Reliability of an interneuron response depends on an integrated sensory state
Abstract
The central nervous system transforms sensory information into representations that are salient to the animal. Here we define the logic of this transformation in a Caenorhabditis elegans integrating interneuron. AIA interneurons receive input from multiple chemosensory neurons that detect attractive odors. We show that reliable AIA responses require the coincidence of two sensory inputs: activation of AWA olfactory neurons that are activated by attractive odors, and inhibition of one or more chemosensory neurons that are inhibited by attractive odors. AWA activates AIA through an electrical synapse, while the disinhibitory pathway acts through glutamatergic chemical synapses. AIA interneurons have bistable electrophysiological properties consistent with their calcium dynamics, suggesting that AIA activation is a stereotyped response to an integrated stimulus. Our results indicate that AIA interneurons combine sensory information using AND-gate logic, requiring coordinated activity from multiple chemosensory neurons. We propose that AIA encodes positive valence based on an integrated sensory state.
Introduction
Sensory environments are complex, and can include multiple signals within and across sensory modalities. Individual stimuli and the combinations in which they occur may differentially signal the presence of beneficial, harmful, or neutral conditions. Accordingly, integration across sensory inputs is an essential function of the nervous system, and the logic of integration is an area of active investigation.
The compact Caenorhabditis elegans nervous system, with 302 neurons and a complete neuronal wiring diagram, is well-suited for studying sensory representation and integration (White et al., 1986; Cook et al., 2019). C. elegans uses volatile and water-soluble cues to detect its food and other relevant stimuli (Bargmann, 2006). Its highly developed chemosensory system senses a large number of attractive and repulsive compounds using over a thousand different G-protein-coupled chemoreceptors, many of which are expressed by eleven pairs of chemosensory neurons in the amphid sensory organ (Robertson and Thomas, 2006). Specific subsets of chemosensory neurons have reproducible functions in chemotaxis and avoidance of chemical stimuli, spontaneous locomotion, physiology, development, and lifespan (Bargmann, 2006). AWA and AWC sensory neurons, for example, drive chemotaxis toward a variety of attractive odors, whereas AWB sensory neurons drive avoidance of certain repellents (Bargmann et al., 1993; Troemel et al., 1997). Notably, sensory neurons may be either activated or inhibited by odors. For example, AWA is activated by the attractive odor diacetyl (Shinkai et al., 2011; Larsch et al., 2013), whereas AWC is inhibited by another attractive odor, isoamyl alcohol (Chalasani et al., 2007).
At the first layer of neuronal integration, the sensory neurons form abundant chemical and electrical synapses onto a half-dozen pairs of interneurons (White et al., 1986). The interneurons regulate behaviors such as reversal frequency, speed, and head turning during thermotaxis, chemotaxis, and spontaneous locomotion (Iino and Yoshida, 2009; Tsalik and Hobert, 2003; Gray et al., 2005; Hendricks et al., 2012). Interneuron responses are variable and complex, and can incorporate feedback from network states and motor systems as well as sensory input (Gordus et al., 2015; Li et al., 2014; Hendricks et al., 2012; Kaplan et al., 2018). Synaptic mechanisms linking interneurons to locomotor states have been defined, but the computations that interneurons perform to integrate multiple sensory inputs are unknown.
The AIA interneuron pair receives chemical or electrical synapses from all eleven pairs of amphid chemosensory neurons, suggesting an integrative function (White et al., 1986) (Supplementary file 1). At a behavioral level, AIA is implicated in the suppression of reversal behavior upon odor addition (Larsch et al., 2015), integration of attractive and repulsive stimuli (Shinkai et al., 2011), olfactory desensitization at short and long timescales (Chalasani et al., 2010; Cho et al., 2016), and regulation of spontaneous reversals (López-Cruz et al., 2019). Functional calcium imaging has demonstrated that AIA can be activated by the attractive odors diacetyl and isoamyl alcohol (Larsch et al., 2013; Chalasani et al., 2010), which are primarily sensed by AWA and AWC neurons, respectively (Bargmann et al., 1993). In both cases, AIA activity rises in the presence of an attractive food-related odor.
Here, we use an optogenetic approach to isolate the connections between individual sensory neurons and AIA. We find that AIA uses AND-gate logic to integrate sensory information. AIA is reliably activated only by coordinated sensory information from multiple neurons, and this activation is mediated by both chemical and electrical synapses. A bistable current-voltage relationship provides a biophysical mechanism for the nonlinear AIA response. Our results suggest that AIA represents a positive valence that is integrated across sensory inputs.
Results
Optogenetic activation of AWA sensory neurons elicits unreliable AIA calcium responses
AWA sensory neurons expressing genetically-encoded calcium indicators such as GCaMP respond to the bacterial odorant diacetyl with fluorescence increases indicating depolarization (Shinkai et al., 2011; Larsch et al., 2013; Zaslaver et al., 2015; Hale et al., 2016; Larsch et al., 2015; Liu et al., 2018). AWA calcium responses are concentration-dependent, with stronger and more rapid responses to 115 nM diacetyl than to 11.5 nM diacetyl, and a rapid rise followed by desensitization within 10 s at 1.15 µM diacetyl (Larsch et al., 2015) (Figure 1A and C). Diacetyl also elicits calcium transients in the AIA interneurons, with desensitization at high diacetyl concentrations (Larsch et al., 2015) (Figure 1B and D). AIA calcium transients are diminished in animals with defects in AWA development or in the AWA diacetyl receptor ODR-10 (Larsch et al., 2015).
To examine AWA-to-AIA synaptic communication in detail, we directly depolarized AWA using the light-activated channel Chrimson expressed under an AWA-selective promoter and recorded GCaMP responses in either AWA or AIA (Klapoetke et al., 2014) (Figure 1A–B, Figure 1—figure supplement 1A–F). Light-activated calcium transients were elicited within one second in Chrimson-expressing AWA neurons and desensitized only slightly over ten seconds. This activation required pre-exposure to retinal and expression of the Chrimson transgene (Figure 1—figure supplement 1B–C), and was comparable in magnitude to the response of AWA at or above 115 nM diacetyl.
To our surprise, AIA responses to AWA::Chrimson stimulation were significantly smaller than AIA responses to any concentration of diacetyl (Figure 1B). To understand the discrepancy between optogenetic stimulation and odor, we examined the dynamics of individual calcium responses instead of averaged traces (Figure 1D and E, Figure 1—figure supplement 2B, and Figure 1—figure supplement 3A) and established robust thresholding parameters to separate responses from non-responses (see Materials and methods and Figure 1—figure supplement 2). The GCaMP fluorescence baseline in AIA was stable, with little spontaneous activity, and nearly all AIA neurons responded to the higher odor concentrations (115 nM and 1.15 µM diacetyl) with an average delay of ~1 s relative to AWA activation (Figure 1C, D and F, and Figure 1—figure supplement 3B–C). At the lowest tested diacetyl concentration (11.5 nM) AIA responded with a lower probability (Figure 1D–F), and a delay of ~2 s relative to AWA responses (Figure 1F, Figure 1—figure supplement 3B). Thus AIA responses to diacetyl are coupled to odor and dose-dependent. These AIA properties differ from those of AIB interneurons, which have spontaneous calcium fluctuations and variable sensory responses at all odor concentrations due to a strong effect of downstream motor state (Gordus et al., 2015; Kato et al., 2015).
At an individual trial level, AWA::Chrimson stimulation elicited AIA responses with a low probability and a delay, and these responses were much less robust than AIA responses elicited by diacetyl at a comparable level of AWA activation (Figure 1D–F). AWA::Chrimson elicited calcium increases in 85% of AWA sensory neurons within one second of optogenetic stimulation, resembling 115 nM diacetyl, but only 56% of the AIA interneurons were activated. Moreover, the AIA calcium transients were delayed by >6 s on average relative to the AWA response (Figure 1F, Figure 1—figure supplement 3B). Thus AIA calcium responses to AWA::Chrimson were unreliable.
Control experiments indicated that these differences were robust to transgenes or stimulus protocols. AWA::Chrimson animals had normal AIA and AWA responses to diacetyl, before or after light stimulation (Figure 1—figure supplements 1D and 3C), and the AIA response latency was not correlated with GCaMP fluorescence levels or AWA::Chrimson transgene expression levels (Figure 1—figure supplement 3D–E). Animals were routinely subjected to two stimulus pulses of light or odor; these responses were slightly biased toward the first stimulus but largely independent (Figure 1—figure supplement 3F–R). Animals that responded to both stimulus pulses had correlated response magnitudes between the two pulses (Figure 1—figure supplement 3J,N and R). However, the latencies of responses were not correlated between the first and second pulse of 11.5 nM diacetyl or AWA::Chrimson stimulation, indicating that reliability is primarily a trial-to-trial property and not due to variation between animals (Figure 1—figure supplement 3I,M and Q).
Close examination of the AIA calcium signals revealed that the response rise dynamics to diacetyl or AWA::Chrimson stimulation were similar; the major differences were in their probability and latency. When aligned to the beginning of an AIA calcium response, positive AIA trials had similar rise times, whether elicited by optogenetic stimulation or by diacetyl (Figure 1—figure supplement 2C–D). These stereotyped properties were robust across experiments and thresholding parameters (Figure 1—figure supplement 2).
Gap junctions mediate AWA-to-AIA communication
AWA cell fate mutants (odr-7) and AWA diacetyl receptor mutants (odr-10) have diminished AIA interneuron responses to diacetyl (Larsch et al., 2015; Sengupta et al., 1994; Sengupta et al., 1996). At the individual trial level, AIA interneuron responses to high (1.15 µM) diacetyl were less reliable in AWA-defective mutants than in wild type (Figure 2A–B). These results indicate that AWA is necessary for strong and reliable AIA interneuron responses to diacetyl, although the optogenetic experiments indicate that AWA activation is not sufficient for reliable AIA responses.
The C. elegans wiring diagram predicts the existence of gap junctions between AWA and AIA neurons, and a recent reanalysis additionally predicts a chemical synapse from AWA to AIA (White et al., 1986; Cook et al., 2019). To assess the importance of the potential chemical synapse, we inhibited AWA synaptic vesicle release by expressing the tetanus toxin light chain, which cleaves the synaptic vesicle protein synaptobrevin (Schiavo et al., 1992; Macosko et al., 2009). AWA::TeTx animals and wild type animals had indistinguishable AIA responses to diacetyl and AWA::Chrimson stimulation, indicating that AWA does not require chemical synapses to activate AIA (Figure 2C, Figure 2—figure supplement 1A). Moreover, AWA::Chrimson stimulation and diacetyl strongly activated AIA in the synaptic transmission mutants unc-13 and unc-18, providing further evidence that chemical synapses are not necessary for AWA to AIA signaling (Figure 3, discussed below).
We next asked whether AWA-to-AIA communication requires gap junctions, which are composed of innexin proteins in invertebrates. AWA and AIA express two innexin genes that contribute to many gap junctions, unc-7 and unc-9, among other innexins (Cao et al., 2017; Bhattacharya et al., 2019). AIA responses to diacetyl were unaffected by single unc-7 or unc-9 mutants, but were less reliable in unc-7 unc-9 innexin double mutants than in wild type (Figure 2D–F and Figure 2—figure supplement 1B,C and E), recapitulating the defects observed in AWA cell fate and receptor mutants (odr-7, odr-10; Figure 2A). AIA responses to AWA::Chrimson stimulation were also less reliable in unc-7 unc-9 innexin double mutants (Figure 2—figure supplement 1D). Expressing an unc-9 cDNA in AWA and AIA neurons of unc-7 unc-9 double mutants rescued the AIA responses to diacetyl, but an unc-9 cDNA with an inactivating point mutation did not rescue (Figure 2F, Figure 2—figure supplement 1E). Together, these results indicate that AWA signals to AIA via gap junctions.
Gap junctions can transmit information symmetrically or asymmetrically between neurons based on properties including voltage-dependent rectification, differential subunit expression between cells, and differential phosphorylation (Goodenough and Paul, 2009). To test whether the predicted electrical synapse between AWA and AIA is bidirectional, we optogenetically stimulated AIA interneurons with a Chrimson transgene and recorded the resulting responses in AWA sensory neurons. AIA responded rapidly and robustly to direct optogenetic stimulation (Figure 2G and H), but AWA responded infrequently, with only small-magnitude calcium responses, to AIA stimulation (Figure 2I and J). The AWA response to AIA stimulation was unchanged in unc-7 unc-9 innexin double mutants, and slightly increased in synaptic transmission mutants, suggesting that multiple synapse types contribute to weak AIA-to-AWA communication (Figure 2G–2J, Figure 2—figure supplement 1F–I). Together, these results suggest that AWA-to-AIA gap junctions preferentially mediate anterograde information flow from sensory neurons to interneurons.
Chemical synapses inhibit AIA
The rapid AIA response to direct AIA optogenetic stimulation indicates that its delayed response to sensory stimuli is not caused by slow intrinsic calcium dynamics, but by other circuit elements. Diacetyl can activate AIA, albeit less reliably, in mutants that lack AWA function or unc-7 and unc-9 innexins. The residual AIA diacetyl response predicts that additional diacetyl-sensing neurons communicate with AIA, most likely through chemical synapses. Indeed, AIA receives chemical synapses from many chemosensory neurons (Supplementary file 1).
We asked how chemical synapses impact AIA responses by examining mutants in unc-13 and unc-18, both of which are deficient in synaptic vesicle release (Richmond, 2007). Unexpectedly, AIA responses to diacetyl were faster and more reliable in animals with defective chemical synapses (Figure 3A, Figure 3—figure supplement 1A). This effect was most evident at low diacetyl concentrations, where the synaptic mutants substantially decreased the latency of the AIA response (Figure 3A). A trend toward faster AIA responses at the higher diacetyl concentrations only reached significance for one of three tested mutants, perhaps reflecting a ceiling effect (Figure 3—figure supplement 1A). Inactivation of dense core vesicle release with an unc-31 mutation did not alter AIA reliability, demonstrating specificity of the effect (Richmond, 2007) (Figure 3—figure supplement 1E). Chemical synapses are thus net inhibitory onto AIA.
The increased reliability of AIA responses in unc-13 and unc-18 mutants was even more striking when AWA was stimulated with AWA::Chrimson (Figure 3B–3D, Figure 3—figure supplement 1B). In the synaptic mutants, half of the AIA neurons responded to AWA::Chrimson within 1.2 s, a dramatic decrease in latency compared to wild type (>6 s) (Figure 3C). The AWA-to-AIA delay in synaptic mutants after AWA::Chrimson stimulation resembled the delay to high concentrations of diacetyl (Figure 3F–G). Control experiments demonstrated that direct AWA responses to optogenetic stimulation did not increase in unc-13 synaptic mutants (Figure 3E, Figure 3—figure supplement 1C–D). Chemical synapses thus inhibit AIA interneurons primarily by decreasing the reliability of AIA’s response at a given level of AWA activation.
Glutamatergic sensory neurons cooperate to inhibit AIA
Eighteen pairs of neurons form chemical synapses onto AIA, six of which are sensory neurons that use glutamate as a neurotransmitter (White et al., 1986; Cook et al., 2019; Serrano-Saiz et al., 2013). Glutamate hyperpolarizes AIA, likely by activating glutamate-gated chloride channels, so these glutamatergic sensory neurons are plausible sources of the synaptic inhibition of AIA (Chalasani et al., 2010; Shinkai et al., 2011; López-Cruz et al., 2019). We selectively inhibited glutamate release using a CRISPR-edited version of the endogenous eat-4 locus, which encodes the major vesicular glutamate transporter in C. elegans; the edited gene enables cell-specific eat-4 excision with flippase recombinase (Lee et al., 1999; López-Cruz et al., 2019) (Figure 4A and B). Selective excision of eat-4 in four tax-4-expressing sensory neurons, AWC, ASE, ASK, and ASG, allowed AWA::Chrimson stimulation to evoke reliable AIA responses similar to those in unc-18 synaptic transmission mutants (Figure 4C, Figure 4—figure supplement 1A). This effect was not observed with either the modified eat-4 locus or flippase expression alone (Figure 4—figure supplement 1A–B).
No individual sensory pair accounted for the full effect of preventing glutamate release from AWC, ASE, ASK, and ASG together (Figure 4D–4H, Figure 4—figure supplement 1A and C), although a significant partial effect was observed upon inhibition of glutamate release from ASK alone (Figure 4D). As previous studies implicated AWC and ASE in diacetyl responses (Larsch et al., 2015), we further examined the effect of chemical synapses from these two neurons. As noted above, the unc-18 mutant has increased reliability of AIA responses to AWA::Chrimson stimulation, but in two independent lines in which unc-18 was selectively rescued in ASE and AWC, AIA interneuron responses were restored to unreliable responses resembling wild type (Figure 4I, Figure 4—figure supplement 1D). This effect was not observed with a control transgene that encoded the inactive unc-18(e234) mutant in AWC and ASE. Synaptic vesicle release from AWC and ASE is therefore sufficient to inhibit AIA activation by AWA. In summary, multiple sensory neurons, including ASK and at least one of AWC and ASE, can release glutamate to inhibit AIA activation.
Multiple sensory neurons detect diacetyl
To explain these results, we hypothesized that glutamatergic sensory neurons tonically inhibit AIA, and are inhibited when diacetyl is present to disinhibit AIA. This hypothesis agrees with previous calcium imaging studies showing that ASK and AWC are active at rest and inhibited by amino acids, pheromones, and certain odors (Wakabayashi et al., 2009; Macosko et al., 2009; Zaslaver et al., 2015; Chalasani et al., 2007; Kato et al., 2014). To extend this observation to diacetyl, we expressed the genetically-encoded calcium indicator GCaMP5A in ASK and AWC, and GCaMP3 in ASE, and determined that ASK and AWC were inhibited by the addition of 1.15 µM diacetyl, whereas ASE was activated by diacetyl at the same concentration (Figure 5A–5C, Figure 5—figure supplement 1A–C). All responses were dose-dependent, with responses that were weaker (ASK) or absent (AWC, ASE) at 11.5 nM diacetyl (Figure 5A–5C, Figure 5—figure supplement 1A–C,E–G). ASH, another glutamatergic sensory neuron that forms chemical synapses onto AIA, did not respond to 1.15 µM diacetyl (Figure 5—figure supplement 1D and H), demonstrating neuronal specificity of the response.
C. elegans sensory neurons are abundantly interconnected by chemical synapses (White et al., 1986). Diacetyl responses in ASK and AWC were unchanged in unc-18 synaptic transmission mutants, suggesting that these neurons detect diacetyl directly (Figure 5—figure supplement 1I–J,L–M). By contrast, diacetyl responses in ASE were eliminated in unc-18 mutants, suggesting that ASE senses diacetyl indirectly via another sensory neuron (Figure 5D, Figure 5—figure supplement 1K). AWA was not the source of the diacetyl response in ASE, as the response was preserved in an odr-10 diacetyl receptor mutant (Figure 5D, Figure 5—figure supplement 1K). ASE receives synapses from many other sensory neurons, but as ASE was not essential or sufficient for AIA activation (Figure 4G–H), we did not pursue it further. Neither ASK, AWC, nor ASE showed a calcium response upon AWA::Chrimson stimulation (Figure 5—figure supplement 1N–O).
In summary, AIA responses are unreliable upon AWA::Chrimson stimulation, which activates only AWA; more reliable to 11.5 nM diacetyl, which activates AWA and inhibits ASK; and highly reliable to 1.15 µM diacetyl, which activates AWA and ASE and inhibits AWC and ASK, leading to disinhibition of AIA (Figure 5H). To ask whether the reduced reliability of AIA responses to 11.5 nM compared to 1.15 µM diacetyl was associated with reduced reliability of sensory neuron responses at low concentrations, we simultaneous recorded AIA, AWA and ASK calcium activity in a small number of animals while delivering pulses of 11.5 nM or 1.15 µM diacetyl. AIA was activated by 12/12 pulses of 1.15 µM diacetyl, and by 6/8 pulses of 11.5 nM diacetyl (Figure 5—figure supplement 2A–B). In each successful trial, AWA was activated, ASK was inhibited, and AIA was activated. In the two trials in which 11.5 nM diacetyl failed to elicit AIA responses, it elicited an AWA response but failed to elicit an ASK response (Figure 5—figure supplement 2B), consistent with a role for multiple sensory inputs in AIA activation.
Combinatorial activation of AIA by isoamyl alcohol-sensing neurons
Since the reliability of the AIA response required inputs from multiple sensory neurons, we asked whether the same logic applied for other odors. Based on previous ablation studies, chemotaxis to diacetyl requires AWA at low concentrations, with a redundant role for AWC at high concentrations (Chou et al., 2001), whereas chemotaxis to another odor and bacterial metabolite, isoamyl alcohol, requires AWC with a minor contribution from AWA (Bargmann et al., 1993; Worthy et al., 2018). Both AWC and AWA respond to isoamyl alcohol with calcium transients (Larsch et al., 2013), suggesting that study of this second odor could test the generality of the AIA activation model.
Previous work showed that AWC is inhibited by 9 and 90 µM isoamyl alcohol (Larsch et al., 2013; Yoshida et al., 2012; Gordus et al., 2015). We found that AWC was also inhibited by 0.9 µM isoamyl alcohol, and that AWA was activated by isoamyl alcohol in a graded fashion at 0.9, 9, and 90 µM isoamyl alcohol (Figure 5F–G, Figure 5—figure supplement 3A–B,D–E). ASK was not inhibited by isoamyl alcohol at any tested concentration (Figure 5E, Figure 5—figure supplement 3C and F).
We then examined AIA interneuron responses to 0.9, 9, and 90 µM isoamyl alcohol. As with diacetyl, AIA calcium responses were unreliable at the lowest concentrations, but became more reliable, with a higher probability and a shorter latency, as isoamyl alcohol concentration increased (Figure 5I–K, Figure 5—figure supplement 3K). At all concentrations, AWC responded first and AIA responded later, near the time of AWA activation (Figure 5—figure supplement 3G–I). Interestingly, a different pattern held for diacetyl, where AWA responded first and AIA responded later, near the time of ASK inhibition (Figure 5—figure supplements 2A–D and 3J).
To test the contributions of different sensory neurons to AIA activation by isoamyl alcohol, we monitored AIA responses in wild type, AWA cell fate mutants (odr-7), AWC and ASE cell fate mutants (ceh-36) (Lanjuin et al., 2003), and odr-7 ceh-36 double mutants. odr-7 mutants had unreliable AIA responses to isoamyl alcohol at all concentrations (Figure 5I–5K, Figure 5—figure supplement 3K). At the higher concentrations of 9 µM and 90 µM isoamyl alcohol, AIA responses were more reliable in the odr-7 and ceh-36 single mutants than in the odr-7 ceh-36 double mutants, indicating that AWA and AWC are partly redundant for AIA activation. At the lower concentration of 0.9 µM isoamyl alcohol, all mutants had unreliable AIA responses, indicating that both AWA and AWC are independently required for AIA activation.
Both diacetyl and isoamyl alcohol represent bacterial food sources (Choi et al., 2016; Worthy et al., 2018). Previous work has shown that the more complex food stimulus of Escherichia coli OP50-conditioned medium activates AWA and ASE and inhibits AWC, ASK, and several other sensory neurons (Zaslaver et al., 2015). We found that OP50-conditioned medium elicited reliable AIA calcium responses with similar latency and rise dynamics to those elicited by high concentrations of diacetyl or isoamyl alcohol (Figure 5L, Figure 5—figure supplement 3L–M). All stimuli that evoked reliable AIA responses engaged multiple sensory neurons, including inhibition of at least one glutamatergic sensory neuron and the activation of AWA (Figure 6D).
AIA has a nonlinear current-voltage relationship
The finding that AIA calcium responses require multiple sensory inputs suggests that AIA is performing a non-additive, nonlinear thresholding calculation. To seek biophysical evidence for this nonlinearity, we recorded AIA voltage responses to electrophysiological current injections in dissected animals using step and ramp injection protocols under current clamp. We found that AIA membrane potential was bistable, with one stable state near the resting potential of −80 mV and a second state near −20 mV (Figure 6A). A rapid transition between these two states occurred with a low current injection threshold near 2–3 pA, at which point the AIA membrane potential rose rapidly to −20 mV (Figure 6A–C). Subsequent increases in stimulus intensity did not greatly increase AIA membrane potential beyond −20 mV. Voltage-clamp recordings indicate that AIA bistability is associated with a region of high membrane resistance between −80 and −20 mV (Figure 6—figure supplement 1A–B).
Simultaneous recordings of membrane potential and GCaMP fluorescence in AIA during current injections revealed GCaMP fluorescence levels that were also bistable and stereotyped, with increases that were correlated with the voltage transition from −80 to −20 mV (Figure 6C). Thus AIA performs a nonlinear rather than an additive computation at a biophysical level.
Discussion
AIA uses AND-gate logic to integrate sensory information
Our work defines the contributions of individual sensory neurons to the activation of a downstream integrating neuron. Multiple C. elegans sensory neurons respond to an attractive odor such as diacetyl or isoamyl alcohol with cell type-specific dynamics and signs. The sensory neurons signal the presence of odor to AIA using electrical synapses (AWA) or glutamatergic chemical synapses (AWC, ASK). AIA does not respond reliably to input from a single sensory neuron. Rather, AIA is activated by coordinated inputs from multiple sensory neurons, functioning as an AND-gate that requires both activation by AWA, and disinhibition from glutamatergic neurons including AWC and ASK.
We propose the following model for AIA integration (Figure 6D). In the absence of food odor, glutamatergic sensory neurons release glutamate that activates glutamate-gated chloride channels on AIA (López-Cruz et al., 2019). The chloride current decreases AIA membrane resistance, preventing AIA activation via the AWA-AIA electrical synapse. In the presence of food odor, glutamatergic sensory neurons are inhibited, the glutamate-gated chloride channels close, and AIA disinhibition and increased AIA resistance make it more sensitive to depolarizing inputs. At the same time, food odors activate AWA, driving AWA depolarization and calcium action potentials (Liu et al., 2018), and transmission of this information through AWA-to-AIA gap junctions depolarizes AIA to the plateau potential of −20 mV.
The properties of the AND-gate and the contributing neurons explain both the low probability of an AIA response and the delayed response to weak odor stimuli. For both diacetyl and isoamyl alcohol, AIA activation lags behind the first sensory neuron to detect odor, matching the arrival of the second input. The sensory neurons, in turn, can contribute to this delay. At low odor levels, the inhibition of the glutamatergic sensory neurons reported by calcium imaging is graded and gradual. Similarly, the first AWA action potential lags behind stimulus onset by about a second (Liu et al., 2018). As a result, the synchronization of the disinhibitory and excitatory sensory inputs required for the AND-gate are delayed at low stimulus levels.
AIA membrane potential is bistable, with a sharp threshold that separates stable states near −80 mV and −20 mV. The depolarized state resembles the plateau potentials first reported in C. elegans RMD motor neurons (Mellem et al., 2008) and subsequently observed in a number of other C. elegans neurons that have a high resistance regime between stable low and high voltage states (Liu et al., 2018). The intrinsic bistability of AIA creates a threshold nonlinearity that shapes the response to synaptic inputs. The synaptic inputs are predicted to increase depolarizing current (through AWA gap junctions) and to increase membrane resistance (by closing glutamate-gated chloride channels), and therefore could have the multiplicative effect on voltage classically defined by Ohm’s law. A preferential effect of AWA gap junctions on depolarizing current, and inhibitory chemical synapses on membrane resistance and shunting, provide a plausible mechanism for the AIA AND-gate.
The AND-gate is an established motif in transcriptional regulation (Buchler et al., 2003), and bears similarity to the related concept of coincidence detection in neural circuits (Koch, 1999). In contrast with computational models of neurons based on additive inputs onto the target neuron (Dayan and Abbott, 2001; McCulloch and Pitts, 1943), the AND-gate computation is nonlinear and multiplicative (Koch, 1999). In the context of AIA, this logical computation requires multiple sensory neurons to report the presence of an attractant, an integrative step that may filter out environmental noise. Based on our results with diacetyl and isoamyl alcohol, it appears that different combinations of sensory neurons can generate the disinhibition and excitation necessary for reliable AIA activation.
It is interesting to compare AIA to another well-characterized olfactory interneuron, AIB. AIB activity rises when odors such as isoamyl alcohol are removed, triggering aversive behaviors such as reversals and turns (Chalasani et al., 2007). The coupling of strong odor stimuli to AIB activity is less reliable than coupling to AIA: stimuli that drive AIA responses in over 90% of trials, drive AIB responses in only 57% of trials (Gordus et al., 2015). At a circuit level, AIB variability results from its integration of sensory input with the downstream motor state (Gordus et al., 2015), and AIB activity is highly correlated with the activity of other neurons that drive reversals (Gordus et al., 2015; Kato et al., 2015). AIA activity has not been studied systematically with respect to network states, although it appears to rise during transitions to forward locomotion (Laurent et al., 2015). In the C. elegans wiring diagram, AIA receives three times as many synapses from sensory neurons as from interneurons, and AIB receives equal numbers of synapses from sensory neurons and interneurons, perhaps explaining why AIA activity is more closely coupled to sensory state, and less to motor state, than AIB (White et al., 1986). That said, motor states were not examined in this work, and they could contribute to the remaining variability in AIA activation. For example, even when the response probability to odors is high, the magnitude of the AIA calcium response can vary somewhat between trials, and neither AIA response magnitude nor response duration were analyzed systematically here. Further investigation could uncover input from network states on these and other quantitative aspects of the AIA response.
Excitation and disinhibition by electrical and chemical synapses
In C. elegans, behavioral functions of many neurons are known – for example, AIA supports forward movement in the presence of odor, reversals upon odor removal, and aversive olfactory learning – but the associated synaptic computations are not as well understood. As predicted by the wiring diagram, our results indicate that AWA primarily excites AIA via an electrical synapse, in the context of chemical synapses from other neurons. Mixed chemical-electrical synapses are present in many escape circuits, including insect giant fibers, goldfish Mauthner cells, and the C. elegans backward command neurons (Allen and Murphey, 2007; Phelan et al., 2008; Liu et al., 2017). In these escape circuits, the same cells form chemical and electrical synapses with each other to maximize speed and efficiency. By contrast, AIA receives chemical and electrical synapses from different sensory neurons to enable integration.
At a synaptic level, the combination of excitatory and disinhibitory inputs of AIA resemble interactions at the glomeruli of the rat inferior olivary nucleus (Pereda et al., 2013). Principal cells in the inferior olive are coupled via electrical synapses, and are uncoupled when neurons from the deep cerebellar nuclei release GABA at adjacent chemical synapses. The inhibitory synapses shunt excitatory current flow at the electrical synapse (Pereda et al., 2013). Synchronized firing in this system has features of an AND-gate, with distinct states that depend on both electrical and chemical synapses.
At a circuit level, the roles of excitation and inhibition in an AND-gate differ from their roles in many circuits. Excitation and inhibition are most frequently observed as balanced inputs, tightly correlated in time and space, poising circuits to detect small changes with precision (Okun and Lampl, 2008; Denève and Machens, 2016). In the AND-gate logic employed by AIA, excitation and inhibition alternate, switching the neuron’s activity state. A related disinhibition logic, with a more complex mechanism, is used in mammalian cortical and hippocampal circuits, where VIP-expressing interneurons disinhibit information flow through excitatory circuits by GABAergic inhibition of local inhibitory interneurons (Pi et al., 2013; Turi et al., 2019).
AIA activity as a readout of positive valence
We suggest that AIA signals an integrated, positive sensory valence, as represented through the activity state of multiple sensory neurons. In addition to the odors studied here, further evidence for combinatorial integration by AIA comes from behavioral studies in which animals cross an aversive copper barrier, sensed by the glutamatergic ASH sensory neurons, to reach an attractive diacetyl source (Shinkai et al., 2011). The copper-diacetyl behavioral interaction requires ASH glutamatergic inhibition of AIA via the glutamate-gated chloride channel GLC-3. In this context, the integrated sensory state detected by AIA includes a repellent as well as an attractant, and a different combination of sensory inputs. The behavioral role of AIA activity is complex, but it is interesting that the combinations of sensory neurons that activate AIA are partly distinct from the roles of those sensory neurons in chemotaxis. For example, AWA is not necessary for chemotaxis to isoamyl alcohol, but has an important role in AIA activation by that odor, and the converse holds for ASK and diacetyl.
The existence of a complete wiring diagram for C. elegans poses a challenge: are there core computations performed by repeated circuit motifs? We speculate that the combination of inhibitory chemical synapses, excitatory gap junctions, and intrinsic bistability, all of which are present in multiple C. elegans neurons, could represent the core features of an AND-gate circuit motif. This possibility can be tested by further experiments.
Materials and methods
C. elegans growth
Request a detailed protocolWe used standard genetic and molecular techniques (Brenner, 1974). C. elegans strains were maintained at 22°C on Nematode Growth Media (NGM; 51.3 mM NaCl, 1.7% agar, 0.25% peptone, 1 mM CaCl2, 12.9 µM cholesterol, 1 mM MgSO4, 25 mM KPO4 at pH 6) plates seeded with LB-grown Escherichia coli OP50 bacteria. Animals had constant access to food for at least three generations prior to experiments. Experiments were performed on young adult hermaphrodites. All strains and full genotypes are listed in Supplementary file 5.
Stimulus preparation
Request a detailed protocolStimulus solutions were freshly prepared each experimental day by serially diluting from a pure stock of diacetyl (2,3-butanedione; Sigma-Aldrich 11038, CAS 431-03-8; stored at 4°C) or isoamyl alcohol (EMD AX-1440–6, CAS 123-51-3; stored at 4°C), or by directly dissolving NaCl (Fisher Chemical S271-1, CAS 7647-14-5) into S Basal buffer (0.1 M NaCl, 5.74 mM K2HPO4 and 44.1 mM KH2PO4 at pH 6, 5 µg/ml cholesterol). E. coli OP50 conditioned medium was prepared by seeding 30 ml NGM buffer without agar or cholesterol with a colony of OP50 bacteria and shaking the culture at 37°C overnight such that optical density was ~0.2 by the morning of the experiment. The OP50 culture was filtered (0.22 µm Millex GP) before the experiment and NGM was used as the buffer control for these experiments. All stimulus solutions contained 1 mM (-)-tetramisole hydrochloride (Sigma L9756, CAS 16595-80-5) to paralyze the body wall muscles and were stored in brown glass vials, except for those used to simultaneously image AIA, AWA, and ASK. For simultaneous imaging experiments, stimulus solutions did not contain (-)-tetramisole hydrochloride and were stored in 30 ml plastic syringes that had been separately soaked in ethanol or water overnight.
Calcium imaging of individual neurons
Request a detailed protocolThe calcium imaging protocol was adapted from Larsch et al. (2015). Animals expressing GCaMP were selected as L4 larvae the evening before the experiment and picked to new OP50 plates. For Chrimson experiments, L4 animals were transferred to freshly seeded plates of 5x concentrated OP50-seeded LB with or without 5 µM all-trans retinal (Sigma R2500; CAS 116-31-4) and housed overnight in complete darkness.
Before beginning the experiment, we selected animals for visible GCaMP fluorescence and gently washed them in S Basal buffer. We then loaded ~10 animals of two genotypes or conditions into separate arenas of a custom-fabricated two-arena polydimethylsiloxane (PDMS; Sigma 761036, made from 9:1 base:curing agent, Sylgard 184) imaging devices that had been de-gassed in a vacuum dessicator for at least 5 min. We chose ~10 animals per arena to maximize the number of replicates within a condition or genotype without sacrificing proper flow. Animals were paralyzed in darkness for ~90 min in buffer + tetramisole before the start of the recording.
Experiments were performed on a Zeiss AxioObserver A1 inverted microscope fit with a 5x/0.25 NA Zeiss Fluar objective. A Hamamatsu Orca Flash 4 sCMOS camera was mounted to the microscope using a 0.63x c-mount adapter to increase field of view. We delivered 474 nm wavelength light with a Lumencor SOLA-LE lamp. We used Metamorph 7.7.6 software to control image acquisition and light pulsing in addition to rapid stimulus switching (National Instruments NI-DAQmx connected to an Automate Valvebank 8 II actuator that controls a solenoid valve), odor selection (Hamilton 8-way distribution valve), and activation of an external red LED for Chrimson stimulation (Mightex Precision LED Spot Light, 617 nm, PLS-0617–030 s; attached to Chroma ET605/50x filter to narrow band to 605 ± 25 nm). For Chrimson experiments, red light intensity was 15 mW/cm2.
For odor-only calcium imaging experiments, we detected GCaMP signals after illumination with 165 mW/cm2 474 nm light, strobed at a 10 ms per 100 ms (10 fps) exposure duty cycle to reduce motion artefacts and GCaMP photobleaching.
For optogenetic experiments, we used a lower light intensity at 474 nm to reduce blue light activation of the Chrimson channel. To define conditions, AWA neurons expressing GCaMP and Chrimson were tested at 474 nm light intensities ranging from 15 mW/cm2 with a 10% duty cycle to 165 mW/cm2 with constant illumination (Figure 1—figure supplement 1D). Strobed (10% duty cycle) 40 mW/cm2 light captured fluorescent GCaMP signals in the AIA neurite, and retained full AWA responses to 617 nm Chrimson illumination and odor stimuli (Figure 1—figure supplement 1D), although with a higher level of background noise than under strong illumination.
Animals received two pulses of the tested stimulus, and both pulses were pooled for analyses, with the exception of Figure 1—figure supplements 1D–E and 3F–R. In experiments with multiple odor concentrations, we delivered odors in order of increasing concentration. In Chrimson experiments with diacetyl controls (Figure 1—figure supplement 1D and Figure 1—figure supplement 3C), or diacetyl experiments with NaCl controls (Figure 5—figure supplement 1D and H), the control was delivered last. To confirm proper odor flow, we delivered a pulse of fluorescein dye at the end of the experiment; assays in which flow was impeded were discarded.
Raw fluorescence values were measured using a custom ImageJ script from Larsch et al. (2013), which measures the average intensity of a 4 × 4 pixel square and subtracts the local background intensity. For all sensory neurons, the square captured the soma; for AIA, it captured the middle of the neurite. Animals that moved too much for the tracking script, and animals with no visible AIA soma, were discarded. Occasionally, the tracking script inserted NaN (Not a Number) instead of a fluorescence intensity; pulses with NaN values were discarded. The tracking script is semi-automated to reduce experimenter bias. Each background-subtracted raw fluorescence trace was first normalized to generate ΔF/F0, where F0 was the median of the 10 s (100 frames) before the odor pulse onset. Traces were then smoothed by five frames such that each frame t represented the mean of t-2 frames to t+2 frames.
In a preliminary experiment, we delivered 28 AWA::Chrimson pulses to wild type animals, and 24 pulses to unc-13(e51) mutant animals (not included in analyses because we used a slightly different protocol). This experiment captured a statistically significant difference in cumulative response time profiles between wild type and unc-13 AIA responses to AWA::Chrimson stimulation (Kolmogorov-Smirnov test, p: 0.007, D test statistic: 0.470). Guided by this result, all experiments reported here included at least 17 light or odor pulses (and typically >30) per genotype or condition.
We delivered two light or odor pulses per animal, tested 5–10 animals per PDMS device arena, tested 2–16 arenas per genotype or condition, and tested 2–10 genotypes or conditions per experimental block. The number of arenas per genotype or condition depended on the number of genotypes or conditions included in an experimental block, such that each genotype or condition was tested on at least two days with fresh odorant solutions or retinal plates, tested in both the upper and lower arenas of a PDMS device (see Figure 1—figure supplement 1A), and tested simultaneously with the control at least once. The n used for analysis refers to individual stimulus pulses. Statistical analyses include all genotypes or conditions in an experimental block, corrected for multiple comparisons that in some cases include genotypes or conditions we do not present. The genotypes or conditions included in an experimental block depended on the hypothesis we were testing. We included a wild type-to-unc-18(e234) comparison in several independent experimental blocks, and the cumulative response time profiles for the separate and combined experimental blocks are shown in Figure 3D.
Determining response latency times
Request a detailed protocolFor AWA, AIA, ASE, and ASH, a calcium trace was deemed a ‘response’ at the first frame t at which the mean smoothed ΔF/F0 of t to t+12 frames exceeded two standard deviations of the mean of the 10 s pre-stimulus ΔF/F0, and the mean time derivative of t to t+1 frame exceeded one standard deviation of the mean time derivative of the 10 s pre-stimulus ΔF/F0. The threshold of 2 standard deviations of the pre-stimulus ΔF/F0 marks an inflection point in the number of traces categorized as ‘responses’ to buffer, in the absence of odor or Chrimson stimulus. This threshold was therefore the lowest, or most inclusive, threshold possible without including an excessive number of obvious non-responses, or false positives. The threshold of 2 standard deviation of the mean time derivative of the pre-stimulus ΔF/F0 marks an inflection point in the number of traces categorized as ‘nonresponses’ to each stimulus; that is, higher thresholds exclude responses. Therefore, this threshold is the highest threshold possible without excluding an excessive number of responses, or false negatives. Notably, at the threshold of 2 standard deviations of the pre-stimulus ΔF/F0, the number of responses are largely independent of the time derivative threshold, which we used to constrain the timing of the event. More details on thresholding procedures can be found in Figure 1—figure supplement 2.
To determine ASK and AWC response latency, but not for other calculations, each ASK and AWC calcium trace was scaled such that the minimum value was 0 and the maximum value was 1. The calcium trace was deemed a ‘response’ at the first frame t at which the mean scaled ΔF/Fmax of t to t+10 frames was below two standard deviations of the mean of the 10 s pre-stimulus ΔF/F0, and either the time derivative of t to t+10 frames was below 0.5 standard deviations, or the time derivative to t to t+5 frames was below 1.15 standard deviations, of the mean of the 10 s pre-stimulus ΔF/F0.
To compare the variability of response latencies, we compared the cumulative response time profiles. We used the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test to compare these distributions since this test would capture both the latencies and probability of response. Although the figures show only 5 s of stimulus, the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test compared distributions for 10 s of stimulus. Details of each test, including the D test statistic, can be found in Supplementary file 2.
Comparing AWA-to-AIA lag times
Request a detailed protocolTo calculate the mean lag between AWA and AIA responses (Figure 1—figure supplement 3B, Figure 3G), we subtracted the frame at which 50% of AWA neurons had responded from the frame at which 50% of AIA neurons had responded to a given stimulus. We performed this calculation 1000 times from randomly bootstrap-sampled populations that had the same n as the true population, sampled with replacement. The standard deviation of the bootstrapped distribution was used as the standard error of the bootstrapped mean. The lag between simultaneously recorded AWA and AIA responses in 24 animals was calculated by subtracting each animal’s AIA response initiation time from its AWA response initiation time (Figure 5—figure supplement 2D). These times were determined separately by eye by an individual blinded to the identity of the neuron and animal number.
Measuring response magnitudes in sensory neurons
Request a detailed protocolTo calculate ASK, AWC, ASE, and ASH response magnitudes to a given stimulus, we subtracted the mean ΔF/F0 of 10 frames (1 s) prior to stimulus delivery from the mean ΔF/F0 of the final 10 frames within the odor pulse. Because AWA and AIA responses often adapt during the stimulus pulse, we defined AWA and AIA response magnitudes as the maximum ΔF/F0 within the 10 s stimulus pulse.
To compare AIA or AWA magnitudes between genotypes or conditions, we included only calcium traces that represent detectable responses as described in the Determining Latency Times section. To determine whether there was an appreciable ASK, AWC, or ASH response to a given stimulus, or AWA response to isoamyl alcohol, we included all calcium traces.
We statistically tested differences with either an ordinary one-way ANOVA with Dunnett’s multiple comparisons test for experiments with more than two conditions, or an unpaired t-test for experiments with only two conditions. For ASK, AWC, ASE, AWA, and ASH responses to diacetyl, isoamyl alcohol, or NaCl, we used responses to S Basal – S Basal buffer switches as the control. For ASK, AWC, and ASE responses to AWA::Chrimson stimulation in Figure 5—figure supplement 1O, we used a paired t-test to compare response magnitudes to the change in ΔF/F0 within a similar time window prior to the light pulse. Details of each test can be found in Supplementary file 3.
Calculating rise times
Request a detailed protocolTo calculate rise times of AIA responses, we included only calcium traces that represent detectable responses as described in the Determining Latency Times section. We calculated the rise time by subtracting the time at which AIA reached 33% of its peak magnitude from the time at which AIA reached 66% of its peak magnitude for each response. We compared rise times to various stimuli using an ordinary one-way ANOVA with Dunnett’s multiple comparisons test (three comparisons to AWA::Chrimson for Figure 1—figure supplement 2D, three comparisons to 1.15 µM diacetyl for Figure 5—figure supplement 3M, and three comparisons to wild type for Figure 4—figure supplement 2M).
Simultaneous calcium imaging of multiple neurons
Request a detailed protocolAnimals expressing soluble GCaMP5A in AIA, and nuclear GCaMP6s in ASK, AWA, and several other sensory neurons, were selected as L4s the evening before the experiment and transferred to fresh bacterial plates overnight. Adult animals were transferred to an unseeded agar plate, then transferred into S Basal buffer containing 1 mM (-)-tetramisole hydrochloride as a paralytic agent. After 10 min, individual animals were loaded into a custom-fabricated PDMS microfluidic chamber in S Basal buffer, and either 11.5 nM or 1.15 µM diacetyl was delivered to the animal’s nose under controlled conditions (Chronis et al., 2007). Fluorescence changes were monitored on a Zeiss Axiovert 100TV wide-field inverted microscope fitted with a 40x/1.3 Zeiss Plan Apochromat oil objective. Metamorph 7.7.8.0 software controlled stimulus switching and image acquisition through an Andor iXon+ EMCCD camera (as described above, Calcium Imaging of Individual Neurons). Light was delivered with a Lumencor SOLA-LE lamp, passed through a 1.3 ND filter, and narrowed to 484–492 nm using a CHROMA 49904-ET Laser Bandpass filter set. Illumination was constant and at full power, which may have elicited some light responses in ASK.
We used ImageJ’s ‘Align slices in stack’ plugin to correct for small amounts of motion. We then measured fluorescence values by selecting regions of interest that captured the AIA neurite, AWA nucleus, and ASK nucleus without background subtraction. We scaled fluorescence values separately for each neuron from 0 to 1 for visualization purposes. Many videos were discarded because fluorescence from other neurons prevented measurement of our neurons of interest, because our neurons of interest could not be captured in the same focal plane, or because the animal rotated during the video.
Electrophysiological recordings
Request a detailed protocolElectrophysiological recording was performed as previously described (Liu et al., 2018). Briefly, an adult animal was immobilized on a Sylgard-coated (Sylgard 184, Dow Corning) glass coverslip in a small drop of DPBS (D8537; Sigma) by applying a cyanoacrylate adhesive (Vetbond tissue adhesive; 3M) along the dorsal side of the head region. A puncture in the cuticle away from the head was made to relieve hydrostatic pressure. A small longitudinal incision was then made using a diamond dissecting blade (Type M-DL 72029 L; EMS) between two pharyngeal bulbs along the glue line. The cuticle flap was folded back and glued to the coverslip with GLUture Topical Adhesive (Abbott Laboratories), exposing the nerve ring. The gluing and dissection were performed under an Olympus SZX16 stereomicroscope equipped with a 1X Plan Apochromat objective and widefield 10x eyepieces. The coverslip with the dissected preparation was then placed into a custom-made open recording chamber (~1.5 ml volume) and treated with 1 mg/ml collagenase (type IV; Sigma) for ~10 s by hand pipetting. The recording chamber was subsequently perfused with the desired extracellular solution using a custom-made gravity-feed perfusion system for ~10 ml. All experiments were performed with the bath at room temperature. We used an upright microscope (Axio Examiner; Carl Zeiss, Inc) equipped with a 40x water immersion lens and 16x eyepieces to view the preparation. AIA neurons were identified using either GFP or GCaMP5A fluorescent markers. We made electrodes by using a laser pipette puller (P-2000; Sutter Instruments) to pre-pull borosilicate glass pipettes (BF100-58-10; Sutter Instruments) with resistance (RE) of 15–20 MΩ and back-filling them with the desired intracellular solutions. We used a motorized micromanipulator (PatchStar Micromanipulator; Scientifica) to control the electrodes. The pipette solution was (all concentrations in mM): [K-gluconate 115; KCl 15; KOH 10; MgCl2 5; CaCl2 0.1; Na2ATP 5; NaGTP 0.5; Na-cGMP 0.5; cAMP 0.5; BAPTA 1; Hepes 10; Sucrose 50], with pH adjusted with KOH to 7.2, osmolarity 320–330 mOsm. The standard extracellular solution was: [NaCl 140; NaOH 5; KCL 5; CaCl2 2; MgCl2 5; Sucrose 15; Hepes 15; Dextrose 25], with pH adjusted with NaOH to 7.3, osmolarity 330–340 mOsm. Liquid junction potentials were calculated and corrected before recording. Whole-cell current clamp and voltage clamp experiments were conducted on an EPC-10 amplifier (EPC-10 USB; Heka) using PatchMaster software (Heka). Two-component capacitive compensation was optimized at rest, and series resistance was compensated to 50%. Analog data were filtered at 2 kHz and digitized at 10 kHz or 50 kHz. For quality control, only patch clamps with a seal resistance above 1 GΩ and uncompensated series resistance below 100 MΩ were accepted for further analysis (seal resistance ranged from 2 to 10 GΩ; series resistance ranged from 17 to 95 MΩ for most accepted recordings). For the 1 pA/step experiment, a short −10 pA hyperpolarizing pre-pulse was added to each current injection step in the stimulation protocol to reset membrane potential to rest. Voltage or current measurement and data analysis were conducted using Fitmaster (Heka) and exported to OriginProt (OriginLab) for graphing. To reduce file size, electrophysiological recording traces shown in figures were re-sampled at 1 kHz by averaging adjacent data points.
Simultaneous electrophysiology and calcium imaging
Request a detailed protocolThe electrophysiology recording setup was as described above. GCaMP fluorescence in AIA neurons expressing soluble GCaMP5A was captured with a CoolSNAP HQ2 Camera (Photometrics) controlled by MetaMorph software. The onset of imaging acquisition and electrophysiology recording was synchronized by a TTL signal sent through Patchmaster. We illuminated the sample with a SpectraX Lumencor solid-state light source. Patch-clamping was performed under DIC illumination, and the sample was switched to fluorescent illumination once a whole-cell recording configuration was formed to begin simultaneous recording. Imaging was set at 20 MHz, binning at 2 and 50 fps. To quantify GCaMP fluorescence, we used ImageJ to capture the mean fluorescence of a hand-selected area around the AIA neurite. We bleach-corrected the resulting trace by fitting a line to the non-stimulus portions of the calcium trace, then subtracted and subsequently dividing the raw trace by the fitted line to achieve the ΔF/F0.
Data availability
All data generated or analyzed during this study, including source data, are included in the manuscript and supporting files.
References
-
The chemical component of the mixed GF-TTMn synapse in Drosophila Melanogaster uses acetylcholine as its neurotransmitterEuropean Journal of Neuroscience 26:439–445.https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05686.x
-
WebsiteChemosensation in C. elegans WormBook, ed. The C. elegans research communityWormBook. Accessed October 25, 2006.
-
The Caenorhabditis elegans odr-2 gene encodes a novel Ly-6-Related protein required for olfactionGenetics 157:211–224.
-
BookTheoretical Neuroscience: Computation and Mathematical Modeling of Neural SystemsCambridge: The MIT Press.
-
Efficient codes and balanced networksNature Neuroscience 19:375–382.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4243
-
Gap junctionsCold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Biology 1:a002576.https://doi.org/10.1101/cshperspect.a002576
-
Parallel use of two behavioral mechanisms for chemotaxis in Caenorhabditis elegansJournal of Neuroscience 29:5370–5380.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3633-08.2009
-
Sensorimotor integration in Caenorhabditis elegans : a reappraisal towards dynamic and distributed computationsPhilosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 373:20170371.https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2017.0371
-
Independent optical excitation of distinct neural populationsNature Methods 11:338–346.https://doi.org/10.1038/nmeth.2836
-
BookBiophysics of Computation: Information Processing in Single NeuronsNew York: Oxford University Press.
-
A circuit for gradient climbing in C. elegans chemotaxisCell Reports 12:1748–1760.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2015.08.032
-
A logical calculus of the ideas immanent in nervous activityThe Bulletin of Mathematical Biophysics 5:115–133.https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02478259
-
Action potentials contribute to neuronal signaling in C. elegansNature Neuroscience 11:865–867.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2131
-
Gap junction-mediated electrical transmission: regulatory mechanisms and plasticityBiochimica Et Biophysica Acta (BBA) - Biomembranes 1828:134–146.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbamem.2012.05.026
-
WebsiteSynaptic function WormBook, Ed. the C. elegans research communityWormBook. Accessed December 7, 2007.
-
WebsiteThe putative chemoreceptor families of C. elegans Wormbook ed. The C. elegans research communityWormBook. Accessed January 6, 2006.
-
Functional mapping of neurons that control locomotory behavior in Caenorhabditis elegansJournal of Neurobiology 56:178–197.https://doi.org/10.1002/neu.10245
-
In vivo calcium imaging of OFF-responding ASK chemosensory neurons in C. elegansBiochimica Et Biophysica Acta (BBA) - General Subjects 1790:765–769.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbagen.2009.03.032
-
The structure of the nervous system of the nematode Caenorhabditis elegansPhilosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 314:1–340.https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.1986.0056
-
Odour concentration-dependent olfactory preference change in C. elegansNature Communications 3:739.https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms1750
Article and author information
Author details
Funding
Chan Zuckerberg Initiative
- May Dobosiewicz
- Qiang Liu
- Cornelia I Bargmann
Howard Hughes Medical Institute
- May Dobosiewicz
- Qiang Liu
- Cornelia I Bargmann
Kavli Neural Systems Institute
- Qiang Liu
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Acknowledgements
We thank Philip Kidd, Sagi Levy, Aylesse Sordillo, Elias Scheer, Du Cheng, Audrey Harnagel, Alejandro López-Cruz, Likui Feng, James Lee, Javier Marquina-Solis, Andrew Gordus, Andrew Leifer, Vanessa Ruta, and Shai Shaham for thoughtful discussions and comments on the manuscript. We thank Mei Zhen for the unc-7 unc-9 double mutant strain. This work was supported by the Howard Hughes Medical Institute, of which CIB was an investigator, by a grant from the Kavli Neural Systems Institute to QL, and by the Chan Zuckerberg Initiative.
Copyright
© 2019, Dobosiewicz et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 4,285
- views
-
- 613
- downloads
-
- 35
- citations
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Neuroscience
General anesthesia (GA) is an indispensable procedure necessary for safely and compassionately administering a significant number of surgical procedures and invasive diagnostic tests. However, the undesired stress response associated with GA causes delayed recovery and even increased morbidity in the clinic. Here, a core hypothalamic ensemble, corticotropin-releasing hormone neurons in the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus (PVHCRH neurons), is discovered to play a role in regulating sevoflurane GA. Chemogenetic activation of these neurons delay the induction of and accelerated emergence from sevoflurane GA, whereas chemogenetic inhibition of PVHCRH neurons accelerates induction and delays awakening. Moreover, optogenetic stimulation of PVHCRH neurons induce rapid cortical activation during both the steady and deep sevoflurane GA state with burst-suppression oscillations. Interestingly, chemogenetic inhibition of PVHCRH neurons relieve the sevoflurane GA-elicited stress response (e.g., excessive self-grooming and elevated corticosterone level). These findings identify PVHCRH neurons modulate states of anesthesia in sevoflurane GA, being a part of anesthesia regulatory network of sevoflurane.
-
- Neuroscience
Pain is a private experience observable through various verbal and non-verbal behavioural manifestations, each of which may relate to different pain-related functions. Despite the importance of understanding the cerebral mechanisms underlying those manifestations, there is currently limited knowledge of the neural correlates of the facial expression of pain. In this functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study, noxious heat stimulation was applied in healthy volunteers and we tested if previously published brain signatures of pain were sensitive to pain expression. We then applied a multivariate pattern analysis to the fMRI data to predict the facial expression of pain. Results revealed the inability of previously developed pain neurosignatures to predict the facial expression of pain. We thus propose a facial expression of pain signature (FEPS) conveying distinctive information about the brain response to nociceptive stimulations with minimal or no overlap with other pain-relevant brain signatures associated with nociception, pain ratings, thermal pain aversiveness, or pain valuation. The FEPS may provide a distinctive functional characterization of the distributed cerebral response to nociceptive pain associated with the socio-communicative role of non-verbal pain expression. This underscores the complexity of pain phenomenology by reinforcing the view that neurosignatures conceived as biomarkers must be interpreted in relation to the specific pain manifestation(s) predicted and their underlying function(s). Future studies should explore other pain-relevant manifestations and assess the specificity of the FEPS against simulated pain expressions and other types of aversive or emotional states.