The effects of psychosocial stress on dopaminergic function and the acute stress response
Abstract
Chronic psychosocial adversity induces vulnerability to mental illnesses. Animal studies demonstrate that this may be mediated by dopaminergic dysfunction. We therefore investigated whether long-term exposure to psychosocial adversity was associated with dopamine dysfunction and its relationship to psychological and physiological responses to acute stress. Using 3,4-dihydroxy-6-[18F]-fluoro-l-phenylalanine ([18F]-DOPA) positron emission tomography (PET), we compared dopamine synthesis capacity in n = 17 human participants with high cumulative exposure to psychosocial adversity with n = 17 age- and sex-matched participants with low cumulative exposure. The PET scan took place 2 hr after the induction of acute psychosocial stress using the Montréal Imaging Stress Task to induce acute psychosocial stress. We found that dopamine synthesis correlated with subjective threat and physiological response to acute psychosocial stress in the low exposure group. Long-term exposure to psychosocial adversity was associated with dampened striatal dopaminergic function (p=0.03, d = 0.80) and that psychosocial adversity blunted physiological yet potentiated subjective responses to acute psychosocial stress. Future studies should investigate the roles of these changes in vulnerability to mental illnesses.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.46797.001Introduction
Chronic psychosocial adversity increases the risk of mental illnesses including schizophrenia and depression (van Os et al., 2010; Howes and Murray, 2014; Parker, 1983). These adverse factors include developmental psychological trauma (Bendall et al., 2008) and adult life events (situations or occurrences that bring about a negative change in personal circumstances and involve threat) (Beards et al., 2013; Brown and Birley, 1968). Several lines of evidence indicate a potential causative component to these relationships as these risk exposures demonstrate dose-response relationships (Janssen et al., 2004; Morgan and Fisher, 2007; Pedersen and Mortensen, 2001). Reverse causality in the form of recall bias does not appear to be driving these associations (Cutajar et al., 2010), and the cessation of stressor reduces the risk of illness (Kelleher et al., 2013). However, we lack a precise mechanistic understanding of how exposure to these risk factors induces vulnerability to mental illness, and why these exposures all increase risk. Understanding this is important to identify targets for prevention and novel treatment. One common component underlying these factors is exposure to psychosocial stress (Howes and Murray, 2014) via activation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis and the sympathetic nervous system as part of the normal biological stress response (Taylor, 2010).
The striatum is functionally connected to the threat detection system (Haber, 2014). Animal research has demonstrated that acute stressors including aversive stimuli induce a pronounced activation of the dopamine system in terms of dopamine neuron population activity (i.e. the numbers of neurons firing) and with regard to amphetamine-induced behaviours (Valenti et al., 2011). Long-lasting changes in dopamine function occur after single stress exposures, including altered responsivity to future stimulation (Holly and Miczek, 2016) in a manner similar to that induced by drugs of abuse (Saal et al., 2003) and such that stress plays a powerful role in the initiation, escalation, and relapse to drug abuse via dopaminergic mechanisms (Koob and Volkow, 2016).
In humans, childhood sexual abuse is associated with elevated urinary dopamine metabolites in childhood (De Bellis et al., 1994) and acute psychosocial stressors induce greater dopamine release in people with low self-reported maternal care (Pruessner et al., 2004). Stress-induced elevations in cortisol levels have been directly correlated with amphetamine-induced dopamine release (Wand et al., 2007) on the one hand, while corticotrophin-releasing hormone administration results in dopamine release on the other (Payer et al., 2017). In terms of long-term exposure, using fMRI in humans, there is evidence that institutional neglect is associated with reduced striatal reward function, which is mediated by the dopamine system (Mehta et al., 2010), and similar findings have been observed in prospective cohorts following childhood adversity (Dillon et al., 2009) suggestive of a possible causal relationship between childhood adversity and alterations of the dopamine system. Furthermore, maltreatment-associated reduced striatal function is associated with adverse outcomes including disrupted attachment (Takiguchi et al., 2015) and depression (Hanson et al., 2015). One study (Oswald et al., 2014) found positive associations between childhood trauma and amphetamine-induced dopamine release, which may be due to the phenomenon of cross-sensitization. Furthermore, within people who are at ultra-high clinical risk of psychosis raised dopamine synthesis capacity has been reported in those patients with high levels of childhood adversity (Egerton et al., 2016). In light of these findings, we wanted to examine the relationships between the autonomic, endocrine and subjective threat responses to an acute psychosocial stressor and dopaminergic function.
The relationships between neurobiological pathways and stress-induced physiological and subjective responses have yet to be fully elucidated in humans. Studies of psychosocial stressors and dopamine function have typically investigated risk factors in isolation, despite the fact that the risk factors cluster together and may share common underlying mechanisms (Hjern et al., 2004; Morgan and Fisher, 2007; Wicks et al., 2005). Since associations between one exposure and outcomes remain after controlling for the other exposures (Schäfer and Fisher, 2011), it is likely that additive effects and/or synergistic effects operate between risk factors (Morgan et al., 2014; Guloksuz et al., 2015; Lataster et al., 2012; Morgan et al., 2008; Schäfer and Fisher, 2011). Furthermore, there is evidence that childhood trauma increases risk of psychopathology in response to adult stressors (McLaughlin et al., 2010). It is also likely that ethnic minority status can increase the risk of psychopathology through social isolation, experiences of discrimination, victimisation and social defeat, which are all considered stressors (Morgan et al., 2010; Bécares et al., 2009; Cooper et al., 2008; Cooper et al., 2017; Selten and Cantor-Graae, 2005; Selten et al., 2012; Sharpley et al., 2001; Tidey and Miczek, 1996). Cognitive models propose that minority status is associated with greater levels of social threat (Combs et al., 2002; Morgan and Fisher, 2007). Indeed we have found evidence that black minority ethnic status is associated with greater amygdalar activation to out-group (i.e. white faces) than vice versa, neurobiological correlates of these theories/findings (McCutcheon et al., 2018) (McCutcheon et al., 2018). Taken with findings that experiences of racism are correlated with amygdala activation to white faces in black individuals (Greer et al., 2012), this suggests that ethnic minority status is associated with functional alterations in the brain circuits involved in threat processing. As minority ethnicity status has been found to be a chronic stressor and increase the risk of mental illness via chronic stress rather than a genetic component (Akdeniz et al., 2014), we included minority ethnic status as a stressor. The combined effect of the risk factors on dopamine function in humans is unknown. Furthermore, previous studies (Egerton et al., 2016; Oswald et al., 2014; Pruessner et al., 2004) in humans have investigated childhood factors alone. Given animal evidence that exposure to mild stressors potentiate dopaminergic activity whilst severe chronic stressors is associated with dopaminergic blunting (Holly and Miczek, 2016), a key outstanding question remains - what is the effect of chronic adversity, across both child and adult stages of life, on dopaminergic function? We therefore aimed to investigate the effects of exposures to multiple psychosocial risk factors for psychosis on dopaminergic function and the acute stress response. Given the findings of dopaminergic dysfunction associated with childhood maltreatment presented above, we hypothesised that healthy humans with a high cumulative exposure to psychosocial stressors would have altered striatal dopamine synthesis, compared to humans with a low exposure. We also sought examine the relationship between dopaminergic function and the subjective threat and physiological responses to acute psychosocial stress using the Montréal Imaging Stress Task (MIST), a validated stress task involving mental arithmetic under negative social appraisal (Dedovic et al., 2005; Lederbogen et al., 2011). We sought to measure salivary α-amylase, secreted from the parotid gland in response to adrenergic activity and a marker of stress-induced adrenergic activity (van Stegeren et al., 2006) which is associated with a faster increase during psychosocial stress than salivary cortisol (Maruyama et al., 2012), and mean arterial pressure (MAP), the product of cardiac output and total peripheral resistance, reflecting organ perfusion and providing a physiological measure of sympathetic activation.
Results
Participant characteristics and scan parameters
Seventeen HA participants were recruited to the study. All reported high levels of psychological stress exposure in childhood and adulthood (Table 1). Seventeen LA participants were recruited and, as expected, had significantly lower levels of childhood and adult stressors than the HA group (Table 1). Clinical rating scales are reported in Table 1. HA scored significantly higher than LA on subclinical measures of depressive symptoms (BDI), the degree to which previous stressors were having an impact on their lives in the week prior to scanning (BIE) and aberrant salience (ASI).
There was no significant group difference in the amount of radioactivity injected or specific activity (Table 1). There was no significant difference in whole striatal or subdivision volumes between the groups (Table 1).
Sample characteristics and scan parameters
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.46797.002Sample characteristic | LA ( = 17) | HA ( = 17) | pa | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age, years [mean(SD)] | 27.6 | (7.8) | 29.2 | (7.2) | 0.54 |
Sex, n | nine female, eight male | eight female, nine male | 1.00 | ||
Ethnicity, n | 17 WB | 4 BA, 1BB, 4 BC, 6 ME, 1 OE, 1 WB | <0.001 | ||
Childhood Adversity | |||||
CTQ [mean(SD)] | 3.8 | (5.2) | 15.3 | (16.1) | 0.01 |
Parental loss (parental separation with loss of parental contact and/or death and/or going into foster care and/or being adopted) during childhood, n | 0 | 13 | <0.001 | ||
Childhood sexual abuse | 0 | 6 | 0.02 | ||
Adult Adversity | |||||
Number of adverse life events over last 6 months [mean(SD)] | 0.5 (0.9) | 2.6 (1.9) | 0.001 | ||
Life events score over last 6 months [mean(SD)] | 15.1 (37.0) | 72.3 (55.7) | <0.01 | ||
Clinical Scores | |||||
BDI [mean(SD)] | 2.7 (3.8) | 6.5 (5.6) | 0.03 | ||
BAI [mean(SD)] | 4.8 (6.7) | 9.7 (10.2) | 0.11 | ||
IES-6 [mean(SD)] | 1.7 (2.3) | 7.7 (7.6) | 0.01 | ||
O-LIFE [mean(SD)] | 7.2 (6.5) | 13.1 (9.5) | 0.07 | ||
ASI [mean(SD)] | 5.7 (5.8) | 11.6 (7.5) | 0.02 | ||
Current Drug Usec,d | |||||
Tobacco cigarette smokers in last 3 months (n) | three user, 14 non-users | four users, 13 non-users | 1.00 | ||
Tobacco use in whole sample (cigarettes/day) [mean(SD)] | .4 | (1.5) | 1.7 | (3.6) | 0.19 |
Alcohol use in last 3 months (n) | 15 users, two non-users | 14 users, three non-users | 1.00 | ||
Alcohol use (UK alcohol units/week) [mean(SD)] | 10.2 | (9.0) | 7.0 | (8.9) | 0.30 |
Scan parameter | |||||
Injected dose (MBq) [mean(SD)] | 143.4 | (7.7) | 142.9 | (7.7) | 0.85 |
Specific activity (MBq/µmol) [mean(SD)] | 35.3 | (6.7) | 41.4 | (15.4) | 0.14 |
Whole striatal volume (mm3) [mean(SD)] | 16,842 | (5094) | 15,741 | (4,601) | 0.54 |
Associative striatal volume (mm3) [mean(SD)] | 10,460 | (3202) | 9771 | (2885) | 0.54 |
Limbic striatal volume (mm3) [mean(SD)] | 2005 | (610) | 1897 | (547) | 0.61 |
Sensorimotor striatal volume (mm3) [mean(SD)] | 4375 | (1314) | 4072 | (1189) | 0.51 |
-
Abbreviations: ASI, Aberrant Salience Inventory; BA, black African; BAI; Beck Anxiety Inventory; BB, black British; BC, black Caribbean; BDI, Beck Depression Inventory; CTQ, Childhood Trauma Questionnaire; IES-6, Brief Impact of Events Scale;, mixed ethnicity; OE, other ethnicity; O-LIFE, Oxford-Liverpool Inventory of Feelings and Experiences; SEAT, Social Environment Assessment Tool; WB, White British.
a Independent-samples t-tests for variables with normal data distributions; Mann-Whitney U tests for variables with non-normal data distributions; χ2-tests for dichotomous variables.
-
bGroups were compared on a dichotomised ethnicity variable (white British vs ethnic minority).
c 1 UK alcohol unit = 10 mL (~7.88 g) alcohol.
Striatal dopaminergic function
Kicer was significantly reduced in HA relative to LA in the whole striatum (t32 = 2.27, p=0.03; Figure 1). Secondary analysis in each striatal subdivision showed that this reduction reached significance in the limbic and associative subdivisions (Table 2).

Striatal dopamine synthesis capacity in Low Adversity (LA, n = 17) and High Adversity participants (HA, n = 17).
Dopamine synthesis capacity was significantly reduced in HA compared with LA (t32 = 2.27, p=0.03). Error bars indicate standard errors.
[18F]-DOPA Kicer (min−1) by group
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.46797.004VOI | LA ( = 17) | HA ( = 17) | Group comparisonsa | Effect size | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | (SD) | Mean | (SD) | tdf | p | (Cohen’s D) | |
STR | 0.0133 | (0.0014) | 0.0124 | (0.0013) | 2.2732 | 0.03 | 0.80 |
AST | 0.0133 | (0.0011) | 0.0124 | (0.0010) | 2.2832 | 0.03 | 0.81 |
LST | 0.0140 | (0.0015) | 0.0128 | (0.0010) | 2.6932 | 0.01 | 0.95 |
SMST | 0.0132 | (0.0013) | 0.0125 | (0.0011) | 1.1732 | 0.10 | 0.41 |
-
Abbreviations: AST, associative striatum; LST, limbic striatum; Kicer, influx rate constant; SMST, sensorimotor striatum; STR, whole striatum; VOI, volume of interest.
a Independent-samples t-tests.
As the amount of smoking differed in the groups and heavy smoking can influence dopamine function (Bloomfield et al., 2014; Salokangas et al., 2000), we performed an ANCOVA to examine whether smoking was influencing our findings. When co-varying for amount of current cigarette use, the group difference remained significant in the limbic striatum only (F1,30 = 5.2, p=0.029, η2p=0.15).
Psychosocial stress-induced effects
There were no differences between the groups on subjective or biological measures of stress response at baseline (Table 3) apart from a statistical trend (p=0.07, d = 0.67) towards greater amylase concentrations in the HA group compared to the LA group. Response to psychosocial stress is shown in Table 4 and Figure 2. The HA group showed a heightened subjective response to psychosocial stress evidenced by Threatened (p=0.04, d = 0.86) compared to the LA group. By contrast, the HA showed a blunted physiological response to psychosocial stress, as evidenced by an attenuated increase in mean arterial blood pressure (MAP), p=0.03, d = 0.81 and trend for a lower increase in cortisol (p=0.06, d = 0.69). Some participants had a negative area under the curve due a reduction in salivary cortisol levels associated with the task.
Baseline stress reactivity in Low Adversity (LA) and High Adversity (HA) groups at prior to acute psychosocial stress challenge
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.46797.005Measure | LA ( = 17) | HA ( = 17) | Group comparisons | Effect size | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | (SD) | Mean | (SD) | tdf | P | (Cohen’s D) | |
Threatened (mm) | 6.75 | (15.52) | 4.58 | (6.00) | 0.4626 | 0.65 | 0.18 |
Cortisol (U/mL) | 3.93 | (2.74) | 5.12 | (3.54) | 1.0830 | 0.29 | 0.38 |
Amylase (U/mL) | 178.42 | (173.83) | 92.79 | (53.13) | 1.8330 | 0.07 | 0.67 |
MAP (mmHg) | 89.67 | (9.45) | 90.38 | (9.63) | 0.2129 | 0.84 | 0.07 |
-
Abbreviations: HA, high adversity; LA, low adversity; MAP, mean arterial pressure.
Acute response to psychosocial stress challenge in Low Adversity (LA) and High Adversity (HA) groups
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.46797.006Measure | LA ( = 17) | HA ( = 17) | Group comparisons | Effect size | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | (SD) | Mean | (SD) | tdf | p | (Cohen’s D) | |
Threatened (AUC) | 191.25 | (587.99) | 780.83 | (764.33) | 2.3126 | 0.04 | 0.86 |
Cortisol (AUC) | 122.34 | (156.49) | 11.75 | (166.14) | 1.9430 | 0.06 | 0.69 |
Amylase (AUC) | 1616.67 | (5750.66) | 1015.84 | (2740.24) | 0.3730 | 0.72 | 0.13 |
MAP (AUC) | 153.30 | (90.04) | 79.31 | (92.09) | 2.2629 | 0.03 | 0.81 |
-
Abbreviations: HA, high adversity; LA, low adversity; MAP, mean arterial pressure.

The High Adversity group showed a heightened subjective response and a blunted physiological response.
Panel A shows subjective Threatened responses; Panels B (Cortisol) and C (Mean Arterial Blood Pressure) show physiological response. Data show mean (+ /- SEM).
The relationships between physiological and subjective measures
We conducted correlations between the primary outcome of interest (dopamine synthesis capacity in the whole striatum) and variables showing differences in response to acute psychosocial stress (AUC for threatened, cortisol and mean arterial blood pressure). Extreme bivariate outliers (Cook’s distance >1) were removed (n = 7 from threat, n = 2 from cortisol, n = 5 for MAP). In the low adversity group, striatal dopamine synthesis capacity correlated with psychosocial stress-induced threat (r = 0.73, p=0.001, Figure 3A) and mean arterial blood pressure (r = −0.62, p=0.013, Figure 3B). There were no correlations between striatal dopamine synthesis capacity and measures of acute psychosocial response in the high adversity group.

Correlations between striatal dopamine synthesis capacity and acute response to psychosocial stress.
(A) Whole striatal dopamine synthesis capacity was positively correlated with stress-induced threat in the Low Adversity group (r = 0.73, p=0.001) but not the High Adversity group (r = −0.03, p=0.934). (B). Whole striatal dopamine synthesis capacity was negatively correlated with stress-induced threat and mean arterial blood pressure in the Low Adversity group (r = −0.62, p=0.013) but not the High Adversity group (r = 0.23, p=0.395). Extreme bivariate outliers have been removed from the figures.
As we found that dopamine synthesis capacity is correlated with both subjective and physiological response to stress in LA, we performed a regression analysis to identify which subregion is the best predictor for the physiological measures. Regression analyses did not identify which striatal subregion was the best predictor of the physiological measures (p>0.08).
Discussion
Our main finding is that chronic exposure to psychosocial stressors is associated with significantly reduced striatal dopamine synthesis capacity, particularly in the limbic (ventral) striatum. In addition, we found evidence that striatal dopamine synthesis capacity correlated with both the physiological and subjective responses to an acute psychosocial stressor. Chronic stress exposure is associated with a dissociation between physiological and psychological acute stress responses in the form of an attenuated stress-induced increase in blood pressure alongside a potentiated stress-induced subjective response. These findings support our hypothesis that high cumulative exposure to psychosocial adversity would be associated with altered dopamine synthesis capacity.
Interpretation of findings
Acute stress is associated with increased dopaminergic and autonomic output in animals and humans (Imperato et al., 1989; Wand et al., 2007) and recent human evidence indicates that corticotrophin-releasing hormone administration results in dopamine release (Payer et al., 2017). Animal research indicates that dopaminergic neurons are strongly excited by acute stress and aversive stimuli during adulthood (Brischoux et al., 2009; Cohen et al., 2012; Wenzel et al., 2015; Zweifel et al., 2011). Long-lasting neuroadaptive changes on VTA dopamine neurons have been observed after a single stress exposure, demonstrating that acute stress can alter VTA dopamine neuron responsivity to future stimulation (Saal et al., 2003). Exposure to a single acute stressor can also promote long-lasting neuroplastic changes in VTA dopamine neurons in a manner similar to exposure to recreational drugs (Dong et al., 2004; Graziane et al., 2013; Niehaus et al., 2010; Saal et al., 2003). Our results extend these findings and are consistent with evidence from animal models whereby subcortical dopamine transmission is blunted in response to multiple stressors in adulthood (Chrapusta et al., 1997; Gresch et al., 1994).
Whilst stress exposure in animals during the juvenile period and adolescence has a very different effect from to chronic stress in adulthood, our findings are also broadly consistent with developmental stress models (Brake et al., 2004; Meaney et al., 2002). Likewise, early maternal deprivation models in the very early juvenile period (from post-natal day 5) have been associated with hypodopaminergic behaviours in later life including reduced or attenuated responses to acute stress, conditioned locomotion, locomotor activity, and dopamine agonist-induced locomotion with amphetamine, alongside potentiated dopamine antagonist-induced decreases in anticipatory responses (Matthews et al., 1996; Hall et al., 1999). Exposure to repeated longer-term stressors leads to decreases in striatal dopamine function including nucleus accumbens dopamine output (Mangiavacchi et al., 2001), reduced cocaine-induced nucleus accumbens dopamine release (Shimamoto et al., 2011; Holly and Miczek, 2016), and reduced striatal dopamine receptor availability (Meaney et al., 2002; Brake et al., 2004). One explanation for the group difference is that long-term exposure to psychosocial stress is associated with dopaminergic, particularly in the limbic striatum, autonomic and endocrine downregulation. There is evidence of regional specificity in the direction of effects of acute vs chronic stress exposure. Acute and repeated stress activates the entire dopamine system projecting to much of the striatum (Valenti et al., 2011), in particular the associative (dorsal) striatum where object salience is important, whereas in chronic stress-induced depression (Holly and Miczek, 2016), the blunting occurs primarily in the neurons projecting to the ventromedial striatum (Moreines et al., 2017), where reward-related variables are processed. These are therefore likely to be different systems mediating the dopamine stress response that varies with duration of stress exposure, and with the induction of anxiety (acute or repeated stress) vs. depression (chronic stress). Our findings are consistent with Koob's opponent process model, where acute stress activates the dopamine system, which upon chronic exposure leads to a compensatory downregulation (Koob et al., 1997).
However, this interpretation is not consistent with all findings (Butterfield et al., 1999; Tidey and Miczek, 1996). These discrepant findings are likely to reflect differences in the stress paradigm employed as mild stressors tend to potentiate dopamine function and severe/chronic stressors tend to reduce activity (Holly and Miczek, 2016), which could be consistent with an adaptive role for dopamine in response to mild stressors, but chronic uncontrollable stress hijacking this system. For example, maternal deprivation and isolation of neonatal rats was associated with increased dopamine release (Hall et al., 1999; Kosten et al., 2003), whilst unavoidable stress administered over one week and three weeks was associated with a decrease in nucleus accumbens dopamine output (Mangiavacchi et al., 2001). Likewise, rats under a 10-day episodic defeat paradigm had a sensitised dopamine response in the nucleus accumbens, whilst when under a 5-week continuous subordination paradigm they exhibited a suppressed dopamine response (Miczek et al., 2011).
Our findings are consistent with a fMRI study with found that adolescents who had suffered severe early life deprivation exhibited ventral striatal hyporesponsivity during anticipation of monetary reward (Mehta et al., 2010). Yet, findings of increased ventral striatal dopamine release in response to psychosocial stress in humans who reported insufficient early life maternal care (Pruessner et al., 2004), and positive associations between childhood adversity and amphetamine-induced dopamine release are not consistent with these results (Oswald et al., 2014). Since the HA group reported high levels of adverse psychosocial experiences throughout their lives, it is therefore possible that exposure to moderate stressors results in an initial sensitisation of dopaminergic function whereas repeated exposures to severe stressors result in a subsequent down-regulation. Alternatively, it is possible that early life stress is non-linearly related to the responsivity of the cortisol and dopamine systems (Del Giudice et al., 2011). Other factors such as type of stressor may also cause different dopaminergic, autonomic and endocrine effects as work to date has demonstrated that different schedules, intensities, or modalities of stressor presentation can result in dramatically different behavioural and physiological responses (Holly and Miczek, 2016) and these stressor-specific effects are appear highly region specific. Taken together with findings of stress-induced increases in dopaminergic measures (e.g. Fulford and Marsden, 1998; Hall et al., 1999; Kosten et al., 2003; Tidey and Miczek, 1996), the nature, intensity, and schedule of repeated stress may be critical, such that mild or intermittent stressors appear to potentiate basal VTA dopamine neuron activity and more severe or chronic uncontrollable stressors appear to reduce basal VTA dopamine activity, and the response to later stressors of a different nature is generally cross-sensitized (Holly and Miczek, 2016). An alternative explanation for the seemingly discrepant finding is that different parts of dopaminergic system undergo divergent responses to repeated stress (Brake et al., 2004). Additionally, differences in stress-induced dopaminergic outputs have even been observed within the same single neuron projections depending on the location of glutamatergic inputs (Finlay and Zigmond, 1997).
Whilst our measures of stress-induced change in endocrine and physiological function returned to basal levels, we cannot exclude the possibility that inducing acute psychosocial stress prior to PET scanning had an effect on the result. Few studies have measured dopamine synthesis in the period following an acute stressor. One study, for example, found that dopamine synthesis was reduced following restraint stress (Demarest et al., 1985) which may have been due to the activation of inhibitory feedback mechanisms. It therefore remains possible, albeit unlikely, that our findings are due to acute up-regulation of inhibitory feedback mechanisms including stress-induced dopamine release inhibiting dopamine synthesis via autoreceptors (Castro et al., 1996). Alternatively, since corticosteroids regulate tyrosine hydroxylase activity (the rate-limiting step in the dopamine synthesis pathway) (Meyer, 1985), and there is evidence that acute corticosteroids are associated with subsequent decreased striatal dopamine synthesis (Lindley et al., 1999), it is possible, therefore, that our findings are due to participants with chronic stress being sensitized to the acute stress-induced decreases in dopamine synthesis via corticosteroid mediated pathways.
Coherence between the emotional, endocrine and autonomic stress outcome systems has been assumed by some but has been questioned by others (Campbell and Ehlert, 2012; Mauss et al., 2005). Thus, our findings of divergent physiological and subjective emotional responses associated with chronic stressors in the current study are therefore of interest to the field. Repeated exposure to corticosteroids can lead to attenuation of HPA axis activity through negative feedback mechanisms (Karssen et al., 2005) and so it is not surprising that repeated exposure to stressors would be associated with a reduced autonomic response to acute stress, as has been found in adolescent survivors of developmental trauma (Gooding et al., 2016). Propranolol and dexamethasone attenuate the autonomic and endocrine responses to acute psychosocial stress but not the psychological response (Ali et al., 2017) indicating that these responses can be dissociated. Nonetheless, the mechanisms underlying the dissociation between these measures and subjective emotions are unclear and further research is needed to elucidate these dissociative mechanisms. One possibility is that this dissociation reflects a shift away from dopaminergic mechanisms to a more cortisol based stress response. Our findings of exposure to stressors being associated with autonomic effects are also important in the context of understanding the mechanisms underlying the well-established associations between cardiovascular disease and both depression (Penninx, 2017) and psychosis (Osborn et al., 2007).
Our findings of heightened stress-induced threat are consistent with the threat-anticipation model of delusion formation (Freeman, 2007) and evidence that elevated sensitivity to socio-environmental stress via enhanced threat anticipation in daily life may be important psychological processes underlying the association between childhood adversity and psychosis (Reininghaus et al., 2016). Given our findings of decreased dopamine synthesis capacity associated with high psychosocial stressors, it remains possible that repeated psychosocial stressors in childhood increase the risk of psychotic symptoms by hypodopaminergic or non-dopaminergic processes. However, Thompson et al. (2013) found that patients with comorbid schizophrenia and substance dependence (associated with a blunted dopamine system) had reduced amphetamine-induced dopamine release. Yet, despite a blunted dopamine response, this study found the classically described relationship between dopamine release and increase in psychotic symptoms, which may be due to D2 receptor super-sensitivity (Seeman and Seeman, 2014). Alternatively, as these participants were healthy it remains possible that these participants have resilience and so the observed findings may be due to adaptive allostatic down-regulations (McEwen, 1998).
Although the case–control design of this study is not able to confirm a causative relationship between psychosocial stress and dopamine dysfunction, these findings warrant further research into potential causative mechanisms. Our findings, particularly of relationships between cumulative stress exposure and the dopamine system, may be important for understanding how exposures to multiple stressors induce changes in the dopamine system, and how these relate to both vulnerability to and resilience against mental illnesses. It would be important to consider these findings in light of a putative role of the dopaminergic system in the pathophysiology anhedonia in depression (Nutt, 2006), the role of dopamine in social motivation (Love, 2014), and findings that striatal neurons can incorporate social reward into their computations (Báez-Mendoza et al., 2013; Schultz, 2016). Our findings may be highly relevant in terms of our understanding of addiction, as a history of exposure to aversive stimuli is strongly associated with later addictive behaviour, with both clinical and preclinical work demonstrating that stress plays a powerful role in the initiation, escalation, and relapse to drug abuse (Shaham et al., 2000; Sinha, 2007; Sinha, 2009).
Strengths and limitations
A strength of our study is that it specifically examined the effect of multiple psychosocial risk factors to examine their combined effect since these factors often cluster together in the general population (Hjern et al., 2004; Teicher et al., 2016; Wicks et al., 2005) and so it is not possible to disentangle the different types of psychosocial adversity due to lack of power as we are unable to directly contrast the effect of single risk factors with multiple exposures, or determine if risk factors have synergistic effects. Likewise, we did not co-vary for the different psychosocial risks as analysis of covariance is suboptimal at adjusting for factors when groups differ significantly in their covariates (Miller and Chapman, 2001). We chose to recruit participants with high levels of stress exposure in early development and adulthood, because early developmental stressors increase the risk of psychopathology following adult stressors (McLaughlin et al., 2010). However, a potential limitation of our combination of early developmental and adult stressors is that it may confound the early life (i.e. likely programming) effects with the later in life acute stressors which occur after developmental sensitive periods.
The PET scan was conducted following exposure to acute psychosocial stress exposure. We sought to reduce the length of time between task and PET scan to reduce variance in time and to limit the variance associated with normal fluctuations in dopamine synthesis capacity that is to limit the temporal separation between the stress task and the PET scan. Whilst acute stress increases dopamine release (Pruessner et al., 2004), it is not clear if this has an acute effect on striatal dopamine synthesis capacity. Therefore, it is possible that the close temporal proximity of the stress task to the PET means that what was observed during the PET scan could have been influenced by the stress task. Thus, one possible explanation for our findings could be reduced change in dopamine synthesis capacity following stress in the high adversity group relative to the low stress group, which would be consistent with Koob’s model (Koob et al., 1997). However, we are not aware of evidence indicating that an acute stressor can alter the parameters that contribute to our index (i.e. the activity of aromatic acid decarboxylase) in the timeframe used in our study. Nonetheless, a controlled study comparing stress and no stress prior to PET to investigate the effects is needed to definitively determine if acute stress alters dopamine synthesis capacity. Given this, it is possible that an optimal experimental design would allow more time to pass between the stressor and the PET scan.
A further consideration is that our groups were not matched for ethnicity. Ethnicity is associated with differences in allele frequencies of dopaminergic genetic variants (Gelernter et al., 1993) and striatal D2/3 receptor availability (Wiers et al., 2018) and so we cannot exclude the possibility that this is driving our results. Nonetheless, the study by Wiers and colleagues (Wiers et al., 2018) found ethnicity was associated with dorsal (rather than ventral) striatal dopamine receptor effects and that the effects of ethnicity on dopamine receptor availability were not driven by variation in dopamine candidate genes suggesting that their results are influenced by socioeconomic factors and therefore psychosocial stressors per se. Since our effects were most pronounced in the ventral (rather than dorsal) striatum and we found relationships between psychosocial stressors and ventral striatal dopamine synthesis, it is therefore unlikely that our results can be explained by ethnicity. Our participants were of multiple ethnic groups and so we were unable to determine the effects of psychosocial stress associated with immigration accounting for ethnicity. Future studies of individual risk factors are needed to examine what type of risks may be driving our findings. Finally, as our study is cross-sectional we cannot rule out reverse causality and a longitudinal design is required to distinguish between the interpretations discussed above.
We cannot exclude the possibility that recall bias may be confounding our findings as the measures of psychosocial stress rely on self-report and it was therefore not possible to independently verify the histories of psychosocial stressors. As such, the assessment of childhood trauma may be liable to recall bias in depressed patients (Lewinsohn and Rosenbaum, 1987). However, measures of childhood trauma have been demonstrated to remain stable over time and to be independent of the current degree of abuse-related psychopathology (Paivio, 2001). Despite ongoing concerns that retrospective reporting overestimates associations between abuse and adult psychopathology (Gilbert et al., 2009), there is evidence that prospective and retrospective measures of abuse predict similar rates of mental illness (Scott et al., 2012), recall bias accounts for less than 1% of variance in measures of child abuse (Fergusson et al., 2011) and good reliability and validity has been reported for retrospective self-reports of early experiences obtained from individuals with psychotic disorder and so this is unlikely to be significantly confounding our results (Fisher et al., 2011). Nonetheless, difficulties remain in measuring and quantifying emotional neglect due in part to its highly subjective nature (Watson et al., 2014). We did not control for genetic influences, apart from excluding individuals with a family history of psychosis and so genetic differences between exposed and unexposed groups might contribute to our finding. Nonetheless, previous work in our laboratory (Stokes et al., 2013) on heritability of striatal dopamine synthesis capacity found that individual-specific environmental factors, rather than genetic factors, had the greatest effect on the limbic striatum which is consistent with the interpretation that the psychosocial exposures have contributed to our finding of striatal hypodopaminergia. A further limitation is that we did not account for menstrual cycle phase in our analysis, given effects on the HPA axis (Kirschbaum et al., 1999).
These findings show that long-term exposure to psychosocial stressors is associated with reduced striatal dopamine synthesis capacity, particularly in the limbic subdivision of the striatum, alongside a de-coupling of the acute stress response such that emotional responses are potentiated whilst cardiovascular and endocrine responses tended to be blunted. Further work is needed to understand what processes contribute to this decoupling and how this may contribute to the development of mental illness.
Materials and methods
This study was approved by the National Research Ethics Service (12/LO/1955) and the Administration of Radioactive Substances Advisory Committee (ARSAC). The study was conducted in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration. All participants provided informed written consent to participate after an oral and written explanation of the study.
Participant recruitment
Request a detailed protocolWe recruited two groups of healthy volunteers, one exposed to multiple risk factors (exposed high adversity group, ‘HA’)) and one not exposed (unexposed low adversity group, ‘LA’), from throughout the UK via public advertisement, newspaper advertisement and national media engagement. Responding individuals were then screened via telephone. LA ‘controls’ were individually matched to the HA group on the basis of age (+ /- 5 years) and sex. Inclusion criteria for all participants were: age 18–45 years, good physical health and capacity to give written informed consent. Exclusion criteria for all participants included: a personal history of psychiatric illness including substance abuse but not Nicotine Use Disorders; a history of psychotic illness in first degree relatives; evidence of an at risk mental state (Yung et al., 2005) and contraindications to PET including pregnancy, nursing mothers, severe obesity and previous clinical procedures involving exposure to significant ionizing radiation within the last year.
Additional inclusion criteria for the HA (exposed) group included at least one childhood stressor and at least two adult stressors. These were ascertained by structured clinical interview. Childhood trauma self-reports were triangulated with the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ) (Bernstein and Fink, 1998) and the Childhood Experiences of Care and Abuse (CECA) (Bifulco et al., 2005).
Childhood stressors: Childhood (before age 16 years); History of childhood (before age 16 years) adversity defined as one or more of the following: parental loss (including separation with loss of parental contact and/or death and/or going into foster care and/or being adopted) and/or abuse (including physical, sexual abuse or neglect) and/or bullying (i.e. peer abuse) and/or major disaster and/or war and/or admission to hospital with life-threatening medical problem.
Adult stressor: Minority ethnic status; a significant life event defined as a bereavement, moving house, a change in job or financial circumstances, a new family member being born, a breakdown of a significant relationship, and/or unemployment within the last six months.
Additional inclusion criteria for the LA group included no exposure to the childhood factors listed above, ethnic majority status and no significant adverse life events in the last 6 months.
Psychosocial assessments
Request a detailed protocolAssessments included the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; Beck et al., 1996) Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI) (Beck et al., 1988), Aberrant Salience Inventory (Cicero et al., 2010), the CTQ and an adapted bullying questionnaire (Olweus, 1996). Detailed histories of life events over the preceding 6 months were obtained via the List of Threatening Events (Brugha and Cragg, 1990), and a life events score then calculated from these events based on the Holmes and Rahe life events stress scale (Holmes and Rahe, 1967); Brief Impact of Events Scale (IES-6) (Thoresen et al., 2010).
PET scans
Request a detailed protocolParticipants were asked to fast for 5 hr and to refrain from smoking tobacco for 2 hr before imaging. On the day of the PET scan, urine drug screen (Monitect HC12, Branan Medical Corporation, Irvine, California) confirmed no recent drug use, and a negative urinary pregnancy test was required in all female participants. Head position was marked and monitored via laser crosshairs and video camera, and minimized using a head-strap. We used a Siemens Biograph 6 TruePoint PET-CT scanner (Siemens Healthcare, Erlangen, Germany). A computed tomography (CT) scan (effective dose = 0.36 mSv) was acquired for attenuation and model-based scatter correction prior to each PET scan. A target dose of approximately 150 MBq of [18F]-DOPA was administered by bolus intravenous injection at the start of PET imaging. Emission data were acquired in list mode for 95 min, reconstructed in a 128 × 128 matrix with 2.6x zoom via filter back projection with a three dimensional 5 mm full-width half-maximum Gaussian image filter and re-binned into 32 timeframes (comprising eight 15 s frames, three 60 s frames, five 120 s frames, and sixteen 300 s frames).
Image analysis
Request a detailed protocolTo correct for head movement in the scanner, non-attenuation-corrected dynamic images were denoised using a level 2, order 64 Battle-Lemarie wavelet filter. Nonattenuation-corrected images were used for the realignment algorithm as they include greater scalp signal, improving re-alignment compared with attenuated-corrected images (Turkheimer et al., 1999). Frames were realigned to a single ‘reference’ frame, acquired 10 min post-injection, employing a mutual information algorithm (Studholme et al., 1996). The transformation parameters were then applied to the corresponding attenuated-corrected dynamic images. The realigned frames were then summated, creating a movement-corrected dynamic image, which was used in the analysis. The cerebellar reference region (Kumakura and Cumming, 2009) was defined using a probabilistic atlas (Martinez et al., 2003), and as previously described, regions of interest (ROI) in the whole striatum and its functional sub-divisions (Haber, 2014) were delineated to create an ROI map (Egerton et al., 2010). SPM8 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm) was then used to normalize the ROI map together with the tracer-specific ([18F]-DOPA) template (Egerton et al., 2016; Howes et al., 2009) to each individual PET summation image. This nonlinear transformation procedure allowed ROIs to be automatically placed on individual [18F]-DOPA PET dynamic images. The influx rate constants (Kicer, written as Ki in some previous publications (Howes et al., 2013) for the entire striatal ROI and the functional subdivisions bilaterally were calculated compared with uptake in the reference region using a graphical approach adapted for a reference tissue input function (Egerton et al., 2016).
Psychosocial stress paradigm
Request a detailed protocolWe induced psychosocial stress using the Montreal Imaging Stress Task (MIST) (Pruessner et al., 2004) 2 hr before PET scanning. The rationale for conducting the PET scan on the same day as the stress task was to reduce the variance in the time between the measures. Participants were aware that one of the measures would be inducing psychosocial stress; however, they were only told after the MIST was completed that this was the task to induce psychosocial stress, and they were then debriefed. They were told beforehand that they could stop the experimental procedures at any time. During the MIST, participants were asked to solve mental arithmetic problems first under a control condition during which no time constraint or feedback were present, and subsequently under the experimental condition where time and difficulty were automatically adjusted to result in a 30–40% error rate. During the experimental condition, we continuously made participants aware of their suboptimal performance via a visual performance bar and scripted verbal negative feedback delivered approximately every 1 min, where a confederate researcher reminded participants that they were performing much worse than average. The MIST was administered using pairs of researchers who were balanced for sex and ethnicity (i.e. male and female researchers, white British and minority ethnicities). There were 2 × 4 min blocks of control MIST control followed by a brief rest, and then 2 pairs of 2 × 4 min blocks of the experimental version including feedback and brief rest. We assessed subjective threat assessed before the task, at the end of control condition, after each 10 min paired block of MIST, and twice upon completion of the task at 30 min and 60 min after starting the experimental task (see Figure 4). We used visual analogue scales to measure subjective threat. Salivary cortisol and α-amylase (Engert et al., 2011) samples were taken at the same time points as the visual analogue scales. Heart rate and blood pressure recordings were taken at 3 min intervals during the control MIST and two experimental MIST conditions, with four readings in each of these three conditions.

Experimental procedures.
MIST, Montreal Imaging Stress Test; MAP, Mean Arterial Blood Pressure; VAS, visual analogue scale.
Power calculation
Request a detailed protocolIn a study of test-retest reliability of [18F]-DOPA PET (Egerton et al., 2010) striatal Kicer had an intraclass correlation coefficient of approximately 0.9 [mean (SD) Kicer = 0.01417 (0.00127)min−1 (test) and 0.01381 (0.00127)min−1 (re-test)]. Previous [18F]-DOPA uptake work has found an effect size of 1.25 in patients with schizophrenia (Howes et al.) which compares well with previous studies: 1.89 (Meyer-Lindenberg et al., 2002), 1.57 (McGowan et al., 2004). On the basis that large effect sizes are observed in disorders of dopamine function, this study was powered to anticipate an effect size of d = 1.00 when comparing differences between HA and LA groups. Therefore, to achieve a power of 0.8, with an effect size of 1.0, a = 0.05, using independent t-tests, n = 17 participants would be required per group.
Statistical analysis
Request a detailed protocolNormality of distribution and homogeneity of variance were assessed using Kolmogorov–Smirnov and Levene’s tests respectively, and diagnostic plots. The primary analysis was for Group (HA/LA) differences in striatal dopamine synthesis capacity. The primary region of interest was the whole striatum. Exploratory analyses were performed in the functional subdivisions of the striatum. Independent samples t-tests were used for normally distributed data, Mann–Whitney U-tests for non-normally distributed data, and the χ2 test for dichotomous variables. Where the assumption of homogeneity of variance was violated for independent samples t-tests, p values were adjusted to assume unequal variance. Group differences in acute response to psychosocial stress were investigated by calculating the Area Under the Curve (AUC) using the trapezoid method. The AUC was calculated from the last measurement taken during the control MIST condition up until the last available post-task measurement. For subjective ratings (VAS), salivary cortisol and amylase, AUC was calculated from readings taken from control MIST (10 min), Active MIST 1 (20 min), Active MIST 2 (30 min), Post Active MIST 2 (40 min) and Post Task (70 min). For blood pressure (MAP), AUC was calculated from Control MIST (19 min), Active MIST 1 (22 minutes, 25 min, 28 min), Active MIST 2 (31 minutes, 34 min, 37 min), and Post Active MIST 2 (40 min). Prior to calculating the AUC, data were corrected for baseline performance by subtracting baseline scores from all subsequent time points. Independent sample t-tests were used to compare groups at baseline, to compare response to acute psychosocial stress using AUC. Pearson correlational analyses were conducted separately in each of the groups among variables showing group differences. Bivariate outliers (Cook’s distance >1) were excluded prior to correlational analyses.
Data availability
The raw data from this study are available on written request to the Chief Investigator. This restriction is due to sensitive data on human research participants. Processed data files for Figures 1 and 2, and table 2 are provided in Source data 1.
References
-
Life events and psychosis: a review and meta-analysisSchizophrenia Bulletin 39:740–747.https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbt065
-
Fear of racism, employment and expected organizational racism: their association with healthThe European Journal of Public Health 19:504–510.https://doi.org/10.1093/eurpub/ckp071
-
An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: psychometric propertiesJournal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 56:893–897.https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.56.6.893
-
BookManual for the Beck Depression Inventory-IISan Antonio: Psychological Corporation.
-
Childhood trauma and psychotic disorders: a systematic, critical review of the evidenceSchizophrenia Bulletin 34:568–579.https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbm121
-
BookChildhood Trauma Questionnaire: A Retrospective Self-Report ManualSan Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.
-
The childhood experience of care and abuse questionnaire (CECA.Q): validation in a community seriesBritish Journal of Clinical Psychology 44:563–581.https://doi.org/10.1348/014466505X35344
-
Dopamine function in cigarette smokers: an [¹⁸F]-DOPA PET studyNeuropsychopharmacology 39:2397–2404.https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2014.87
-
Crises and life changes and the onset of schizophreniaJournal of Health and Social Behavior 9:203–214.https://doi.org/10.2307/2948405
-
The list of threatening experiences: the reliability and validity of a brief life events questionnaireActa Psychiatrica Scandinavica 82:77–81.https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.1990.tb01360.x
-
Victimization of women and its impact on assessment and treatment in the psychiatric emergency settingPsychiatric Clinics of North America 22:875–896.https://doi.org/10.1016/S0193-953X(05)70131-3
-
Acute psychosocial stress: does the emotional stress response correspond with physiological responses?Psychoneuroendocrinology 37:1111–1134.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.12.010
-
The aberrant salience inventory: a new measure of psychosis pronenessPsychological Assessment 22:688–701.https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019913
-
Ethnic differences in subclinical paranoia: an expansion of norms of the paranoia scaleCultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology 8:248–256.https://doi.org/10.1037/1099-9809.8.3.248
-
Schizophrenia and other psychotic disorders in a cohort of sexually abused childrenArchives of General Psychiatry 67:1114–1119.https://doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.147
-
Urinary catecholamine excretion in sexually abused girlsJournal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 33:320–327.https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199403000-00004
-
The Montreal imaging stress task: using functional imaging to investigate the effects of perceiving and processing psychosocial stress in the human brainJournal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience : JPN 30:319–325.
-
The adaptive calibration model of stress responsivityNeuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 35:1562–1592.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.11.007
-
Adversity in childhood linked to elevated striatal dopamine function in adulthoodSchizophrenia Research 176:171–176.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2016.06.005
-
Structural equation modeling of repeated retrospective reports of childhood maltreatmentInternational Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research 20:93–104.https://doi.org/10.1002/mpr.337
-
The effects of stress on central dopaminergic neurons: possible clinical implicationsNeurochemical Research 22:1387–1394.https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1022075324164
-
Reliability and comparability of psychosis patients' retrospective reports of childhood abuseSchizophrenia Bulletin 37:546–553.https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbp103
-
Suspicious minds: the psychology of persecutory delusionsClinical Psychology Review 27:425–457.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2006.10.004
-
Child abuse, resting blood pressure, and blood pressure reactivity to psychosocial stressJournal of Pediatric Psychology 41:5–14.https://doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/jsv040
-
Ventral tegmental area dopamine revisited: effects of acute and repeated stressPsychopharmacology 233:163–186.https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-015-4151-3
-
The social readjustment rating scaleJournal of Psychosomatic Research 11:213–218.https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3999(67)90010-4
-
Elevated striatal dopamine function linked to prodromal signs of schizophreniaArchives of General Psychiatry 66:13–20.https://doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2008.514
-
Stress-induced enhancement of dopamine and acetylcholine release in limbic structures: role of corticosteroneEuropean Journal of Pharmacology 165:337–338.https://doi.org/10.1016/0014-2999(89)90735-8
-
Childhood abuse as a risk factor for psychotic experiencesActa Psychiatrica Scandinavica 109:38–45.https://doi.org/10.1046/j.0001-690X.2003.00217.x
-
Childhood trauma and psychosis in a prospective cohort study: cause, effect, and directionalityAmerican Journal of Psychiatry 170:734–741.https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12091169
-
Opponent process model and psychostimulant addictionPharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 57:513–521.https://doi.org/10.1016/S0091-3057(96)00438-8
-
Neurobiology of addiction: a neurocircuitry analysisThe Lancet Psychiatry 3:760–773.https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(16)00104-8
-
Recall of parental behavior by acute depressives, remitted depressives, and nondepressivesJournal of Personality and Social Psychology 52:611–619.https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.52.3.611
-
Glucocorticoid effects on mesotelencephalic dopamine neurotransmissionNeuropsychopharmacology 21:399–407.https://doi.org/10.1016/S0893-133X(98)00103-1
-
Oxytocin, motivation and the role of dopaminePharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 119:49–60.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pbb.2013.06.011
-
Long-term behavioral and neurochemical effects of chronic stress exposure in ratsJournal of Neurochemistry 79:1113–1121.https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1471-4159.2001.00665.x
-
Imaging human mesolimbic dopamine transmission with positron emission tomography. part II: amphetamine-induced dopamine release in the functional subdivisions of the striatumJournal of Cerebral Blood Flow & Metabolism 23:285–300.https://doi.org/10.1097/01.WCB.0000048520.34839.1A
-
Stress, adaptation, and disease. allostasis and allostatic loadAnnals of the New York Academy of Sciences 840:33–44.https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.1998.tb09546.x
-
Presynaptic dopaminergic dysfunction in schizophrenia: a positron emission tomographic [18F]fluorodopa studyArchives of General Psychiatry 61:134–142.https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.61.2.134
-
Hyporesponsive reward anticipation in the basal ganglia following severe institutional deprivation early in lifeJournal of Cognitive Neuroscience 22:2316–2325.https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21394
-
Biochemical effects of corticosteroids on neural tissuesPhysiological Reviews 65:946–1020.https://doi.org/10.1152/physrev.1985.65.4.946
-
Misunderstanding analysis of covarianceJournal of Abnormal Psychology 110:40–48.https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.110.1.40
-
Cumulative social disadvantage, ethnicity and first-episode psychosis: a case-control studyPsychological Medicine 38:1701–1715.https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291708004534
-
Migration, ethnicity, and psychosis: toward a sociodevelopmental modelSchizophrenia Bulletin 36:655–664.https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbq051
-
Drugs of abuse and stress impair LTP at inhibitory synapses in the ventral tegmental areaEuropean Journal of Neuroscience 32:108–117.https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07256.x
-
The role of dopamine and norepinephrine in depression and antidepressant treatmentThe Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 67 Suppl 6:3–8.
-
BookThe Revised Olweus Bully/Victim QuestionnaireBergen: University of Bergen.https://doi.org/10.1037/t09634-000
-
Parental 'affectionless control' as an antecedent to adult depression. A risk factor delineatedArchives of General Psychiatry 40:956–960.https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1983.01790080038005
-
Corticotropin-releasing hormone and dopamine release in healthy individualsPsychoneuroendocrinology 76:192–196.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.11.034
-
Evidence of a dose-response relationship between urbanicity during upbringing and schizophrenia riskArchives of General Psychiatry 58:1039–1046.https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.58.11.1039
-
Depression and cardiovascular disease: epidemiological evidence on their linking mechanismsNeuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 74:277–286.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.07.003
-
High levels of dopamine activity in the basal ganglia of cigarette smokersAmerican Journal of Psychiatry 157:632–634.https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.157.4.632
-
Childhood trauma and psychosis - what is the evidence?Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience 13:360–365.
-
Dopamine reward prediction-error signalling: a two-component responseNature Reviews Neuroscience 17:183–195.https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2015.26
-
Childhood maltreatment and DSM-IV adult mental disorders: comparison of prospective and retrospective findingsBritish Journal of Psychiatry 200:469–475.https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.111.103267
-
Is schizophrenia a dopamine supersensitivity psychotic reaction?Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry 48:155–160.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2013.10.003
-
Risk of psychiatric treatment for mood disorders and psychotic disorders among migrants and Dutch nationals in Utrecht, The NetherlandsSocial Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 47:271–278.https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-010-0335-7
-
Social defeat: risk factor for schizophrenia?British Journal of Psychiatry 187:101–102.https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.187.2.101
-
Stress-induced relapse to heroin and cocaine seeking in rats: a reviewBrain Research Reviews 33:13–33.https://doi.org/10.1016/S0165-0173(00)00024-2
-
Understanding the excess of psychosis among the African-Caribbean population in England. review of current hypothesesThe British Journal of Psychiatry. Supplement 40:s60–s68.
-
The role of stress in addiction relapseCurrent Psychiatry Reports 9:388–395.https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-007-0050-6
-
Automated 3-D registration of MR and CT images of the headMedical Image Analysis 1:163–175.https://doi.org/10.1016/S1361-8415(96)80011-9
-
The effects of childhood maltreatment on brain structure, function and connectivityNature Reviews Neuroscience 17:652–666.https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2016.111
-
Striatal dopamine release in schizophrenia comorbid with substance dependenceMolecular Psychiatry 18:909–915.https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2012.109
-
Brief measure of posttraumatic stress reactions: impact of event Scale-6Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 45:405–412.https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-009-0073-x
-
Multiresolution analysis of emission tomography images in the wavelet domainJournal of Cerebral Blood Flow & Metabolism 19:1189–1208.https://doi.org/10.1097/00004647-199911000-00003
-
Salivary alpha amylase as marker for adrenergic activity during stress: effect of betablockadePsychoneuroendocrinology 31:137–141.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.05.012
-
Childhood trauma in bipolar disorderAustralian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 48:564–570.https://doi.org/10.1177/0004867413516681
-
Social adversity in childhood and the risk of developing psychosis: a national cohort studyAmerican Journal of Psychiatry 162:1652–1657.https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.162.9.1652
-
Association of genetic ancestry with striatal dopamine D2/D3 receptor availabilityMolecular Psychiatry 23:1711–1716.https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2017.208
-
Mapping the onset of psychosis: the comprehensive assessment of At-Risk mental statesAustralian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 39:964–971.https://doi.org/10.1080/j.1440-1614.2005.01714.x
Decision letter
-
Christian BüchelSenior and Reviewing Editor; University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Germany
In the interests of transparency, eLife includes the editorial decision letter and accompanying author responses. A lightly edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the most substantive concerns; minor comments are not usually included.
Thank you for submitting your article "The effects of chronic psychosocial stress on dopaminergic function and psychiatric imaging group" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by three peer reviewers, and the evaluation has been overseen by Christian Büchel as the Senior and Reviewing Editor. The reviewers have opted to remain anonymous.
The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.
Summary:
This paper is on chronic psychosocial adversity and vulnerability to mental illness. The authors follow the lead that dopamine might be an important link here and used [18 F]-DOPA PET to compare dopamine synthesis capacity of participants with high cumulative exposure to psychosocial adversity and controls (both groups n=17). The results show (i) that DA synthesis was correlated with subjective threat and physiological response to acute psychosocial stress, (ii) psychosocial stress dampened striatal dopaminergic and (iii) psychosocial adversity blunted physiological subjective responses to acute psychosocial stress.
Essential revisions:
Apart from addressing all points raised by the reviewers (see below), the authors should put particular emphasis on the following topics:
1) Explain and justify why the stress task (MIST) was performed two hours prior to the PET scanning.
2) How can acute control and experimental effects be disentangled and how is the interaction with chronic adversity resolved?
3) The sections regarding basic science and dopamine population activity needs clarifications.
Reviewer #1:
In this manuscript, the authors examine the impact of prior exposure to psychosocial stress on indices of DA function in humans. The paper is quite interesting and potentially important; however, there are some issues that need to be addressed:
1) The link with animal literature as written is confusing with regard to DA release and stress vs. synthesis capacity in humans. One potential caveat is that overall release in animals in response to stressors may not be the critical variable; since the authors are studying synthesis capacity (i.e., number of terminals that are active), a better comparison would be with DA neuron population activity (i.e., number of neurons firing) which was shown in studies of repeated stress by Valenti and Grace. These data would be more consistent and less confusing than the literature cited, which confounds release, metabolites, and activation.
2) Which regions of the DA system are activated vs. blunted with acute vs. chronic stress is an important variable. Acute and repeated stress activate the entire DA system projecting to much of the striatum (Valenti and Grace), in particular the associative striatum where object salience is important, whereas in chronic stress-induced depression (Holly and Miczek), the blunting occurs primarily in the neurons projecting to the ventromedial striatum (Moreines and Grace), where reward-related variables are processed. Therefore these are different systems mediating the DA stress response that varies with duration of stress exposure, and with the induction of anxiety (acute or repeated stress) vs. depression (chronic stress). This is consistent with Koob's opponent process model, where acute stress activates the DA system, which upon chronic exposure leads to a compensatory down-regulation.
3) Stress in Juvenile/adolescence has very different effects than chronic stress in adults. Therefore it is not valid to compare repeated adult stress with juvenile stress factors.
4) Discussion interpretation of findings – Holly and Miczek did not record from DA neurons.
5). The section on maternal deprivation occurs during very early juvenile period (i.e., postnatal day 7-8), which is far earlier than the juvenile/adolescent stress referred to in the paper.
Reviewer #2:
The authors present findings on dopamine synthesis capacity (using PET) in individuals with high exposure to adversity as compared to matched controls with low adversity (each group with N=17). PET scans are performed two hours after an acute psychosocial stress task (MIST), were participants with high exposure to adversity show enhanced threat experience, but lower arterial blood-pressure and trend-wise lower cortisol responses.
I am intrigued by these findings, since the author link a basal neurotransmitter function to cumulative adversity across different experiences in a non-clinical population. My excitement is however damped due to the order of the MIST and the PET scans: If the authors expect an effect of stressful events (chronically) on dopamine function, the PET scans would also be influenced (acutely) by the preceding stress-experience.
The authors need to clarify this effect. Since the design cannot be changed, the least would be a clear statement in the Abstract and main text.
The authors need to provide a power analyses. This study has a low number of subjects (I know that this might be common for PET studies), hence the authors need to show convincingly that the effects are well powered.
An additional factor, which needs to be considered (e.g., by covariation) is the amount of smoking in both groups. Even though the numbers of smokers is equal in both groups, the amount of smoking differs and has been shown to influence dopamine neurotransmission.
In the mediation analysis, the limbic subdivision of the striatum was chosen (subsection “The relationships between physiological and subjective measures”). The motivation for this subregion in the text ("connectivity to the threat system") and in the Materials and methods ("Exploratory analyses were performed in the functional subdivisions of the striatum.") is not very strong. I would suggest to perform regression analyses to identify which subregion is actually the best predictor for the physiological measures.
Reviewer #3:
In the study described by Bloomfield and colleagues, authors wanted to investigate whether exposure to chronic psychosocial stress, as assessed through a combination of childhood and adult stressors, would be associated with dopamine synthesis capacity. Authors exposed 34 subjects (17 with high and 17 with low cumulative stress exposure) to the Montreal Imaging stress task (MIST) and measured subjective, physiological and endocrine stress responses; two hours later, subjects were scanned for their dopamine synthesis capacity using 18F-Dopa in a Positron Emission Tomography experiment. Authors report that dopamine synthesis correlated with subjective threat and physiological response to acute psychosocial stress, and that high cumulative stress dampened striatal dopaminergic function.
Any PET study is a serious investment in time and resources and therefore authors should be commended to have undertaken this endeavor. The research question is not completely new as previous studies have investigated the possible role of stress in dopamine function, also using PET (and the authors make reference to this). What is unusual is the combination of early life and adult stressors to create a high cumulative lifetime adversity group; I have not come across this before. One can certainly argue whether this represents a useful approach as it confounds the early life (likely programming) effects with the later in life acute stressors which occur past the critical development periods. In fact, some authors (e.g., Ellis and colleagues with their work on life history theory) argue that early life adversity changes the stress systems to better prepare you for the adult life that is likely to be similar. In that sense, early life stress that is followed by adult stress might thus represent the expected outcome for the organism, that it is now better prepared for.
Be that as it may, I would simply have wished for a stronger justification for the particular chosen experimental approach as I was unclear on what exactly made the authors select these criteria. Also, why minority ethnic status was considered an adult stressor per se which was considered to be on par with bereavement or unemployment was not immediately clear to me and I would like to see more references justifying this approach.
What might be a potentially bigger concern had to do with the methodological approach chosen by the authors – if I understood the Materials and methods section correctly, authors did the stress task (the MIST) two hours prior to the PET scanning. Is the assumption that the stress would have the effect on the dopamine synthesis two hours after stress cessation? If that was the case, this should be made very explicit to the reader, and then I would like to see a section in the manuscript where this is systematically explained; right now, the reader is left in the dark about the choice of this exact rationale. Further, there was no stress – control comparison; subjects performed the first block of control followed by directly the experimental condition of the MIST; how can the authors be sure that what they then observe in the PET is specific to the stress part of the experimental manipulation? This is completely unclear to me, and I (and I am assuming the average reader as well) would benefit from a more thorough explanation of this part of the study.
[Editors' note: further revisions were requested prior to acceptance, as described below.]
Thank you for resubmitting your work entitled "The Effects of Chronic Psychosocial Stress on Dopaminergic Function and the Acute Stress Response" for further consideration at eLife. Your revised article has been favorably evaluated by Christian Büchel as the Senior and Reviewing Editor, and three reviewers.
The manuscript has been improved but there is one remaining issue that needs to be addressed before acceptance:
The data does not allow to say that DA synthesis is driven by early adversity alone, because it could be the interaction of childhood adversity and acute stress manipulation. The reviewers and the editor think that this does not lower the importance of this paper, but that it needs to be made clear in the paper, including the title (see below).
Reviewer #2:
The author responded to my comments and made an effort to clarify critical points in the manuscript.
The authors agree that the current study cannot disentangle chronic and acute stress, which is why I will consult with the other reviewers if the term "chronic" should be removed/changed from the title.
Reviewer #3:
The manuscript has benefited from the revision and now appears substantially improved. The authors have addressed all major points raised previously.
For the major points, the first point that was raised by this reviewer, i.e. the question of why a high cumulative lifetime adversity group was created, was answered thoroughly and convincingly, resulting in helpful revision in both the Introduction and the Discussion.
The second point previously raised, i.e. why a stress task preceded the PET scan by two hours, was answered by stating that the rationale was to reduce the variance in time. Here I am less enthusiastic that this is a sufficient response. Why would it be important to reduce the variation in time? On the other hand, by putting these two tests so close to each other, my fear would be that potential after effects from the stressor might have influenced what was observed during the PET scan. Authors do clarify that they are not aware of any evidence indicating that an acute stressor can have an effect on the parameters looked at in the PET, but then again, a controlled study comparing stress and no stress prior to PET to investigate the effects probably does not exist at this point, so obviously it is not known. Any number of things can occur two hours after an acute stressor – e.g., immune system changes, genomic effects of glucocorticoids, refractory period of hpa axis responsivity, which might or might not have an effect on the subsequent PET scan.
Thus, the decision to induce acute stress so close before the PET scan is in my opinion problematic, in the absence of a good rationale. Just to avoid the impression that it isn't, and to prevent other researchers from copying this design, it might be worth adding that having more time pass after an acute stressor might have been the superior choice.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.46797.013Author response
Essential revisions:
Apart from addressing all points raised by the reviewers (see below), the authors should put particular emphasis on the following topics:
1) Explain and justify why the stress task (MIST) was performed two hours prior to the PET scanning.
Please see our response to reviewer 2 (below).
2) How can acute control and experimental effects be disentangled and how is the interaction with chronic adversity resolved?
We have addressed the reviewers comments relating to this below. We have commented on this in the Discussion:
“However, a potential limitation of our combination of early developmental and adult stressors is that if may confound the early life (i.e. likely programming) effects with the later in life acute stressors which occur after developmental sensitive periods. […] Future work is therefore needed to disentangle the effects of acute vs. chronic stressors on the dopamine system.”
3) The sections regarding basic science and dopamine population activity needs clarifications.
We have clarified these sections as requested by the reviewers.
Reviewer #1:
In this manuscript, the authors examine the impact of prior exposure to psychosocial stress on indices of DA function in humans. The paper is quite interesting and potentially important; however, there are some issues that need to be addressed:
1) The link with animal literature as written is confusing with regard to DA release and stress vs. synthesis capacity in humans. One potential caveat is that overall release in animals in response to stressors may not be the critical variable; since the authors are studying synthesis capacity (i.e., number of terminals that are active), a better comparison would be with DA neuron population activity (i.e., number of neurons firing) which was shown in studies of repeated stress by Valenti and Grace. These data would be more consistent and less confusing than the literature cited, which confounds release, metabolites, and activation.
Thank you for this suggestion. We have separated the animal from the human work. We have removed studies investigating stress-induced dopamine release from the animal work and added the following text to the paragraph on animal work:
“Animal research has demonstrated that acute stressors including aversive stimuli induce a pronounced activation of the dopamine system in terms of dopamine neuron population activity (i.e. the numbers of neurons firing) and with regards to amphetamine-induced behaviours (Valenti et at.).”
2) Which regions of the DA system are activated vs. blunted with acute vs. chronic stress is an important variable. Acute and repeated stress activate the entire DA system projecting to much of the striatum (Valenti and Grace), in particular the associative striatum where object salience is important, whereas in chronic stress-induced depression (Holly and Miczek), the blunting occurs primarily in the neurons projecting to the ventromedial striatum (Moreines and Grace), where reward-related variables are processed. Therefore these are different systems mediating the DA stress response that varies with duration of stress exposure, and with the induction of anxiety (acute or repeated stress) vs. depression (chronic stress). This is consistent with Koob's opponent process model, where acute stress activates the DA system, which upon chronic exposure leads to a compensatory down-regulation.
Thank you very much indeed for raising these important points. We have added the following text to the Discussion:
“There is evidence of regional specificity in the direction of effects of acute vs. chronic stress exposure. […] Our findings are consistent with Koob's opponent process model, where acute stress activates the dopamine system, which upon chronic exposure leads to a compensatory down-regulation (Koob et al., 1997)”.
3) Stress in Juvenile/adolescence has very different effects than chronic stress in adults. Therefore it is not valid to compare repeated adult stress with juvenile stress factors.
We have edited the relevant section of the Discussion to make it clearer when we are referring to developmental vs. adult stress exposure. We have revised the text as follows:
"Our results extend these findings and are consistent with evidence from anima/ models whereby subcortical dopamine transmission is blunted in response to multiple stressors in adulthood (Chrapusta, Wyatt and Masserano, 1997; Gresch et al., 1994)... Whilst stress exposure in animals during the juvenile period and adolescence has a very different effect from to chronic stress in adulthood, our findings are also broadly consistent with developmental stress models (Brake et al., 2004). "
4) Discussion interpretation of findings – Holly and Miczek did not record from DA neurons.
Reference to the review by Holly and Miczek has been replaced with reference to the study by Saal et al., 2003.
5). The section on maternal deprivation occurs during very early juvenile period (i.e., postnatal day 7-8), which is far earlier than the juvenile/adolescent stress referred to in the paper.
We have made the timing of maternal deprivation studies clear in the text:
"Likewise, early maternal deprivation models in the very early juvenile period (from post-natal day 5) have been associated with hypodopaminergic behaviours…”
Reviewer #2:
The authors present findings on dopamine synthesis capacity (using PET) in individuals with high exposure to adversity as compared to matched controls with low adversity (each group with N=17). PET scans are performed two hours after an acute psychosocial stress task (MIST), were participants with high exposure to adversity show enhanced threat experience, but lower arterial blood-pressure and trend-wise lower cortisol responses.
I am intrigued by these findings, since the author link a basal neurotransmitter function to cumulative adversity across different experiences in a non-clinical population.
My excitement is however damped due to the order of the MIST and the PET scans: If the authors expect an effect of stressful events (chronically) on dopamine function, the PET scans would also be influenced (acutely) by the preceding stress-experience. The authors need to clarify this effect. Since the design cannot be changed, the least would be a clear statement in the Abstract and main text.
The study design was the same for both groups i.e. both groups underwent PET scans on the same day as acute psychosocial stress. Whilst we cannot exclude the possibility of an interaction (see below), it is unlikely that acute changes in dopamine synthesis are driving our results.
The Abstract has been amended as follows:
"The PET scan took place two hours after the induction of acute psychosocial stress using the Montréal Imaging Stress Task to induce acute psychosocial stress."
We have also added the following text in the limitations section of the Discussion:
"The PET scan was conducted following exposure to acute psychosocial stress exposure. Whilst this increases dopamine release (Pruessner et al., 2004), it is not clear if this has an acute effect on striatal dopamine synthesis capacity. […] A further study is needed to determine if acute stress alters dopamine synthesis capacity.”
The authors need to provide a power analyses. This study has a low number of subjects (I know that this might be common for PET studies), hence the authors need to show convincingly that the effects are well powered.
We have added a power calculation as follows:
"In a study of test-retest reliability of [18F]-DOPA PET (Egerton et al., 2010) striatal Kper had an intraclass correlation coefficient of approximately 0.9 [mean (SD) = 0.01417(0.00127)min-1 (test) and 0.01381(0.00127)min-1 (re-test)]. […] Therefore, to achieve a power of 0.8, with an effect size of 1.0, a=0.05, using independent t-tests, n=17 participants would be required per group. "
An additional factor, which needs to be considered (e.g., by covariation) is the amount of smoking in both groups. Even though the numbers of smokers is equal in both groups, the amount of smoking differs and has been shown to influence dopamine neurotransmission.
The following has been inserted into the text:
"As the amount of smoking differed in the groups and heavy smoking can influence dopamine function (Bloomfield et al., 2014; Salokangas et al., 2000) we performed an ANCOVA to examine whether smoking was influencing our findings. When co-varying for amount of current cigarette use, the group difference remained significant in the limbic striatum only 5.2, p=.029, "Ép=. 15)."
In the mediation analysis, the limbic subdivision of the striatum was chosen (subsection “The relationships between physiological and subjective measures”). The motivation for this subregion in the text ("connectivity to the threat system") and in the Materials and methods ("Exploratory analyses were performed in the functional subdivisions of the striatum.") is not very strong. I would suggest to perform regression analyses to identify which subregion is actually the best predictor for the physiological measures.
The limbic striatum was chosen because of its role in the stress response, its functional connectivity to the threat detection system and the largest effects sizes for group differences observed in our study. However, regression analyses did not identify which sub-region was the best predictor of the physiological measures (p>.08). We have therefore removed the mediation analysis from the manuscript and inserted the following into the text:
"Regression analyses did not identify which striata/ sub-region was the best predictor of the physiological measures (p>.08)".
Reviewer #3:
[…] Any PET study is a serious investment in time and resources and therefore authors should be commended to have undertaken this endeavor. The research question is not completely new as previous studies have investigated the possible role of stress in dopamine function, also using PET (and the authors make reference to this).
What is unusual is the combination of early life and adult stressors to create a high cumulative lifetime adversity group; I have not come across this before. One can certainly argue whether this represents a useful approach as it confounds the early life (likely programming) effects with the later in life acute stressors which occur past the critical development periods. In fact, some authors (e.g., Ellis and colleagues with their work on life history theory) argue that early life adversity changes the stress systems to better prepare you for the adult life that is likely to be similar. In that sense, early life stress that is followed by adult stress might thus represent the expected outcome for the organism, that it is now better prepared for.
Be that as it may, I would simply have wished for a stronger justification for the particular chosen experimental approach as I was unclear on what exactly made the authors select these criteria. Also, why minority ethnic status was considered an adult stressor per se which was considered to be on par with bereavement or unemployment was not immediately clear to me and I would like to see more references justifying this approach.
We are grateful to the reviewer for raising these important points. Findings from epidemiological studies have found that early developmental adversity increases the risk of psychopathology in response to stressors in adulthood (e.g. McLaughlin et al., 2010) are not consistent with the theory that experiencing childhood trauma is protective against risk of mental illness in response to adult stressors.
We have revised the justification for our experimental approach, including the rationale for including minority ethnic status, in the Introduction as follows:
"Studies of psychosocial stressors and dopamine function have typically investigated risk factors in isolation, despite the fact that the risk factors cluster together and may share common underlying mechanisms (Hjern, Wicks, and Dalman; Morgan and Fisher; Wicks et al., 2005). […] Given the findings of dopaminergic dysfunction associated with childhood maltreatment presented above, we hypothesised that healthy humans with a high cumulative exposure to psychosocial stressors would have altered striatal dopamine synthesis, compared to humans with a low exposure."
We have added the following to the Discussion:
"We chose to recruit participants with high levels of stress exposure in early development and adulthood, because early developmental stressors increase the risk of psychopathology following adult stressors (McLaughlin et al., 2010). However, a potential limitation of our combination of early developmental and adult stressors is that it may confound the early life (i.e. likely programming) effects with the later in life acute stressors which occur after developmental sensitive periods. "
What might be a potentially bigger concern had to do with the methodological approach chosen by the authors – if I understood the Materials and methods section correctly, authors did the stress task (the MIST) two hours prior to the PET scanning. Is the assumption that the stress would have the effect on the dopamine synthesis two hours after stress cessation? If that was the case, this should be made very explicit to the reader, and then I would like to see a section in the manuscript where this is systematically explained; right now, the reader is left in the dark about the choice of this exact rationale. Further, there was no stress – control comparison; subjects performed the first block of control followed by directly the experimental condition of the MIST; how can the authors be sure that what they then observe in the PET is specific to the stress part of the experimental manipulation? This is completely unclear to me, and I (and I am assuming the average reader as well) would benefit from a more thorough explanation of this part of the study.
We apologise that this was not clear enough. The rationale for conducting the PET scan on the same day as the stress task was to reduce the variance in the time between the measures. We did not have assumptions that acute stress exposure would alter subsequent dopamine synthesis capacity. Whilst we cannot exclude the possibility that such effects may be contributing to our results, we are not aware of evidence indicating that an acute stressor can alter two parameters that contribute to our index (i.e. the number of available dopamine vesicles and/or the activity of aromatic acid decarboxylase) in this timeframe. The rationale for exploring the relationships between stress-induced measures and dopamine synthesis capacity is implicit is provided in the manuscript. We have included these points as follows:
Materials and methods:
"The rationale for conducting the PET scan on the same day as the stress task was to reduce the variance in the time between the measures."
Discussion:
"However, we are not aware of evidence indicating that an acute stressor can alter the parameters that contribute to our index (i.e. the activity of aromatic acid decarboxylase) in the timeframe used in our study. "
[Editors' note: further revisions were requested prior to acceptance, as described below.]
The manuscript has been improved but there is one remaining issue that needs to be addressed before acceptance:
The data does not allow to say that DA synthesis is driven by early adversity alone, because it could be the interaction of childhood adversity and acute stress manipulation. The reviewers and the editor think that this does not lower the importance of this paper, but that it needs to be made clear in the paper, including the title (see below).
We are grateful to Professor Buchel and colleagues for their further review. We have made changes to the paper including the title.
Reviewer #2:
The author responded to my comments and made an effort to clarify critical points in the manuscript.
The authors agree that the current study cannot disentangle chronic and acute stress, which is why I will consult with the other reviewers if the term "chronic" should be removed/changed from the title.
We are grateful to the reviewer. We have removed the term "chronic" from the title. The title now reads: "The Effects of Psychosocial Stress on Dopaminergic Function and the Acute Stress Response".
Reviewer #3:
[…] For the major points, the first point that was raised by this reviewer, i.e. the question of why a high cumulative lifetime adversity group was created, was answered thoroughly and convincingly, resulting in helpful revision in both the Introduction and the Discussion.
The second point previously raised, i.e. why a stress task preceded the PET scan by two hours, was answered by stating that the rationale was to reduce the variance in time. Here I am less enthusiastic that this is a sufficient response. Why would it be important to reduce the variation in time? On the other hand, by putting these two tests so close to each other, my fear would be that potential after effects from the stressor might have influenced what was observed during the PET scan. Authors do clarify that they are not aware of any evidence indicating that an acute stressor can have an effect on the parameters looked at in the PET, but then again, a controlled study comparing stress and no stress prior to PET to investigate the effects probably does not exist at this point, so obviously it is not known. Any number of things can occur two hours after an acute stressor – e.g., immune system changes, genomic effects of glucocorticoids, refractory period of hpa axis responsivity, which might or might not have an effect on the subsequent PET scan.
Thus, the decision to induce acute stress so close before the PET scan is in my opinion problematic, in the absence of a good rationale. Just to avoid the impression that it isn't, and to prevent other researchers from copying this design, it might be worth adding that having more time pass after an acute stressor might have been the superior choice.
We are grateful to the reviewer for their feedback. In performing the PET scan so close to the stress task we sought to reduce the length of time between task and PET scan to reduce variance associated with normal fluctuations in dopamine synthesis capacity. We agree is possible that close temporal proximity of the stress task to the PET means that it is possible that what was observed during the PET scan could have been influenced by the stress task. Whilst we not aware of any evidence indicating that an acute stressor can have an effect on the parameters looked at in the PET, a controlled study comparing stress and no stress prior to PET to investigate the effects is needed to definitively address this possibility. Given this, it is possible therefore that an optimal experimental design would allow more time to pass between the stressor and the PET scan. These points have been clarified in the text as below:
"The PET scan was conducted following exposure to acute psychosocial stress exposure. […] Nonetheless, a controlled study comparing stress and no stress prior to PET to investigate the effects is needed to definitively determine if acute stress alters dopamine synthesis capacity. Given this, it is possible that an optimal experimental design would allow more time to pass between the stressor and the PET scan.”
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.46797.014Article and author information
Author details
Funding
Medical Research Council (MC-A656-5QD30)
- Oliver Howes
National Institute for Health Research
- Michael AP Bloomfield
National Institute for Health Research (University College London Hospitals Biomedical Research Centre)
- Michael AP Bloomfield
Wellcome Trust (094849/Z/10/Z)
- Oliver Howes
University College London
- Michael AP Bloomfield
National Institute for Health Research (Biomedical Research Centre at South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and King’s College London)
- Oliver Howes
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Acknowledgements
We are very grateful to Yvonne Lewis and everyone at Imanova (now Invicro), Professor Vivette Glover, Drs David Bonsall and Dr Lucia Valmaggia for their assistance with this study. This work was funded by a Medical Research Grant to Professor Howes. Dr Bloomfield is funded by a UCL Excellence Fellowship and supported by the National Institute for Health Research University College London Hospitals Biomedical Research Centre.
Ethics
Human subjects: This study was approved by the National Research Ethics Service (12/LO/1955) and the Administration of Radioactive Substances Advisory Committee (ARSAC). The study was conducted in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration. All participants provided informed written consent to participate after an oral and written explanation of the study.
Senior and Reviewing Editor
- Christian Büchel, University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Germany
Version history
- Received: March 12, 2019
- Accepted: October 12, 2019
- Version of Record published: November 12, 2019 (version 1)
Copyright
© 2019, Bloomfield et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 20,962
- Page views
-
- 1,030
- Downloads
-
- 45
- Citations
Article citation count generated by polling the highest count across the following sources: Scopus, Crossref, PubMed Central.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Neuroscience
Perceptual decisions about sensory input are influenced by fluctuations in ongoing neural activity, most prominently driven by attention and neuromodulator systems. It is currently unknown if neuromodulator activity and attention differentially modulate perceptual decision-making and/or whether neuromodulatory systems in fact control attentional processes. To investigate the effects of two distinct neuromodulatory systems and spatial attention on perceptual decisions, we pharmacologically elevated cholinergic (through donepezil) and catecholaminergic (through atomoxetine) levels in humans performing a visuo-spatial attention task, while we measured electroencephalography (EEG). Both attention and catecholaminergic enhancement improved decision-making at the behavioral and algorithmic level, as reflected in increased perceptual sensitivity and the modulation of the drift rate parameter derived from drift diffusion modeling. Univariate analyses of EEG data time-locked to the attentional cue, the target stimulus, and the motor response further revealed that attention and catecholaminergic enhancement both modulated pre-stimulus cortical excitability, cue- and stimulus-evoked sensory activity, as well as parietal evidence accumulation signals. Interestingly, we observed both similar, unique, and interactive effects of attention and catecholaminergic neuromodulation on these behavioral, algorithmic, and neural markers of the decision-making process. Thereby, this study reveals an intricate relationship between attentional and catecholaminergic systems and advances our understanding about how these systems jointly shape various stages of perceptual decision-making.
-
- Neuroscience
The relationship between obesity and human brain structure is incompletely understood. Using diffusion-weighted MRI from ∼30,000 UK Biobank participants, we test the hypothesis that obesity (waist-to-hip ratio, WHR) is associated with regional differences in two micro-structural MRI metrics: isotropic volume fraction (ISOVF), an index of free water, and intra-cellular volume fraction (ICVF), an index of neurite density. We observed significant associations with obesity in two coupled but distinct brain systems: a prefrontal/temporal/striatal system associated with ISOVF and a medial temporal/occipital/striatal system associated with ICVF. The ISOVF~WHR system colocated with expression of genes enriched for innate immune functions, decreased glial density, and high mu opioid (MOR) and other neurotransmitter receptor density. Conversely, the ICVF~WHR system co-located with expression of genes enriched for G-protein coupled receptors and decreased density of MOR and other receptors. To test whether these distinct brain phenotypes might differ in terms of their underlying shared genetics or relationship to maps of the inflammatory marker C-reactive Protein (CRP), we estimated the genetic correlations between WHR and ISOVF (rg = 0.026, P = 0.36) and ICVF (rg = 0.112, P < 9×10−4) as well as comparing correlations between WHR maps and equivalent CRP maps for ISOVF and ICVF (P<0.05). These correlational results are consistent with a two-way mechanistic model whereby genetically determined differences in neurite density in the medial temporal system may contribute to obesity, whereas water content in the prefrontal system could reflect a consequence of obesity mediated by innate immune system activation.