemo signals. To gain insight into the cell types, developmental gene expression patterns, and functional differences amongst neurons, we performed single-cell transcriptomics of the mouse vomeronasal sensory epithelium. Our analysis reveals diverse cell types with gene expression patterns specific to each, which we made available as a searchable web resource accessed from https://www.scvnoexplorer.com. Pseudo-time developmental analysis indicates that neurons originating from common progenitors diverge in their gene expression during maturation with transient and persistent transcription factor expression at critical branch points. Comparative analysis across two of the major neuronal subtypes that express divergent GPCR families and the G-protein subunits Gnai2 or Gnao1, reveals significantly higher expression of endoplasmic reticulum (ER) associated genes within Gnao1 neurons. In addition, differences in ER content and prevalence of cubic membrane ER ultrastructure revealed by electron microscopy, indicate fundamental differences in ER function.