Visually-updated hand state estimates modulate the proprioceptive reflex independently of motor task requirements
Abstract
Fast signaling from vision and proprioception to muscle activation plays essential roles in quickly correcting movement. Though many studies have demonstrated modulation of the quick sensorimotor responses as depending on context in each modality, the contribution of multimodal information has not been established. Here, we examined whether state estimates contributing to stretch reflexes are represented solely by proprioceptive information or by multimodal information. Unlike previous studies, we newly found a significant stretch-reflex attenuation by the distortion and elimination of visual-feedback without any change in motor tasks. Furthermore, the stretch-reflex amplitude reduced with increasing elimination durations which would degrade state estimates. By contrast, even though a distortion was introduced in the target-motor-mapping, the stretch reflex was not simultaneously attenuated with visuomotor reflex. Our results therefore indicate that the observed stretch-reflex attenuation is specifically ascribed to uncertainty increase in estimating hand states, suggesting multimodal contributions to the generation of stretch reflexes.
Introduction
Our limb movements often deviate from planned trajectories due to imperfect learning of limb dynamics (Shadmehr and Mussa-Ivaldi, 1994), interactions between our body and the environment (Lackner and Dizio, 1994; Lacquaniti and Maioli, 1989), or the internal noise of neural processing (Apker and Buneo, 2012; van Beers et al., 2004). To correct movements from these deviations and to stabilize the motor system, feedback control mechanisms are essential. In addition to voluntary correction, a quick implicit sensorimotor process greatly contributes to feedback control.
For instance, a stretch reflex provides the function of feedback control, driven by proprioceptive input. When muscle spindles detect muscle stretch occurring with postural change, the muscle is activated quickly and involuntarily to compensate for the posture or movement error, which is one of the important functions of the stretch reflex (Marsden et al., 1981). The stretch reflex occurs earlier (~30–100 ms) than the voluntary response (>~100 ms), yet it also frequently shows goal-directed modulation. Long-latency stretch reflexes (~50–100 ms), which are mainly yielded via transcortical loop (Evarts and Tanji, 1976; Kimura et al., 2006; Pruszynski et al., 2011), in particular exhibit flexible modulation depending on various contexts such as the instability of environments (Shemmell et al., 2009), task instructions (Evarts and Tanji, 1976; Hammond et al., 1956; Shemmell et al., 2009), the direction of an up-coming external force field (Kimura et al., 2006; Kimura and Gomi, 2009), and the spatial properties of visual targets (Nashed et al., 2012; Pruszynski et al., 2008; Yang et al., 2011). Additionally, a visuomotor reflexive response also contributes to quick feedback control. Various studies have demonstrated functional modulation of quick corrective responses evoked by visual perturbations (cursor shift or background visual motion) (Abekawa and Gomi, 2010; Franklin and Wolpert, 2008; Gomi et al., 2006; Knill et al., 2011; Saijo et al., 2005).
Properly regulating goal-directed feedback control requires that feedback gain is adjusted with consideration to both context and current body states, including posture and motion of limb. One theory to account for the computational aspect of feedback control is the optimal feedback control (OFC) framework (Scott, 2004; Todorov and Jordan, 2002). That theory proposed that our brain continuously updates state estimates of the body by combining sensory inflow and internal prediction, in order to provide a state-dependent motor command according to the control policy (Dimitriou et al., 2013; Izawa and Shadmehr, 2008; Liu and Todorov, 2007; Wagner and Smith, 2008). Additionally, a number of studies have suggested that the brain integrates multimodal sensory inputs in a statistically optimal manner for perception (Ernst and Banks, 2002; van Beers et al., 2002) and voluntary motor control (Ronsse et al., 2009). However, for reflex control it is still under debate whether feedback responses are calculated from multisensory integration or are only generated by signals within a single modality (Cluff et al., 2015; Oostwoud Wijdenes and Medendorp, 2017).
As such, the present study examined how altering the properties of visual information, namely by distorting or eliminating visual feedback, affected stretch reflexes. Previous studies of multimodal integration (Ernst and Banks, 2002; van Beers et al., 2002) predict that these visual changes increase uncertainty of visual information, resulting in more weighting of proprioceptive information. If reflex gain is tuned according to this sensory-weighting, we could expect an enhancement of stretch reflex. On the other hand, these visual changes will reduce the reliability of limb state estimates, due to the combination of visual and proprioceptive signals. If reflexes are regulated according to the reliability of the multimodal state estimates, reflex gain could be reduced to suppress erroneous corrective outputs. Actually, Izawa and Shadmehr (2008) have shown that the amplitude of a quick visuomotor response is reduced with increased visual uncertainty. However, this finding was limited to unimodal sensory conditions. It remains unknown whether visual uncertainty also affects proprioceptive reflexes. Therefore, to investigate the effect of multimodal integration on quick sensorimotor control, and to differentiate that effect from goal-dependent modulations, here we focused on the modulation of stretch reflexes by visual information, while keeping hand movements identical across conditions. A part of this data has been preliminarily reported elsewhere (Ito and Gomi, 2017; Ito and Gomi, 2015).
Results
Stretch reflex modulation by visual rotation in visually-guided reaching
In the first experiment, we examined whether distortion of visual feedback alters the stretch reflex gain by applying visual rotations. Participants performed visually-guided wrist flexion, where they moved a cursor representing their hand position toward a visual target (Figure 1A). In each experimental block, a particular angle rotation (0°, 45°, 90°, 135°, or 180°) was introduced to the movement of the visual cursor (Figure 1B). Importantly, locations of the start and the target were also rotated, so that the required hand movements (wrist flexion) were identical across the blocks.
To characterize the effect of visual rotation on the stretch reflex in the flexor (agonist) muscle, a mechanical perturbation (MP) was applied at the wrist joint in the extension direction during wrist flexion movement (Figure 2A) for participants in the Agonist group (n = 18). Importantly, variabilities of the endpoint locations of unperturbed trials significantly increased in the large angle rotations (135° and 180°, p<0.05 by the post-hoc comparison after one-way ANOVA with p=3.65 × 10−5, F(4, 17)=7.67, partial η2 = 0.31) as shown in Figure 2B, although averaged movement profiles (movement durations, endpoint biases, and peak velocities) of unperturbed trials did not differ significantly from baseline (0°) by adding visual rotations (Figure 2—figure supplement 1A - C). We also confirmed that participants were correctly attending to the cursor movements, by checking correctly shifted movement endpoints in the catch trials (see Figure 2—figure supplement 1D) and short reaction times (198 ± 16.1 ms) to the cursor shift in the catch trials of all rotation conditions. Figure 2C shows temporal patterns of the wrist flexor muscle activities (rEMG) in three visual rotation conditions of 0°, 90°, and 180°. The rEMGs clearly increased around 30–100 ms after the perturbation onset in all three conditions, but peak amplitudes look different. To characterize the stretch reflex modulation, we quantified EMG of short- and long-latency components separately (middle and right panels of Figure 2D). Interestingly, the amplitudes of the long-latency stretch reflex were significantly changed by visual rotation angle (one-way ANOVA, p=5.45 × 10−9, F(4, 17)=24.77, partial η2 = 0.59). Post-hoc analyses indicated that long-latency stretch reflexes in the greater visual rotation (≥90°) conditions were smaller than that in the baseline (0°) condition (p<0.05). This means that the gain of the stretch reflex was reduced by introducing a large directional discrepancy between actual hand and visual cursor motions. This reduction is not explained by any changes in the background level of EMG activity (BGA in left panel of Figure 2D) which did not differ across conditions (p=0.087, F(4, 17)=2.13, partial η2 = 0.11). This modulation pattern for the long-latency stretch reflex was not observed for the short-latency stretch reflex (middle panel of Figure 2D), although we did find that the short latency components of 90° and 135° were significantly smaller than the baseline condition (p<0.05 by post-hoc comparison after ANOVA with p=6.07 × 10−4, F(4, 17)=5.58, partial η2 = 0.25). These results suggest that the reduction of the stretch reflex amplitude more robustly occurred in the long-latency component than the short-latency one.
To examine the effect in the extensor (antagonist) muscle, MP was applied in the flexion direction during the movement (Figure 3A) for participants in the Antagonist group (n = 10). As observed in the Agonist group, endpoint variations significantly increased for the large angle rotations (135° and 180°, p<0.05 by post-hoc comparison after ANOVA with p=1.27 × 10−4, F(4, 9)=7.78, partial η2 = 0.46) from the baseline (Figure 3B) while averaged movement profiles of unperturbed trials were not significantly different among visual rotation conditions (Figure 3—figure supplement 1A - C). Figure 3C shows an example of the temporal patterns of wrist extensor muscle activities (rEMG) in three visual rotation conditions of 0°, 90°, and 180°. As in the Agonist group, significant modulation of the long-latency stretch reflex dependent on the visual rotation angles (one-way ANOVA, p=0.011, F(4, 9)=3.83, partial η2 = 0.30) was observed (Figure 3D). Post-hoc analysis showed significant reduction of the long-latency stretch reflex amplitude from baseline for the greater rotation angles (≥90°). We did not find a significant difference in the level of background muscle activity (p=0.68, F(4, 9)=0.572, partial η2 = 0.060), or short-latency stretch reflex amplitude (p=0.30, F(4, 9)=1.26, partial η2 = 0.12) across the visual rotation angles. In summary, amplitudes of the long-latency stretch reflexes decreased in both the agonist and antagonist muscles, implying that distortions of visual feedback cause reductions in the gain of proprioceptive feedback, regardless of muscle acting direction.
Effect of visual feedback on online modulation of stretch reflex
In the second experiment, we additionally tested the contribution of online visual feedback to the modulation of the stretch reflex. Participants (n = 10) performed the wrist flexion task under normal and mirror-reversed visual feedback (Figure 4A) in separate experimental blocks. In half of the trials randomly selected in each block, the visual cursor was eliminated just after the hand movements started as shown in the bottom panels in Figure 4A. The averaged kinematic (wrist angle) profiles of mechanically unperturbed trials (dashed curves in Figure 4B) did not differ largely by changing the motion direction of visual feedback, or by eliminating the visual cursor. Indeed, a two-way ANOVA comparing peak velocities did not find significant effect of factor for either visual feedback type (p=0.57, F(1, 9)=0.35, partial η2 = 0.037) or cursor visibility (p=0.68, F(1, 9)=0.18, partial η2 = 0.020). As done in the analysis of Experiment 1, we examined the effects of our visual manipulation on the endpoint variability of movements (Figure 4C). A two-way ANOVA with factors of visual feedback type and cursor visibility, showed that both mirror-reversed visual feedback (p=2.8 × 10−3, F(1, 9)=16.51, partial η2 = 0.64) and the elimination of the visual cursor (p=8.5 × 10−6, F(1, 9)=81.13, partial η2 = 0.90) increased the endpoint variability of movements. But the interaction of the two effects was not significant (p=0.10, F(1, 9)=3.26, partial η2 = 0.27).
Figure 4D shows temporal patterns of EMG responses evoked by the MPs in the four conditions of a representative participant. As shown in these patterns, long-latency components were clearly modulated in different conditions, whose average and inter-subject variabilities are shown in Figure 4E. A two-way ANOVA showed a significant main effect of the factor of visual feedback type on the amplitude of the long-latency stretch reflex (Normal vs Mirror, p=6.0 × 10−4, F(1, 9)=26.53, partial η2 = 0.74), indicating that the long-latency stretch reflex was smaller with the mirror-reversed visual feedback than with the normal visual feedback.
There was also a significant main effect of cursor visibility on the amplitude of the long-latency stretch reflex (Show vs Hide, p=1.6 × 10−3, F(1, 9)=19.67, partial η2 = 0.69), indicating that eliminating the online visual cursor also reduced the long-latency stretch reflex gain. As observed in the endpoint variability, the ANOVA did not find significant interaction between the visual feedback type and the cursor visibility (p=0.41, F(1, 9)=0.75, partial η2 = 0.077). In other words, the effect of the distorted visual feedback and the effect of the visibility of the online cursor independently contributed to the modulation of the stretch reflex gain. In particular, the significant effect of cursor visibility demonstrates the contribution of visual feedback to the online tuning of the stretch reflex because the cursor was eliminated just after the movement start. Note that, although we found small but significant differences in movement durations and endpoints among conditions (Figure 4—figure supplement 1), probably due to the cursor elimination and mirror-reversal, we found significant effect of neither visual feedback type (p=0.61, F(1, 9)=0.27, partial η2 = 0.029) nor cursor visibility (p=0.091, F(1, 9)=3.58, partial η2 = 0.28) on the level of background EMG activity. Nor were those visual manipulations associated with significant effects (visual feedback type, p=0.47, F(1, 9)=0.57, partial η2 = 0.060; cursor visibility, p=0.31, F(1, 9)=1.18, partial η2 = 0.12) on the movement speed immediately before the MP application in the perturbed trials. Therefore, we can exclude the possibility that the observed modulation of the long-latency stretch reflex was caused by differences in background muscle activity or by differences in movement dynamics across conditions.
In the above analyses of Experiments 1 and 2, we consistently observed that the long-latency reflex component decreased and that the endpoint variance increased in response to altered or eliminated visual feedback. Actually, the negative correlation between the changes in reflex amplitude and in endpoint variance was statistically significant in the Agonist group of Experiment 1 (mean r = −0.46, 1000 bootstrap 95% CI [−0.62, −0.26]) and in Experiment 2 (mean r = −0.73, 1000 bootstrap 95% CI [−0.79, −0.66]). Since the imprecision of endpoints can be ascribed to the increased uncertainty in estimating hand states, the stretch reflex gain reductions could be ascribed to an increase in the uncertainty of the state estimation.
Theoretically, uncertainty of online state estimation gradually increases during movement for a certain duration after elimination of visual feedback (Wolpert et al., 1995). Therefore, to further examine our hypothesis, we varied the duration of cursor elimination in Experiment 3 (n = 10). As depicted in Figure 5A, in the baseline visual feedback condition (Show), the visual cursor was shown throughout the wrist flexion movement. In the other three cursor-elimination conditions (Short-hide, Middle-hide, and Long-hide. See Materials and methods for details), the visual cursor disappeared when the hand passed a certain location. Although movement durations and speeds were not identical among participants (Figure 5—figure supplement 1A and C), actual durations of cursor elimination until the perturbations were appropriately varied among the three hide conditions as shown in Figure 5B. As a result, we found a significant increase in endpoint variability of the movements as the duration of cursor elimination became longer (Figure 5C, p=1.3 × 10−7, F(3, 9)=23.00, partial η2 = 0.72), which could be due to an increase in the variability of state estimation. Note that we did not find a significant effect on movement duration, endpoints, peak velocity, nor BGA (Figure 5—figure supplement 1A - D). Meanwhile, stretch reflex amplitude significantly decreased with longer eliminations of the visual cursor (Figure 5D, p=2.6 × 10−3, F(3, 9)=6.11, partial η2 = 0.40). Importantly, correlation between reflex amplitude and endpoint variability was statistically significant (mean r = −0.62, 1000 bootstrap 95% CI [−0.830.33]). This negative relationship is consistent with the aforementioned interpretation, namely that stretch reflex gains are tuned in a manner, which depends on the certainty of the hand state estimation, which in turn utilizes online visual feedback.
Anti-reaction attenuates quick visuomotor response but not stretch reflex
In the above experiment, we examined the effects of distortion and elimination of visual feedback on the stretch reflex gain. While we hypothesized that uncertainty of multimodal state estimates causes changes in the reflex gain, another possible explanation is that the visual manipulation induces a ‘general inhibition’ of quick sensorimotor control (Beritov, 1968). If this is the case, decreases in response amplitude should not be specific to the visuomotor reflex, but may instead widely affect sensorimotor gains, including the proprioceptive reflex. Therefore, in the fourth experiment we examined whether gain reductions of quick visuomotor responses are always accompanied by gain reductions of the stretch reflex. To separately create similar complexities of hand-state dependent and hand-state independent visuomotor tasks, we used mirror-reversal and anti-reaction conditions. Participants (n = 8) performed wrist flexion toward the target as in the previous experiments (Baseline in Figure 6). They were additionally required to correct their hand movements in response to sudden target jumps (Forward/Backward in Figure 6) in order to evaluate reflexive visuomotor responses evoked by target jumps as well as stretch reflex responses. The Pro/Anti-reaction session (Figure 6A) tested the effect of the anti-reaction task on these responses, and the Normal/Mirror vision session (Figure 6B) tested the effects of mirror-reversal of visual feedback on these responses.
Figure 7A shows the temporal patterns of hand accelerations (top panels) and EMG (Flexor: middle panels; Extensor: bottom panels) of a particular participant in the Pro and Anti tasks. The hand acceleration patterns diverged around 200 ms after the target jump (top left panel). As reported in previous studies (Day and Lyon, 2000; Franklin and Wolpert, 2008), acceleration defined response onset (shown as triangles) was clearly later in the Anti task (top right panel) than in the Pro task (top left panel). Similarly, the EMG response was seen around 120 ms after the target jump in the Pro task, while retardation of flexor and extensor response onset was observed in the Anti task.
We calculated group mean and individual trends of the response latency trigged by target jump for the Pro and Anti tasks (Figure 7—figure supplement 1). Since the shortest response latency of EMG among all participants was 120.0 ms for Pro and 180.0 ms for Anti, we calculated the mean EMG activities observed in a time window of 115–175 ms as a measure of the reflexive visuomotor muscle responses (v-EMG) in the analyses below.
Figure 7B shows an example of hand accelerations (top panels) and EMG (Flexor: middle panels; Extensor: bottom panels) for target jumps and for no target-jump (baseline) in the Normal and Mirror blocks. In the Normal blocks, flexor muscle activity to forward target jump clearly increased in the time window for characterizing a visuomotor reflex (shaded time range). Similarly, increases in the activity of the extensor muscle for the backward target jump in the Normal blocks were found in this time window. Those flexor and extensor activities produced positive and negative changes in acceleration respectively, which resulted in quick correction of hand motion. In contrast, in the Mirror blocks, both flexor and extensor muscle activities started to change after that time window, according to the direction of the target jump.
To examine the condition dependency of the reflexive visuomotor muscle responses (v-EMG) quantitatively, we calculated the differences between v-EMG amplitudes of forward and backward target jump trials (Δv-EMG). Figure 8A shows the group mean of Δv-EMG in Normal/Mirror and in Pro/Anti-reaction sessions. Note that movement profiles are shown in Figure 8—figure supplement 1. Paired t-test showed a significant reduction of flexor Δv-EMG (p=0.013, t(7) = 3.31, d = 1.6) in the Mirror blocks compared to that in the Normal blocks. Similarly, flexor Δv-EMG was significantly smaller in the Anti-reaction block than in the Pro-reaction block (p=0.0088, t(7) = 3.60, d = 2.3). Compatible results were also obtained for the extensor muscle (Figure 8—figure supplement 2). These results indicate that quick visuomotor reaction (reflexive visuomotor response) could not be generated (i.e., was almost completely eliminated) in Mirror and Anti-reaction blocks.
In contrast, we did not find any significant difference in long-latency stretch reflexes between Pro- and Anti-reaction blocks (Left panel of Figure 8B, p=0.78, t(7) = −0.29, d = 0.020) while the stretch reflex was smaller in the Mirror blocks than in the Normal blocks (Right panel in Figure 8B, p=0.0062, t(7) = 3.86, d = 0.66). These results therefore indicate that reductions of visuomotor reflexes are not always accompanied by gain reductions of the stretch reflex.
The above contrast between modulations of reflexive visuomotor and proprioceptive-motor responses does not support the hypothesis that gain reductions of the stretch reflex are due to a general inhibition of reflexive motor responses, operating across feedback modalities. Instead, it specifically supports the hypothesis that the stretch reflex gain reduction observed in this study can be ascribed to the uncertainty of state estimation caused by distorted or eliminated visual feedback.
Discussion
The present study assessed the contribution of visual feedback to the gain modulation of the stretch reflexes. The first and second experiments showed that distorted visual hand cursor feedback and elimination of the cursor during movement, in both case significantly reduced the amplitude of long-latency stretch reflexes. The third experiment examined the temporal development of the reflex gain reduction after the cursor elimination, and the forth experiment demonstrated a dissociation between the gain modulations of the quick proprioceptive-motor and visuomotor responses by using different visual feedback conditions. Here we discuss a new aspect of the regulation mechanisms of the stretch reflex.
Visual stimulus effects on motor correction
By using distorted visual feedback (e.g., visual rotation and mirror reversal), several studies have investigated the contribution of the visuomotor transformations to the process of generating a visually evoked corrective response. However, it was unclear whether visual information affects only the visuomotor reflex. The present study demonstrates that the distortion of visual feedback can indeed reduce the gain of the stretch reflex, indicating a contribution of visual information to the tuning of the proprioceptive reflex as well.
Under conditions of mirror-reversed visual feedback, an early response to sudden cursor shifts is initially generated toward the incorrect direction (Gritsenko and Kalaska, 2010; Kasuga et al., 2015; Telgen et al., 2014), but participants can learn to suppress this incorrect response after extensive training. In addition, participants can attenuate the early reaching correction evoked by a target jump in anti-target jump tasks (Gritsenko and Kalaska, 2010). From those observations, one could imagine that suppressions of visuomotor reflexes are also accompanied by suppression of the stretch reflex, due to a possible general inhibition of reflexive sensorimotor processing (Beritov, 1968). Results of Experiment 4, however, do not accord with this idea. The amplitude of the stretch reflex under the anti-target jump condition was comparable to that under the pro-target jump condition, while it decreased as a result of the mirror-reversal of visual feedback. This discrepancy clearly indicates that the suppression of the visuomotor reflex does not necessarily lead to attenuation of the stretch reflex. Instead, our data imply that stretch reflex modulation occurs when distortion of visual feedback affects the state estimation of the motor system, as in the cases of the rotation and mirror-reversed conditions. Importantly, the anti-target jump task requires a change of the mapping from the visual target to the motor command, which is irrelevant to hand position representation. Therefore, this task would not affect the hand state estimation process of the motor system.
Crucially, we also found that altering visual-feedback increased the endpoint variability of movements in the current study. Many visual rotation studies (e.g., Cressman and Henriques, 2009; Mazzoni and Krakauer, 2006; Saijo and Gomi, 2012; Saijo and Gomi, 2010) have focused on the bias effects (endpoint shift) during two dimensional movements, while cursor elimination is well known to increase the variability of endpoints (Elliott, 1988; Proteau, 1992) and the uncertainty of state estimation (Wolpert et al., 1995). In contrast, we did not observe any biasing of movement endpoints in our rotated conditions, relative to the baseline condition. This may be because our one dimensional movement was much simpler than the two dimensional movements used in previous studies. In general, large angles of visual rotation, as well as mirror-reversals, increase reaction times (e.g., Fernandez-Ruiz et al., 2011; Saijo and Gomi, 2010; Telgen et al., 2014). This suggests that these visuomotor transformations are too complex for the nervous system to easily reconcile the mismatch between visual and proprioceptive information. As a result, movement trajectories are expected to become variant by visual feedback distortion, even after some amount of learning. This has been shown in a previous study (Buch et al., 2003). These observations therefore imply that distorted visual feedback could deteriorate the integration of visual and proprioceptive signals, in turn limiting the ability of the system to represent hand states, resulting in large variances of movement endpoints for large visual rotation conditions.
Stretch reflex modulation
Previous studies demonstrated context-dependent modulations of the long-latency stretch reflex according to task instruction, action intention, postural state, and external force field, as mentioned in the Introduction. Meanwhile, few studies (Crevecoeur et al., 2016; Mutha et al., 2008; Yang et al., 2011) have investigated how visual information contributes to the modulation of stretch reflexes. Mutha et al. (2008) measured stretch reflexes when goal location changed at the onset of reaching movements, and found increases in the long-latency stretch reflexes toward a new goal location followed by a late (voluntary) increase in muscle activity. This suggests a goal-directed modulation of stretch reflexes by visual information. However, that study did not examine whether visual feedback is utilized in representing limb state for the tuning of stretch reflexes.
To examine this point, we manipulated the properties of visual feedback (visual rotation and removing online cursor) without altering motor-task goals. As shown in the results of Experiments 1 and 2, changes in visual feedback reduced the amplitude of long-latency stretch reflexes without any changes in baseline movements or muscle activity (Figures 2, 3 and 4). These results suggest a new role of visual information, distinct from that used for goal-directed modulation of stretch reflexes. In contrast to our results, Crevecoeur et al. (2016) reported less contribution of visual feedback to stretch reflexes because they did not find any modulation in the corrective response to mechanical perturbations by the elimination of a visual cursor. This dissociation in reflex modulation in the current and previous studies could be due to task differences. In particular, the previous study employed a relatively static regulation task, where participants were required to recover a hand position to the position just before the mechanical perturbation. Since participants could detect the static postures before and after the perturbation using their proprioceptive information, they potentially completed the task by relying less on visual feedback. Meanwhile, in our experimental paradigm, participants should rely on visual information because they had to reach visual targets shown in the external workspace rather than recovering a posture. This relatively high importance of visual feedback present in our experimental task has brought to light the significant impact of vision on the stretch reflex.
Uncertainty in state estimates affects feedback gain
The stretch reflex modulation shown by our visual manipulation clearly supports the contribution of visual information of hand states to stretch reflex gain tuning. However, the observed reduction of stretch reflex amplitude cannot alone be explained by a simple sensory re-weighting between vision and proprioception. Since the theory of optimal sensory integration predicts a heavier weight on proprioception with uncertainty in visual information (Ernst and Banks, 2002; van Beers et al., 2002), an increase in the stretch reflex gain should be expected. However, we observed a decrease in stretch reflex gain.
To avoid possible inappropriate or harmful motor output due to unreliable state estimates, feedback gain should be reduced as mentioned in the Introduction. Indeed, several experimental studies have demonstrated that uncertainty of state estimate impacts feedback gain in online motor control. For example, Izawa and Shadmehr (2008) systematically manipulated the ambiguity of target location and showed that increased uncertainty in the visually estimated state causes reduction in the gain of visuomotor correction. Other studies have shown a decrease in the visuomotor reflex immediately after a dynamic update of visual information due to a target jump (Dimitriou et al., 2013) or saccade (Abekawa and Gomi, 2015), suggesting that uncertainty caused by a visual update decreases the reflex gain. Likewise, the present experimental manipulation of visual feedback could have made visual information unreliable (Saijo and Gomi, 2012; Wei and Körding, 2010). Theoretically, with the low reliability of visual feedback, limb states obtained by integrating visual and proprioceptive information also becomes less reliable, even though the optimal integration process maintains as much reliability as possible. In such situations, lower feedback gain would be preferable because larger feedback gain increases the risk of enlarging movement error by generating a large incorrect response due to erroneous state estimation. Consequently, if we assume the generation of stretch reflexes involves integration of multiple sensory sources, it is reasonable to expect that uncertainty in visual feedback will reduce stretch reflex amplitude, compared to situations where visual information is fully available.
Interestingly, Franklin et al. (2012) showed increases rather than decreases in the visuomotor reflex during force field adaptation, suggesting that uncertainty of limb or environment dynamics causes up-regulation of reflex gain. The optimal direction of the gain modulation may therefore depend on where the uncertainty originates. In situations with uncertainty in body dynamics or an unpredictable environment, higher gain would be required for quick error correction caused by the uncertainty (Franklin and Wolpert, 2008; Franklin et al., 2012; Shemmell et al., 2009). However, if, as in our study, uncertainty exists in sensory information, smaller gain is desirable (Izawa and Shadmehr, 2008; Körding and Wolpert, 2004) because it is not beneficial to generate a possibly erroneous response based on unreliable state estimates.
Next, we will consider how the reduction of stretch reflex gain can be explained by integrating sensory signals from different modalities. Even if we assume the different processing delays of proprioceptive and visual signals (Crevecoeur et al., 2016), decreases in the stretch reflex would not be predicted. Instead, we need to consider the possibility that feedback gain is regulated according to the reliability of limb states estimated via multimodal integration. For instance, according to the optimal control model, one possibility explaining reflex gain reduction is that the optimal policy calculating optimal feedback gain is modified to evaluate the uncertainty of the state representation. In such a case, the short latency stretch reflex, which might not be involved in high level state estimation, could be altered by top-down regulation, as well as the long latency stretch reflex. The simultaneous reductions in short-latency and long-latency components in Experiment 1 (Agonist group, 90° and 135° conditions) may be partly explained by this hypothesis. Another possibility is that the uncertainty increase in estimated states induces a reduction of the filter gain in the state estimation process, which could lead to a tracking delay in state estimation, resulting in a reduction of the corrective command to a transient perturbation. This possibility was examined by a model for visuomotor response (Izawa and Shadmehr, 2008). By considering the current findings regarding the influence of state uncertainty on the regulation of the proprioceptive-motor response shown in the current study, it seems possible that future studies will be able to extend the computational model to include multimodal integration for reaching movement. In both possibilities, to realize the predefined speed of movement we may need to assume that at least a part of the motor command is generated in a feedforward fashion, as has been examined previously (Bastian, 2006; Kawato, 1999; Sabes, 2000; Saijo and Gomi, 2010; Wolpert et al., 1998).
Visual effect on stretch reflexes in multiple stages of the motor control hierarchy
Additive modulations of the stretch reflex by visual discrepancy and elimination observed in Experiment 2 (Figure 4E) suggest that the stretch reflex is regulated by multiple factors in the motor-control hierarchy. One possible account is that the gain of the stretch reflex is regulated at two different stages of sensorimotor processing: motor planning and online-control. Many previous studies demonstrated that feedback gain is set before starting movement, depending on various contexts (Ahmadi-Pajouh et al., 2012; Evarts and Tanji, 1976). This planned reflex control would occur not only in a preset manner (Bonnard et al., 2004) but also in a time-varying manner (Kimura and Gomi, 2009). In addition to such planned reflex control, the reflex gain could also be updated during movement according to online sensory information (Mutha et al., 2008). Therefore, independent and additive gain control mechanisms could be assumed to exist in the neural processing of both movement planning and online-control.
Another plausible account of the additive effect in reflex gain change is that it is due to a gradual increase in uncertainty by cursor elimination combined with uncertainty caused by visual feedback distortion. As shown in Experiment 3, stretch reflex gain decreased with movement variability (Figure 5C and D), possibly related to the increase in uncertainty of state estimation over time (Wolpert et al., 1995). If the cursor elimination gradually increases the uncertainty of the state estimates under both of the normal and distorted visual feedback conditions, this account would predict similar temporal decays of reflex gain during the movements in those two conditions, which is consistent with the observed additive effect of visual feedback distortion and elimination on the reflex gain reduction.
In both of possible accounts above, the uncertainty of state estimation is an essential factor in stretch-reflex gain control. The present study demonstrated for the first time a contribution of visual feedback of the limbs to the modulation of the stretch reflexes independent of motor task. The results suggest that multimodal integration is involved in state estimation and that the uncertainty of this state estimation underlies the functional tuning of automatic feedback control. Understanding this hidden linkage between state uncertainty and reflex modulation is extremely important since regulation of proprioceptive-motor loop is essential in a wide range of motor controls.
Materials and methods
Participants
In total, 48 healthy volunteers (16 males, 32 females; age range 20–49, average 32 ± 9.1) participated in a series of experiments. Out of all the participants, four were tested in three of the experiments, seven were tested in two of the experiments, and the rests were tested in one experiment. Since seven participants out of 35 in Experiment1 could not satisfy a prerequisite of the task performance (See Data collection and analysis for details of the prerequisite), we excluded them from the analysis. All of the volunteers were right-handed. All gave written informed consent to participate in the experiments. All of the experimental protocols were approved (H28-011 and H31-007) by NTT Communication Science Laboratories Ethics Committee.
Apparatus
Participants sat in front of a horizontally set screen with their right hand tightly fixed to a custom-made manipulandum (maximum torque of 7.0 Nm) for wrist joints as shown in Figure 1. The right forearms of the participants were held on an armrest and tightly fastened by a belt. Hand movement was restricted to one degree-of-freedom movement (flexion and extension of wrist joint in a horizontal plane). The manipulandum was controlled by a digital signal processor (iBIS DSP7101A, MTT Co., Tokyo, Japan) at a 2000 Hz control frequency. Vision of the participant’s actual hand was occluded by a screen placed over the hand. Feedback of hand movement was provided by a visual cursor that was displayed on the screen via a projector (K335, Aser Inc, New Taipei City, Taiwan). Visual cursor position was updated as wrist angle changed. The instructed movement duration was 750 ms in all experiments, which was communicated to participants via beeping sounds presented at start of the trial and at the point of expected movement completion. To control movement speed, if the hand reached the middle point of the flexion earlier than 375 ms after trial onset, the trial was discarded and immediately restarted. In addition, if the hand movement stopped later than 750 ms after the start, an alert message was displayed on the screen. Visual and auditory stimuli were controlled via custom made programs using MATLAB (Mathworks Inc, Natick, MA, USA) and Cogent graphics toolbox (developed by John Romaya at the LON at the Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience). A photodiode was placed on the corner of the screen to detect the timings of stimulus changes, and its signal was recorded simultaneously with the other signals.
Experiment 1
Request a detailed protocolThe purpose of the experiment was to examine the effect of distortion of visual feedback on the amplitude of the stretch reflex. In the baseline condition, the visual cursor moved along the x-axis (left-right direction) according to the change in wrist angle, where displacement of the cursor x was calculated as x = -αθ. Here, θ was a wrist flexion angle from straight hand posture, and α (0.44 cm/deg) was the visual feedback gain. The origin of the coordinates was 25 cm away from the position on the screen above the rotation center of the manipulandum.
Participants were required to move a visual cursor from a starting point (x = −10.0 cm, θ = −22.5°) toward a visual target (x = −10.0 cm, θ = 30°) by flexing their wrists. We prepared five types of experimental blocks. In each block, visual rotation of the cursor movement was introduced in only one of the following directions: 0°, 45°, 90°, 135°, or 180°, around the origin of the coordinates (Figure 1B). Here, the visual locations of start and goal markers were also rotated to the same degree as the cursor rotations. In other words, required hand movement remained constant, while the degree of visual rotation changed in a block-dependent manner. The visual cursor was present throughout the trial, during both initial reaching movement (wrist flexion) and subsequent return movement (wrist extension).
In order to evoke stretch reflexes, a mechanical perturbation (half sine-wave torque pattern with 50 ms duration, 2.0 Nm peak amplitude) was applied randomly in 25% of trials. The perturbation was applied in the direction of wrist extension to evoke stretch reflexes from flexor muscles for the halfone group of the participants (Agonist group, n = 18), and was applied in the opposite direction to evoke stretch reflexes from extensor muscles for the others (Antagonist group, n = 10). The perturbation was initiated when the hand passed a predefined trigger position (θ = 3.75°). Note that actual hand positions when the perturbation was applied were slightly more flexed (mean ± SD across conditions: 11.03°±0.14°) than the trigger position due to a system delay in our experimental setup. There was no statistical difference between the wrist angles at which perturbations were applied in all the experimental blocks (Agonist group, p=0.34, F(4, 17)=1.16, partial η2 = 0.064; Antagonist group, p=0.15, F(4, 9)=1.78, partial η2 = 0.17). To keep the participants from executing hand movement without visual feedback, catch trials were included, in which the cursor position abruptly shifted (±12°) at the midpoint of the reaching movement. Participants were asked to compensate for the shift by bringing the cursor to the target as quickly as possible when the shift was perceived.
The experimental block consisted of 20 trials (five for no visual perturbation and no mechanical perturbation (N), five for mechanical perturbation (MP), five for forward cursor shift, and five for backward cursor shift. All trials were ordered randomly). All of the five experimental blocks with different visual rotation angles were ordered pseudo-randomly and repeated five times.
Experiment 2
Request a detailed protocolIn this experiment, we examined the contribution of online visual feedback to the tuning of stretch reflexes. For this purpose, we hid the visual cursor in half the trials, in addition to a directional change of visual feedback.
As in Experiment 1, participants (n = 10) were asked to make wrist flexion movements. We prepared two types of experimental blocks where the direction of cursor movement differed (Figure 4A). In one type of block, visual feedback moved in the same direction as actual hand motion, like with the 0° condition in Experiment 1 (Normal block). In another type of block, the direction of cursor movement was mirror-reversed relative to the actual hand movement (Mirror condition), which was nearly equal to the 180° condition in Experiment 1.
In both conditions, hand posture at the start was identical (θ = −22.5°), and three target postures (Standard: Std, θ = 22.5°; Near, θ = 16.5°; and Far, θ = 28.5°) were randomly applied, to prevent participants from performing the movement without visual information. In the Mirror condition, start and target locations were also mirror-reversed so that required hand movements were identical to those in the Normal condition. For half of the trials for each target, the hand cursor was shown throughout the trial (Show trials). In the other half, the hand cursor was hidden just after the detection of movement onset ( > 30 deg/s) (Hide trials), and reappeared after the stop ( < 30 deg/s). In the Show and Hide trials for the Std target, the mechanical perturbation (MP) extended the wrist when hand passed a trigger position (θ = 0°) to evoke the stretch reflexes in 40% of trials. In the other 60% of trials for the Std target, MP was not applied (N). Each experimental block consisted of 40 trials (12 Std w/o MP, 8 Std with MP, 10 Near, and 10 Far), half of which were Hide trials. We repeated a pair of Normal and Mirror blocks (order was randomized) six times. The order of all trials in each block was randomly shuffled. Conditions are summarized in Table 1.
Experiment 3
Request a detailed protocolThis experiment was designed to extend the earlier examination of the contribution of online visual feedback to stretch reflex amplitude, by manipulating the duration of cursor elimination. As in the other experiments, participants (n = 10) performed wrist flexion task from a starting point (θ = −22.5°) to a visual target (standard target: Std, θ = 45°), as shown in Figure 5A. In a baseline visual feedback condition (Show), the visual cursor was displayed at a fingertip position throughout the flexion movement. In the other three cursor-elimination conditions (Short-hide, Middle-hide, and Long-hide), the visual cursor disappeared when hand passed a certain location for each condition (+33.8° flexion for Short-hide, +16.9° flexion for Middle-hide, and +1.0° flexion from the starting point for Long-hide) as depicted in Figure 5A and reappeared after the movement stopped to show endpoint position. In randomly selected trials (40% of each condition), the mechanical perturbation (MP) suddenly extended the wrist when the hand reached a constant trigger position (+50.6° flexion from the starting point) to evoke the stretch reflex. In the remaining trials (60%) with a Std target, the mechanical perturbation was not applied (N).
To prevent participants from memorizing target location, we also required them to reach two additional targets (Near target, θ = 39° and Far target, θ = 51°) with Show and Long-hide conditions, in which the mechanical perturbation was not applied (N). One experimental block consisted of 80 trials (10 trials each for Show and the three hide conditions with Std target, 10 trials each for Show and Long-hide with Near target, and 10 trials each for Show and Long-hide with Far target) and was repeated six times. Trial conditions in each block are summarized in Table 2.
Experiment 4
Request a detailed protocolThe purpose of this experiment is to examine whether gain modulations of visual and proprioceptive reflexes occur jointly or independently. The experiment consisted of two types of sessions: Pro/Anti-reaction session and Normal/Mirror vision session. The latter session was included again in this experiment so as to compare the reflex modulations of those conditions within the same participants. Each participant (n = 8) performed both of the sessions, and session order was counter-balanced across participants. In each session, we measured both the stretch reflex evoked by mechanical perturbations and the visuomotor responses elicited by target jumps.
The session involving Pro/Anti-reaction blocks was designed to characterize the effect of the anti-reaction condition on visuomotor and proprioceptive-motor responses. Throughout this session, the moving direction of the visual hand cursor was consistent with that of the actual hand. Participants were instructed to flex their wrists from the start position (θ = −22.5°) to the standard target (θ = 22.5°) with the assistance of a visual cursor. As shown in the left panels of Figure 6A, small markers were shown as possible positions of the jumped target. In Pro-reaction blocks (Pro), participants were also asked to correct their hand movement as quickly as possible when the target jumped forward or backward (±12°, 30% trials for each, as drawn in the middle and right panels of Figure 6A). The target jump occurred when the hand passed a position of θ = 0°. In Anti-reaction blocks (Anti), participants were asked to correct their hand movement in the direction opposite to the target-jump direction and to bring the hand cursor to a small gray marker placed on the opposite side to the jumped target as depicted in the second row panels of Figure 6A. Namely, if the target jumped forward, the hand cursor needed to be moved backward, and vice versa. In both types of blocks, participants were required to move the visual cursor to the new target (marker) location within a duration of 400 ms after the target jump. In other trials (20% of all trials), mechanical perturbations were applied in the same manner as Experiment 2.
The session involving Normal/Mirror blocks was designed to compare visuomotor and proprioceptive-motor responses in these different visual feedback (Normal vs Mirror) conditions (Figure 6B). In the Mirror block, visual information (i.e., cursor, start position, and target) was displayed as a mirror-reversed image of that in the Normal block, as shown in Figure 6B. In both the Normal and Mirror blocks, a forward or backward target jump (±12°, as drawn in the middle and right panels of Figure 6B) occurred in pseudo-randomly chosen trials (30% of trials for each jump). The time limit for the adjustment in this session was the same as in the Pro/Anti-reaction session. The mechanical perturbation was applied in pseudo-randomly selected trials (20%) as in the Pro/Anti-reaction blocks.
One experimental block consisted of 60 trials (12 baselines, 12 mechanical perturbations, 18 forward jumps, and 18 backward jumps, in random order). In each session, participants performed two types of blocks, two times each, and the whole order of these blocks was randomized. The order of sessions was counter-balanced across participants. All conditions are summarized in Tables 3 and 4.
Data collection and analysis
Request a detailed protocolHand angle was measured with a rotary encoder (resolution of 0.0055°) attached to the manipulandum and sampled at 500 Hz. Its velocity and acceleration were obtained by calculating the differences between the flexion angles at each sampling frame and applying a low-pass filter (fourth order Butterworth filter, 40 Hz cutoff frequency). To evaluate movement accuracy and duration, start and end of hand movements were estimated for each trial. The start was determined as the point where hand velocity exceeded 5% of its peak, and the end was estimated as the point where the hand velocity became less than 5% of its peak and remained below this value for 300 ms. As an index of movement precision, standard deviation of movement endpoints was calculated.
Electromyography (EMG) data were measured from wrist flexor (FCR: Flexor carpi radialis) and extensor muscle (ECR: Extensor carpi radialis) of right hand using surface electrodes (Ag-AgCl disposable electrode, GE Healthcare Japan, Tokyo, Japan). The signals were sampled at 2000 Hz after filtering (0.53–1000 Hz) and amplification (MME-3116, Nihon Kohden, Tokyo, Japan). That data was high-pass filtered (zero-phase lag, fourth order Butterworth filter, with 50 Hz cutoff frequency) to remove motion artifacts and then was rectified. The rectified EMG (rEMG) was aligned on the onset timing of the mechanical perturbation to calculate the inter-trial average. To evaluate the amplitude of stretch reflex components, mean activities in constant time windows from the perturbation onset (background activities: from −50 to 0 ms, short-latency: from 30 to 50 ms, long-latency: from 50 to 100 ms) were calculated with reference to previous studies (Lee and Tatton, 1982; Cluff and Scott, 2013). To quantify inter-subject variation, the amplitude of the rEMG was normalized using reference activities during isometric contraction against reference torque of 1 Nm, which was recorded before starting the experiment. For visualization, we applied low-pass filter (zero-phase lag fourth order Butterworth filter with cut-off frequency of 100 Hz) to the rEMG and used it in plotting muscle activities during trials.
To examine the relationship between stretch reflex and movement variability, we evaluated the correlation coefficient between them across conditions. For each condition, we calculated the mean amplitude of the long-latency stretch reflex and the standard deviation of endpoints in unperturbed trials. Then, the correlation coefficient of these values across conditions was computed for each participant. We evaluated the statistical significance of the group mean of the correlation coefficients by a confidence interval with bootstrap resampling method.
In Experiment 4, we evaluated the quick motor responses evoked by target jumps as well as stretch reflexes. Angular accelerations of hand motion were temporally aligned using the jump onset of the visual target. To calculate response latency from the target jump, one-tailed successive t-tests (Crevecoeur et al., 2016; Diamond et al., 2015; Prablanc and Martin, 1992) were performed between the two acceleration data sets of the forward jump and for the backward jump for each participant. We defined the response onset as the first time at which the p-values of the t-tests were lower than 0.05 at 10 consecutive samples. We also calculated the response latency from EMG signal of both flexor and extensor muscles. The rEMG was low-pass filtered (100 Hz cutoff frequency, zero-phase lag, fourth order Butterworth filter) to obtain signal envelope, and then its response onset was calculated in a similar manner (but 50 consecutive samples) with that of acceleration.
To characterize the changes of the reflexive visuomotor responses, we evaluated the early component of muscle response elicited by the target jump. Previous studies (Day and Lyon, 2000; Franklin and Wolpert, 2008) showed that participants exhibit reflexive response toward the target jump direction in the Pro task, but cannot reverse it from the target jump direction in the Anti task. Therefore, in this study, the reflexive component (v-EMG) was calculated by taking average of rEMG during the interval between the response onsets in the Pro and Anti tasks.
In Experiment 1, a subset of participants showed a tendency to change their muscle activity due to co-contraction resulting from distortions of visual feedback. Since stretch reflexes induced by mechanical perturbation are known to scale with background muscle activity (Bedingham and Tatton, 1984), background changes caused by co-contraction could affect the amplitude of the stretch reflex. To rule out this possibility, participants were excluded from analysis (n = 7) if their mean background activity in any visual rotation condition was outside of ±25% range of the total average across conditions.
To remove outliers, we discarded trials from the analysis if the time average of the background muscle activity, the long-latency stretch reflex, or the reflexive visuomotor responses deviated by more than two standard deviations from the median of each condition. In addition, unperturbed trials were also discarded if movement duration or endpoint position was more than three times the median absolute deviation away from the median of each condition. In total, 2.2% trials were excluded, and the most trials removed from an individual across all experiments was 4.9%.
Experimental design and statistical analysis
Request a detailed protocolAll of the experiments and analyses were conducted using a within-subject design. We conducted repeated measures ANOVA to test statistical differences of behavioral data among conditions. Results of Experiment 1 were analyzed by one-way ANOVA with a factor of rotation angle of visual feedback followed by Tukey-HSD test as post-hoc analysis. Results of Experiment 2 were analyzed by two-way repeated measures ANOVA with factors of cursor direction (Normal or Mirror) and cursor visibility (Show or Hide). Results of Experiment 3 were analyzed by one-way ANOVA with a factor of locations of eliminating the visual feedback followed by Tukey-HSD test as post-hoc analysis. Results of Experiment 4 were analyzed by paired t -test to see significant differences between trial types for each session. The number of participants (sample size) was determined by referring to previous studies of stretch reflex modulation (Kimura et al., 2006; Kurtzer et al., 2008). To examine the short latency reflex, a larger number of participants were recruited by referring to a previous study (Weiler et al., 2019).
Data availability
All data generated or analysed during this study are included in the manuscript. Source data files have been provided for Figures 2, 3, 4, 5, and 8 for all experiments.
References
-
Spatial coincidence of intentional actions modulates an implicit visuomotor controlJournal of Neurophysiology 103:2717–2727.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.91133.2008
-
Online gain update for manual following response accompanied by gaze shift during arm reachingJournal of Neurophysiology 113:1206–1216.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00281.2014
-
Preparing to reach: selecting an adaptive Long-Latency feedback controllerJournal of Neuroscience 32:9537–9545.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4275-11.2012
-
Contribution of execution noise to arm movement variability in three-dimensional spaceJournal of Neurophysiology 107:90–102.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00495.2011
-
Learning to predict the future: the cerebellum adapts feedforward movement controlCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology 16:645–649.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2006.08.016
-
Dependence of EMG responses evoked by imposed wrist displacements on Pre-existing activity in the stretched musclesCanadian Journal of Neurological Sciences / Journal Canadien Des Sciences Neurologiques 11:272–280.https://doi.org/10.1017/S0317167100045534
-
Rapid feedback responses correlate with reach adaptation and properties of novel upper limb loadsJournal of Neuroscience 33:15903–15914.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0263-13.2013
-
Sensory recalibration of hand position following visuomotor adaptationJournal of Neurophysiology 102:3505–3518.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00514.2009
-
Dynamic multisensory integration: somatosensory speed trumps visual accuracy during feedback controlThe Journal of Neuroscience 36:8598–8611.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0184-16.2016
-
Voluntary modification of automatic arm movements evoked by motion of a visual targetExperimental Brain Research 130:159–168.https://doi.org/10.1007/s002219900218
-
The temporal evolution of feedback gains rapidly update to task demandsJournal of Neuroscience 33:10898–10909.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5669-12.2013
-
The influence of visual target and limb information on manual aimingCanadian Journal of Psychology/Revue Canadienne De Psychologie 42:57–68.https://doi.org/10.1037/h0084172
-
Reflex and intended responses in motor cortex pyramidal tract neurons of monkeyJournal of Neurophysiology 39:1069–1080.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1976.39.5.1069
-
Relation between reaction time and reach errors during visuomotor adaptationBehavioural Brain Research 219:8–14.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2010.11.060
-
Visuomotor feedback gains upregulate during the learning of novel dynamicsJournal of Neurophysiology 108:467–478.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.01123.2011
-
Specificity of reflex adaptation for task-relevant variabilityJournal of Neuroscience 28:14165–14175.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4406-08.2008
-
Spatiotemporal tuning of rapid interactions between visual-motion analysis and reaching movementJournal of Neuroscience 26:5301–5308.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0340-06.2006
-
Rapid online correction is selectively suppressed during movement with a visuomotor transformationJournal of Neurophysiology 104:3084–3104.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00909.2009
-
Nervous gradation of muscular contractionBritish Medical Bulletin 12:214–218.https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordjournals.bmb.a069553
-
ConferenceMirror-reversed visual feedback reduces quick somatomotor response evoked by mechanical perturbationThe Society for Neuroscience 45th Annual Meeting.
-
ConferenceRotated visual feedback of self-movement affects long-latency stretch reflexThe Society for Neuroscience 47th Annual Meeting.
-
On-line processing of uncertain information in visuomotor controlJournal of Neuroscience 28:11360–11368.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3063-08.2008
-
Learning feedback and feedforward control in a mirror-reversed visual environmentJournal of Neurophysiology 114:2187–2193.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00096.2015
-
Internal models for motor control and trajectory planningCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology 9:718–727.https://doi.org/10.1016/S0959-4388(99)00028-8
-
Temporal development of anticipatory reflex modulation to dynamical interactions during arm movementJournal of Neurophysiology 102:2220–2231.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.90907.2008
-
Flexible, task-dependent use of sensory feedback to control hand movementsJournal of Neuroscience 31:1219–1237.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3522-09.2011
-
Rapid adaptation to coriolis force perturbations of arm trajectoryJournal of Neurophysiology 72:299–313.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1994.72.1.299
-
The role of preparation in tuning anticipatory and reflex responses during catchingThe Journal of Neuroscience 9:134–148.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.09-01-00134.1989
-
Long latency reflexes to imposed displacements of the human wrist: dependence on duration of movementExperimental Brain Research 45:207–216.https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00235780
-
Evidence for the flexible sensorimotor strategies predicted by optimal feedback controlJournal of Neuroscience 27:9354–9368.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1110-06.2007
-
Reliability and efficacy of the long-latency stretch reflex in the human thumbThe Journal of Physiology 316:47–60.https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1981.sp013771
-
An implicit plan overrides an explicit strategy during visuomotor adaptationJournal of Neuroscience 26:3642–3645.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5317-05.2006
-
Influence of the behavioral goal and environmental obstacles on rapid feedback responsesJournal of Neurophysiology 108:999–1009.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.01089.2011
-
State estimation for early feedback responses in reaching: intramodal or multimodal?Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience 11:1–8.https://doi.org/10.3389/fnint.2017.00038
-
Automatic control during hand reaching at undetected two-dimensional target displacementsJournal of Neurophysiology 67:455–469.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1992.67.2.455
-
Rapid motor responses are appropriately tuned to the metrics of a visuospatial taskJournal of Neurophysiology 100:224–238.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.90262.2008
-
The planning and control of reaching movementsCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology 10:740–746.https://doi.org/10.1016/S0959-4388(00)00149-5
-
Large-field visual motion directly induces an involuntary rapid manual following responseJournal of Neuroscience 25:4941–4951.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4143-04.2005
-
Effect of visuomotor-map uncertainty on visuomotor adaptationJournal of Neurophysiology 107:1576–1585.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00204.2011
-
Optimal feedback control and the neural basis of volitional motor controlNature Reviews Neuroscience 5:532–545.https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1427
-
Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor taskThe Journal of Neuroscience 14:3208–3224.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-05-03208.1994
-
Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordinationNature Neuroscience 5:1226–1235.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn963
-
The role of execution noise in movement variabilityJournal of Neurophysiology 91:1050–1063.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00652.2003
-
Shared internal models for feedforward and feedback controlJournal of Neuroscience 28:10663–10673.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5479-07.2008
-
Uncertainty of feedback and state estimation determines the speed of motor adaptationFrontiers in Computational Neuroscience 4:11.https://doi.org/10.3389/fncom.2010.00011
-
Spinal stretch reflexes support efficient hand controlNature Neuroscience 22:529–533.https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-019-0336-0
-
Internal models in the cerebellumTrends in Cognitive Sciences 2:338–347.https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01221-2
-
Rapid motor responses quickly integrate visuospatial task constraintsExperimental Brain Research 211:231–242.https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-011-2674-3
Article and author information
Author details
Funding
Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (JP16H06566)
- Hiroaki Gomi
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by Grants-in-Aid for Scientific Research (JP16H06566) from Japan Society for the Promotion of Science to HG. We thank J De Havas for comments on the manuscript.
Ethics
Human subjects: All gave written informed consent to participate in the experiments. All of the experimental protocols were approved (H28-011, H31-007) by NTT Communication Science Laboratories Ethics Committee.
Copyright
© 2020, Ito and Gomi
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 1,823
- views
-
- 181
- downloads
-
- 13
- citations
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Neuroscience
Gamma oscillations in brain activity (30–150 Hz) have been studied for over 80 years. Although in the past three decades significant progress has been made to try to understand their functional role, a definitive answer regarding their causal implication in perception, cognition, and behavior still lies ahead of us. Here, we first review the basic neural mechanisms that give rise to gamma oscillations and then focus on two main pillars of exploration. The first pillar examines the major theories regarding their functional role in information processing in the brain, also highlighting critical viewpoints. The second pillar reviews a novel research direction that proposes a therapeutic role for gamma oscillations, namely the gamma entrainment using sensory stimulation (GENUS). We extensively discuss both the positive findings and the issues regarding reproducibility of GENUS. Going beyond the functional and therapeutic role of gamma, we propose a third pillar of exploration, where gamma, generated endogenously by cortical circuits, is essential for maintenance of healthy circuit function. We propose that four classes of interneurons, namely those expressing parvalbumin (PV), vasointestinal peptide (VIP), somatostatin (SST), and nitric oxide synthase (NOS) take advantage of endogenous gamma to perform active vasomotor control that maintains homeostasis in the neuronal tissue. According to this hypothesis, which we call GAMER (GAmma MEdiated ciRcuit maintenance), gamma oscillations act as a ‘servicing’ rhythm that enables efficient translation of neural activity into vascular responses that are essential for optimal neurometabolic processes. GAMER is an extension of GENUS, where endogenous rather than entrained gamma plays a fundamental role. Finally, we propose several critical experiments to test the GAMER hypothesis.
-
- Neuroscience
Motivation depends on dopamine, but might be modulated by acetylcholine which influences dopamine release in the striatum, and amplifies motivation in animal studies. A corresponding effect in humans would be important clinically, since anticholinergic drugs are frequently used in Parkinson’s disease, a condition that can also disrupt motivation. Reward and dopamine make us more ready to respond, as indexed by reaction times (RT), and move faster, sometimes termed vigour. These effects may be controlled by preparatory processes that can be tracked using electroencephalography (EEG). We measured vigour in a placebo-controlled, double-blinded study of trihexyphenidyl (THP), a muscarinic antagonist, with an incentivised eye movement task and EEG. Participants responded faster and with greater vigour when incentives were high, but THP blunted these motivational effects, suggesting that muscarinic receptors facilitate invigoration by reward. Preparatory EEG build-up (contingent negative variation [CNV]) was strengthened by high incentives and by muscarinic blockade, although THP reduced the incentive effect. The amplitude of preparatory activity predicted both vigour and RT, although over distinct scalp regions; frontal activity predicted vigour, whereas a larger, earlier, central component predicted RT. The incentivisation of RT was partly mediated by the CNV, though vigour was not. Moreover, the CNV mediated the drug’s effect on dampening incentives, suggesting that muscarinic receptors underlie the motivational influence on this preparatory activity. Taken together, these findings show that a muscarinic blocker impairs motivated action in healthy people, and that medial frontal preparatory neural activity mediates this for RT.