Navigating the garden of forking paths for data exclusions in fear conditioning research
Abstract
In this report, we illustrate the considerable impact of researcher degrees of freedom with respect to exclusion of participants in paradigms with a learning element. We illustrate this empirically through case examples from human fear conditioning research, in which the exclusion of ‘non-learners’ and ‘non-responders’ is common – despite a lack of consensus on how to define these groups. We illustrate the substantial heterogeneity in exclusion criteria identified in a systematic literature search and highlight the potential problems and pitfalls of different definitions through case examples based on re-analyses of existing data sets. On the basis of these studies, we propose a consensus on evidence-based rather than idiosyncratic criteria, including clear guidelines on reporting details. Taken together, we illustrate how flexibility in data collection and analysis can be avoided, which will benefit the robustness and replicability of research findings and can be expected to be applicable to other fields of research that involve a learning element.
Introduction
In the past decade, efforts to understand the impact of undisclosed flexibility in data collection and analysis on research findings have gained momentum – for instance in defining and excluding ‘outliers’ (Simmons et al., 2011). This flexibility has been referred to as ‘researcher degrees of freedom’ (Simmons et al., 2011) or ‘the garden of forking paths’ (Gelman and Loken, 2013) to reflect the fact that each decision during data processing and/or analysis will take the researcher down a different ‘path’. Importantly and concerningly, these different paths can lead to fundamentally different end-points (i.e., results and associated conclusions) despite an identical starting point (i.e., raw data) (Silberzahn et al., 2018). Often, researchers take a certain path without malicious intent to obtain favorable results (e.g., ‘p-hacking’; Head et al., 2015): the decision to follow a certain path may be based on unawareness of alternative paths (due to lack of specific background knowledge) or the researcher following the most obvious path from an individual perspective. The latter is influenced by the scientific environment, the research question at stake or practices previously published by researchers in the field.
Admittedly, there is substantial ambiguity in what constitutes ‘the best decision’ for data analysis, and none of the available options may be necessarily incorrect (Simmons et al., 2011; Silberzahn et al., 2018). More precisely, different paths in the garden of forking paths may be more or less appropriate for different research questions, experimental designs, outcome measures or samples. Consequently, it is notoriously difficult for researchers, particularly those new to a field, to make informed and hence appropriate decisions. As a matter of fact, it is difficult to anticipate the number of different paths available and the consequences of choosing one over the other, or to come up with facts that truly justify choosing one path over the other – even for experts in a field. However, simply choosing a particular path because others chose it before (i.e., adopting published exclusion criteria) can also be highly problematic, as decisions often hinge on study-specific characteristics that do not invariantly apply to other studies.
We argue that it is important to raise awareness to this issue. Specifically, we think that it is critical to discuss both the rationale behind and the consequences associated with taking different analytical paths in general and in specific sub-fields of research. Here, we exemplarily take up this discussion for human fear conditioning research as a case example for tasks with a learning element grounded in recent discussions in science in general (Flake and Fried, 2019; John et al., 2012) and in fear conditioning research specifically (Lonsdorf et al., 2017; Lonsdorf et al., 2019). Fear conditioning is a typical paradigm employed to study (emotional) learning and memory processes with a particularly strong translational perspective (Lonsdorf et al., 2017; Vervliet et al., 2013). Questions addressed in the field of human fear conditioning are often concerned with consolidation, retrieval, generalization or modification of conditioned responses. Hence, it has often been claimed that the study of these processes requires the acquisition of a robust conditioned response as a precondition. Therefore, participants are often (routinely) excluded from analyses if they appear to not have learned (‘non-learners’) or not have been responsive to the experimental stimuli (‘non-responders’) during fear acquisition training, in which one conditional stimulus (CS+) predicts an upcoming aversive unconditioned stimulus (US) and another conditional stimulus does not (CS–) (Lonsdorf et al., 2017; Pavlov, 1927).
Critically, ‘non-learning’ is most often defined as a failure to show discrimination between the CS+ and CS– in skin conductance responses (SCRs) – the most common outcome measure in the field (Lonsdorf et al., 2017). This practice may seem trivial at first glance and has been referred to as exclusion of ‘non-learners’, ‘performance-based exclusion’ or even ‘exclusion of outliers'. Yet, defining a set of characteristics to identify individuals who ‘did not learn’ is operationalized in very heterogeneous ways across studies. The same applies to the criteria that determine what constitutes a’ non-responder’ during fear acquisition training.
In addition to the heterogeneity in operationalization, other problems of performance-based exclusion of participants are worth noting: definitions of ‘non-learners’ are typically based on SCRs only (for exceptions see Ahmed and Lovibond, 2019; Oyarzún et al., 2019) and ‘non-learners’ are typically excluded from all analyses, that is, all experimental phases and outcome measures of a study. As SCRs are not a pure measure of either learning or fear, but rather reflect arousal levels (Hamm et al., 1993) that serve as proxies for fear learning, classification into ‘learners’ and ‘non-learners’ on the basis of this single outcome measure may induce substantial sample bias. First, defining 'non-learning' on one single outcome measure, such as SCRs, ignores the fact that successful CS+/CS– differentiation may be present in other outcome measures (Hamm et al., 1993) such as fear potentiated startle (FPS) or ratings of fear and contingencies (i.e., cognitive awareness of the CS+/US contingencies). As such, ‘non-learning’ as defined on a single outcome measure such as SCRs cannot comprehensively capture ‘non-learning’. Second, the level of responding in SCRs and CS+/CS– discrimination has been shown to be associated with a vast number of individual difference factors (Lonsdorf and Merz, 2017; Boucsein et al., 2012) such as age and sex (for a discussion see Boucsein et al., 2012), ethnicity (Alexandra Kredlow et al., 2017; Boucsein et al., 2012), genetic make-up (Garpenstrand et al., 2001), use of oral contraceptives (Merz et al., 2018b) or personality traits (Naveteur and Freixa I Baque, 1987). Consequently, excluding participants from an experiment as 'non-learners' may pre-select specific sub-samples and thus may thus severely hamper the generalizability and interpretation of the findings. Importantly, this practice may be a threat to and a limitation of the clinical translation of findings because it potentially leads to the selective exclusion of specific and highly relevant sub-groups. In fact, a recent meta-analysis suggests that patients suffering from anxiety disorders show overgeneralization of fear responding, which is enhanced when responding to the CS– (Duits et al., 2015), which may lead to reduced CS+/CS– discrimination if the response to the CS+ is comparable.
The concerns discussed above are merely based on theoretical considerations. Below, we aim to address the important and controversial topic of exclusion of ‘non-learners’ and ‘non-responders’ in human fear conditioning research empirically. We set out to provide an overview and inventory of the exclusion criteria that are currently employed in the field by means of a systematic literature search following PRISMA guidelines (Moher et al., 2009), covering a publication period of six months. Importantly, we distinguish between ‘non-learners’ (based on task performance, that is, CS+/CS– discrimination) and ‘non-responders’ (based on a lack of responsiveness) as assessed using SCRs. We expect the identified criteria for ‘non-learners’ and ‘non-responders’ to be characterized by noticeable heterogeneity (thus allowing for considerable researcher degrees of freedom) across studies. We thus aim to (1) raise awareness and (2) illustrate the impact of applying different exclusion criteria features (i.e., forking paths) on results and interpretation through case examples exemplified by the re-analyses of existing data sets. Finally, we aim to (3) provide methodologically informed, evidence-based recommendations for future studies with respect to defining and handling ‘non-learners’ and ‘non-responders’.
Results
Definition of performance-based exclusion of participants (‘non-learners’) and number of participants excluded across studies
Slightly fewer than one fourth of the records (i.e., 22%; 11 out of 50 records comprising 14 individual studies as three records reported two studies each) included in the systematic literature search employed performance-based exclusion of participants (i.e., SCR ‘non-learners’, Figure 1).
Strikingly, every single one of these records used an idiosyncratic definition to define ‘non-learners’, yielding a total of eleven different definitions in the short period of six months (see Appendix 1—table 1). The percentages of excluded participants varied from 2% to 74% (Figure 2A) of the respective study sample. Definitions differed in i) the experimental (sub-)phases to which they were applied (i.e., whether the full phase or only the first half, second half or even single trials were considered), ii) the number of trials that the exclusion was based on (varying between one and eight single trials), iii) the CS+/CS– discrimination cutoff applied (varying between <0 µS and <0.1 µS), and iv) the CS+ type (only non-reinforced or all CS+ trials) considered. The different forking paths and their frequency resulting from these combinations are displayed in Figure 2B.
The cutoff for CS+ versus CS– discrimination used to identify a ‘non-learner’ varied between <0 µS and <0.1 µS, with most records excluding participants as ‘non-learners’ if they showed either a negative discrimination (<0 µS) and/or no discrimination (≤0 µS). These criteria apply if the SCR amplitude in response to the CS– was higher than and/or equal to the amplitude elicited by the CS+. Furthermore, most records required this criterion to be fulfilled only during the last half or the full fear acquisition training phase. Of note, the number of trials included in the same ‘phase’ category is contingent on the experimental design and hence does not represent a homogeneous category (‘last half’ may include five trials for one study comprising 10 trials in total but 10 trials for a different study employing 20 trials in total.
Applying the identified performance-based exclusion criteria to existing data: a case example
We applied the identified cutoff criteria to an existing data set (Data set 1) to exemplify the part of the sample that would be excluded when applying different cutoff criteria (shown in different colors from yellow to dark blue in Figure 3) based on the most frequently used phase restriction: the last half of fear acquisition training. CS+/CS– discrimination was calculated on the basis of raw (A) or log-transformed, range-corrected (log, rc) scores (B), because it is not usually reported which data are used to classify ‘learners’ vs. ‘non-learners’. Strikingly, the proportion of participants that are excluded is higher when CS+/CS– discrimination is calculated on the basis of raw data rather than log-transformed and range-corrected data (despite employing the same criteria) in particular for the highest ‘non-learner’ <0.01 µS cutoff (76.7% versus 52.6%, respectively) (see Figure 3—figure supplement 1 for details).
In addition, we included a case example of two hypothetical individuals that differ in raw SCR amplitudes (ID#1: low and ID#2: high), but importantly show the same discrimination ratio (4:1) between CS+ and CS–(see Figure 3A). These two case examples illustrate that high CS+/CS– discrimination cutoffs, such as excluding individuals with discrimination scores < 0.1 µS as ‘non-learners’, favor individuals with high SCR raw amplitudes.
Unsurprisingly, the exclusion group defined by a CS+/CS– discrimination cutoff <0 µS showed inverse discrimination (CS–>CS+, not significant in raw SCRs [p=0.117]; significant in log,rc SCRs [p = 0.021]). Strikingly and more importantly, most cumulative exclusion groups, as established by defining ‘non-learners’ by the CS+/CS– discrimination different cutoffs in SCRs in the literature, in fact show statistically significant CS+/CS– discrimination (see Appendix 2 for details and a brief discussion).
Note that despite the different color coding, which serves illustrative purposes only, the groups are in practice cumulative. More precisely, the groups illustrated by lighter colors are always contained in the darker colored groups when applying the respective cutoffs. For example, the group excluded when employing a cutoff of <0.1 µS (mid blue) also comprises the groups already excluded for the lower cutoffs of = 0.05 µS (light blue),<0.05 µS (turquoise), = 0 µS (light green) and <0 µS (yellow). For illustrative purposes, the different groups are treated as separate groups in this figure.
Exploratory analyses of consistency of classification (‘learners’ vs. ‘non-learners’) across outcome measures and criteria employed
The convergence of non-discrimination across different outcome measures was investigated by testing for CS+/CS– discrimination in fear ratings in individuals with different amounts of CS+/CS– discrimination in SCRs as defined by the criteria described above. In fact, individuals with non-significant and inverse CS+/CS– discrimination (i.e., ≤0 µS) in SCRs showed significant CS+/CS– discrimination in fear ratings (t31 = 9.69, pbonf_corr < 0.000000001, d = 1.71, see Figure 4—figure supplement 1). Importantly, all cumulative exclusion groups showed significant CS+/CS– discrimination in fear ratings (all p ’s< 0.002, see Appendix 3—table 1).
We also illustrate (Figure 4) that the classification as ‘learners’ and ‘non-learners’ changes if two features (CS+/CS- discrimination cutoff and full vs. last half of acquisition training phase) of the criteria are changed (as illustrated in their full variation in Figure 2B).
The potential sample bias with respect to individual differences induced by employing different performance-based exclusion criteria: a re-analysis of existing data and a case example
Regarding the impact of performance-based exclusion on the pre-selection for certain individual differences, Figure 5 shows that the distributions of trait anxiety were shifted to the left (i.e., towards lower scores) with higher SCR CS+/CS– discrimination cutoffs. More precisely, this means that, in this sample, highly anxious individuals display smaller CS+/CS– discrimination in SCRs, and that excluding individuals who display low discrimination scores will lead to the exclusion of anxious individuals.
In fact, we observed a main effect of ‘Exclusion group’ on trait anxiety score (F[4,263] = 219.2, p<0.001, ƞP2 = 0.77). All exclusion groups (corresponding to the color coding in Figure 5) differ significantly from each other in their trait anxiety scores (all pbonf_corr ≤ 0.001), except for the group that did not show any CS+/CS– discrimination (=0 µS, light green, however n = 6 only), which showed significantly higher trait anxiety scores (mean ± SD STAI score: 43.8 ± 6.1) than the group with the largest CS+/CS– discrimination only (i.e., ≥0.1 µS, dark blue, n = 88, mean ± SD STAI score: 36.6 ± 8.5, pbonf_corr ≤ 0.001, CI [0.133 to 0.279]). Nevertheless, trait anxiety scores in this group (light green) were not significantly larger than those in the group with the negative discrimination (i.e., <0 µS, yellow, n = 89, mean ± SD STAI score: 40.5 ± 9.7, pbonf_corr = 0.10, CI [−0.004 to 0.142]), the group with a small discrimination score (i.e.,>0 µS but <0.05 µS, light blue, n = 43, mean ± SD STAI score 37.9 ± 7.9, pbonf_corr = 1.0, CI [−0.054 to 0.094]) or the group with the middle discrimination score (i.e., >0.05 µS but <0.1 µS, mid blue, n = 42, mean ± SD STAI score 38 ± 10.2, pbonf_corr = 0.11, CI [−0.005 to 0.145]).
Definition of ‘non-responders' and numberof participants excluded across studies
Thirty-two percent (i.e., 16 records) of the records in our systematic literature search included a definition and exclusion of ‘non-responders’, with percentages of participants excluded as a result of non-responding ranging between 0% and 14% (see Figure 6A). A single study (Chauret et al., 2014; Oyarzún et al., 2012) reported % ‘non-learners’ and % ‘non-responders’ as a single value (see Appendix 1—table 2). The definitions differed in: i) the stimulus type(s) used to define ‘non-responding’ (CS+ reinforced, CS+ unreinforced, all CS+s, CS–, US), ii) the SCR minimum amplitude criterion used to define a responder (varying between 0.01 µS and 0.05 µS; visual inspection), and iii) the percentage of trials for which these criteria have to be met (see Figure 6B and Appendix 1—table 2), as well as a combination thereof.
‘Non-responding’ was most commonly defined as not showing a sufficient number of responses to the conditioned stimuli (CS+ and CS–), less frequently by the absence of responses to the US or any stimulus (CS+, CS- or US), and in two cases by the absence of responses to the CS+ or context (CXT+) specifically (see Figure 6B). Not surprisingly, the percentage of excluded participants differed substantially depending on the stimulus type used to define ‘non-responding’ (CS based, 0–10%; CS+/CXT+ based, 10–11%; US based, 0–4%; CS and US based, 11–14%; pre-experimental test based, 5%; Figure 6A).
Despite these differences in the stimulus types used to define ‘non-responding’ in the first place, studies differed widely in the amplitude cutoff criterion to be exceeded in order to qualify as a response (see Figure 6B) as well as in the percentage of trials in which this cutoff had to be met (see Appendix 1—table 2).
The question of what (physiological) ‘non-responders’ during fear acquisition training are and how to identify them might be elucidated by investigating the number of ‘non-responses’ across trial types (CS and US) across data sets, and whether ‘non-responding’ to the US predicts ‘non-responding’ to the CS or vice versa. As expected from Figure 6A, the number of ‘non-responses’ to the US was low (as was also the case in our data [10%, Data set 1]), while the number of ‘non-responses’ to the CS (48.29%) was substantially higher – in particular for the CS– (58.6%; CS+ ‘non-responses’: 37.9%, see Figure 7A). This pattern, exemplarily illustrated here in one data set is representative of a larger number of data sets (see Appendix 4, table 1 for details). Furthermore, in our data (Data set 1), all individuals that did not react to the US in more than two thirds of the US trials also showed no responses to the CS (n = 3 of N = 119). To summarize, this provides the first evidence that ‘non-responding’ to the US may predict ‘non-responding’ to the CS but not vice versa. Furthermore, our data also suggest a positive correlation between the number of ‘non-responses’ to the US and the number of ‘non-responses’ to the CS (see Figure 7B for statistics).
Discussion
In this article, we showed that participant exclusion in fear conditioning research is common (i.e., 40% of records included) and characterized by substantial operationalizational heterogeneity of definitions for ‘non-learners’ and (physiological) ‘non-responders’. Furthermore, we provide case-examples that illustrate: i) the futility of some definitions of ‘non-learners’ (i.e., when those classified as ‘non-learners’ in fact show significant discrimination on both ratings and SCRs as illustrated in Appendix 3 and Appendix 2, respectively) when applied to our data; and ii) the potential sample bias induced by excluding ‘non-learners’ with respect to individual differences. Furthermore, we provide an overview of SCR ‘non-responses’ to different stimulus types (CS+, CS– and US) across different data sets (see Appendix 4—tables 1 and 2) as a guide for developing evidence-based criteria to define ‘non-responders’. Together, we believe that this work contributes to: i) raising awareness of some of the problems associated with performance-based exclusion of participants (‘non-learners’) and of how this exclusion is implemented, ii) facilitating decision-making on which criteria to employ and not to employ, iii) enhancing transparency and clarity in future publications, and thereby iv) fostering reproducibility and robustness as well as clinical translation in the field of fear conditioning research and beyond.
‘Non-learners’: conclusions, caveats and considerations
Operationalizational heterogeneity is illustrated by every single record in our systematic literature search (covering a six months period) that employed definitions of ‘non-learners’ using a set of idiosyncratic criteria. The true number of definitions in the field applied over decades will be even substantially larger. In the records included here, 6–52% of participants were excluded (disregarding one study reporting percentages of ‘non-learners’ and ‘non-responders’ together with 74%; cf. Figure 2A), which substantially exceed the percentages recently put forward for ‘non-learning’ exclusions (Marin et al., 2019) that were suggested to lie between 4% (Chauret et al., 2014) and 19% (Oyarzún et al., 2012).
If several thousand analytical pipelines can be applied, the likelihood of false positives is high (Munafò et al., 2017) and the temptation of their opportunistic (ab)use must be considered a threat. Hence, a constructive discussion on where to go from here and how to not get lost in the garden of forking paths is important. This being said, we do acknowledge that certain research questions or the use of different recording equipment (robust lab equipment vs. novel mobile devices such as smartwatches) may potentially require distinct data-processing pipelines and the exclusion of certain observations (Silberzahn et al., 2018; Simmons et al., 2011), and hence it is not desirable to propose rigid and fixed rules for generic adoption. Procedural differences, in particular the inclusion of outcome measures that require certain triggers to elicit a response (such as startle responses or ratings) have also been shown to impact on the learning process itself (Sjouwerman et al., 2016). Rather, we call for a reconsideration of methods in the field and want to raise awareness to the pitfalls of adopting exclusion criteria from previously published work without critical evaluation of whether these apply meaningfully to one’s own research. Furthermore, we want to promote the adoption of transparent reporting of data processing, recording and analyses and strive to suggest standards in the field to reduce heterogeneity based on idiosyncratic customs rather than methodological and theoretical considerations (see Box 1).
Box 1.
List of reporting details, potential difficulties and recommendations when excluding ‘non-learners’ (performance-based exclusion) and/or ‘non-responders’ with a focus on SCRs.
Please note that this Box can be annotated online.
(A) General reporting details | |||
---|---|---|---|
What to report? | Why is this considered important? | What can go wrong or be ambiguous? | Recommendations on how to proceed |
Details on data recording and response quantification pipeline | • because differences in data recording and quantification (i.e., response scoring) can make a substantial difference | • report recording equipment and all settings used (e.g., filter) • report software used for response quantification • report precise details of response quantification | |
Minimal response criterion (μS) to define a valid SCR | • to define valid responses | • minimally detectable amplitude (e.g., 0.01, 0.02, 0.03, 0.05 µS, etc.) may be sample- and equipment-specific • no clear recommendations (existing guidelines provide a range of 0.01 to 0.05 µS) because this is influenced by noise level and equipment | • test different minimal response criteria in the data set and define the cutoff empirically. In our experience (Data set 1), a cutoff was easily determined empirically by visually inspecting responses at different cutoffs (e.g., <0.01 µS, between 0.01 µS and 0.02 µS) and by evaluating their discrimination from noise |
Whether the first CS+ and/or the first CS– trial is included or not, and information on trial sequence | • no learning can be evident in the first trial, as the first US may occur at the earliest at the end of the CS+ and hence after the scoring window for the CS+-induced SCR • if the first trial is a CS–, no learning can have taken place as the US has not been presented yet • inclusion of the first trial (or the first trials in partial reinforcement protocols) may thus artificially reduce CS+/CS– discrimination | • in fully randomized partial reinforcement protocols, US presentations may cluster in the first or last half of the acquisition training, which will impact on CS+/CS–discrimination in SCRs | • careful experimental design with respect to trial-sequences (in particular in partial reinforcement protocols) • report whether the first trial for both CS+ and CS– is excluded because it may induce noise and bias CS+/CS– discrimination towards non-discrimination and as the first trial is sensitive to trial sequence effects |
Precise number of trials considered (if applicable for each trial type including reinforced and non-reinforced CS+ trials in case of partial reinforcement) | • often difficult/ambiguous to infer this information from the 'Materials and methods' section of a reporta • number of trials that the ‘last half’ or ‘full phase’ refers to is contingent on experimental design and hence ambiguous and imprecise (see Figure 2B) | • precision in reporting rather than relying on the reader making the right inferences • specify clearly the number of trials per stimulus type that are comprised in the ‘last half’ or ‘full phase’ • provide a justification (theoretical and/or empirical) for this decisionb | |
Details of whether results were based on raw or transformed data | • typically, transformations are required to allow interpretation of the reported results and to meet the assumptions of commonly statistical models | • report details of transformation (e.g., logarithmized [log/LN], range-corrected, square-root) including the number of trials considered (for each stimulus type) and the sequence of transformations applied and specific formula (e.g., for range-correction) • provide justification for any applied transformation (e.g., violation of assumption of normal distribution of residuals) | |
Precise number of excluded participants and specific reasons | • often difficult/ambiguous to infer this information from the 'Materials and methods' section of a reporta | • different researchers have different opinions on what ‘exclusion’ is (e.g., having individuals discontinue after a first experimental day based on performance should be considered and reported as exclusion) | • report a breakdown of specific reasons for exclusions with respective n’s |
(B) Specific reporting details for exclusion of ‘non-learners’ | |||
What to report? | Why is this considered important? | What can go wrong or be ambiguous? | Recommendations on how to proceed |
CS+/CS– discrimination is calculated on the basis of raw SCR or transformed (e.g., logarithmized [log/LN], range-corrected, square-root) scores | • the same criteria lead to different proportions of excluded individuals when applying them to raw or transformed data (see Figure 3A and B) | • exact details of transformations (optimally calculation formulas) need to be included for full transparency and reproducibility | |
Minimal differential (CS+ vs. CS) cutoff for ‘non-learning’ in μS | • different cutoffs lead to very different proportions of individuals excluded (see Figure 3) | • exact details on cutoffs need to be included for full transparency and reproducibility | |
On what outcome measures is ‘non-learning’ determined? | • ‘non-learners’ do not necessarily converge across different outcome measures (Appendix 3, Figure 4—figure supplement 1) | • all outcome measures recorded need to be reported | • ‘non-learning’ should not be based on a single outcome measure or a clear justification needs to be provided as to why a single measure is considered meaningful |
If ‘non-learning’ is determined by responding during fear acquisition training, which trial types and number of trials per trial type were considered? | • depending on the criteria employed, the same individual may be classified as ‘learner’ or ‘non-learner’ (see Figure 4) | • classification as ‘non-learner’ should be based on differential scores (CS+ vs. CS–), and the number of trials included for this calculation should be clearly justified. Providing a generally valid recommendation regarding the number of trials to be included is difficult because it critically depends on experimental design choices | |
If ‘non-learning ‘criteria are used, do they differ from criteria that the researcher or the research group used in previous publications? If yes, why were the criteria changed? | • provide explicit justifications on why different criteria were used previously and presently | • report differences between present and previous criteria used including references and justifications | |
Did ‘non-learners’ really fail to learn? | • important as a manipulation check but note that the absence of a statistically significant CS+/CS– discrimination effect in a group on average cannot be taken to imply that all individuals in this group do not show meaningful CS+/CS–discrimination | • individuals classified as ‘non-learners’ may in fact show significant CS+/CS– discrimination in SCRs (see Appendix 2) or in other outcome measures (see Figure 3—figure supplement 1 and Appendix 4) and hence fail the manipulation check | • do the groups classified as ‘non-learners’ and ‘learners’ differ significantly in discrimination, and do ‘non-learners’ really not discriminate in SCRs and other outcome measures? Report the data on this group graphically and/or statistically in the supplementary material (do not report the full sample with and without exclusions only) |
Are results contingent on the exclusion of ‘non-learners’? | • important to allow for transparency and to evaluate the impact of the results | • it is not clearly defined when results differ meaningfully when excluding and including ‘non-learners’ | • provide results with and without exclusion of ‘non-learners’ • additional analyses can be provided as supplementary material. When results are not contingent on the exclusion of ‘non-learners’, it is sufficient to mention this briefly in the results of the main manuscript (e.g., results are not contingent on the exclusion of ‘non-learners’) • if the results of the main analyses and hence the main conclusions change when ‘non-learners’ are excluded, this needs to be included in the main manuscript , and the implications need to be adequately discussed. Please note that this does not necessarily invalidate findings but can refine them |
Descriptive statistics for excluded ‘non-learners’ | • important to allow for transparency and evaluation of the potential sample biases introduced | • report sex, age, anxiety levels, awareness | |
(C) Specific reporting details for exclusions of ‘non-responders’ | |||
What to report? | Why is this considered important? | What can go wrong or be ambiguous? | Recommendations on how to proceed |
Whether ‘non-responses’ are calculated on the basis of raw SCR or transformed (e.g., logarithmized [log/LN], range-corrected, square-root) scores | • the same criteria lead to different proportions of excluded individuals when applying to raw or transformed data (see Figure 3A and B) | • exact details of transformations (optimally calculation formulas) need to be included for full transparency and reproducibility | |
Minimal cutoff for ‘non-responses’ in μS | • it is often difficult/ambiguous to infer this information from the 'Materials and methods' section of a reporta • higher cutoffs could unnecessarily reduce the sample size | • exact details on cutoffs need to be included for full transparency and reproducibility | |
Was ‘non-responding’ determined in a pre-experimental phase such as forced-breathing or US calibration? | • determining ‘non-responding’ during a pre-experimental phase may help to detect malfunctioning of the equipment and allow this to be corrected prior to data acquisition • classification of ‘non-responders’ independent of the experimental task and its specifications (e.g., number of US presentations) | • electrodes may detach between the pre-experimental phase and fear acquisition training | • report details of pre-experimental phase • classification in SCR ‘non-responders’ should be based on a pre-experimental phase if no US presentations occur during the experiment, such as in case of threat of shock experiments, observational conditioning, extinction or return of fear tests |
If ‘non-responding’ is determined by responding during fear acquisition training, what trial types are considered? | • frequency of ‘non-responding’ differs substantially between different stimuli (CS and US) but also between CS+ and CS– (see Figure 7A) | • ‘non-responding’ to the US may be due to technical failure (i.e., no US was administered) | • classification in SCR ‘non-responders’ should not be based on SCRS elicited by CS (CS+, CS– or both), but should be based on US responding • a question on the estimated number of US presented during fear acquisition training (and all other phases) may serve as a manipulation check |
Descriptive statistics for excluded ‘non-responder’ | • important to allow for transparency and evaluation of the potential sample biases introduced | • report sex, age, anxiety levels, awareness |
-
a based on our experience with extracting this information from literature identified in the systematic literature search reported in this manuscript.
b ‘others have done this previously’ is not an acceptable justification in our point of view.
Yet, there are many other critical considerations worth discussing beyond the heterogeneous criteria used to define ‘non-learners’ and their impact on the outcome of statistical tests:
First, ‘performance-based exclusion of participants’ is often based on a single outcome measure (typically SCRs), despite multiple measures being recorded (for exceptions see Ahmed and Lovibond, 2019; Belleau et al., 2018; Oyarzún et al., 2012). Importantly, ‘fear learning’ cannot be reliably inferred by means of SCRs, because SCRs capture arousal-related processes and can only be used as a proxy to infer ‘fear learning’ as fear is closely linked to arousal (Hamm and Weike, 2005). Relatedly, the fact that physiological proxies of ‘fear’ do not map onto ‘fear’ itself has been discussed extensively (LeDoux, 2012; LeDoux, 2014).
Second, but related, individuals that fail to show CS+/CS– discrimination in SCRs may show substantial discrimination, as an indicator of successful learning, in other outcome measures such as ratings of fear, US expectancy or fear potentiated startle (Hamm and Weike, 2005; Marin et al., 2019), as illustrated here for fear ratings (see Figure 4—figure supplement 1 and Appendix 3—table 1).
Third, a common justification for excluding ‘non-learners’ is that it is not possible to investigate extinction- or return-of-fear-related phenomena in individuals who ‘did not learn’. To our knowledge, there is some evidence (Craske et al., 2008; Plendl and Wotjak, 2010; Prenoveau et al., 2013) that this theoretical assumption does not necessarily hold true, (i.e., CS+/CS– discrimination during fear acquisition training does not necessarily predict CS+/CS– discrimination during other experimental phases) (Gerlicher et al., 2019). An empirical investigation of this, however, would go beyond this manuscript’s scope.
Fourth, we provided empirical evidence that those classified as a group of ‘non-learners’ in SCRs in the literature (sometimes referred to as ‘outliers’) on the basis of the identified definitions in fact displayed significant CS+/CS– discrimination when applied to our own data. An exception to this was using cut offs in differential responding of <0.05 µS (note, however, that a non-significant CS+/CS– discrimination effect in the group of ‘non-learners’ as a whole cannot be taken as evidence that all individuals in this group do not in fact display meaningful or statistically significant CS+/CS– discrimination). Hence, in addition to the many conceptual problems we raised here, the operationalization of ‘non-learning’ in the field failed its critical manipulation check given that those classified as ‘non-learners’ show clear evidence of learning as a group (i.e., CS+/CS– discrimination, see Appendix 2—table 1).
Fifth, we illustrate a concerning sample bias that is introduced by performance-based participant exclusion. CS+/CS– discrimination in SCRs during fear acquisition training has been linked to a number of individual difference factors (Lonsdorf and Merz, 2017) and, naturally, selecting participants on the basis of SCR CS+/CS– discrimination will also select them on the basis of these individual differences (illustrated by our case example on trait anxiety, Figure 5). In our case example, we illustrate that excluding ‘non-learners’ biases the sample towards low anxiety scores, which hampers the generalizability and replicability of findings: i) the effect may only exist in low-anxiety individuals but not in the general population, and ii) as fear acquisition is a clinically relevant paradigm, pre-selection in favor of low-anxiety individuals might represent a threat to the clinical translation of the findings. Many studies in the field of fear conditioning research aim to develop behavioral or pharmacological manipulations to enhance treatment effects or aim to study mechanisms that are relevant for clinical fear and anxiety. Hence, it is highly problematic that these studies may exclude individuals who show response patterns that mimic responses typically observed in anxiety patients when excluding ‘non-learners’. In fact, patients suffering from anxiety disorders have been shown to be characterized by generalization of fear from the CS+ to the CS– (Duits et al., 2015).
Sixth, as illustrated by our case example (Figure 3), high CS+/CS– discrimination cutoffs generally favor individuals with high SCR amplitudes despite potentially identical ratios between CS+ and CS– amplitudes, which may introduce a sampling bias for individuals characterized by high arousal levels that probably have biological underpinnings. Relatedly, future studies need to empirically address which criteria for SCR transformation and exclusions are more or less sensitive to baseline differences (for an example from startle responding see Bradford et al., 2015; Grillon and Baas, 2002).
In summary, in light of the many (potential) problems associated with performance-based exclusion of participants, we forcefully echo Marin et al.’s conclusion that one needs "to be cautious when excluding SCR non-learners and to consider the potential implications of such exclusion when interpreting the findings from studies of conditioned fear" (Marin et al., 2019, abstract). Routinely, excluding participants who are intentionally or unintentionally characterized by specific individual differences represents a major threat to generalizability, replicability and potentially clinical translation of findings, as results might be contingent on a specific sub-sample and specific sample characteristics. This is also true when researchers are interested in the study of general processes. Furthermore, by excluding these individuals from further analyses, we may miss the opportunity to understand why some individuals do not show discrimination between the CS+ and the CS– in SCRs (or other outcome measures) or whether this lack of discrimination is maintained across subsequent experimental phases. It can be speculated that this lack of discrimination may carry meaningful information – at least for a subsample.
‘Non-responders’: conclusions, caveats and considerations
In addition to ‘non-learners’, ‘non-responders’ are also often excluded during fear conditioning research. We showed that the definition of ‘non-responders’, like that of ‘non-learners’, varies widely across studies. Heterogeneity in definitions manifests in different cutoff criteria for what is considered a valid response, the number of trials and the stimulus type(s) considered (Appendix 1—table 2, Figure 6). Surprisingly, most definitions are based on CS responses (i.e., SCRs to the CS+ and/or CS–) and only few are based on US responses. This highlights a potentially problematic overlap between ‘non-learners’ and ‘non-responders’: ‘non-responding’ to the CS (i.e., CS+ and CS– or CS+ only) is not necessarily indicative of physiological ‘non-responding’ – especially if high cutoffs are used. In fact, ‘non-responding’ to the CS may, or at least in some cases, reflect the absence of learning-based patterns in physiological responding – which may carry important information. Having observed the striking differences in percentages of ‘non-responses’ to the US (10%) and CS (48%) observed in our data (see Figure 7 and Appendix 4—table 1), we suggest that physiological ‘non-responding’ cannot and should not be determined on the basis of the absence of responding to the CS.
More globally, the group of ‘non-responders’, as defined by the criteria identified here, probably lumps together several sub-groups: individuals (1) for whom technical problems resulted in no valid SCRs, (2) who fell asleep or did not pay attention, (3) who cognitively learned the CS+/US contingencies but did not express the expected corresponding responses in SCRs, and (4) who were attentive to the experiment but did not learn the contingencies (i.e., unaware participants) and hence did not show the expected SCR patterns (Tabbert et al., 2011).
In summary, although excluding physiological ‘non-responders’ makes sense (in terms of a manipulation check and independent of the hypothesis), we consider defining ‘non-responders’ on the basis of the absence of SCRs to the CS as problematic (dependent on the hypothesis). We suggest that physiological SCR ‘non-responders’ should be defined on the basis of US responses during fear acquisition training or to strong stimuli during pre-conditioning phases such as US calibration, startle habituation or forced breathing (reliably eliciting strong SCRs). If ‘non-responding’ to the US (during fear acquisition training) is used, it is difficult to suggest a universally valid cutoff with respect to the number or percentage of required valid US responses, because this critically depends on a number of variables such as hardware and sampling rate used. It remains an open question for future work whether data quality of novel mobile devices (e.g., smartwatches) for the acquisition of SCRs differs from traditional, robust lab-based recordings and how this would impact on the frequency of exclusions based on SCRs. Appendix 4 suggests that the cutoff may typically range between 1/3 and 2/3 of valid responses but may be data-set specific. US-based criteria are of course not trivial in multiple-day experiments, in which certain experimental days do not involve the presentation of US or involve few temporally clustered US presentations (i.e., reinstatement), or in paradigms not involving direct exposure to the US (i.e., observational or instructional learning; Haaker et al., 2017). In these cases, the other options listed above are strongly preferred to CS based criteria.
Where do we go from here?
In this work, we have comprehensively illustrated and argued that most of the current definitions employed to define ‘non-learners’ and ‘non-responders’ have to be considered as theoretically and empirically problematic. It is not sufficient, however, to raise awareness to these problems and the practical question of ‘Where do we go from here?’ remains to be addressed. What can we do to avoid getting lost in the garden of forking paths of exclusion criteria? Here, we would like to offer several solutions to improve practices in the field, which we expect to foster robustness, replicability and potentially clinical translation of findings: (1) transparency in reporting, (2) adopting open science practices, (3) increasing the level and quality of reporting and (4) graphical data presentation, (5) manipulation checks, and (6) fostering critical evaluation. We refer to see Box 1 for specific recommendations.
More precisely, transparency can be enhanced ‘if observations are eliminated, authors must also report what the statistical results are if those observations are included’, as suggested by Simmons and colleagues, nearly a decade ago (Simmons et al., 2011, Table 2). Here, we echo this call that this recommendation should be implemented routinely in data reporting pipelines when employing performance-based participant exclusions (‘non-learners’) in fear conditioning research. We also call for a transparent and adequate reporting in the results (in brief) and discussion section rather than providing this information exclusively in the appendix. This being said, it is important to point out that should a finding turn out to be contingent on the exclusion of ‘non-learners’, this does not necessarily invalidate this finding. On the contrary, it may further specify the finding or hint to possible mechanisms and/or boundary conditions – yet inferences on boundary conditions should be made carefully (Hardwicke and Shanks, 2016). Relatedly, adopting an open science culture will facilitate transparent reporting of exclusion criteria (Nosek et al., 2015) and will minimize the risk of exploiting heterogeneous definitions in the field. Registered reports (Hardwicke and Ioannidis, 2018), publicly available data including those from excluded participants and pre-registration (Munafò et al., 2017) of definitions and analysis pipelines (Ioannidis, 2014), as well as openly accessible lab-specific standard operational protocols (SOPs), may also be helpful.
We acknowledge, however, that transparent reporting and particularly pre-registration of exclusion criteria is not trivial in light of the unsatisfactory quality and level of detail in reporting in the field of fear conditioning research. It was striking that the compilation of exclusion criteria (‘non-learners’ and ‘non-responders’, see Appendix 1—tables 1 and 2) employed in the records included in our systematic literature search required extensive personal exchange with the authors because the definitions provided were often insufficient, ambiguous or incorrect. It is our responsibility as authors, reviewers and editors to improve these reporting standards to an acceptable level. As a guidance, Box 1 provides a compilation of reporting details that we consider important to include in both pre-registered protocols and publications (an editable online version of Box 1 is available to allow for further development, see Box caption).
Our recommendations to improve the level of reporting details and transparency extends to the graphical illustration of results, which should optimally allow for a complete presentation of data (Weissgerber et al., 2015) without risking obscuring important patterns, providing detailed distributional information rather than merely presenting summary statistics (see Weissgerber et al., 2015 for a discussion). Such visualization options include, for instance, scatterplots, box plots, histograms, violin plots as well as their combination (see also Figure 5) in so called ‘rain cloud plots’ (see Allen et al., 2018 for a tutorial in R, Matlab and Phyton) and utilizing colors or color gradients to visualize different groups of individuals (for instance ‘learners’ and ‘non-learners’) or discrimination scores. This will provide readers with the opportunity to evaluate the presented results and conclusions independently and comprehensively.
Finally, if criteria for ‘non-learners’ or ‘non-responders’ are employed to exclude participants from data analyses (or continuation of the experiment), we recommend that a sanity or manipulation check should be performed to determine whether – for instance - ‘non-learners’ really did not learn (i.e., really do not show significant CS+/CS– discrimination). We have empirically illustrated that most definitions of ‘non-learners’ fail this manipulation check (Appendix 2—table 1). Yet, it may not be feasible in all cases to determine such statistics, as these may not be appropriate for small samples and correspondingly small sub-groups of ‘non-learners’. Relatedly, we urge authors to justify adequately all details of the exclusion criteria (if applied) – both theoretically and practically. Furthermore, we encourage authors, reviewers and editors alike to critically evaluate whether exclusions and applied criteria are warranted in the first place and appropriate in the specific context (vs. mere adopting published or previously employed criteria) and whether these exclusion criteria are transparently reported and discussed if results hinge on them (Steegen et al., 2016).
Furthermore, future work should empirically address the question of how to best define ‘non-learning’ in particular in light of different outcome measures in fear conditioning studies, which capture different aspects of defensive responding (Jentsch et al., 2020; Lonsdorf et al., 2017).
Final remarks
In closing, the field of fear conditioning has been plagued with a lack of consensus on how to define and treat ‘non-learners’ and ‘non-responders’, which not seldomly impacts review processes and generates unnecessary lengthy discussions for editors, reviewers and authors. We argue that it is neither ethical (due to an excessive waste of tax money and human resources) nor scientifically meaningful to exclude up to two thirds of a sample. If only one third of the population performs ‘as expected’ in the experiment, experimental designs, data recording and processing techniques as well as definitions need to be reconsidered. We have shown that findings derived from such highly selective sub-samples may not generalize to other samples or to the general population, and as a consequence might be a threat to clinical translation. Most problematically, however, findings derived from such highly selective samples have been routinely and invariantly generalized to reflect ‘general principles’ and ‘processes’ in the past. Not surprisingly, such findings have also suffered replication failures. As such, exclusions of ‘non-learners’ can in fact be dangerous if not handled transparently (as suggested above), because they may bias and confuse a whole research field and may push research along a misleading path. Thus, we suggest recommendations and consensus suggestions, and recommend that common practices should be critically evaluated before we adopt them in future work, so that the field follows a path towards more robust and replicable research findings.
Materials and methods
This project has been pre-registered on the Open Science Framework (OSF) (Lonsdorf et al., 2019, March 22; retrieved from https://osf.io/vjse4).
A systematic literature search was performed according to PRISMA guidelines (Moher et al., 2009) covering all publications (including e-pubs ahead of print) in PubMed during the six months prior to the 22nd March 2019, using the following search terms: threat conditioning OR fear conditioning OR threat acquisition OR fear acquisition OR threat learning OR fear learning OR threat memory OR fear memory OR return of fear OR threat extinction OR fear extinction. In case of author corrections, we included the original study that the correction referred to unless this study itself was already included on the basis of the publication date.
From the identified 854 records listed in PubMed, 152 were included in stage 2 screening (abstract) and 86 were retained for stage 3 screening (full text). Finally, 50 records were included (see Figure 1 for details) that reported results for (1) SCRs as an outcome measure from (2) the fear acquisition training phase (3) in human participants.
Extraction of criteria for ‘non-learners’ and ‘non-responders’
Request a detailed protocolThe 50 records were screened in-depth and information derived from each record was entered into a template file agreed on by the authors prior to literature screening (available from the OSF pre-registration https://osf.io/vjse4). We distinguished between ‘non-learners’ and ‘non-responders’. We considered an exclusion to be an exclusion of ‘non-learners’ if it was based on the key task performance –that is, CS+/CS– discrimination in SCRs. Exclusions were considered as exclusion of ‘non-responders’ when based on general (physiological) responding (i.e., not based on CS+/CS– discrimination). Participants who were explicitly excluded because of clear-cut and well-described technical problems, such as abortion of data recording or electrode disattachment, were not included in any definition. Criteria for defining ‘non-learners’ (see Appendix 1—table 1) and ‘non-responders’ (see Appendix 1—table 2) were extracted if applicable for the respective study. In case information in the publication was insufficient or ambiguous, the corresponding authors were contacted and asked for clarification.
Re-analysis of existing data applying the identified exclusion criteria
Request a detailed protocolOne aim of this work was to illustrate empirically the impact of different exclusion criteria on the study outcome and interpretation. To achieve this aim, we initially planned to re-analyze existing data sets and to exclude participants on the basis of the identified definitions, which was expected to demonstrate that results are not robust across the various definitions of ‘non-learners’ and ‘non-responders’ employed. More precisely, we planned to calculate CS+/CS– discrimination across different data sets for all definitions identified by the systematic literature search and to generate corresponding correlation matrices as well as the percentages of zero and non-responses (see pre-registration: https://osf.io/vjse4). Because the exclusion criteria identified through the systematic literature search were even more heterogeneous than expected, and as it was difficult to agree on a key outcome to quantify the impact of exclusion criteria, we eventually concluded that such extensive re-analyses would not add much to the tabular and graphical illustration of this heterogeneity. Instead, we provide illustrative case examples for: (i) the proportion of individuals excluded on the basis of the identified exclusion criteria for ‘non-learners’ (Figure 3) and (ii) the potential sample bias with respect to individual differences (exploratory aim) induced by employing different exclusion criteria features (i.e., discrimination cutoff; Figure 5). As planned, (iii) we provide the percentage of non-responses to the CS+, CS–, CS+ and CS– combined, and the US across different studies, as well as empirical information on the association between CS and US based non-responding as a base to guide empirical recommendations.
Data processing, statistical analyses and figures were generated with R version 3.6.0 (2019-04-26) using the following packages: cowplot, dplyr, ggplot2 (Wickham, 2009), ggrigdes, car, ez, lsr, psychReport, lubridate, RColorBrewer and flipPlot packages. Sanky plots were generated with help of https://app.displayr.com.
Data sets
Data set 1
Request a detailed protocolData set 1 is part of the baseline measurement of an ongoing longitudinal fear conditioning study. Here, fear ratings and SCR data from the first test-timepoint (T0) were included (N = 119, 79 females, mean ± SD age of 25 ± 4 years) whereas fMRI data were not used. All participants gave written informed consent to the protocol which was approved by the local ethics committee (PV 5157, Ethics Committee of the General Medical Council Hamburg).
Data set 1 is employed to illustrate a case example for the proportion of participants excluded when employing different CS+/CS– discrimination cutoffs (‘non-learners’, Figure 3) as well as the number of zero-responses across different stimulus types (‘non-responders’) and their association (Figure 7). Furthermore, we aimed to test exploratively whether even in groups defined as ‘non-learners’ a significant CS+/CS– discrimination on SCR and fear ratings can be detected (all results presented in the Appendix are based on Data set 1).
Paradigm and stimuli
Request a detailed protocolThe two-day paradigm consisted of habituation and acquisition training (day 1) and extinction training and recall testing (day 2) without any contingency instructions provided. Here, only data from the acquisition training phase (100% reinforcement rate) were used. CS were two light grey fractals, presented 14 times each in a pseudo-randomized order for 6–8 s (mean: 7 s). Visual stimuli were identical for all participants, but allocation to CS+ and CS– was counterbalanced between participants. During inter-trial intervals (ITIs), a white fixation cross was shown for 10–16 s (mean: 13 s). All stimuli were presented on a light gray background and controlled by Presentation software (Version 14.8, Neurobehavioral Systems, Inc, Albany California, USA).
The electrotactile stimulus, serving as US, consisted of three 10 ms electrotactile rectangular pulses with an interpulse interval of 50 ms (onset: 200 ms before CS+ offset) and was administered to the back of the right hand of the participants. It was generated by a Digitimer DS7A constant current stimulator (Welwyn Garden City, Hertfordshire, UK) and delivered through a 1 cm diameter platinum pin surface electrode (Speciality Developments, Bexley, UK). The electrode was attached between the metacarpal bones of the index and middle finger. US intensity was individually calibrated in a standardized step-wise procedure aiming at an unpleasant, but still tolerable level.
SCRs
Request a detailed protocolSCRs were semi-manually scored by using a custom-made computer program (EDA View) as the first response from trough to peak 0.9–3.5 s after CS onset (0.9–2.5 s after US onset) as recommended (Boucsein et al., 2012; Sjouwerman and Lonsdorf, 2019). The maximum rise time was set to 5 s. Data were down-sampled to 10 Hz. Each scored SCR was checked visually, and the scoring suggested by EDA View was corrected if necessary (e.g., the foot or trough was misclassified by the algorithm). Data with recording artifacts or excessive baseline activity (i.e., more than half of the response amplitudes) were treated as missing data points and excluded from the analyses. SCRs below 0.01 μS or the absence of any SCR within the defined time window were classified as non-responses and set to 0. The threshold of 0.01 μS for this data set was determined empirically by visually inspecting response specifically above and below this cutoff, which suggested that in this data set, responses > 0.01 μS can be reliably identified. ‘Non-responders’ (N = 3) were defined as individuals who showed more than two thirds of non-responses to the US (10 or more non-responses out of 14 US trials, see Appendix 4—table 2). Three individuals were classified as ‘non-responders’ and these individuals did not show any responses to the CS either. The three participants classified as ‘non-responders’ (see above) were only excluded for the analyses of ‘non-learners’. Raw SCR amplitudes were normalized by taking the natural logarithm and range-corrected by dividing each logarithmized SCR by the maximum amplitude (maximum SCR to a CS or a US) per participant and day.
Fear ratings
Request a detailed protocolFear ratings were provided by participants through ratings on a visual analog scale (VAS) on the screen asking ‘how much stress, fear, and tension’ they experienced when they last saw the CS+ and CS–. The fear ratings used for the purpose of this manuscript are those obtained after fear acquisition training (no ratings were acquired during this phase). Answers were given within 5 s on the VAS, which ranged from 0 (answer = none) to 25 (answer = maximum) by using a button box. Pressing the buttons moved a bar on the VAS to the aimed value and answers were logged in by pressing another button. Non-registered ratings were considered as missing values (8.4%).
Statistical analysis
Request a detailed protocolTo test whether exclusion groups differ in CS+/CS– discrimination, a mixed ANOVA with CS+/CS– discrimination in SCR or fear ratings as the dependent variable and the between-subjects factor ‘Exclusion group’ and the within-subject factor ‘CS-type’ was performed. Note that it is circular to test for differences in SCR CS+/CS– discrimination between groups that were selected on the basis of different SCR CS+/CS– discrimination cutoffs in the first place. Still, it is relevant to test whether all groups classified as ‘non-learners’ in the literature do indeed fail to show evidence of learning, which would be indicated by a lack of significant CS+/CS– discrimination in SCRs in this case. In essence, this is a test to evaluate whether the exclusion criteria used in the literature do indeed achieve what they purport to do, that is, classify a group of participants that do not show evidence of learning. To test whether these exclusion groups discriminated in SCRs and fear ratings, exclusion groups were cumulated, and t-tests were performed for each cumulative group (see Appendices 2 and 3, respectively). We acknowledge, however, that the absence of a statistically significant CS+/CS– discrimination effect in a group on average cannot be taken to imply that all individuals in this group do not show meaningful CS+/CS– discrimination. As such, this is a rather conservative test. To correct for multiple testing, all p-values deriving from t-tests were adjusted using the Bonferroni procedure. As effect size, Cohen’s d was reported for t-tests and partial eta-squared for ANOVAs. To illustrate the association between the non-responses to the US and the non-responses to the CS, a Spearman rank correlation test was computed.
Data set 2
Request a detailed protocolFor the purpose of this manuscript, a final sample of 268 individuals (195 female, mean ± SD age of 25 ± 4 years) was re-analyzed. This sample is reported in a recent pre-print (Sjouwerman et al., 2018) in which we observed an association between trait anxiety and CS+/CS– discrimination in SCRs. Here, the re-analysis and graphical illustration of these data serve the purpose of a case example to illustrate the potential sample bias that may be induced by employing performance-based exclusion (Figure 5).
Paradigm and stimuli
Request a detailed protocolA detailed experimental description is included in the preprint Sjouwerman et al. (2018). In brief, participants underwent a 100% reinforcement fear acquisition training phase in a behavioral laboratory setting, including 9 CS+ and 9 CS– trials, presented for 6 s each. Consequently, 9 US presentations were included that coincided 100 ms prior to CS+ offset. Trials were interleaved by 10–13 s ITIs with a white fixation cross presented on a black background. Black geometrical shapes served as CS, and electrical stimulation delivered by a DS7A electrical stimulator (Digitimer, Welwyn Garden City, UK) onto the outer surface of the right hand served as US. The intensity of the US was individually calibrated with a stair-case procedure in order to reach an unpleasant but tolerable level. Not of interest to the current case example were the acoustic startle probes (95 dB(A) burst of white noise) presented to elicit a startle response in two thirds of all acquisition trials, as well as three fear-rating blocks probed intermittently during fear acquisition training. Startle probes were presented 4 or 5 s post‐CS onset, and 5 or 7 s post‐ITI onset. No contingency instructions were given.
SCRs
Request a detailed protocolSCRs were quantified as the first SCR within 0.9–4.0 s after stimulus onset (CS or US) and were scored semi-manually from trough-to-peak using a custom-made program. Signal increases smaller than 0.02 µS were treated as non-responses, that is set to 0. (Please note that this cut-off was not empirically determined as in Data set 1 but adopted from the previous publication of Data set 2. As we present re-analyses here, we decided not to change the cut-off to maintain comparability.) Responses confounded by recording artifacts, such as responses moving beyond the sampling window, excessive baseline activity, or electrode detachment were treated as missing values. Raw response amplitudes per trial were log-transformed and range-corrected for the maximum CS or US response per participant. Individuals not showing any valid SCR (i.e., missing or zero responses) in more or equal than two thirds (≥6 out of 9, see Appendix 4—table 2) of US trials were treated as physiological ‘non-responders’ (n = 19) and were consequently excluded from graphical illustration and the statistical analysis. In addition, 31 participants were excluded prior to physiological processing, either because of abortion of the experiment or due to technical failures during data acquisition (e.g. errors during saving, overwritten logfile, or missing markers), leaving 307 out of 357 individuals with valid SCR data for fear acquisition training. Of these 307 participants, 39 had incomplete STAI-T data (Spielberger et al., 1983) resulting in a final sample size for this case example of 268 individuals (195 female, mean ± SD age of 25 ± 4 years).
Statistical analysis
Request a detailed protocolTo test whether different exclusion groups differ in their mean trait anxiety levels, a univariate ANOVA with STAI-T score as the dependent variable and exclusion group as the independent variable was carried out. Post hoc pairwise t-tests were conducted to compare trait anxiety scores between the different exclusion group levels. The post hoc tests were corrected for multiple testing, using the Bonferroni correction method. 95% family wise confidence levels were determined using TukeyHSD tests.
Appendix 1
Definition of performance-based exclusion of participants (‘non-learners’) and numbers of participants excluded across studies
Appendix 2
Applying the identified performance-based exclusion criteria to existing data: a case example
In this case example based on Data set 1 (see main manuscript), we tested whether CS+/CS– discrimination in SCRs does indeed differ between the different exclusion groups as defined by the cut-offs retrieved from the literature (see Figure 2B). Note that this is somewhat circular as exclusion groups are defined by different SCR CS+/CS– cutoffs, which then are used in an analysis in which differential SCRs are the dependent measure. However, that this is exactly what is sometimes done in the literature (see main manuscript).
Still, this is an important manipulation check to test empirically whether those classified in a group of ‘non-learners’ in the literature do indeed show no evidence of learning, which would be indicated by comparable SCRs to the CS+ and the CS– (i.e., no significant discrimination). Here, we test this for cumulative exclusion groups. Note that this is only a rough manipulation check, as a non-significant CS+/CS– discrimination effect in the whole group (e.g., those showing a CS+/CS– discrimination <0.05 µS based on raw scores) cannot be taken as evidence that all individuals in this group do not display meaningful or statistically significant CS+/CS– discrimination. More precisely, half of this group who did not meet the cut-off of 0.05µS in CS+/CS– discrimination do show a negative or zero discrimination score, which may bias the group average score towards non-discrimination. Yet, statistically testing for discrimination within each exclusion group (e.g. specifically in the group showing a discrimination between >0 and < 0.05 µS) is not unproblematic.
Appendix 3
Exploratory analyses on consistency of classification (‘learners’ vs. ‘non-learners’) across outcome measures and criteria employed
Throughout the main manuscript and particularly in the discussion, we highlight that differential (CS+>CS–) SCRs alone cannot be taken to infer ‘learning’ (Figure 4—figure supplement 1).
Appendix 3—table 1 provides statistical information on CS+/CS– discrimination in fear ratings in (cumulative) exclusion groups as defined by CS+/CS– discrimination in SCRs.
Appendix 4
Definition of ‘non-responders ‘and amount of participants excluded across studies
In the main manuscript, we discuss different frequencies of ‘non-responding’ to different experimental stimuli (e.g., US, CS+ and CS– in isolation or in combination), which inherently lead to different exclusion frequencies when classifying ‘non-responders’ on the basis of different types of stimuli. As there is little empirical work on the frequency of ‘non-responses’ to the US, CSs (i.e., CS+ and CS–) and CS+ only to base recommendations on, we compiled this information across 20 different data sets (see Appendix 4—table 1), including information on SCR response quantification specifications (i.e., minimum amplitude, scoring approach) and procedural details during fear acquisition training (i.e., number of CS and US presentations). These data sets were provided by different co-authors involved in this manuscript.
In addition, Appendix 4—table 2 provides information on the number and percentage of individuals in a sample showing SCR ‘non-responses’ to a certain number of US presentations during fear acquisition training as well as mean number and percentage of CS responses (CS refers to the CS+ and CS– combined) in these individuals to guide the development of empirically based criteria to define SCR ‘non-responders’.
Data availability
The minimal data sets (data set 1 and data set 2, both represent re-analysis of existing data), which were analysed during the current study, as well as code for figure production are are available at OSF under https://osf.io/mkxqe/ and DOI: https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/MKXQE.
References
-
Rule-based processes in generalisation and peak shift in human fear conditioningQuarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 72:118–131.https://doi.org/10.1177/1747021818766461
-
Contingency awareness as a prerequisite for differential contextual fear conditioningCognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience 19:811–828.https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-018-00666-z
-
Cortico-limbic connectivity changes following fear extinction and relationships with trait anxietySocial Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 13:1037–1046.https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsy073
-
Publication recommendations for electrodermal measurementsPsychophysiology 49:1017–1034.https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2012.01384.x
-
The conditioning and extinction of fear in youths: what's sex got to do with it?Biological Psychology 100:97–105.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.06.001
-
Optimizing inhibitory learning during exposure therapyBehaviour Research and Therapy 46:5–27.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2007.10.003
-
Effects of cortisol on reconsolidation of reactivated fear memoriesNeuropsychopharmacology 40:3036–3043.https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2015.160
-
Preextinction stress prevents Context-Related renewal of fearBehavior Therapy 49:1008–1019.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2018.03.001
-
Updated meta-analysis of classical fear conditioning in the anxiety disordersDepression and Anxiety 32:239–253.https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22353
-
Human fear conditioning is related to dopaminergic and serotonergic biological markersBehavioral Neuroscience 115:358–364.https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.115.2.358
-
L-DOPA improves extinction memory retrieval after successful fear extinctionPsychopharmacology 236:3401–3412.https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-019-05301-4
-
Emotional learning, hedonic change, and the startle probeJournal of Abnormal Psychology 102:453–465.https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.102.3.453
-
The neuropsychology of fear learning and fear regulationInternational Journal of Psychophysiology 57:5–14.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.01.006
-
Mapping the universe of registered reportsNature Human Behaviour 2:793–796.https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0444-y
-
The extent and consequences of p-hacking in sciencePLOS Biology 13:e1002106.https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002106
-
Renewal of conditioned fear in a novel context is associated with hippocampal activation and connectivitySocial Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 11:1411–1421.https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsw047
-
How to make more published research truePLOS Medicine 11:e1001747.https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1001747
-
Temporal dynamics of conditioned skin conductance and pupillary responses during fear acquisition and extinctionInternational Journal of Psychophysiology 147:93–99.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2019.11.006
-
Fear extinction retention: is it what we think it is?Biological Psychiatry 85:1074–1082.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2019.02.011
-
Cortisol effects on fear memory reconsolidation in womenPsychopharmacology 233:2687–2697.https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-016-4314-x
-
Reactivation of the unconditioned stimulus inhibits the return of fear independent of cortisolFrontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 13:254.https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00254
-
Stress disrupts the reconsolidation of fear memories in menPsychoneuroendocrinology 77:95–104.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.11.027
-
Secondary extinction reduces reinstatement of threat expectancy and conditioned skin conductance responses in human fear conditioningJournal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry 62:103–111.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2018.09.007
-
Exposure to stress attenuates fear retrieval in healthy menPsychoneuroendocrinology 41:89–96.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.12.009
-
Neural underpinnings of cortisol effects on fear extinctionNeuropsychopharmacology 43:384–392.https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2017.227
-
Let's talk about sex … differences in human fear conditioningCurrent Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 23:7–12.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.01.021
-
I feel safe when i know: contingency instruction promotes threat extinction in high intolerance of uncertainty individualsBehaviour Research and Therapy 116:111–118.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2019.03.004
-
A manifesto for reproducible scienceNature Human Behaviour 1:0021.https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-016-0021
-
Individual differences in electrodermal activity as a function of subjects' anxietyPersonality and Individual Differences 8:615–626.https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(87)90059-6
-
Avoidance behavior prevents modification of fear memory during reconsolidationPsychological Reports 15:33294118811116.https://doi.org/10.1177/0033294118811116
-
Dissociation of within- and between-session extinction of conditioned fearJournal of Neuroscience 30:4990–4998.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6038-09.2010
-
Human fear conditioning and extinction: timing is everything…or is it?Biological Psychology 92:59–68.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.02.005
-
Many analysts, one data set: making transparent how variations in analytic choices affect resultsAdvances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science 1:337–356.https://doi.org/10.1177/2515245917747646
-
Latency of skin conductance responses across stimulus modalitiesPsychophysiology 56:e13307.https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13307
-
Increasing transparency through a multiverse analysisPerspectives on Psychological Science 11:702–712.https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691616658637
-
Influence of contingency awareness on neural, electrodermal and evaluative responses during fear conditioningSocial Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 6:495–506.https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsq070
-
Comparison of emotional processing assessed with fear conditioning by interpersonal conflicts in patients with depression and schizophreniaPsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences 73:116–125.https://doi.org/10.1111/pcn.12805
-
Reduced Fear-Conditioned pain modulation in experienced meditators: a preliminary studyPsychosomatic Medicine 80:799–806.https://doi.org/10.1097/PSY.0000000000000634
-
Fear extinction and relapse: state of the artAnnual Review of Clinical Psychology 9:215–248.https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185542
-
Genetic influences on the acquisition and inhibition of fearInternational Journal of Psychophysiology 98:499–505.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.10.007
Article and author information
Author details
Funding
Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (LO 1980/2-1)
- Tina B Lonsdorf
- Christian J Merz
Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (LO 1980/1-1)
- Tina B Lonsdorf
Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (Project B07 44541416)
- Tina B Lonsdorf
Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (316803389 - SFB1280)
- Christian J Merz
Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (WE 5873/1-1)
- Julia Wendt
Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (WE 5873/5-1)
- Julia Wendt
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Acknowledgements
The project is part of a European wide network (EIFEL-ROF network) of researchers working on meta-research topics in the field of fear conditioning research, which is funded by the German Research foundation (DFG; Grant ID LO1980/2-1) to TBL and CJM. Data used for re-analyses in the main manuscript paper are derived from projects funded by the DFG to TBL (Data set 1: Collaborative Research Center project number 44541416 TRR 58, sub-project B07. Data set 2: LO 1980/1-1). Data reported in Appendix 1—table 1 are also funded by the German Research Foundation to CJM (Collaborative Research Center SFB 1280 project number 316803389, sub-project A09) and JW (WE 5873/1–1 and WE 2762/5–1). The authors represent eight different research groups across three European countries (Germany, the Netherlands, and Belgium).
We thank Prof. Dr. Matthias Gamer, University of Würzburg, for providing EDA View for SCR response quantification.
The authors thank Manuel Kuhn, Jan Haaker, Tanja Jovanovic and Dean Mobbs for helpful suggestions during discussions on this project. We also thank Jan Haaker for help with initial literature screening.
Ethics
Human subjects: Study 1: All participants gave written informed consent to the protocol which was approved by the local ethics committee (PV 5157, Ethics Committee of the General Medical Council Hamburg). Study 2: All participants gave written informed consent to the protocol which was approved by the Ethical Review Board of the German Psychological Association (TL072015).
Copyright
© 2019, Lonsdorf et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 3,555
- views
-
- 427
- downloads
-
- 102
- citations
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Neuroscience
Neural implants have the potential to restore lost sensory function by electrically evoking the complex naturalistic activity patterns of neural populations. However, it can be difficult to predict and control evoked neural responses to simultaneous multi-electrode stimulation due to nonlinearity of the responses. We present a solution to this problem and demonstrate its utility in the context of a bidirectional retinal implant for restoring vision. A dynamically optimized stimulation approach encodes incoming visual stimuli into a rapid, greedily chosen, temporally dithered and spatially multiplexed sequence of simple stimulation patterns. Stimuli are selected to optimize the reconstruction of the visual stimulus from the evoked responses. Temporal dithering exploits the slow time scales of downstream neural processing, and spatial multiplexing exploits the independence of responses generated by distant electrodes. The approach was evaluated using an experimental laboratory prototype of a retinal implant: large-scale, high-resolution multi-electrode stimulation and recording of macaque and rat retinal ganglion cells ex vivo. The dynamically optimized stimulation approach substantially enhanced performance compared to existing approaches based on static mapping between visual stimulus intensity and current amplitude. The modular framework enabled parallel extensions to naturalistic viewing conditions, incorporation of perceptual similarity measures, and efficient implementation for an implantable device. A direct closed-loop test of the approach supported its potential use in vision restoration.
-
- Neuroscience
Neuromodulatory inputs to the hippocampus play pivotal roles in modulating synaptic plasticity, shaping neuronal activity, and influencing learning and memory. Recently, it has been shown that the main sources of catecholamines to the hippocampus, ventral tegmental area (VTA) and locus coeruleus (LC), may have overlapping release of neurotransmitters and effects on the hippocampus. Therefore, to dissect the impacts of both VTA and LC circuits on hippocampal function, a thorough examination of how these pathways might differentially operate during behavior and learning is necessary. We therefore utilized two-photon microscopy to functionally image the activity of VTA and LC axons within the CA1 region of the dorsal hippocampus in head-fixed male mice navigating linear paths within virtual reality (VR) environments. We found that within familiar environments some VTA axons and the vast majority of LC axons showed a correlation with the animals’ running speed. However, as mice approached previously learned rewarded locations, a large majority of VTA axons exhibited a gradual ramping-up of activity, peaking at the reward location. In contrast, LC axons displayed a pre-movement signal predictive of the animal’s transition from immobility to movement. Interestingly, a marked divergence emerged following a switch from the familiar to novel VR environments. Many LC axons showed large increases in activity that remained elevated for over a minute, while the previously observed VTA axon ramping-to-reward dynamics disappeared during the same period. In conclusion, these findings highlight distinct roles of VTA and LC catecholaminergic inputs in the dorsal CA1 hippocampal region. These inputs encode unique information, with reward information in VTA inputs and novelty and kinematic information in LC inputs, likely contributing to differential modulation of hippocampal activity during behavior and learning.