Tactile sensory channels over-ruled by frequency decoding system that utilizes spike pattern regardless of receptor type

  1. Ingvars Birznieks  Is a corresponding author
  2. Sarah McIntyre
  3. Hanna Maria Nilsson
  4. Saad S Nagi
  5. Vaughan G Macefield
  6. David A Mahns
  7. Richard M Vickery
  1. Faculty of Medicine, UNSW Sydney, Australia
  2. Neuroscience Research Australia, Australia
  3. Western Sydney University, Australia
  4. Linköping University, Sweden
  5. School of Medicine, Western Sydney University, Australia
  6. The Baker Heart and Diabetes Institute, Australia

Abstract

The established view is that vibrotactile stimuli evoke two qualitatively distinctive cutaneous sensations, flutter (frequencies < 60 Hz) and vibratory hum (frequencies > 60 Hz), subserved by two distinct receptor types (Meissner’s and Pacinian corpuscle, respectively), which may engage different neural processing pathways or channels and fulfil quite different biological roles. In psychological and physiological literature, those two systems have been labelled as Pacinian and non-Pacinian channels. However, we present evidence that low-frequency spike trains in Pacinian afferents can readily induce a vibratory percept with the same low frequency attributes as sinusoidal stimuli of the same frequency, thus demonstrating a universal frequency decoding system. We achieved this using brief low-amplitude pulsatile mechanical stimuli to selectively activate Pacinian afferents. This indicates that spiking pattern, regardless of receptor type, determines vibrotactile frequency perception. This mechanism may underlie the constancy of vibrotactile frequency perception across different skin regions innervated by distinct afferent types.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.46510.001

Introduction

The sense of touch comprises a range of different perceptual qualities subserved by several distinct mechanoreceptor types and associated afferent nerve fibres in the skin (Johnson, 2001; Vallbo and Johansson, 1984). Observations that different receptor types are tuned to different stimulus features and have distinct response profiles have led researchers to conclude that different receptor types are the inputs to separate neural ‘channels’ dedicated to processing of those features (Bolanowski et al., 1994; Gescheider, 1976; Gescheider et al., 2004; Hyvarinen et al., 1968; Sretavan and Dykes, 1983). In the glabrous skin, there are two types of fast adapting (FA) afferents which at the threshold level display characteristic U-shaped tuning curves to sinusoidal vibrotactile stimuli: FAI (or RA) afferents innervating Meissner’s corpuscles are preferentially activated at frequencies up to 60 Hz, while FAII (or Pacinian - PC) afferents innervating Pacinian corpuscles have much lower response thresholds, and are most sensitive to higher frequencies (>100 Hz) (Talbot et al., 1968). At the border between those two frequency domains, at about 60 Hz, there is a qualitative change in sensation from flutter to vibratory hum (Gescheider, 1976; LaMotte and Mountcastle, 1975; Talbot et al., 1968), which is used as further justification for psychophysical segregation into Pacinian and non-Pacinian channels. This scheme has engendered speculation that the Pacinian channel may not possess neural circuits for processing low-frequency spiking patterns characteristic of low-frequency sinusoidal stimuli, and therefore cannot produce a perceptual experience outside the high-frequency domain. These findings are based on laboratory testing using sinusoidal stimuli in which acceleration and periodicity are linked and thus Pacinian corpuscles would not respond at low frequencies. An intriguing question is the extent to which frequency processing circuitry is specialised for afferent type (FAI vs FAII) in their optimal sinusoidal frequency response range? There is a big gap in our knowledge, as sinusoidal stimuli inherently do not allow activation of FAII afferents at low frequencies and thus functionally are not representative for a wide variety of natural stimuli involving discrete mechanical transients associated with motor control or surface structures with low spatial frequency.

We addressed this question by using brief pulsatile mechanical stimuli that enabled us to create arbitrary time-controlled spike trains of any frequency and pattern in the responding FAII afferents and thus investigate the perceptual properties of those spiking patterns (for details see Birznieks and Vickery, 2017). By setting the amplitude of the mechanical pulses below the FAI activation threshold, we first established that low-frequency discharge in FAII afferents (the Pacinian channel) can indeed cause conscious perception of a tactile stimulus at frequencies as low as 6 Hz. We then investigated the perceptual properties of low-frequency FAII afferent discharge by comparing them with those elicited by sinusoidal stimuli driving predominantly FAI afferents (the non-Pacinian or RA channel). We tested whether low-frequency discharge in FAII afferents evoked a clear identifiable percept of frequency and whether it was analogous to that evoked by sinusoidal stimuli within flutter range that primarily activates FAI afferents. Finally, we evaluated frequency discrimination capacity mediated by FAII afferents.

Results

Detection thresholds mediated by FAII afferents at low frequencies

Detection thresholds for pulsatile stimuli (Figure 1b) evoking low-frequency discharge exclusively in FAII afferents (the Pacinian channel) were measured at two frequencies within the flutter range (6 and 24 Hz) and for comparison at two frequencies in the vibratory hum range (100 and 200 Hz). For pulsatile stimuli, the detection thresholds on the finger were low at all frequencies: 1.3 (± 0.6 mean ± SD; n = 6) μm at the lowest (6 Hz) frequency and 0.7 (± 0.2; n = 6) μm at the highest (200 Hz) (Figure 1a). Regardless of the frequency, the perceptual thresholds for pulsatile stimuli were well below response threshold for FAI afferents (Johansson et al., 1982; Saal et al., 2017; Talbot et al., 1968), and thus could only have been mediated by the FAII afferents through the Pacinian channel. The detection thresholds for sinusoidal stimuli (Figure 1b) were considerably higher within flutter range frequencies and, as expected, steeply decreased with increasing frequency from 28 (± 6; n = 6) μm at 6 Hz to 0.7 (± 0.2; n = 6) μm at 200 Hz (Figure 1a). This reflects a shift from activation of FAI afferents, which have thresholds around 10–15 μm even at their characteristic frequencies, to activation of the much more sensitive FAII afferents which have thresholds for sinusoidal stimulation below 1 μm at their characteristic frequencies (Johansson et al., 1982; Saal et al., 2017; Talbot et al., 1968).

Detection thresholds.

(A) Vibrotactile detection thresholds on the finger across frequency ranges for sinusoidal and pulsatile stimuli (n = 12). Shaded area represent ± 95% confidence intervals. (B) An example of the sinusoidal and pulsatile waveforms.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.46510.002

Perceptual properties of low-frequency discharge rate in Pacinian channel

We next examined whether the Pacinian channel is capable of conveying a sense of vibration frequency within the flutter range. To do this, the amplitude of the pulsatile vibrotactile stimuli was kept at the level of 3 µm regardless of repetition rate (frequency), which is well below the activation thresholds of FAI afferents. The amplitudes for comparison sine waves were selected after conducting intensity matching to the 3 µm pulsatile stimulus in pilot experiments and are presented in Table 1 of the Materials and methods. Given variability in intensity matching between subjects, we verified that frequency judgements were insensitive to amplitude changes (Figure 2—figure supplement 1). The apparent frequency of a 20 or 40 Hz FAII-driven pulsatile stimulus was obtained from participants’ comparisons of the pulsatile (P) and sinusoidal (S) stimuli in the following combinations PP, SP, with SS as a control (Figure 2). From these comparisons, we calculated the point of subjective equality (PSE) of frequency. The physical frequency defined as repetition rate for pulsatile or frequency for sinusoidal stimuli used as test stimulus was either 20 or 40 Hz.

Figure 2 with 1 supplement see all
Point of subjective equality (PSE) obtained using two interval forced choice paradigm.

The test stimulus was either sinusoidal or pulsatile presented at 20 Hz and 40 Hz. The test stimulus was compared with a range of comparison frequencies: 10, 14, 18, 22, 26, 30 Hz with 20 Hz test stimulus; and 25, 31, 37, 43, 49, 55 Hz with 40 Hz test stimulus. Black horizontal lines represent mean ± 95% confidence intervals (n = 12).

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.46510.004
Figure 2—source data 1

PSE values for individual subjects.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.46510.006
Table 1
Amplitudes and frequencies used in each experimental condition.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.46510.007
ConditionTest stimulusComparison stimulusComparison frequencies
PP20 Hz, Pulsatile, 3 µm
40 Hz, Pulsatile, 3 µm
Pulsatile, 3 µm
Pulsatile, 3 µm
10, 14, 18, 22, 26, 30 Hz
25, 31, 37, 43, 49, 55 Hz
SS20 Hz, Sinusoidal, 150 µm
40 Hz, Sinusoidal, 40 µm
Sinusoidal, 150 µm
Sinusoidal, 40 µm
10, 14, 18, 22, 26, 30 Hz
25, 31, 37, 43, 49, 55 Hz
SP20 Hz, Sinusoidal, 150 µm
40 Hz, Sinusoidal, 40 µm
Pulsatile, 3 µm
Pulsatile, 3 µm
10, 14, 18, 22, 26, 30 Hz
25, 31, 37, 43, 49, 55 Hz

FAII afferent activation in flutter range creates frequency percept

In the PP condition, participants compared the frequency of a pulsatile test stimulus with that of six pulsatile comparison stimuli. The PSEs obtained from the psychometric curves were very close to the physical frequencies of the presented test stimuli: 20.0 (19.5–20.5; 95% confidence interval, CI) Hz for the 20 Hz test, and 40.8 (39.8–41.7) Hz for the 40 Hz test stimulus (Figure 2; n = 12). The narrow CI values indicate that pulsatile stimuli evoked perceptions with a well-defined frequency. The PSE values obtained with sinusoidal stimuli (SS condition) using six sinusoidal comparison frequencies were 19.5 (18.8–20.2) Hz for the 20 Hz test and 41.7 (39.8–43.7) Hz for the 40 Hz test stimulus (Figure 2; n = 12). Repeated measures two-way ANOVA indicated no difference between types of stimuli (pulsatile or sinusoidal) used (F(1, 11)=0.131, p=0.72).

Frequency percept mediated by low-frequency discharge in FAII afferents is analogous to that evoked by sinusoidal stimuli

The PSE for the 20 Hz pulsatile test stimulus was 22.0 (19.7–24.4; 95% CI) Hz when determined in comparison to six sinusoidal frequencies (PS condition), which was no different from the 20 Hz stimulus (p=0.09, n = 12; one sample two-tailed t-test). This indicates that both pulsatile and sinusoidal low-frequency stimuli generate a percept of identical frequency within the flutter frequency range. For 40 Hz pulsatile test stimulus, the PSE assessed in comparison with sinusoidal stimuli was 43.6 (39.8–47.4; 95% CI) Hz; again, this was not different from 40 Hz (p=0.06, n = 12; one sample two-tailed t-test).

Frequency discrimination capacity mediated by FAII afferents within the flutter range

Weber fractions that were mediated exclusively by FAII afferents within the flutter frequency range were just as low as the Weber fractions determined with sinusoidal stimuli mediated predominantly by FAI afferents (Figure 3). Two-way repeated measures ANOVA indicated that the FAII afferents provided frequency discrimination in the flutter range that was no different from sinusoidal stimuli predominantly mediated by FAI afferents (F(1, 11)=0.004, p=0.949). However, there was an effect of frequency (F(1, 11)=29.00, p=0.0002) indicating that the size of the Weber fraction is affected by frequency and not afferent type providing this input. Weber fractions were lower at the higher frequency (40 Hz) than they were at 20 Hz for both pulsatile and sinusoidal stimuli (0.21 vs 0.14 and 0.19 vs 0.15, respectively; n = 12).

The Weber's fraction of just noticeable difference in frequency.

For details refer to legend of Figure 2.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.46510.008

Discussion

Our study provides strong evidence that low-frequency discharge of FAII afferents providing input to the Pacinian channel can mediate a clear perception of vibration with easily identifiable and distinguishable frequency characteristics within the flutter range. We also established that frequency perception signalled exclusively by the activity of FAII afferents is directly comparable with the perceptual features of flutter frequency sensation evoked by corresponding sinusoidal stimuli that naturally activate predominantly FAI afferents (non-Pacinian channel). As the afferent type and thus spatial properties of the population activation evoking this sensation (FAII afferents with large receptive fields) are different from that evoking the same frequency sense with low-amplitude sinusoidal stimulation (FAI afferents with small localised receptive fields), there are likely to be some differences in the quality of sensation beyond the frequency quality. Differences may also relate to the higher synchrony of activation across afferent fibres induced with pulsatile stimuli than with sinusoidal stimuli, due to their tighter time-envelope.

To test whether there are inherent differences in neural mechanisms involved in frequency analysis via the Pacinian and non-Pacinian channels, frequency discrimination ability was tested. The Weber fraction is generally used to characterise the smallest frequency differences reliably detected by subjects as a fraction of the comparison frequency. The Weber fraction is around 0.2 in the flutter range and at high frequencies has been reported to have a slightly higher value of around 0.3; however there is significant variation depending on methodology used (Bensmaïa et al., 2005; Goble and Hollins, 1994). The discriminative ability of the Pacinian channel was not previously reported at frequencies outside the high-frequency range with which it is normally associated in vibrotactile perception. The Weber fractions determined for the perceived frequency of vibration driven via the FAII and the FAI channels were a close match at both frequencies tested (20 and 40 Hz), and show a similar decrease from 20 to 40 Hz. This suggests that common neural mechanisms of frequency discrimination might be exploited by both channels, as the existence of independent mechanisms having such close agreement at frequencies outside the usual operating range of one of the channels seems less likely.

Recent psychophysical evidence demonstrated that there is interaction between the FAI and FAII inputs, by showing the assimilation effect, where a frequency in the range of one channel can influence perceived frequency on the other channel (Kuroki et al., 2017). We suggest that our data extends this, and represents evidence of the functional consequence of the recently discovered extensive convergence of FAI- and FAII- derived inputs onto S1 cortical neurons (Carter et al., 2014; Pei et al., 2009; Saal et al., 2015). It is known that about 70–80% of rapidly adapting neurons in somatosensory cortex S1 show convergent inputs deriving from both afferent classes, and it may be that these neurons are responsible for this generalised frequency processing. Saal et al. (2015) made an interesting observation that input from FAI afferents determines the cortical neuron response rate due to its net excitatory drive, while the more temporally-precise PC-channel has a balanced excitatory-inhibitory drive that can control the precise spike timing, which is useful in various encoding schemes (Birznieks and Vickery, 2017; Andrew Hires et al., 2015; Johansson and Birznieks, 2004; Prsa and Huber, 2018; Saal et al., 2016). This difference did not appear to affect frequency perception in the current study, as Weber fractions were found to be similar regardless of whether we used pulsatile stimuli exclusively activating FAII afferents in a time-controlled manner or used sinusoidal stimuli predominantly activating FAI afferents, presumably with less temporal precision due to the slow rising phase of the sinusoid.

A consequence of generalised neural processing for frequency, regardless of the source of afferent input, is that it would support constancy of vibrotactile frequency perception across different skin regions innervated by different afferent types. For example, the frequency perception on the hairy skin of the arm is not noticeably different from that of the glabrous skin (Mahns et al., 2006; McIntyre et al., 2016), despite it having neither Meissner (FAI) nor Pacinian (FAII) receptors; instead vibrotactile stimuli are signalled by field units and hair follicle units (Vallbo et al., 1995). This also accords with natural stimulation, which is often of a sufficiently high amplitude to activate multiple types of tactile afferents (Johansson et al., 1982), and activate receptors across different skin types.

Functionally, it means that FAII afferents and the Pacinian channel are well suited for detecting fast discrete mechanical transients with low repetition rate as might arise during object manipulation or exploration of surfaces with sparsely distributed sharp asperities or ridges. The evidence that low-frequency signals arising from FAII afferents are consciously perceived and easily discriminated strongly suggests that they are biologically important and are likely to be utilised by neural circuits dedicated to motor control of the hand. In regard to new technology development, the exquisite sensitivity of FAII afferents combined with their role in tactile perception and motor control makes them a useful target when designing haptic and teleoperated devices.

Conclusions

In this study we obtained evidence that low-frequency spike trains in FAII afferents (Pacinian channel) can readily induce a vibratory percept with the same low frequency attributes as signalled by FAI afferents (Meissner’s, non-Pacinian channel). It has become evident that perception of vibrotactile frequency depends on the discharge pattern of the active afferents, rather than the afferent type that is active. Low frequency spike trains in FAII afferents can induce a vibratory percept which has the same frequency attributes as that induced by sinusoidal stimuli. These new findings raise questions about whether much of the observed functional dichotomy between Pacinian and non-Pacinian channels relates to behavioural interpretations of the stimulus rather than to the type of receptor that the signal originates from. In addition, our proposed universal frequency decoding system would help explain the perceptual constancy of vibrotactile frequency perception which is a prominent problem in tactile system where distinct human skin regions and types (e.g. glabrous and hairy) functionally encode the same physical features of stimuli using remarkably different receptor types tuned for different stimulus features.

These findings are consistent with the growing evidence of extensive convergence of inputs from different afferent types onto neurons in the primary somatosensory cortex. Finally, these findings indicate the need to review the functional and neurophysiological basis on which processing of vibrotactile stimuli is attributed to Pacinian and non-Pacinian channels.

Materials and methods

Subjects

Research participants were healthy volunteers aged 20 to 26 years without any known history of neurological disorders which would affect the somatosensory system. Ethics approval was obtained from the UNSW Human Research Ethics Committee, and all participants signed a consent form. The participants were reimbursed for their time. There were six participants (four female) in the detection threshold experiments and 12 (6 female) in the frequency perception experiments. Five participants were in both experiments. The sample size was determined by pilot studies to determine effect size, and according to accepted practice in psychophysical experiments. No individual subjects or data outliers were excluded from the data analyses.

Apparatus

The mechanical stimulation probe was a metal ball 5 mm in diameter at the end of a metal rod driven by a V4 shaker (Data Physics, San Jose). To drive the shaker, analogue output signals were amplified by a Signalforce 30W Power Amplifier (Data physics, San Jose, USA). The displacement of the stimulation probe was monitored using an OptocoNCDT 2200–10 laser displacement sensor (Micro-Epsilon, Ortenburg, Germany) with a resolution of 0.15 μm at 10 kHz.

Stimulus delivery was controlled by a CED data acquisition system (CED, Cambridge, UK) consisting of hardware (CED Power 1401 MkII) and software (Spike2 7.07). Custom made Spike2 and MATLAB (MathWorks Inc, Natick, MA) scripts were used to control the delivery of pulsatile and sinusoidal stimuli, and to record stimulus measurements and button presses made by the participant.

The stimuli were delivered to the finger pad of the right index finger. The arm, hand and stimulated finger were positioned and held in place with the aid of a vacuum pillow (GermaProtec, Kristianstad, Sweden). The pillow, filled with small foam balls, was moulded around the participant’s arm, and the air was then pumped out to hold its shape. The probe was positioned on the finger with a force of 50 g; the probe protracted from this rest position. White noise was delivered through headphones to eliminate auditory cues associated with the mechanical stimulator. Participants made responses by pressing buttons with the unstimulated hand.

Vibration stimulus

Request a detailed protocol

A stereotyped brief pulsatile mechanical stimulus with a protraction time of only 2 ms was used to control the spiking pattern in recruited afferents (Figure 1a). As the duration of the mechanical stimulus was comparable to the refractory period of the action potential, each mechanical stimulation event generated only a single time-controlled spike in responding afferents (Birznieks and Vickery, 2017). Each mechanical pulse is a reproducible and uniform event which ensures that the same population of afferents will be excited regardless of the rate at which these pulses are repeated.

Detection thresholds

Request a detailed protocol

Detection thresholds were measured for pulsatile and sinusoidal stimuli at four frequencies: 6, 24, 100 and 200 Hz. Thresholds were determined on the fingertips. All together thresholds were tested in 16 conditions (2 waveforms x 4 frequencies x 2 locations). The thresholds for two types of stimuli were measured together in a single session with their trials pseudo-randomly interleaved. Each testing session lasted about 10 min, with a total of 8 sessions for each participant.

To measure detection thresholds, we used a two-interval forced-choice (2IFC) procedure, where in each trial participants were presented with two time intervals, indicated with audio cues (Figure 4a). The intervals were each 1 s long, with a 0.5 s gap in between. One interval contained the vibration, and the other did not. Participants had to indicate which interval, the first or the second, contained the vibration stimulus. The interval containing the stimulus varied randomly, with each containing the stimulus equally often throughout the experiment.

Experimental protocols.

(A) Structure of the detection threshold task. (B) Structure of the frequency perception task. (C) Point of subjective equality (PSE) determined on the psychometric curve.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.46510.010

To calculate detection thresholds, we used the QUEST package implemented in Psychtoolbox-3 (http://psychtoolbox.org) for MATLAB. We defined the threshold as the intensity at which the stimulus could be correctly identified for 82% of trials, and was given by the mean of the posterior distribution function. For each threshold estimate, 41 trials were conducted. To determine the amplitude of the vibration to present on each trial, we used a Bayesian adaptive QUEST protocol (Watson and Pelli, 1983), operating on the log-transformed amplitudes. The prior threshold estimate depended on the waveform and frequency (80 µm for 6 Hz sinusoidal, 5 µm for 24 Hz sinusoidal and 3 µm for 100 and 200 Hz sinusoidal, and for all pulsatile stimuli). The amplitude of the vibration on each trial was determined by the QUEST algorithm in most cases. The exceptions were the first trial, which was fixed at the prior threshold estimate for that stimulus, and every tenth trial, which was three times the value suggested by QUEST, to give participants a few easy trials. The actual amplitude delivered was measured and this value, along with the participant’s response, was returned to the QUEST algorithm on each trial.

Frequency perception

Request a detailed protocol

To measure frequency perception, we used a similar 2IFC procedure as described above. However, in this case, both stimulus intervals presented to the participant contained vibration stimulation. The participant was required to indicate which of the two intervals contained the vibration with the highest frequency, the first or the second. One interval contained the test stimulus of fixed frequency, and the other contained the comparison stimulus, which varied in frequency from trial to trial (Figure 4b). Six comparison frequencies were paired with the test stimulus 20 times each, in a random sequence.

Frequency perception was tested in three experimental conditions, with different combinations of stimulus waveforms: PP, in which a test stimulus with a pulsatile waveform was compared to comparison stimuli with pulsatile waveforms; SS, in which a sinusoidal test was compared to sinusoidal comparison stimuli; and SP, in which a sinusoidal test was compared to pulsatile comparison stimuli. Each condition was tested with two test stimuli of different frequencies: 20 and 40 Hz. The features of the stimuli used in the experiment are fully described in Table 1. For each condition, we randomly interleaved the trials from the 20 and 40 Hz tests. Data collecting from each participant was divided into six sessions lasting approximately 10 min each.

Custom MATLAB scripts were used to analyse the frequency perception data (McIntyre, 2019; copy archived at https://github.com/elifesciences-publications/touch-frequency-perception). Logistic regression was applied to the data to produce the psychometric function, relating the frequency of the comparison stimulus to the proportion of trials that the participant said the comparison was a higher frequency than the test. From the psychometric function, we calculated measures of both the perceived frequency of the test stimulus, and the frequency discrimination sensitivity. The perceived frequency of the test stimulus is given by the point of subjective equality (PSE), the comparison frequency at the 50% point on the psychometric curve (Figure 4c) (Birznieks and Vickery, 2017). The PSE is the point where the participant is equally likely to say the comparison frequency is higher or lower than the test. Discrimination sensitivity is given by the Weber fraction, the one half difference between the 75% point and the 25% on the psychometric function, divided by the frequency of the test stimulus (LaMotte and Mountcastle, 1975).

The amplitude of all pulsatile stimuli was 3 µm which was approximately three times the sensory threshold found in our detection threshold experiment. At this amplitude, we expect only Pacinian (FAII) afferents to respond, as threshold for recruitment of FAI afferents, even at their preferred frequency within the flutter range, is no lower than 10 µm. The shape (acceleration profile) of the pulsatile stimulus used in this study corresponds to a waveform of sinusoidal stimulus > 250 Hz. For stimuli at this frequency, FAI afferents typically respond at amplitudes about 10 fold higher than the 3 µm stimuli used for selective FAII afferent activation in this study (Freeman and Johnson, 1982; Johansson et al., 1982; Saal et al., 2017; Talbot et al., 1968). The response of one FAII afferent recorded by microneurography in a human subject, evoked by 3 μm pulsatile stimuli at 20 imp/s, is shown in Figure 5a. For comparison, the response of one FAI afferent to 20 Hz sinusoidal stimuli also responding at 20 imp/s is shown in Figure 5b. The response thresholds with pulsatile stimuli are shown in Figure 5c–e. The FAI afferent starts responding sporadically at 30 μm and becomes entrained 1:1 with a stimulus amplitude of 35 μm.

Afferent responses with 20 Hz stimuli.

(A) FAII afferent response to pulsatile stimuli 3 µm in amplitude. (B) FAI afferent response to sinusoidal stimuli 150 µm in amplitude. (C–D) The same FAI afferent as in B, response to pulsatile stimuli at various amplitudes: no response with 25 µm (subthreshold) stimulus; sporadic firing at 30 µm; and entrainment at 35 µm. Note that firing pattern in A, B and E is identical regardless of stimulus or afferent type.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.46510.011

For the sinusoidal stimuli, we chose amplitudes such that the perceived intensity of all stimuli were approximately equal (see Table 1). Given that there is inter- and intra- subject variability, we verified that intensity cues were not used for frequency judgements. We conducted control experiments in five subjects (n = 5) who conducted the same experimental protocol illustrated in Figures 2 and 3 at 40 Hz, but with two different amplitudes (randomly interleaved) for the comparison frequencies. For the sinusoidal comparisons, the standard was 60 µm, and the comparisons were 40 µm (sine low) and 90 µm (sine high). For the pulsatile comparisons, the standard was 6 µm, and the comparisons were 3 µm (pulse low) and 10 µm (pulse high). The data in Figure 2—figure supplement 1 show that amplitude of the sinusoidal or pulsatile stimuli had no effect on PSE or Weber’s fractions indicating that subjects can readily judge frequency while ignoring any intensity cues.

Statistical analysis

Request a detailed protocol

One sample two-tailed t-test (n = 12) was used to test whether the PSE obtained in psychophysics experiments using either pulsatile or sinusoidal stimuli in 12 subjects rendered the same result as physical frequency of the periodic mechanical stimulus of the same type. In this test, PSE obtained by comparing pulsatile stimulus (test stimulus) with sinusoidal stimuli (comparison stimuli) was compared to the expected PSE if test and comparison stimuli would be of the same type (sinusoidal).

Two-way repeated measures ANOVA was performed to analyse the effects on PSE by two repeated measures (within subject; n = 12) factors: type of stimulus (pulsatile, sinusoidal) and frequency (20 Hz, 40 Hz). Two-way repeated measures ANOVA was performed to analyse the effects on Weber’s fraction (frequency discrimination capacity) by two repeated measures (within subject; n = 12) factors: type of stimulus (pulsatile, sinusoidal) and frequency (20 Hz, 40 Hz).

For statistical analyses on the calculated thresholds, PSEs and Weber fractions, and for generating graphs, GraphPad Prism software was used (GraphPad Software Inc, La Jolla).

Data availability

Data analysed during this study are included in the manuscript and supporting files. Source data files have been provided for Figures 1, 2 and 3.

References

    1. Gescheider GA
    (1976)
    Evidence in support of the duplex theory of mechanoreception
    Sensory Processes 1:68–76.
  1. Book
    1. McIntyre S
    2. Birznieks I
    3. Andersson R
    4. Dicander G
    5. Breen PP
    6. Vickery RM
    (2016) Temporal Integration of Tactile Inputs from Multiple Sites
    In: Bello F, Kajimoto H, Visell Y, editors. Haptics: Perception, Devices, Control, and Applications: 10th International Conference, EuroHaptics 2016. London: Springer International Publishing. pp. 204–213.
    https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-42321-0_19
    1. Vallbo AB
    2. Johansson RS
    (1984)
    Properties of cutaneous mechanoreceptors in the human hand related to touch sensation
    Human Neurobiology 3:3–14.

Article and author information

Author details

  1. Ingvars Birznieks

    1. School of Medical Sciences, Faculty of Medicine, UNSW Sydney, Sydney, Australia
    2. Neuroscience Research Australia, Sydney, Australia
    3. Biomedical Engineering and Neuroscience, MARCS Institute, Western Sydney University, Sydney, Australia
    Contribution
    Conceptualization, Resources, Software, Formal analysis, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Visualization, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing
    For correspondence
    i.birznieks@unsw.edu.au
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0003-4916-1254
  2. Sarah McIntyre

    1. Neuroscience Research Australia, Sydney, Australia
    2. Biomedical Engineering and Neuroscience, MARCS Institute, Western Sydney University, Sydney, Australia
    3. Linköping University, Linköping, Sweden
    Contribution
    Conceptualization, Software, Formal analysis, Supervision, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing—review and editing
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
  3. Hanna Maria Nilsson

    1. Neuroscience Research Australia, Sydney, Australia
    2. Linköping University, Linköping, Sweden
    Contribution
    Formal analysis, Investigation, Visualization, Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
  4. Saad S Nagi

    1. Linköping University, Linköping, Sweden
    2. School of Medicine, Western Sydney University, Sydney, Australia
    Contribution
    Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Writing—review and editing
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0001-8773-8232
  5. Vaughan G Macefield

    1. Neuroscience Research Australia, Sydney, Australia
    2. School of Medicine, Western Sydney University, Sydney, Australia
    3. The Baker Heart and Diabetes Institute, Melbourne, Australia
    Contribution
    Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Writing—review and editing
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
  6. David A Mahns

    School of Medicine, Western Sydney University, Sydney, Australia
    Contribution
    Formal analysis, Supervision, Methodology, Writing—review and editing
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
  7. Richard M Vickery

    1. School of Medical Sciences, Faculty of Medicine, UNSW Sydney, Sydney, Australia
    2. Neuroscience Research Australia, Sydney, Australia
    Contribution
    Conceptualization, Resources, Software, Formal analysis, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Visualization, Methodology, Writing—review and editing
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared

Funding

National Health and Medical Research Council (APP1028284)

  • Ingvars Birznieks
  • Vaughan G Macefield
  • Richard M Vickery

Australian Research Council (DP170100064)

  • Ingvars Birznieks

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC) project grant to IB, RMV, and VGM. We would like to thank Mr Kevin KW Ng for help conducting control experiments investigating the effect of amplitude on frequency perception.

Ethics

Human subjects: Ethics approval was obtained from the UNSW Human Research Ethics Committee HC11074, HC16245 and all participants signed a consent form.

Copyright

© 2019, Birznieks et al.

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.

Metrics

  • 2,376
    views
  • 288
    downloads
  • 43
    citations

Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Ingvars Birznieks
  2. Sarah McIntyre
  3. Hanna Maria Nilsson
  4. Saad S Nagi
  5. Vaughan G Macefield
  6. David A Mahns
  7. Richard M Vickery
(2019)
Tactile sensory channels over-ruled by frequency decoding system that utilizes spike pattern regardless of receptor type
eLife 8:e46510.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.46510

Share this article

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.46510

Further reading

    1. Neuroscience
    Bhanu Shrestha, Jiun Sang ... Youngseok Lee
    Research Article

    Sour taste, which is elicited by low pH, may serve to help animals distinguish appetitive from potentially harmful food sources. In all species studied to date, the attractiveness of oral acids is contingent on concentration. Many carboxylic acids are attractive at ecologically relevant concentrations but become aversive beyond some maximal concentration. Recent work found that Drosophila ionotropic receptors IR25a and IR76b expressed by sweet-responsive gustatory receptor neurons (GRNs) in the labellum, a peripheral gustatory organ, mediate appetitive feeding behaviors toward dilute carboxylic acids. Here, we disclose the existence of pharyngeal sensors in Drosophila melanogaster that detect ingested carboxylic acids and are also involved in the appetitive responses to carboxylic acids. These pharyngeal sensors rely on IR51b, IR94a, and IR94h, together with IR25a and IR76b, to drive responses to carboxylic acids. We then demonstrate that optogenetic activation of either Ir94a+ or Ir94h+ GRNs promotes an appetitive feeding response, confirming their contributions to appetitive feeding behavior. Our discovery of internal pharyngeal sour taste receptors opens up new avenues for investigating the internal sensation of tastants in insects.

    1. Neuroscience
    Rossella Conti, Céline Auger
    Research Article

    Granule cells of the cerebellum make up to 175,000 excitatory synapses on a single Purkinje cell, encoding the wide variety of information from the mossy fibre inputs into the cerebellar cortex. The granule cell axon is made of an ascending portion and a long parallel fibre extending at right angles, an architecture suggesting that synapses formed by the two segments of the axon could encode different information. There are controversial indications that ascending axon (AA) and parallel fibre (PF) synapse properties and modalities of plasticity are different. We tested the hypothesis that AA and PF synapses encode different information, and that the association of these distinct inputs to Purkinje cells might be relevant to the circuit and trigger plasticity, similar to the coincident activation of PF and climbing fibre inputs. Here, by recording synaptic currents in Purkinje cells from either proximal or distal granule cells (mostly AA and PF synapses, respectively), we describe a new form of associative plasticity between these two distinct granule cell inputs. We show for the first time that synchronous AA and PF repetitive train stimulation, with inhibition intact, triggers long-term potentiation (LTP) at AA synapses specifically. Furthermore, the timing of the presentation of the two inputs controls the outcome of plasticity and induction requires NMDAR and mGluR1 activation. The long length of the PFs allows us to preferentially activate the two inputs independently, and despite a lack of morphological reconstruction of the connections, these observations reinforce the suggestion that AA and PF synapses have different coding capabilities and plasticity that is associative, enabling effective association of information transmitted via granule cells.