Pre-stimulus phase and amplitude regulation of phase-locked responses are maximized in the critical state

  1. Arthur-Ervin Avramiea
  2. Richard Hardstone
  3. Jan-Matthis Lueckmann
  4. Jan Bím
  5. Huibert D Mansvelder
  6. Klaus Linkenkaer-Hansen  Is a corresponding author
  1. Department of Integrative Neurophysiology, Center for Neurogenomics and Cognitive Research (CNCR), Amsterdam Neuroscience, Netherlands
  2. Neuroscience Institute, New York University School of Medicine, United States
  3. Technical University of Munich, Germany
  4. Czech Technical University in Prague, Czech Republic

Abstract

Understanding why identical stimuli give differing neuronal responses and percepts is a central challenge in research on attention and consciousness. Ongoing oscillations reflect functional states that bias processing of incoming signals through amplitude and phase. It is not known, however, whether the effect of phase or amplitude on stimulus processing depends on the long-term global dynamics of the networks generating the oscillations. Here, we show, using a computational model, that the ability of networks to regulate stimulus response based on pre-stimulus activity requires near-critical dynamics—a dynamical state that emerges from networks with balanced excitation and inhibition, and that is characterized by scale-free fluctuations. We also find that networks exhibiting critical oscillations produce differing responses to the largest range of stimulus intensities. Thus, the brain may bring its dynamics close to the critical state whenever such network versatility is required.

Introduction

Understanding how neurons coordinate their activity to produce emergent dynamics and behaviors is crucial to understanding how the brain gives rise to conscious perception. An influential approach for elucidating neural correlates of consciousness (Dehaene and Changeux, 2011; Engel and Singer, 2001; Tononi and Koch, 2008) has been that of threshold-stimulus detection (Li et al., 2014; Linkenkaer-Hansen et al., 2004; Wyart and Tallon-Baudry, 2009). In these tasks, subjects are given a stimulus that is set at the edge of perception, meaning that on 50% of trials subjects perceive it, and on 50% of trials they do not. In trials where the stimulus is perceived, primary sensory regions exhibit larger event-related potentials (Pins and Ffytche, 2003), larger event-related desynchronization (Vidal et al., 2015), and stronger phase-locking to the stimulus (Palva et al., 2005). However, what causes some trials to evoke stronger ERPs, event-related desynchronization, or phase-locking than other trials—with dramatic consequences for perception—cannot be explained by differences in the stimuli, which are held identical throughout the threshold-stimulus detection experiments. The alternative is that awareness of stimuli depends on brain processes that unravel around the time of the stimulation, for example that pre-stimulus neuronal activity biases stimulus processing and conscious perception (Aru et al., 2012).

One candidate mechanism for gating stimulus processing is alpha oscillations (Jensen and Mazaheri, 2010), which inhibit unwanted stimuli from reaching consciousness (Capilla et al., 2014; Händel et al., 2011; Jensen et al., 2012; Jensen and Mazaheri, 2010; Thut et al., 2006). Fluctuations in alpha oscillations, possibly modulated by attention (Macdonald et al., 2011), may determine whether a stimulus is perceived or not. This was confirmed in threshold-stimulus experiments relating higher pre-stimulus alpha activity to decreased stimulus detection (Ergenoglu et al., 2004; Hanslmayr et al., 2005; Linkenkaer-Hansen et al., 2004). In addition to amplitude, the pre-stimulus alpha phase also predicts stimulus awareness (Busch et al., 2009; Mathewson et al., 2009; VanRullen, 2016).

Regardless of the attentional state, neuronal oscillations in the alpha band exhibit spontaneous amplitude fluctuations. However, these fluctuations are not random, but exhibit a specific temporal structure: autocorrelations with scale-free decay that extends up to time scales of hundreds of seconds (Linkenkaer-Hansen et al., 2001), also called long-range temporal correlations (LRTC). This scale-free character of the oscillations emerges in neuronal networks with balanced excitatory and inhibitory forces (Poil et al., 2012) and is considered a sign of critical-state dynamics (Bak et al., 1987; Chialvo, 2010). Thus, oscillations exhibiting LRTC are referred to as ‘critical oscillations’. Criticality in the brain has also been associated with scale-free neuronal avalanches, and different computational models have used this link to indicate that critical dynamics are beneficial for processing information (Beggs and Plenz, 2003), for example evoked responses show the largest dynamic range (Gautam et al., 2015; Kinouchi and Copelli, 2006; Larremore et al., 2011) and this has subsequently been confirmed experimentally (Gautam et al., 2015; Shew et al., 2009). However, it is not known whether critical-state dynamics of alpha oscillations is beneficial for stimulus processing. Importantly, it remains entirely unknown whether pre-stimulus bias of post-stimulus processing depends on brain dynamics being close to the critical point.

To answer this question, we consider the network response in terms of phase-locking to the stimulus, which is thought to play a role in reorganizing neuronal networks for efficient stimulus processing (Hanslmayr et al., 2005; Min et al., 2007; Sauseng et al., 2007; Voloh and Womelsdorf, 2016). We rely on an extension of a model of critical oscillations (CROS) (Poil et al., 2012), which exhibits dynamics comparable to human M/EEG (Dalla Porta and Copelli, 2019). We use the model to investigate the network phase-locking response as a function of network connectivity, stimulus size, and pre-stimulus alpha amplitude and phase. We then related short-term fluctuations in information processing in these networks, with a long-term characteristic of the oscillatory dynamics: their level of criticality. To evaluate the criticality of alpha oscillations, we used detrended fluctuation analysis (DFA, see Materials and methods), which can discriminate between random fluctuations in the amplitude of oscillations and critical oscillations with long-range temporal correlations. In addition, DFA has the advantage, over other measures of criticality such as neuronal avalanches, that it can be estimated noninvasively at electrode level. This will allow future experiments with EEG/MEG recordings to test whether for example the criticality of sensory cortices biases stimulus processing. We show that pre-stimulus amplitude and phase regulation occurs for critical, but not for subcritical or supercritical networks. Thus, critical-state dynamics in a neuronal network is associated with versatile functions, allowing it to flow between low- and high-responsivity states on sub-second time scales. In addition, we show that a critical network can differentiate the widest range of stimulus sizes. Together, our results suggest that understanding how close neuronal networks are to criticality is essential for understanding their function.

Results

Unstimulated network produces multi-level criticality

To test the effect of critical oscillations on neuronal network functionality, we adapted a model of ongoing neuronal activity (CROS) (Poil et al., 2012) (see Materials and methods). CROS consists of 75% excitatory and 25% inhibitory integrate-and-fire neurons arranged in a 50 × 50 grid (Figure 1—figure supplement 1A). The two parameters that need to be set when creating a network are the percentage of neurons within a local range (circular radius four neurons) that each excitatory and each inhibitory neuron connects to (Figure 1—figure supplement 1A). The firing rate grew monotonously for both excitatory and inhibitory neurons, as we increased excitatory connectivity (Figure 1—figure supplement 1B,C). Networks oscillated in the 8–16 Hz range (Figure 1—figure supplement 1D), with the power of the oscillation increasing as we move towards an excitation-dominated regime (Figure 1—figure supplement 1E). Importantly, the model exhibits multi-level criticality, with spatial and temporal power-law scaling behavior in network activity, which arises through a common mechanism of balanced E/I connectivity (Figure 1—figure supplement 1F–J). To illustrate the importance of the level of criticality on network function, we will show detailed data from three networks throughout the paper: a subcritical (45% excitatory/90% inhibitory connectivity - blue), a critical (60%/75% - green), and a supercritical one (75%/60% - red)(e.g., Figure 1E).

Figure 1 with 3 supplements see all
Dynamic range is maximized when network exhibits critical-state dynamics.

(A) A stimulus is connected to a random subset of excitatory neurons on the grid. (B) We stimulated 1800 excitatory neurons in three example subcritical, critical and supercritical networks. The top plots show the total number of excitatory (red) and inhibitory (blue) spikes with millisecond resolution. Plots below show individual spikes for all 2500 excitatory and inhibitory neurons in the networks. In all cases, the response to the stimulus is oscillatory with a periodicity characteristic of an alpha (8–16 Hz) rhythm. (C) The phase-locking factor at a time point post-stimulus is calculated as the normalized vector sum of phase angles across trials (inset left). A value of 1 corresponds to all trials having the same phase, a value of 0 corresponds to a uniform distribution of phases. Post-stimulus phase-locking response to a stimulus is dependent on the size of the stimulus. Shown for different stimulus sizes in a critical network. (D) Dynamic range was calculated as the orders of magnitude of neurons stimulated between the 10th and 90th percentile, of a sigmoid fit to the phase-locking response, at a time point where the network shows response to the stimulus (150 ms post-stimulus). Shown for example sub-critical (blue), critical (green) and super-critical (red) networks. (E) Dynamic range is dependent on E/I connectivity balance. The black line indicates critical neuronal avalanches, as measured when the networks were not stimulated. The networks used to illustrate the functionality of different criticality regimes are shown as triangles on the phase-space with the colors of sub-critical (blue), critical (green) and super-critical (red). (F) Dynamic range of phase-locking response increases with long-range temporal correlations (LRTC) at 150 ms post-stimulus (Spearman correlation). Networks binned based on average DFA for each of the 256 excitatory/inhibitory connectivity parameter combinations, and dynamic range calculated as the mean of networks in that bin.

Dynamic range of phase-locking response is strongest for critical networks

Previously, it was shown that criticality maximizes the dynamic range of evoked firing-rate responses (Gautam et al., 2015; Kinouchi and Copelli, 2006; Larremore et al., 2011). However, event-related responses in large oscillatory networks also reflect a reset in the oscillatory phase following stimulation, leading to the phase-locking of the oscillation to the stimulus (Makeig et al., 2002; Min et al., 2007; Palva et al., 2005). As such, the firing rate and phase-locking responses reflect fundamentally different aspects of network activity, for example evoked firing-rate responses may increase the overall network activity without affecting the phase of oscillatory activity in a particular frequency band. In fact, evoked firing-rate responses are not an appropriate measure of network response in networks with strong oscillations: they fluctuate continuously with the phase of the ongoing oscillation, making it difficult to judge the firing-rate response in absolute values, that is without relating it to the ongoing oscillation. In that respect, the phase-locking response is a more appropriate way to detect changes in network activity occurring due to external stimulation.

To test whether criticality maximizes the dynamic range also for phase-locked oscillatory responses, we attached a stimulus to n excitatory neurons distributed randomly on the grid (Figure 1A). The stimulus had the same weight as any of the excitatory to excitatory synapses in the network (winput = wEE = 0.0085), and lasted for one time step (1 ms). We found that the stimulus response in our networks is indeed oscillatory, which may result in stimulus phase-locking (Figure 1B). We then investigated how changing the stimulus size, n, altered the phase-locking response of a network to the stimulus. In Figure 1C, we can see that a critical network increases the level of post-stimulus phase-locking as stimulus size is increased. This shows that the CROS model is capable of showing oscillatory responses to a stimulus, as has been seen in human subjects (Palva et al., 2005). We calculated the dynamic range, which gives an indication of how well a network can give differing responses to stimuli of different sizes (see Materials and methods). Critical networks show a wider dynamic range than their sub-/super-critical counterparts (Figure 1D–F), and this effect is robust over a wide range of post-stimulus latencies (Figure 1—figure supplement 2). Interestingly, in the case of supercritical networks, the strong, ongoing oscillatory activity prevents the phase-locking response from saturating (PLF ~1), even at the largest stimulus sizes (Figure 1D, Figure 2—figure supplement 1). Another factor that can shape stimulus response, other than stimulus size, is the strength of the external stimulus. We found that an 8-fold increase in the external input strength was sufficient to bring the PLF of supercritical networks to saturation, for the largest stimulus size (Figure 1—figure supplement 3B). However, such a large increase in the input strength also leads to a stronger PLF for the smaller stimulus sizes in the case of subcritical and critical networks, which means they no longer show weak PLF responses reducing their dynamic range. Of all tested strengths and networks, the maximal dynamic range occurs for critical networks at a multiplier ~1 (Figure 1—figure supplement 3C). Overall, our results show that critical networks discriminate the widest range of stimulus sizes through their post-stimulus phase-locking response.

Pre-stimulation amplitude influence on phase-locking requires critical dynamics

The ongoing oscillations showed a phase-locking response in the post-stimulus period (~65–250 ms) to the stimulus, which depended on the criticality of the network (Figure 2A,B), as well as on the stimulus size (Figure 2—figure supplement 1). Considering the complex variation of activity in a critical network, we wondered whether critical networks function differently depending on their instantaneous state. For this purpose, we assessed the network phase-locking response to a stimulus of 5 neurons, and related it to the pre-stimulus amplitude averaged over the −150 to −50 ms time range, in the 8–16 Hz frequency band. We found that critical networks exhibit strong negative pre-stimulus amplitude regulation (Figure 2C–E), whereas sub-critical or super-critical networks show no pre-stimulus amplitude regulation (Figure 2D,E). We confirmed this result statistically by showing that pre-stimulus amplitude regulation was more prominent for networks with stronger LRTC (Figure 2F, Figure 2—figure supplement 2).

Figure 2 with 3 supplements see all
Pre-stimulus amplitude regulation of response requires critical-state dynamics.

(A) Ongoing oscillations phase-lock to stimulus. Example shown for five neurons stimulated, for sub-critical (blue), critical (green), and super-critical networks (red). (B) Phase-locking response at 150 ms post-stimulus depends on the balance between excitation and inhibition, being highest for sub-critical networks, lowest for super-critical networks, and with intermediate values in the critical regime. The locations of the three example networks are highlighted on the phase space by triangles with corresponding colors. black line indicates critical neuronal avalanches. (C) To calculate the PLF response across trials with similar pre-stimulus amplitude, we split trials in 10 bins based on the pre-stimulus amplitude in the −150 to −50 ms time range, for the 8–16 Hz frequency band. Trials belonging to different pre-stimulus amplitude bins show different phase-locking response post-stimulus. Color indicates the percentile interval of the pre-stimulus amplitude bin. (D) Power of pre-stimulus oscillation can alter the phase-locking response of a network to the stimulus, in the presence of critical oscillations. Shown for example sub-critical (blue), critical (green), and super-critical (red) networks. Pre-stimulus amplitude regulation, Regamp, was computed as the Spearman correlation coefficient between the pre-stimulus amplitude percentile bins, and the PLF of the trials in each bin. (E) The strength of pre-stimulus amplitude regulation is dependent on a balance between excitation and inhibition in terms of connectivity. The black line indicates critical neuronal avalanches, as measured when the networks were not stimulated. Here shown for a stimulus size of 5 neurons. (F) Pre-stimulus amplitude regulation (Regamp) is more prominent for stronger LRTC (DFA) at 150 ms post-stimulus (Spearman correlation). DFA was measured for the non-stimulated networks, and Regamp was measured for the same networks, but with a five neuron stimulus. Networks binned based on DFA as in Figure 1F. (G) Critical networks exhibit pre-stimulus amplitude regulation for the widest range of stimuli. Supercritical networks also show regulation, but only at very high stimulus sizes. mean ± sem corresponding to 10 individual networks with the same excitatory/inhibitory connectivity as the selected sub-critical (blue), critical (green), super-critical (red) networks. Horizontal lines at the bottom of the plot show the range of stimuli for which the networks show significant pre-stimulus regulation (two-sided t-test, null hypothesis mu = 0, Bonferroni corrected for 3 × 16 comparisons).

To test whether this function is generic to criticality and not specific to the chosen stimulus size, we investigated the three combinations of excitatory/inhibitory connectivity and found that the critical network—unlike the sub-/super-critical networks—showed significant pre-stimulus amplitude modulation for a wide range of stimulus intensities (Figure 2G). Changing the weight of the external input also affects the range of stimuli over which networks show pre-stimulus amplitude regulation (Figure 2—figure supplement 3B). Nonetheless, the range of stimuli over which the critical networks show pre-stimulus amplitude regulation is superior to that of the subcritical and supercritical networks, regardless of the strength of the input weight (Figure 2—figure supplement 3C).

Pre-stimulation phase influence on phase-locking requires critical dynamics

Having identified a strong interaction between criticality of oscillations and pre-stimulus amplitude on stimulus-evoked phase-locking, we asked whether a similar effect is present for the phase of the pre-stimulus oscillation. For the sub- and super-critical networks there was no pre-stimulus phase regulation of response at a stimulus size of 5 neurons (Figure 3A). In contrast, the critical network showed large differences in post-stimulus phase-locking for different pre-stimulus phase bins (Figure 3A). These networks showed the weakest response on the descending side of the alpha cycle, when the network is inhibited, and the strongest response on the rising side of the alpha cycle, when the network is in an excitable state (Figure 3—figure supplement 1). Looking across the connectivity parameter space, we found that pre-stimulus phase dependence of post-stimulus phase-locking requires a balance in E/I connectivity (Figure 3B), and is significantly correlated with criticality as reflected in LRTC (Figure 3C, Figure 3—figure supplement 2). The pre-stimulus phase influence on phase-locking also generalized to other stimulus sizes. Given the three combinations of excitatory/inhibitory connectivity indicated in Figure 3D, we found that the critical networks show pre-stimulus regulation over the widest range of stimulus sizes. We found that changing the weight of the external input also affects the range of stimuli over which pre-stimulus phase regulation is significant (Figure 3—figure supplement 3B). Nonetheless, pre-stimulus phase-regulation occurs for a wider range of stimuli in critical networks than in their subcritical or supercritical counterparts, regardless of the strength of the input weight (Figure 3—figure supplement 3C).

Figure 3 with 3 supplements see all
Pre-stimulus phase regulation of response requires critical-state dynamics.

(A) The phase of pre-stimulus oscillations can alter the phase-locking response of a network to a stimulus in the presence of critical oscillations. Trials are split into 32 evenly spaced bins based on pre-stimulus phase (−5 ms) with an equal number of trials in each bin. Phase-locking at 150 ms post-stimulus was calculated for each bin. Pre-stimulus phase regulation of response, Regphase, was calculated based on the post-stimulus PLF distribution of the bins and examples are shown for sub-critical (blue), critical (green) and super-critical (red) networks. Super-critical networks show higher PLF per bin than sub-critical networks because the former have stronger alpha oscillations which carry the phase of alpha oscillations for a longer time into the post-stimulus period without adjusting the phase due to the stimulus. (B) Pre-stimulus phase regulation is dependent on E/I connectivity balance. The black line indicates critical neuronal avalanches, as measured when the networks were not stimulated. Here, shown for a stimulus size of 5 neurons. (C) Pre-stimulus phase regulation is more prominent for stronger LRTC at 150 ms post-stimulus (Spearman correlation). DFA was computed for the unstimulated networks, and Regphase for the same networks, but with five neurons stimulated. Networks binned based on DFA exponent, as in Figure 1F. (D) Critical networks exhibit pre-stimulus phase regulation over the widest range of stimulus sizes. Supercritical networks also show regulation, but only at very high stimulus sizes. mean ± sem corresponding to 10 individual networks with the same excitatory/inhibitory connectivity as the selected sub-critical (blue), critical (green), super-critical (red) networks. Black shaded line is the significance threshold calculated as the 95% percentile of pre-stimulus phase regulation based on bootstrapping of pre-stimulus and post-stimulus phases (see Supplementary information - Materials and methods).

The relationship between criticality and network versatility is robust to changes in model parameters

Model parameters were selected through an optimization process which aimed at getting power-law avalanche distributions and robust LRTC (as described in more detail in Materials and methods), but did not include any criteria to maximize the dynamic range of phase-locking response, or pre-stimulus amplitude or phase regulation. Therefore, we expect that the functional consequences of criticality on stimulus processing that emerged out of the model, are generic to criticality, and not specific to the actual parameters. To verify this, we repeated our analysis for two versions of the model where the synaptic weight of excitatory to excitatory connections (wEE) was multiplied by a factor of 0.75, or by a factor of 1.25. Multiplication of wEE by a factor of 0.75 reduced the overall activity in the network, which means that stronger connectivity is required to achieve the same dynamics, leading to a shift up of the critical line through the phase space (Figure 4). Increasing wEE had the opposite effect on the critical line. Importantly, the functional consequences of criticality—maximized dynamic range, pre-stimulus amplitude and phase regulation—followed this shift in the critical line. This suggests that the relationship between criticality and versatility in stimulus processing is not coincidental on the specific choice of parameters, but generic to criticality.

The relationship between criticality and network versatility is robust to changes in the strength of excitatory connections.

Phase-space plots of LRTC (A,E), Dynamic Range (B,F) Amplitude Dependence (C,G), and Phase Dependence (D,H), for networks with the weight of the excitatory to excitatory connections (wEE) multiplied by a factor of 0.75 (top row) or 1.25 (bottom row). black line indicates critical neuronal avalanches, as measured when the networks were not stimulated.

Discussion

In this study, we establish for the first time a link between critical-state dynamics of oscillations and the ability of a network to respond differently depending on the amplitude of ongoing alpha oscillations. This functionality has been investigated in many human studies (Ergenoglu et al., 2004; Hanslmayr et al., 2005; Linkenkaer-Hansen et al., 2004), where a low oscillation power in relevant sensory areas in the pre-stimulus period has been seen as a pre-requisite of conscious perception. In non-relevant areas, on the other hand, it has been suggested that a high oscillation power serves to functionally inhibit that region (Smith et al., 2012). Our findings are also consistent with the notion that alpha oscillations create periodic fluctuations in excitability (Klimesch et al., 2007), which are indexed by the phase of the oscillations. Pre-stimulus phase has been shown to bias perception (Busch et al., 2009; Mathewson et al., 2009; VanRullen, 2016) and we found that this function is also likely to be modulated by the criticality of the network. In addition, we show that oscillatory spiking neuronal networks have an optimal dynamic range when they are critical, which is in agreement with previous work on non-oscillatory networks (Gautam et al., 2015; Kinouchi and Copelli, 2006; Larremore et al., 2011; Shew et al., 2009), and suggests that optimal dynamic range is a generic property of critical systems. Thus, since the level of criticality affects network response in terms of stimulus phase-locking, and phase-locking plays a role in the generation of event-related responses (ERPs) (Makeig et al., 2002; Min et al., 2007), we predict that criticality will also affect the different time components of ERPs.

There is much debate as to the cause of amplitude fluctuations during threshold stimulus detection tasks, with top-down mechanisms such as attention and expectation being suggested (Jensen et al., 2012; Melloni et al., 2011). Here, we show that a network with balanced excitatory and inhibitory connectivity produces spontaneous amplitude fluctuations that have a functional role, meaning that the network goes through periods of high and low sensitivity to external stimuli. In the unbalanced state, the network loses this function and either reliably reacts (in the sub-critical case), or ignores the stimulus (super-critical case). Thus, top-down mechanisms are not required for fluctuations in alpha power; however, it is plausible that attentional mechanisms can actively regulate the level of fluctuations to shift the network’s operating point relative to the critical state, allowing the network to react in the required manner (Wyart and Tallon-Baudry, 2009). How do these findings relate to task performance? Pre-stimulus activity regulation of post-stimulus response has been found beneficial, by enhancing detection of stimuli that are attended to (Capilla et al., 2014) or expected (Mayer et al., 2016), and reducing detection of irrelevant stimuli. The spontaneous fluctuations in amplitude observed at criticality, with impact on conscious perception, may serve to flexibly switch between the different stimuli competing for attention. To this is added another advantage of being in the critical state: it requires minimal attentional modulations to reach the sub- or super-critical states, and thus to enhance aspects of stimuli, for example in the case of stimulus discrimination (Tomen et al., 2014).

However, is this network ability to regulate perception based on pre-stimulus amplitude always optimal for performing a task? In tasks where continuous detection ability is required, fluctuations in pre-stimulus activity will actually decrease task performance in those periods of amplitude that produce smaller post-stimulus response. In addition, the broadening of attention to external stimuli in the critical state, is accompanied by increased susceptibility to task-unrelated mentation, or mind-wandering (Hellyer et al., 2014). When people experience periods of mind-wandering during task-performance, this results in reduced phase-locking to a stimulus (Baird et al., 2014). In these cases a more sub-critical network would actually perform better in terms of constant attention (Irrmischer et al., 2018; Tomen et al., 2014) and a super-critical network would perform better in terms of constant functional inhibition. Our results are in line with the predictions of Cramer et al. (2019), which suggest that the critical state is not optimal for all information processing requirements: complex tasks benefit from criticality, whereas in simple tasks, the high susceptibility to stimuli, memory capacity and entropy of a critical regime might impair performance.

We have shown that critical networks have overall the widest dynamic range of phase-locking response, and pre-stimulus amplitude and phase regulation. However, we also found that supercritical networks can show differential PLF and pre-stimulus regulation for the large stimulus sizes where the PLF response of critical networks saturates. One way in which the brain can take advantage of the information processing benefits of different states is to regulate its dynamics with respect to the critical point in a task-dependent manner (Pfeffer et al., 2018). Zierenberg et al. (2020) show that the brain could also combine networks with different distances to the critical point, such that the resulting ensemble has a dynamic range of stimulus response wider than any of the ensemble networks. This strategy could plausibly be employed to extend the range of stimulus sizes over which pre-stimulus amplitude and phase regulation occur.

Although our computational model abstracts many biophysiological neuronal mechanisms, it captures essential features of neuronal dynamics: alpha oscillations, long-range temporal correlations, and scale-free avalanches, which emerge at a specific balance of excitatory and inhibitory connectivity. We showed that the relationship between criticality and the maximal versatility of network function is not specific to the selection of model parameters, which suggests that it is generic to criticality. Future studies should test whether our results also hold for more physiologically realistic models, for example where excitatory and inhibitory currents balance each other (Haider et al., 2006; Rudolph and Destexhe, 2003).

Previously, non-oscillatory neuronal models have shown maximized dynamic range at criticality (Gautam et al., 2015; Kinouchi and Copelli, 2006; Larremore et al., 2011; Shew et al., 2009). While it is plausible that non-oscillatory models could also show a pre-stimulus ‘amplitude’ regulation effect based on the level of pre-stimulus firing—and that this effect is likely to be maximal at criticality—it is unclear how a phase regulation effect could occur without an ongoing oscillation. Future studies could investigate whether the presence of oscillations is important for pre-stimulus phase and amplitude regulation to occur, in models with a different type of critical phase transition (such as absorbing/sustained) (Del Papa et al., 2017; Levina et al., 2007).

Overall our study highlights the importance of critical-state dynamics (Chialvo, 2010) for understanding neuronal network function, and shows that it is possible to integrate oscillatory mechanisms into this framework. Importantly, while the quantitative hallmarks of criticality require long-time monitoring of spatial and temporal dynamics that are statistically stable, our results show that the concept of critical brain dynamics is compatible with time-varying functions—an important notion in contemporary theories of neuronal oscillations and their role in neuronal communication and in attention.

Materials and methods

Key resources table
Reagent type
(species) or resource
DesignationSource or referenceIdentifiers
Software, algorithmBrian Simulatorhttps://briansimulator.orgRRID:SCR_002998
Software, algorithmMATLABhttps://www.mathworks.com/products/matlabRRID:SCR_001622
Software, algorithmcircular statisticshttps://mathworks.com/matlabcentral/fileexchange/10676-circular-statistics-toolbox-directional-statisticsRRID:SCR_016651
Software, algorithmR Project for Statistical Computinghttp://www.r-project.org/RRID:SCR_001905

CRitical OScillations (CROS) model

Request a detailed protocol

We used an adapted version of the model described in Poil et al. (2012), with synaptic weights optimized for power-law avalanches and long-range temporal correlations. The model was implemented using the Brian2 simulator for spiking neural networks (Stimberg et al., 2014; RRID:SCR_002998). Specifically, we modeled networks of 75% excitatory and 25% inhibitory integrate-and-fire neurons arranged on a 50x50 open grid. Placing neurons on the grid using uniform sampling may result, by chance, in clusters of excitatory or inhibitory neurons. To avoid this, we first positioned inhibitory neurons on the grid according to Mitchell’s best candidate algorithm (Mitchell, 1991), which results in a more even spatial distribution. The remaining spaces were filled with excitatory neurons. Networks differ in their two connectivity parameters, CE and CI, which are the percentage of other neurons within a local range (a circle with radius = 4 neurons centered on the presynaptic neuron) that each excitatory and each inhibitory neuron connects to, respectively. Connectivity parameters were set between 25 and 100% at 5% intervals, and 10 different networks were created for each combination of CE and CI. Border neurons had fewer connections because these neurons had a lower number of neurons in their local range. Within local range, connection probability decreases exponentially with distance. More specifically, the probability, P, of a connection at a distance r was given by:

(1) P(r)=min(αer,1)

where α is optimized separately for excitatory and inhibitory neurons such that the overall connectivity probability within a neuron’s local range is equal to CE or CI, depending on whether the neuron is excitatory or inhibitory. For example in the case of an excitatory neuron i, with connectivity probability CE, where the set of neighboring neurons within the local range is J, and J is the number of neighbors, α will have to satisfy the following equation:

(2) jJP(rj)=jJmin(αerj,1)=CE|J|

As such, we used the Nelder-Mead optimization algorithm to determine the value of α which minimizes the following function:

(3) f(α)=|jJmin(αerj,1)CE|J||

Neuron model

Request a detailed protocol

Neurons were modeled using a synaptic model integrating received spikes, and a probabilistic spiking model. Each time step (dt) of 1 ms starts with each neuron, i, updating the input Ii with spikes received from the presynaptic neurons Ji, together with an exponential synaptic decay:

(4) Ii(t+dt)=(Ii(t)+jJiWijSj(t))(1dtτI)

The weights Wij are fixed, and depend on the type of the pre- and post-synaptic neuron. τI is the decay constant of inputs, and S is a is a binary vector, with Sj=1 if the pre-synaptic neuron j fired in the previous time step, and Sj=0 otherwise.

The activation of a neuron Ai, is then updated with these excitatory and inhibitory inputs, together with an exponential decay, τP, and a baseline activation level A0:

(5) Ai(t+dt)=(Ai(t)+Ii(t))(1dtτP)+A0dtτP

The spiking probability Pis is calculated as a function of the neuron activation A at the current timestep, as follows:

(6) Pis(t)={0, Ai(t)<0 Ai(t), 0Ai(t)11, Ai(t)>1

We determine whether the neuron spikes with the probability PS. If a neuron spikes, the neuron activation A is reset to the reset value, Ar. At the next time step, all neurons that it connects to will have their input updated again according to Equation 4.

Model parameters

Request a detailed protocol

All model parameters were the same as in the original paper (Poil et al., 2012) apart from the synaptic weights. Neuron model: (τI = 9 ms), Synaptic model: Excitatory neurons (τP = 6 ms, A0 = 0.000001, Ar = -2), Inhibitory neurons (τP = 12 ms, A0 =0, Ar = -20). To improve the range and stability of the long-range temporal correlations from the original model, an evolutionary algorithm (Smit and Eiben, 2011) was applied to the synaptic weights. The parameters that could vary were the 2 connectivity parameters (taking values between 0 and 100%), and the natural logarithm of the magnitude of the 4 synaptic weights, WEE, WIE, WEI and WII (taking values between -5 and 1). For each run, a fitness value was calculated based on the avalanches size and duration distributions (see Materials and methods - Neuronal Avalanches), and the LRTCs (see Materials and methods - Detrended fluctuation analysis of long-range temporal correlations).

(7) fitness= 1|1DFA|+|1κsize|+|1κduration|

The optimum weights (Wij, connecting the presynaptic neuron j to the postsynaptic neuron i) found by the algorithm were (WEE = 0.0085,WIE = 0.0085,WEI = −0.569,WII = −2).

Network activity analysis

Request a detailed protocol

A network signal was created by summing the total number of neurons spiking at each time-step with a Gaussian white noise signal of the same length with mean = 0 and σ = 3. This level of white noise was set to allow all networks to achieve a time-varying phase, which is not the case without adding the noise, when there are silent periods in the network. Detrended fluctuation analysis, oscillation power, and phase-locking factor, which are described below, were calculated on this pre-processed signal, with Gaussian noise added. The analysis of neuronal avalanches was performed on the raw signal, consisting of the total number of neurons spiking at each time step.

Oscillation power

Request a detailed protocol

The power spectrum was computed using the Welch method with a Hamming window with 211 FFT points.

Neuronal avalanches

Request a detailed protocol

A neuronal avalanche is defined as a period where neurons are spiking above a certain threshold—in our case set to half median of activity. The size of the avalanche is the number of spikes during this period. We then computed the κ index (Poil et al., 2012; Shew et al., 2009), which determines the difference between the distribution of our data and a power-law, by calculating the average difference of the cumulative distribution of a power-law function, P, (with exponent −1.5 for size and −2.0 for duration) and that of our experimental data, A, at 10 equally spaced points on a logarithmic axis (β) and adding 1.

(8) κ= 110i=110(P(βi)A(βi))+1

A subcritical distribution is characterized by κ <1, and a supercritical distribution by κ >1, whereas κ = 1 indicates a critical network.

Detrended fluctuation analysis of long-range temporal correlations

Request a detailed protocol

Detrended fluctuation analysis (DFA) was used to analyze the scale-free decay of temporal (auto)correlations, also known as long-range temporal correlations (LRTC). The DFA was introduced as a method to quantify correlations in complex data with less strict assumptions about the stationarity of the signal than the classical autocorrelation function or power spectral density (Hardstone et al., 2012; Linkenkaer-Hansen et al., 2001). In addition, DFA facilitates a reliable analysis of LRTC up to time scales of at least 10% of the duration of the signal (Chen et al., 2002; Gao et al., 2006). DFA exponents in the interval of 0.5 to 1.0 indicate scale-free temporal correlations (autocorrelations), whereas an exponent of 0.5 characterizes an uncorrelated signal. The main steps from the broadband signal to the quantification of LRTC using DFA have been explained in detail previously (Hardstone et al., 2012; Linkenkaer-Hansen et al., 2001). In brief, the DFA measures the power-law scaling of the root-mean-square fluctuation of the integrated and linearly detrended signals, F(t), as a function of time-window size, t (with an overlap of 50% between windows). The DFA exponent is the slope of the fluctuation function F(t), and can be interpreted as the strength of the autocorrelations in signals. For our analyses we computed DFA on the amplitude envelope of the signal filtered in the 8–16 Hz range, and the exponent was fit between 2 and 50 s.

Unstimulated networks

Request a detailed protocol

To assess the dynamics of unstimulated networks we allowed connectivity to take values between 25 and 100% at 5% intervals. For all 256 possible parameter combinations of excitatory and inhibitory connectivity, we created 10 different networks and ran each network for 2 × 106 time-steps (2000 s).

Stimulation networks

Request a detailed protocol

To test network response to a stimulus we took one sample network for each combination of excitatory and inhibitory connectivity parameters, and for each run of the network we stimulated n excitatory neurons where n ε {1, 2, 3, 5, 9, 15, 25, 43, 74, 126, 216, 369, 632, 1081, 1800}. The stimulated neurons were randomly distributed across the grid. For all 256 possible parameter combinations of excitatory and inhibitory connectivity, and stimulus size, we used the same 10 different networks as in the unstimulated case, and ran each network for 2 × 106 time-steps (2000 s). During the stimulation runs all neurons in the receptive field simultaneously received a stimulus once in every 500–1500 ms. The stimulus had the same weight as any of the excitatory-excitatory connections in the network (winput = wEE = 0.0085), and was applied simultaneously, for one timestep = 1 ms, to all the n excitatory neurons which the stimulus connected to. Additionally, we investigated the effect of the stimulus strength on network dynamics by modulating winput by a factor of 1/64 to 64 times the original value, on a log2 scale.

Phase-locking factor

Request a detailed protocol

Stimulus response was calculated in the network in terms of the phase-locking factor (Palva et al., 2005). Data were filtered using a one-way causal FIR filter (order 250) between 8 and 16 Hz. The use of a causal FIR filter ensured that pre-stimulus phase and amplitude were not contaminated by the stimulus responses, at the cost of delaying the peak response to the stimulus by 125 ms. The phase at any point in time was obtained by taking the angle of the Hilbert transform. Phase-locking factor (PLF) measures the uniformity of phases across trials at a time-point post-stimulus and was calculated as the mean vector length of all phases (Berens, 2009; RRID:SCR_016651):

(9) PLF=1NiTzi

where T is the set of trials, of size N, and zi is the unit vector whose angle corresponds to the momentary phase in trial i, at the time point for which PLF is calculated.

Dynamic range

Request a detailed protocol

We fit a sigmoid to the PLF response at 150 ms post-stimulus, as a function of the number of neurons stimulated. The dynamic range is calculated as the orders of magnitude of neurons stimulated between the 10% and 90% of the sigmoid we fit (Kinouchi and Copelli, 2006).

Pre-stimulus amplitude regulation of PLF

Request a detailed protocol

To calculate the PLF response corresponding to different levels of pre-stimulus amplitude, we separated trials into 10 percentile bins, based on the average amplitude in the −150 to −50 ms time-range, calculated for the 8–16 Hz frequency band. The percentile binning procedure ensured an equal number of trials across bins, which is important because the PLF value reflects the distribution of phases, but is also determined by the number of trials (e.g., the PLF value for uniformly randomly distributed phases is dependent on the number of trials). PLF at 150 ms post-stimulus was calculated separately for each of the 10 bins. The regulation measure Regamp was calculated as the Spearman correlation between the index of the bin and the PLF for that bin.

Pre-stimulus phase regulation of PLF

Request a detailed protocol

To calculate the pre-stimulus phase regulation of PLF, trials were split into 32 overlapping bins of width π/8 based on the pre-stimulus phase in the 8–16 Hz frequency range at time −5 ms. To get an equal number of trials in each bin, x randomly selected trials from each bin were picked where x was the smallest number of trials in a bin. PLF at 150 ms post-stimulus was calculated separately for each bin. If no phase regulation takes place, a uniform distribution of post-stimulus PLF is expected. The calculation of phase regulation uses the phase-locking factor of each of 32 phase bins as the weight for the unit vector of that phase. The sum of the 32 vectors is then normalized by the sum of all PLFs. For this calculation, we used the circ_r function from the circ_stats package (Berens, 2009; RRID:SCR_016651):

(10) Regphase=1iPPLFiiPPLFizi

where P is the set of 32 bins, zi is the unit vector whose angle corresponds to the phase at the center of each bin i, and PLFi is the phase-locking factor across all trials belonging to bin i. This measure gives a value of Regphase between 0 (PLF is same for all bins) and 1 (PLF is non-zero in one bin, and zero in all others).

Association between criticality and network function

Request a detailed protocol

Criticality was measured by the LRTC (DFA) of alpha oscillations in the non-stimulated networks. Network function was measured by pre-stimulus amplitude and phase modulation, as well as dynamic range, of the stimulated networks. To get robust estimators for criticality as well as our 3 indicators of network function, we averaged the values across all 10 network initializations, for each excitatory/inhibitory connectivity parameter combination (n=256). Before correlating criticality to network function, we had to address another aspect: a disproportionate amount of networks had low DFA. To this end, we binned the n=256 datapoints corresponding to each aspect of network function (pre-stimulus amplitude and phase modulation, dynamic range), by DFA, in bins of size 0.02, covering a DFA range of 0.5 to 0.9. The association between criticality and pre-stimulus amplitude regulation, for example, was computed as the Spearman correlation between the mean values of the 20 DFA bins, and the mean Regamp for the networks belonging to that bin. The same type of pre-processing was performed for pre-stimulus phase regulation and dynamic range. To identify the time ranges where criticality predicts network function, we calculated the Spearman correlation mentioned earlier, for all timepoints between 0 and 500 ms post-stimulus. To correct for multiple comparisons, we used the cluster-based correction with permutation (Nichols and Holmes, 2002), with a significance threshold α=0.001. We will exemplify again the process by looking at the association between criticality and pre-stimulus amplitude regulation, but the steps are the same for the other indicators of network function. First, we calculated the cluster significance threshold, to determine which clusters could have arisen by chance. While keeping the ordering of the DFA bins the same, we permuted the corresponding Regamp values, separately for each timepoint between 0 and 500 ms post-stimulus. We then computed the Spearman correlation coefficients rS between DFA and the permuted Regamp, for each timepoint, and identified all clusters – sets of contiguous timepoints with a p-value for the Spearman correlation pα. We defined cluster size Sc=tTcrSt, where t is a timepoint belonging to the set of contiguous timepoints Tc in cluster c, and rSt is the absolute value of the Spearman correlation coefficient at time t. We then calculated the maximum cluster size Smaxi for the permuted dataset i to be maxcCSc, where C is the set of all clusters C. We repeated this process 10000 times, which gave us a distribution of Smaxi over different permutations of the data. We set the cluster significance threshold TSmax to correspond to the 1-α*100 percentile of Smaxi. We then identified all clusters from the actual (non-permuted data), consisting of contiguous timepoints with with a p-value for the Spearman correlation pα, and kept only those clusters, whose cluster size Sc>TSmax.

Baseline level of pre-stimulus regulation

Request a detailed protocol

For this, we generated 10 synthetic runs (equal to the number of simulations of the CROS model for each excitatory/inhibitory connectivity combination)—each consisting of 2000 pairs of pre/post stimulus phase (equal to the number of times that the CROS model was stimulated throughout a run) drawn from a uniform distribution, and calculated pre-stimulus phase regulation for each of the 10 synthetic runs. Then we computed the average phase regulation across all 10 runs. We repeated this process 10000 times to get the 95% percentile of phase regulation. We used this as a significance threshold to determine whether 10 individual subcritical, critical, or supercritical networks, at a certain stimulus size, showed pre-stimulus phase regulation above what is expected from a random distribution of pre/post-stimulus phases. As seen in Figure 3D, sub-critical networks at strong stimuli show phase regulation well below this threshold. That is because these networks respond reliably to the strong stimuli regardless of the pre-stimulus phase, thus the distribution of responses across phases is more uniform than expected from a random distribution. Some supercritical networks show phase regulation below threshold for the weaker stimuli, because they have strong alpha oscillations which maintain the phase of oscillations similar for a long time after they have been binned uniform.

Robustness of relationship between network versatility and criticality

Request a detailed protocol

To verify that our results are not dependent on the specific model parameters, we used two variants of the model, where the weight of excitatory to excitatory connections (wEE) was multiplied by a factor of 0.75 and 1.25, respectively. For each combination of excitatory and inhibitory connectivity, we created five different networks. We then ran each network for 2 × 106 time-steps (2000 s), for all combinations of stimulus size and weight of excitatory to excitatory connections (wEE).

Data availability

Source code required to run all simulations, as well as datasets and scripts required to generate all figures presented here, are available at https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.12167943.v2.

The following data sets were generated
    1. Avramiea A-E
    2. Hardstone R
    3. Lueckmann J-M
    4. Bím J
    5. Mansvelder HD
    6. Linkenkaer-Hansen K
    (2020) figshare
    Pre-stimulus phase and amplitude regulation of phase-locked responses is maximized in the critical state.
    https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.12167943.v2

References

  1. Book
    1. Mitchell DP
    (1991)
    Spectrally Optimal Sampling for Distribution Ray Tracing
    ACM Siggraph Computer Graphics.
  2. Conference
    1. Smit S
    2. Eiben AE
    (2011)
    Multi-Problem parameter tuning using bonesa
    Artificial Evolution. pp. 222–233.

Decision letter

  1. Floris P de Lange
    Senior and Reviewing Editor; Radboud University, Netherlands

In the interests of transparency, eLife publishes the most substantive revision requests and the accompanying author responses.

Acceptance summary:

There has been much interest in how stimulus processing depends on the phase and amplitude of pre-stimulus oscillatory activity. Using sophisticated computational modeling, this paper demonstrates that near-criticality is required to enable the ability of networks to regulate stimulus response based on pre-stimulus activity. This highlights a potential functional role for the critical state in allowing pre-stimulus states to influence sensory information processing.

Decision letter after peer review:

Thank you for submitting your article "Versatility of neuronal network function is maximized in the critical state" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by two peer reviewers, and the evaluation has been overseen by Floris de Lange as the Senior Editor and Reviewing Editor. The reviewers have opted to remain anonymous.

Summary:

The authors study the stimulus-evoked responses in a previously published model (from the same group), which generates alpha oscillations and long-range temporal correlations. Previous results have shown that this model can exhibit critical dynamics depending on the connection probabilities of excitation and inhibition. The main novel contribution of this work is that it shows that the pre-stimulus biases (alpha frequency amplitude and phase) of the stimulus-evoked responses only occur in networks close to criticality. In particular, they found a strong negative correlation between pre-stimulus alpha amplitude and phase-locking response to stimulus in critical networks. This is consistent with experimental results that show higher alpha activity is correlated with decreased stimulus detection. This result also suggests that attention can modulate the network from the supercritical state where it ignores stimulus to the subcritical state where it reliably responds to stimulus.

In addition, the authors also show that networks at critical state have the largest dynamic range of phase-locking responses to stimulus. However, similar result has been shown in previous work for the dynamic range of firing rate responses.

Essential revisions:

1) Physiological realism.

Please report how the firing rates of excitatory and inhibitory neurons depend on E-I ratio (analog figure to e.g. 1E, but displaying the firing rates).

Please show raster plots of spiking for e.g. 100 randomly chosen neurons; a 250-500 ms window would be good to see; in addition, show population rates for a few seconds; both for the three examples (sub/super/critical).

"E-I balance" in the strict sense refers to excitatory and inhibitory currents within a single neuron canceling each other. Does the model show that?

2) Robustness.

You modified parameters with respect to the first version of the model. Ideally, the effects you show do not depend on these parameter modifications. Could you demonstrate that?

Likewise, would you expect that other models, like Levina, Hermann and Geisel, 2007, or del Papa, Priesemann and Triesch, 2017, would show similar types of effects, even in the absence of oscillations, thus at the transition between absorbing/sustained activity?

3) Dynamic Range.

The dynamic range at criticality is larger than at sub/supercritical models. However, the "working point" or response range also changes considerably. This is in analogy to Zierenberg et al., 2020. How do your results relate on the systematic change of the response range, and to their ideas that combining networks of different distances to criticality would greatly enhance the dynamic range?

4) Writing.

As to the writing, I think this paper is too concise and requires prior knowledge of the previously published papers from this group. I would appreciate more explanations in the main text about the key terminologies and concepts, such as "critical oscillation", "Long-range temporal correlations", "Detrended fluctuation analysis".

5) Data and code sharing.

The raw spike data for the three states (sub/super/critical) should be made available, so that everyone can re-analyse them. The code should be made available as well. Please share the code before acceptance.

6) I didn't find a clear definition of the stimulus input? What's the duration and magnitude? Is it a constant current in time? Why is the stimulus strength defined as the number of stimulated neurons? Would results be similar if stimulus strength is modeled as the magnitude of input current?

7) Figure 1D, if input current is larger, does it expand the dynamic range of the supercritical networks?

8) Figure 1C, Is the phase-locking response related to the onset increase of firing rate? If the network shows onset response in rate, it would result in more rising phases, right? Since previous work has shown criticality maximizes dynamic range of evoked rate responses, how is this result different from previous work?

9) Figure 3D, Similar to my comments above about the dynamics range, I wonder if the range of pre-stimulus regulation depends on the magnitude of input current. If you drive the supercritical network with stronger input for each neuron, does it expand the region of significant regulation.

10) About the balance between excitation and inhibition: I think it should be the product of synaptic weight and connection probability that determines the network dynamics. Why is the critical transition at approximately when excitatory connectivity percentage equals inhibitory connectivity percentage? How does the transition depend on the recurrent weights?

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.53016.sa1

Author response

Essential revisions:

1) Physiological realism.

Please report how the firing rates of excitatory and inhibitory neurons depend on E-I ratio (analog figure to e.g. 1E, but displaying the firing rates).

We have added the phase-space plots of excitatory and inhibitory firing rates to Figure 1—figure supplement 1 and refer to the figure in Results:

“The firing rate grew monotonously for both excitatory and inhibitory neurons, as we increased excitatory connectivity (Figure 1—figure supplement 1B,C).”

Please show raster plots of spiking for e.g. 100 randomly chosen neurons; a 250-500 ms window would be good to see; in addition, show population rates for a few seconds; both for the three examples (sub/super/critical).

We agree it is very interesting to illustrate the spiking activity and have added the requested raster plots for stimulated networks in Figure 1 (Figure 1B in the revised manuscript). This also highlights the oscillatory nature of responses in the CROS model (see point #8 below).

We also made raster plots of spontaneous activity in the unstimulated network. We include this figure here for comparison (Author response image 1); however, we judge it too similar to the new Figure 1B to include it in the manuscript. For the sub-critical, critical and super-critical networks, we selected a representative 500-ms window to exemplify spiking for all neurons, as well as a 5-second window to show the evolution of population activity (Author response image 1). The example sub-critical network exhibits very sparse bursts of activity, the super-critical network is characterized by relentless firing, and the critical network gives rise to avalanche patterns with greater variance in size and spread. Due to the recurrent connectivity of the network, inhibitory activity closely follows excitatory activity. However, excitatory activity dominates when the number of excitatory synapses is much larger than the number of inhibitory synapses, as is the case for the supercritical network (Author response image 1C).

Author response image 1
Network time-series and raster plots for unstimulated networks.

Time-series and raster plots for an example sub-critical (A), critical (B), and super-critical (C) network. (Top and middle rows) Network time-series for excitatory (red) and inhibitory (blue) neurons. (Bottom) Raster plot of activity from highlighted region in top row.

"E-I balance" in the strict sense refers to excitatory and inhibitory currents within a single neuron canceling each other. Does the model show that?

In our model, we use probabilistic integrate-and-fire neurons (i.e., not a current-based integrate-and-fire). Thus, it is not possible to analyze the relative balance of excitatory and inhibitory currents in these neurons. However, E/I balance can be defined at different levels of neuronal organization and, here, we use a network-level approach based on the number of excitatory and inhibitory connections, and find that for a certain balance of connections, the model produces critical-state dynamics. Note, that if we change the strength of excitatory or inhibitory connections then the ratio of excitatory and inhibitory connections required for critical-state dynamics will also change (See new Figure 4 and the reply to point #2 below).

We have addressed this point in the manuscript (Discussion section):

“Although our computational model abstracts many biophysiological neuronal mechanisms, it captures essential features of neuronal dynamics: alpha oscillations, long-range temporal correlations, and scale-free avalanches, which emerge at a specific balance of excitatory and inhibitory connectivity. We showed that the relationship between criticality and the maximal versatility of network function is not specific to the selection of model parameters, which suggests that it is generic to criticality. Future studies should test whether our results also hold for more physiologically realistic models, e.g., where excitatory and inhibitory currents balance each other (Haider et al., 2006; Rudolph and Destexhe, 2003).”

2) Robustness.

You modified parameters with respect to the first version of the model. Ideally, the effects you show do not depend on these parameter modifications. Could you demonstrate that?

The only parameters that were modified compared to the first version of the model were the shape of the local range, which was changed from square to circular, and the synaptic weights, which came out of an optimization process. We have clarified this in the manuscript (Results section):

“Model parameters were selected through an optimization process which aimed at getting power-law avalanche distributions and robust LRTC (as described in more detail in Materials and methods), but did not include any criteria to maximize the dynamic range of phase-locking response, or pre-stimulus amplitude or phase regulation.”

To further support that our results are generic to criticality and not dependent on the specific parameter values, we ran additional simulations and added the following to the Results:

“Therefore, we expect that the functional consequences of criticality on stimulus processing that emerged out of the model, are generic to criticality, and not specific to the actual parameters. To test this, we repeated our analysis for two versions of the model where the synaptic weight of excitatory to excitatory connections (wEE) was multiplied by a factor of 0.75, or by a factor of 1.25. Multiplication of wEE by a factor of 0.75 reduced the overall activity in the network, which means that stronger connectivity is required to achieve the same dynamics, leading to an upward shift of the critical line through the phase space (Figure 4). Increasing wEE had the opposite effect on the critical line. Importantly, the functional consequences of criticality—maximized dynamic range, pre-stimulus amplitude and phase regulation—followed this shift in the critical line. This suggests that the relationship between criticality and versatility in stimulus processing does not require a specific choice of parameters, but is generic to criticality.”

Likewise, would you expect that other models, like Levina, Hermann and Geisel, 2007, or del Papa, Priesemann and Triesch, 2017, would show similar types of effects, even in the absence of oscillations, thus at the transition between absorbing/sustained activity?

We feel that it goes beyond the scope of the present paper to implement other models in order to answer this question with certainty; however, we have added a comment to the manuscript (Discussion):

“Previously, non-oscillatory neuronal models have shown maximized dynamic range at criticality (Shew et al., 2009). While it is plausible that non-oscillatory models could also show a pre-stimulus “amplitude” regulation effect based on the level of pre-stimulus firing—and that this effect is likely to be maximal at criticality—it is unclear how a phase regulation effect could occur without an ongoing oscillation. Future studies could investigate whether the presence of oscillations is important for pre-stimulus phase and amplitude regulation to occur, in models with a different type of critical phase transition (such as absorbing/sustained) (Del Papa et al., 2017; Levina et al., 2007).”

3) Dynamic Range.

The dynamic range at criticality is larger than at sub/supercritical models. However, the "working point" or response range also changes considerably. This is in analogy to Zierenberg et al., 2020. How do your results relate on the systematic change of the response range, and to their ideas that combining networks of different distances to criticality would greatly enhance the dynamic range?

We thank the reviewers for bringing this study to our attention. We now address this point in the Discussion:

“We have shown that critical networks have overall the widest dynamic range of phase-locking response, and pre-stimulus amplitude and phase regulation. However, we also found that supercritical networks can show differential PLF and pre-stimulus regulation for the large stimulus sizes where the PLF response of critical networks saturates. One way in which the brain can take advantage of the information processing benefits of different states is to regulate its dynamics with respect to the critical point in a task-dependent manner (Pfeffer et al., 2018). Zierenberg et al., 2020, show that the brain could also combine networks with different distances to the critical point, such that the resulting ensemble has a dynamic range of stimulus response wider than any of the ensemble networks. This strategy could plausibly be employed to extend the range of stimuli strengths over which pre-stimulus amplitude and phase regulation occur.”

4) Writing.

As to the writing, I think this paper is too concise and requires prior knowledge of the previously published papers from this group. I would appreciate more explanations in the main text about the key terminologies and concepts, such as "critical oscillation", "Long-range temporal correlations", "Detrended fluctuation analysis".

Following the reviewers’ suggestion, we have adjusted the Introduction to define these concepts.

“neuronal oscillations in the alpha band exhibit spontaneous amplitude fluctuations. However, these fluctuations are not random, but exhibit a specific temporal structure: autocorrelations with scale-free decay that extends up to time scales of hundreds of seconds (Linkenkaer-Hansen et al., 2001), also called long-range temporal correlations (LRTC). This scale-free character of the oscillations emerges in neuronal networks with balanced excitatory and inhibitory forces (Poil et al., 2012) and is considered a sign of critical-state dynamics (Bak et al., 1987; Chialvo, 2010). Thus, oscillations exhibiting LRTC are referred to as “critical oscillations”.”

“To evaluate the criticality of alpha oscillations, we used detrended fluctuation analysis (DFA, see Materials and methods), which can discriminate between random fluctuations in the amplitude of oscillations and critical oscillations with long-range temporal correlations.”

5) Data and code sharing.

The raw spike data for the three states (sub/super/critical) should be made available, so that everyone can re-analyse them. The code should be made available as well. Please share the code before acceptance.

The code has been uploaded at:

https://figshare.com/articles/Pre-stimulus_phase_and_amplitude_regulation_of_phase-locked_responses_is_maximized_in_the_critical_state/12167943

6) I didn't find a clear definition of the stimulus input? What's the duration and magnitude? Is it a constant current in time? Why is the stimulus strength defined as the number of stimulated neurons? Would results be similar if stimulus strength is modeled as the magnitude of input current?

We thank the reviewers for suggesting to improve the definition of the stimulus input. We now clearly define the stimulus input in the Materials and methods:

“The stimulus had the same weight as any of the excitatory-excitatory connections in the network (winput = wEE = 0.0085), and was applied simultaneously (i.e., for 1 time step of 1 ms) to all the n excitatory neurons that the stimulus connected to. Additionally, we investigated the effect of the stimulus strength on network dynamics by modulating winput by a factor of 1/64 to 64 times the original value, on a log2 scale.”

We have also added this definition to the Results section:

“The stimulus had the same weight as any of the excitatory-to-excitatory synapses in the network (winput = wEE = 0.0085), and lasted for one time step (1 ms).”

7) Figure 1D, if input current is larger, does it expand the dynamic range of the supercritical networks?

To answer this question, we now explain why the PLF does not reach the maximum value of 1 in our network. In addition, we performed simulation experiments to test stronger stimulus strengths and report the results. We made the following changes to the manuscript (Results):

“Interestingly, in the case of supercritical networks, the strong, ongoing oscillatory activity prevents the phase-locking response from saturating (PLF~1), even at the largest stimulus sizes (Figure 1D, Figure 2—figure supplement 1).”

We acknowledge that another way to affect stimulus response is to alter stimulus strength (Results):

“Another factor that can shape stimulus response, other than stimulus size, is the strength of the external stimulus.”

Subsequently, we report the effect of changing the external input weight on the dynamic range (Results):

“We found that an 8-fold increase in the external input strength was sufficient to bring the PLF of supercritical networks to saturation, for the largest stimulus size (Figure 1—figure supplement 3B). However, such a large increase in the input strength also leads to a stronger PLF for the smaller stimulus sizes in the case of subcritical and critical networks, which means they no longer show weak PLF responses reducing their dynamic range. Of all tested strengths and networks, the maximal dynamic range occurs for critical networks at a multiplier ~1 (Figure 1—figure supplement 3C).”

8) Since previous work has shown criticality maximizes dynamic range of evoked rate responses, how is this result different from previous work?

The primary difference lies in the oscillatory nature of ongoing activity as well as of the stimulus-evoked activity in the CROS model. These properties are closely related to research using mass-neuronal electrophysiological readouts such as M/EEG. To emphasize these points, we have added new text and figures. In particular, (Results):

“Previously, it was shown that criticality maximizes the dynamic range of evoked firing-rate responses (Gautam et al., 2015; Kinouchi and Copelli, 2006; Larremore et al., 2011; Shew et al., 2009).”

Importantly, the CROS model produces EEG-like network oscillations and oscillatory responses to stimuli and, therefore, are suitably investigated for its phase-locking response as now explained (Results):

“However, event-related responses in large oscillatory networks also reflect a reset in the oscillatory phase following stimulation, leading to the phase-locking of the oscillation to the stimulus (Makeig et al., 2002; Min et al., 2007; Palva et al., 2005).”

To better visualize the oscillatory nature of the network, we have added raster plots of a single-trial stimulus response (as requested also in point #1 above) (Results):

“the stimulus response in our networks is indeed oscillatory, which may result in stimulus phase-locking (Figure 1B)”

Subsequently, we use the phase-locking factor to show how strongly different networks phase-lock to the stimulus (Results):

“Figure 2A: Ongoing oscillations phase-lock to stimulus. Example shown for 5 neurons stimulated, for sub-critical (blue), critical (green), and super-critical networks (red).”

Figure 1C, Is the phase-locking response related to the onset increase of firing rate? If the network shows onset response in rate, it would result in more rising phases, right?

As mentioned in our replies above, it is not ideal to use firing-rate when investigating oscillatory phenomena. To further clarify this, we included the following paragraph (Results):

“As such, the firing rate and phase-locking responses reflect fundamentally different aspects of network activity, e.g., evoked firing-rate responses may increase the overall network activity without affecting the phase of oscillatory activity in a particular frequency band. In fact, evoked firing-rate responses are not an appropriate measure of network response in networks with strong oscillations: they fluctuate continuously with the phase of the ongoing oscillation, making it difficult to judge the firing-rate responses in absolute values, i.e., without relating it to the ongoing oscillation. In that respect, the phase-locking response is a more appropriate way to detect changes in network activity occurring due to external stimulation”

Although we consider these valid arguments for why we analyze phase-locking responses instead of increases in firing rates, we have also included below an extensive analysis showing how firing-rate responses can be confounded with the oscillatory activity of the network, and confirming that firing rates are not always coupled to oscillatory responses in our model. We decided to not include these analysis results in the revised manuscript because the CROS model is oscillatory as emphasized in our replies above. We organized the following section as follows: first a short summary of our findings, followed by the corresponding figures, and end with a Materials and methods section. We begin by reiterating the reviewers’ question—"Figure 1C, Is the phase-locking response related to the onset increase of firing rate? If the network shows onset response in rate, it would result in more rising phases, right?”

– Yes, both increases and decreases in firing rate will cause significant phase-locking (Author response image 2), and they do so even in the absence of stimulation. Firing rates fluctuate with the ongoing oscillation, such that increases/decreases in firing rate correspond to specific oscillation phases, leading to high phase-locking factors.

Author response image 2
Change in firing rate corresponds to the phase of the alpha oscillation for networks with strong alpha oscillations in the absence of stimulation.

(A) Trial-averaged baseline-corrected firing rates corresponding to the largest decrease (blue), no substantial change (green), and highest increases (red) in firing rate. Intervals for calculating pre-stimulus baseline and post-stimulus firing rate are shaded in grey. (B) PLF at 150 ms post-stimulus (red shaded line) is high for both the lower and the higher firing-rate percentile bin, in the absence of any stimulation. Horizontal dashed line represents PLF significance threshold (see Materials and methods).

– However, the firing rate is not always coupled with the amplitude of alpha oscillations (Author response image 3). This is important, because both the ongoing activity, and also the stimulus responses, are oscillatory in nature. Thus, it is possible for a network to respond only with a change in the firing rate, without an oscillatory signal conducive to a change in the phase of the ongoing alpha oscillation (which is the main organizational principle in this network), and vice versa. Therefore, the phase-locking response we are measuring here is very different from the firing-rate response that has been measured in previous studies.

– Last, we investigated if the pre-stimulus firing rate regulates post-stimulus phase-locking (Author response image 4). As we have already shown in Author response image 2, the firing rate is strongly related to the oscillation phase. In our model, for networks with strong oscillations the phase of the oscillation diverges slowly, so the firing rate (oscillation phase) at a pre-stimulus time is predictive of the oscillation phase at post-stimulus time. As such pre-stimulus firing rate appears, inadvertently, to regulate post-stimulus PLF in the absence of stimuli (Author response image 4D). However, pre-stimulus amplitude does not have this issue: there is no regulation in unstimulated networks (Author response image 4B).

Author response image 3
Firing rates and amplitude of alpha oscillations are not always coupled.

(A) For unstimulated networks, alpha-oscillation amplitude and firing rate are only strongly coupled in the critical regime. Pearson correlation of mean amplitude and firing rate calculated on 100 ms windows for unstimulated networks. (B,C) Coupling between post-stimulus oscillatory amplitude and firing rate is dependent on stimulus size. Pearson’s correlation coefficient between the firing rate (1–50 ms), and oscillation amplitude for the same time interval. For the weak stimulus (B, n = 5), the critical networks show stronger coupling between firing rate and amplitude compared to their sub/supercritical counterparts. However, for the strong stimulus (n = 1800) critical networks show weaker coupling between firing rate and amplitude, compared to their sub-/super-critical counterparts. (D) Coupling of post-stimulus amplitude and firing rate for the 3 example networks highlighted with triangles on the phase space (A-C), for the entire range of stimulus sizes.

Author response image 4
Networks with strong oscillatory activity show regulation of phase-locking by pre-stimulus firing rate, even when the networks are not stimulated.

(A) Trial-averaged amplitude for unstimulated networks, with trials split into lowest (blue), medium (green) and highest (red) pre-stimulus (grey window) oscillation amplitude. (B) No significant phase-locking for trials used in (A) in the unstimulated condition. (C) Trial-averaged firing rate for unstimulated networks, with trials split into lowest (blue), medium (green) and highest (red) pre-stimulus (grey window) firing rate. (D) Significant phase-locking for trials used in (C) in the unstimulated condition. Red dashed lines indicate post-stimulus time (equivalent to 25 ms post-stimulus corrected for filter effect), horizontal black dashed lines indicate PLF significance threshold (see Materials and methods).

Materials and methods:

PLF significance threshold

To determine whether the post-stimulus PLF for a particular pre-stimulus firing rate or pre-stimulus amplitude percentile bin is significant, we generated 200 random phases equal to the number of trials in a percentile bin. We repeated this process 106 times. PLF values that are above the 95% percentile of the bootstrapped PLFs are considered significant.

Onset increase in firing rate and phase-locking factors

To investigate the relationship between the phase-locking response and the onset increase in firing rate, we chose a supercritical network which exhibits strong alpha oscillations. We ran the network without any stimulation, to investigate whether changes in firing rate can lead to phase-locking, also in unstimulated networks. As such, we analyzed the network as if it were stimulated, by generating a time series of “stimuli”, with the same temporal properties as the runs with stimulation (stimulus spacing at 1 second, with a 500-ms jitter). To measure the onset increase in firing rate, we quantified the firing rate at 1–50 ms post-“stimulus” relative to the firing rate during -275 to -175 ms pre-“stimulus” (corresponding to the -150 to -50 ms pre-stimulus interval used to study pre-stimulus amplitude regulation, after accounting for the 125 ms filter delay). We binned trials in 10 percentile bins based on the difference between post and pre-“stimulus” firing rate. Afterwards, we compared the average firing rate across trials in each bin, with the pre-stimulus firing rate activity subtracted. Signals were filtered in the 8–16 Hz frequency range, and PLF was computed across trials belonging to the same firing-rate percentile bin. Note that, due to the filter delay, the PLF time series (Author response image 2B) is time shifted by 125 ms compared to the firing rate time series (Author response image 2A).

Relationship between firing rate and amplitude of alpha oscillations

For unstimulated networks, we split the 2000 seconds time series into 100 ms windows. We then computed the mean firing rate and mean amplitude of alpha oscillations (8–16 Hz) for each of these short windows, and then correlated the two measures (Pearson). Amplitude windows were time shifted by 125 ms to account for the filter delay. For stimulated networks, we computed the average firing rate between 1 and 50 ms post-stimulus, and the average amplitude at 126 ms to 175 ms post-stimulus, again, accounting for the filter delay. We then correlated firing rate responses with post-stimulus amplitude using Pearson’s correlation for all networks / stimulus sizes.

Pre-stimulus firing-rate regulation of post-stimulus phase-locking response

We ran a supercritical network, without stimulation, for 2000 seconds. We generated a time series of “stimuli” (stimulus spacing at 1 second, 500 ms jitter), to analyze the network as if it were stimulated. To investigate the effect of pre-stimulus amplitude on post-stimulus phase locking, we split the trials in 10 percentile bins, based on the pre-stimulus amplitude between -150 and -50 ms (grey rectangle), and show the results for the 0–10th, 40–50th and 90–100th percentiles. We then computed the phase-locking factor across trials in each bin. To investigate the effect of pre-stimulus firing rate on post-stimulus phase locking, we again split the trials in 10 percentile bins, based on the pre-stimulus firing rate between -275 and -175 ms (grey rectangle), and show the results for the 0–10th, 40–50th and 90–100th percentiles. We then computed the phase-locking factor across trials in each bin. The 125 ms time difference between the pre-stimulus interval for firing rate and amplitude accounts for the filter delay. Also, note that, due to the filter delay, the PLF time series (Author response image 4D) is time shifted by 125 ms compared to the firing rate time series (Author response image 4C).

9) Figure 3D, Similar to my comments above about the dynamics range, I wonder if the range of pre-stimulus regulation depends on the magnitude of input current. If you drive the supercritical network with stronger input for each neuron, does it expand the region of significant regulation.

We now address this in the Results section, for pre-stimulus amplitude regulation:

“Changing the weight of the external input also affects the range of stimuli over which networks show pre-stimulus amplitude regulation (Figure 2—figure supplement 3B).Nonetheless, the range of stimuli over which the critical networks show pre-stimulus amplitude regulation is superior to that of the subcritical and supercritical networks, regardless of the strength of the input weight (Figure 2—figure supplement 3C).”

And also for pre-stimulus phase regulation:

“We found that changing the weight of the external input also affects the range of stimuli over which pre-stimulus phase regulation is significant (Figure 3—figure supplement 3B). Nonetheless, pre-stimulus phase-regulation occurs for a wider range of stimuli in critical networks than in their subcritical or supercritical counterparts, regardless of the strength of the input weight (Figure 3—figure supplement 3C).”

10) About the balance between excitation and inhibition: I think it should be the product of synaptic weight and connection probability that determines the network dynamics. Why is the critical transition at approximately when excitatory connectivity percentage equals inhibitory connectivity percentage? How does the transition depends on the recurrent weights?

As the reviewers correctly point out, the balance of excitation and inhibition is indeed a bi-product of connectivity (number of connections) and the strength of the recurrent weights. The critical line follows a diagonal through the phase space where excitatory and inhibitory connectivity are roughly equal, but the exact positioning of the line is coincidental, i.e., the optimization algorithm could in principle have selected the parameters shown in Figure 4 of the revised manuscript. We showed, when addressing Major Point 2, that by modulating the weight of excitatory and excitatory connections, we can shift the position of the critical line on the phase space, and its position does not affect the relationship between criticality and versatility of information processing (Results):

“Therefore, we expect that the functional consequences of criticality on stimulus processing that emerged out of the model, are generic to criticality, and not specific to the actual parameters. To verify this, we repeated our analysis for two versions of the model where the synaptic weight of excitatory to excitatory connections (wEE) was multiplied by a factor of 0.75, or by a factor of 1.25. Multiplication of wEE by a factor of 0.75 reduced the overall activity in the network, which means that stronger connectivity is required to achieve the same dynamics, leading to a shift up of the critical line through the phase space (Figure 4). Increasing wEE had the opposite effect on the critical line. Importantly, the functional consequences of criticality—maximized dynamic range, pre-stimulus amplitude and phase regulation—followed this shift in the critical line. This suggests that the relationship between criticality and versatility in stimulus processing is not coincidental on the specific choice of parameters, but generic to criticality.”

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.53016.sa2

Article and author information

Author details

  1. Arthur-Ervin Avramiea

    Department of Integrative Neurophysiology, Center for Neurogenomics and Cognitive Research (CNCR), Amsterdam Neuroscience, Amsterdam, Netherlands
    Contribution
    Software, Formal analysis, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology
    Contributed equally with
    Richard Hardstone
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0002-0826-8269
  2. Richard Hardstone

    1. Department of Integrative Neurophysiology, Center for Neurogenomics and Cognitive Research (CNCR), Amsterdam Neuroscience, Amsterdam, Netherlands
    2. Neuroscience Institute, New York University School of Medicine, New York, United States
    Contribution
    Conceptualization, Software, Formal analysis, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology
    Contributed equally with
    Arthur-Ervin Avramiea
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0002-7502-9145
  3. Jan-Matthis Lueckmann

    1. Department of Integrative Neurophysiology, Center for Neurogenomics and Cognitive Research (CNCR), Amsterdam Neuroscience, Amsterdam, Netherlands
    2. Technical University of Munich, Munich, Germany
    Contribution
    Conceptualization, Software, Formal analysis, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0003-4320-4663
  4. Jan Bím

    1. Department of Integrative Neurophysiology, Center for Neurogenomics and Cognitive Research (CNCR), Amsterdam Neuroscience, Amsterdam, Netherlands
    2. Czech Technical University in Prague, Prague, Czech Republic
    Contribution
    Software, Methodology
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0003-2780-5610
  5. Huibert D Mansvelder

    Department of Integrative Neurophysiology, Center for Neurogenomics and Cognitive Research (CNCR), Amsterdam Neuroscience, Amsterdam, Netherlands
    Contribution
    Conceptualization, Supervision
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0003-1365-5340
  6. Klaus Linkenkaer-Hansen

    Department of Integrative Neurophysiology, Center for Neurogenomics and Cognitive Research (CNCR), Amsterdam Neuroscience, Amsterdam, Netherlands
    Contribution
    Conceptualization, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration
    For correspondence
    klaus.linkenkaer@cncr.vu.nl
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0003-2140-9780

Funding

Nederlandse Organisatie voor Wetenschappelijk Onderzoek (612.001.123)

  • Richard Hardstone
  • Klaus Linkenkaer-Hansen

Nederlandse Organisatie voor Wetenschappelijk Onderzoek (406.15.256)

  • Arthur-Ervin Avramiea
  • Klaus Linkenkaer-Hansen

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Acknowledgements

This work was funded by Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO) Physical Sciences Grant 612.001.123 (to KL-H and RH) and NWO Social Sciences grant 406.15.256 (to KL-H and A-EA). We thank Simon-Shlomo Poil for discussions on the manuscript.

Senior and Reviewing Editor

  1. Floris P de Lange, Radboud University, Netherlands

Version history

  1. Received: October 24, 2019
  2. Accepted: April 20, 2020
  3. Accepted Manuscript published: April 23, 2020 (version 1)
  4. Accepted Manuscript updated: April 27, 2020 (version 2)
  5. Version of Record published: May 12, 2020 (version 3)

Copyright

© 2020, Avramiea et al.

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.

Metrics

  • 1,949
    Page views
  • 239
    Downloads
  • 12
    Citations

Article citation count generated by polling the highest count across the following sources: Crossref, PubMed Central, Scopus.

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Arthur-Ervin Avramiea
  2. Richard Hardstone
  3. Jan-Matthis Lueckmann
  4. Jan Bím
  5. Huibert D Mansvelder
  6. Klaus Linkenkaer-Hansen
(2020)
Pre-stimulus phase and amplitude regulation of phase-locked responses are maximized in the critical state
eLife 9:e53016.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.53016

Further reading

    1. Cell Biology
    2. Neuroscience
    Elisabeth Jongsma, Anita Goyala ... Collin Yvès Ewald
    Research Article Updated

    The amyloid beta (Aβ) plaques found in Alzheimer’s disease (AD) patients’ brains contain collagens and are embedded extracellularly. Several collagens have been proposed to influence Aβ aggregate formation, yet their role in clearance is unknown. To investigate the potential role of collagens in forming and clearance of extracellular aggregates in vivo, we created a transgenic Caenorhabditis elegans strain that expresses and secretes human Aβ1-42. This secreted Aβ forms aggregates in two distinct places within the extracellular matrix. In a screen for extracellular human Aβ aggregation regulators, we identified different collagens to ameliorate or potentiate Aβ aggregation. We show that a disintegrin and metalloprotease a disintegrin and metalloprotease 2 (ADM-2), an ortholog of ADAM9, reduces the load of extracellular Aβ aggregates. ADM-2 is required and sufficient to remove the extracellular Aβ aggregates. Thus, we provide in vivo evidence of collagens essential for aggregate formation and metalloprotease participating in extracellular Aβ aggregate removal.

    1. Computational and Systems Biology
    2. Neuroscience
    Marjorie Xie, Samuel P Muscinelli ... Ashok Litwin-Kumar
    Research Article Updated

    The cerebellar granule cell layer has inspired numerous theoretical models of neural representations that support learned behaviors, beginning with the work of Marr and Albus. In these models, granule cells form a sparse, combinatorial encoding of diverse sensorimotor inputs. Such sparse representations are optimal for learning to discriminate random stimuli. However, recent observations of dense, low-dimensional activity across granule cells have called into question the role of sparse coding in these neurons. Here, we generalize theories of cerebellar learning to determine the optimal granule cell representation for tasks beyond random stimulus discrimination, including continuous input-output transformations as required for smooth motor control. We show that for such tasks, the optimal granule cell representation is substantially denser than predicted by classical theories. Our results provide a general theory of learning in cerebellum-like systems and suggest that optimal cerebellar representations are task-dependent.