Modelling the neural code in large populations of correlated neurons
Abstract
Neurons respond selectively to stimuli, and thereby define a code that associates stimuli with population response patterns. Certain correlations within population responses (noise correlations) significantly impact the information content of the code, especially in large populations. Understanding the neural code thus necessitates response models that quantify the coding properties of modelled populations, while fitting large-scale neural recordings and capturing noise correlations. In this paper, we propose a class of response model based on mixture models and exponential families. We show how to fit our models with expectation-maximization, and that they capture diverse variability and covariability in recordings of macaque primary visual cortex. We also show how they facilitate accurate Bayesian decoding, provide a closed-form expression for the Fisher information, and are compatible with theories of probabilistic population coding. Our framework could allow researchers to quantitatively validate the predictions of neural coding theories against both large-scale neural recordings and cognitive performance.
Introduction
A foundational idea in sensory neuroscience is that the activity of neural populations constitutes a ‘neural code’ for representing stimuli (Dayan and Abbott, 2005; Doya, 2007): the activity pattern of a population in response to a sensory stimulus encodes information about that stimulus, and downstream neurons decode, process, and re-encode this information in their own responses. Sequences of such neural populations implement the elementary functions that drive perception, cognition, and behaviour (Pitkow and Angelaki, 2017). Therefore, by studying the encoding and decoding of population responses, researchers may investigate how information is processed along neural circuits, and how this processing influences perception and behaviour (Wei and Stocker, 2015; Panzeri et al., 2017; Kriegeskorte and Douglas, 2018).
Given a true statistical model of how a neural population responds to (encodes information about) stimuli, Bayes’ rule can transform the encoding model into an optimal decoder of stimulus information (Zemel et al., 1998; Pillow et al., 2011). However, when validated as Bayesian decoders, statistical models of neural encoding are often outperformed by models trained to decode stimulus-information directly, indicating that the encoding models miss key statistics of the neural code (Graf et al., 2011; Walker et al., 2020). In particular, the correlations between neurons’ responses to repeated presentations of a given stimulus (noise correlations), and how these noise correlations are modulated by stimuli, can strongly impact coding in neural circuits (Zohary et al., 1994; Abbott and Dayan, 1999; Sompolinsky et al., 2001; Ecker et al., 2016; Kohn et al., 2016; Schneidman, 2016), especially in large populations of neurons (Moreno-Bote et al., 2014; Montijn et al., 2019; Bartolo et al., 2020; Kafashan et al., 2021; Rumyantsev et al., 2020).
Statistically validating theories of population coding in large neural circuits thus depends on encoding models that support accurate Bayesian decoding, effectively capture noise-correlations, and efficiently fit large-scale neural recordings. There are at least two classes of neural recordings for which established models have facilitated such analyses. Firstly, for recordings of binary spike-counts, pairwise-maximum entropy models (Schneidman et al., 2006; Lyamzin et al., 2010; Granot-Atedgi et al., 2013; Tkačik et al., 2013; Meshulam et al., 2017; Maoz et al., 2020) have been used to investigate the structure of the retinal code (Ganmor et al., 2015; Tkačik et al., 2015). Secondly, when modelling dynamic spike-train recordings, generalized linear models (GLMs) have proven effective at modelling spatio-temporal features of information processing in the retina and cortex (Pillow et al., 2008; Park et al., 2014; Runyan et al., 2017; Ruda et al., 2020).
Nevertheless, many theories of neural coding are formulated in terms of unbounded spike-counts (Ma et al., 2006; Beck et al., 2011a; Ganguli and Simoncelli, 2014; Makin et al., 2015; Yerxa et al., 2020), rather than the binary spike-counts of pairwise maximum entropy models. Furthermore, neural correlations are often low-dimensional (Arieli et al., 1996; Ecker et al., 2014; Goris et al., 2014; Rabinowitz et al., 2015; Okun et al., 2015; Semedo et al., 2019), in contrast with the correlations that result from the fully connected, recurrent structure of standard GLMs. Although there are extensions of the GLM approach that capture shared-variability (Vidne et al., 2012; Archer et al., 2014; Zhao and Park, 2017), they seem unable to support exact Bayesian decoding. Similarly, methods such as factor analysis that model unbounded spike-counts as continuous variables have proven highly effective at modelling neural correlations in large-scale recordings (Yu et al., 2009; Cunningham and Yu, 2014; Ecker et al., 2014; Semedo et al., 2019), yet it is also unknown if they can support accurate Bayesian decoding.
Towards modelling spike-count responses and accurate Bayesian decoding in large populations of correlated neurons, we develop a class of encoding model based on finite mixtures of Poisson distributions. Within neuroscience, Poisson mixtures are widely applied to modelling the spike-count distributions of individual neurons (Wiener and Richmond, 2003; Shidara et al., 2005; Goris et al., 2014; Taouali et al., 2016). Outside of neuroscience, mixtures of multivariate Poisson distributions are an established model of multivariate count distributions that effectively capture correlations in count data (Karlis and Meligkotsidou, 2007; Inouye et al., 2017).
Building on the theory of exponential family distributions (Wainwright and Jordan, 2008; Macke et al., 2011b), our model extends previous mixture models of multivariate count data in two ways. Firstly, we develop a tractable extension of Poisson mixtures that captures both over- and under-dispersed response variability (i.e. where the response variance is larger or smaller than the mean, respectively) based on Conway-Maxwell Poisson distributions (Shmueli et al., 2005; Stevenson, 2016). Secondly, we introduce an explicit dependence of the model on a stimulus variable, which allows the model to accurately capture changes in response statistics (including noise correlations) across stimuli. Importantly, the resulting encoding model affords closed-form expressions for both its Fisher information and probability density function, and thereby a rigorous quantification of the coding properties of a modelled neural population (Dayan and Abbott, 2005). Moreover, the model learns low-dimensional representations of stimulus-driven neural activity, and we show how it captures a fundamental property of population codes known as information-limiting correlations (Moreno-Bote et al., 2014; Montijn et al., 2019; Bartolo et al., 2020; Kafashan et al., 2021; Rumyantsev et al., 2020).
We apply our mixture model framework to both synthetic data and recordings from macaque primary visual cortex (V1), and demonstrate that it effectively models responses of populations of hundreds of neurons, captures noise correlations, and supports accurate Bayesian decoding. Moreover, we show how our model is compatible with the theory of probabilistic population coding (Zemel et al., 1998; Pouget et al., 2013), and could thus be used to study the theoretical coding properties of neural circuits, such as their efficiency (Ganguli and Simoncelli, 2014), linearity (Ma et al., 2006), or information content (Moreno-Bote et al., 2014).
Results
A critical part of our theoretical approach is based on expressing models of interest in exponential family form. An exponential family distribution over some data (in our case, neural responses) is defined by the proportionality relation , where are the so-called natural parameters, is a vector-valued function of the data called the sufficient statistic, and is a scalar-valued, non-negative function called the base measure (Wainwright and Jordan, 2008). The exponential family form allows us to modify and extend existing models in a simple and flexible manner, and derive analytical results about the coding properties of our models. We demonstrate our approach with applications to both synthetic data, and experimental data recorded in V1 of anaesthetized and awake macaques viewing drifting grating stimuli at different orientations (for details see Materials and methods).
Extended Poisson mixture models capture spike-count variability and covariability
Our first goal is to define a class of models of stimulus-independent, neural population activity, that model neural activity directly as spike-counts, and that accurately capture single-neuron variability and pairwise covariability. We base our models on Poisson distributions, as they are widely applied to modelling the trial-to-trial distribution of the number of spikes generated by a neuron (Dayan and Abbott, 2005; Macke et al., 2011a). We will also generalize our Poisson-based models with the theory of Conway-Maxwell (CoM) Poisson distributions (Sur et al., 2015; Stevenson, 2016; Chanialidis et al., 2018). The two-parameter CoM-Poisson model contains the one-parameter Poisson model as a special case, however, whereas the Poisson model always has a Fano factor (FF; the variance divided by the mean) of 1, the CoM-Poisson model can exhibit both over- (FF>1) and under-dispersion (FF<1), and thus capture the broader range of Fano factors observed in cortex (Stevenson, 2016).
The other key ingredient in our modelling approach are mixtures of Poisson distributions, which have been used to model complex spike-count distributions in cortex, and also allow for over-dispersion (Shidara et al., 2005; Goris et al., 2014; Taouali et al., 2016; Figure 1A). In our case, we mix multiple, independent Poisson distributions in parallel, as such models can capture covariability in count data as well (see Karlis and Meligkotsidou, 2007 for a more general formulation of multivariate Poisson mixtures than what we consider here). To construct such a model, we begin with a product of independent Poisson distributions (IP distribution), one per neuron. We then mix a finite number of component IP models, to arrive at a multivariate spike-count, finite mixture model (see Materials and methods). Importantly, although each component of this mixture is an IP distribution, randomly switching between components induces correlations between the neurons (Figure 1B,C).
IP mixtures can in fact model arbitrary covariability between neurons (see Materials and methods, Equation 7); however, they are still limited because the model neurons in an IP mixture are always over-dispersed. To overcome this, it is helpful to consider factor analysis (FA), which is widely applied to modelling neural population responses (Cunningham and Yu, 2014). IP mixtures are similar to FA, in that FA represents the covariance matrix of neural responses as the sum of a diagonal matrix that helps capture individual variance, and a low-rank matrix that captures covariance (see Bishop, 2006), and FA and IP mixtures can be fine-tuned to capture covariance arbitrarily well. However, whereas FA has distinct parameters for representing means and diagonal variances, the means and variances in an IP mixture are coupled through shared parameters (see Materials and methods, Equation 6). Our strategy will thus be to break this coupling between means and variances by granting IP mixtures an additional set of parameters based on the theory of CoM-Poisson distributions.
To do so, we first show how to express an IP mixture as the marginal distribution of an exponential family distribution. Note that an IP mixture with components may be expressed as a latent variable model over spike-count vectors and latent component-indices , where . In this formulation we denote the th component distribution by , and the probability of realizing (switching to) the th component by . The mixture model over spike-counts is then expressed as the marginal distribution , of the joint distribution . Under mild regularity assumptions (see Materials and methods), we may reparameterize this joint distribution in exponential family form as
where the vectors and , and matrix are the natural parameters of , and is the Kronecker delta vector defined by if , and 0 otherwise.
This representation affords an intuitive interpretation. In general, the natural parameters of an IP distribution are the logarithms of the average spike-counts (firing rates), and the natural parameters of the first component distribution of an IP mixture are simply . The natural parameters of the th component for are then the sum of the ‘baseline’ parameters and column from the matrix of parameters (Equation 13, Materials and methods). Because the dimension of the baseline parameters is much smaller than the total number of parameters in a given mixture, the baseline parameters provide a relatively low-dimensional means of affecting all the component distributions of the given mixture, as well as the probability distribution over indices (Figure 1D; see Materials and methods, Equation 12 for how the index-probabilities depend on ).
We next extend Equation 1 with the theory of CoM-Poisson distributions, and define the latent variable exponential family
where is the vector of log-factorials of the individual spike-counts, and are a set of natural parameters derived from CoM-Poisson distributions (see Materials and methods). Based on this construction, each component is a product of independent CoM-Poisson distributions, and when , we recover an IP mixture defined by Equation 1 with parameters , , and . The first component of this model has parameters and , and as with the IP mixture, the parameters are translated by column of when . However, the parameters are never translated, and remain the same for each component distribution (Equation 16, Materials and methods, and see Equation 15 for formulae for the index-probabilities ). We refer to models defined by Equation 2 as CoM-based (CB) mixtures, and as CB parameters.
Due to the addition of the CB parameters , a CB mixture breaks the coupling between the spike-count means and variances that is present in the simpler IP mixture (Equation 17, Materials and methods). In Figure 1D–F, we demonstrate how changing the parameters of a CB mixture can concentrate or disperse both the mixture distribution and its components, and that a CB mixture can indeed exhibit under-dispersion.
To validate our mixture models, we tested if they capture variability and covariability of V1 population responses to repeated presentations of a grating stimulus with fixed orientation ( neurons and repetitions of 150 ms duration in one awake macaque; and of duration 70 ms in one anaesthetized macaque). We fit our mixtures to the complete datasets with expectation-maximization (EM, a standard choice for training finite mixture models [McLachlan et al., 2019] see Materials and methods). The CB mixture accurately captured single-neuron variability (Figure 2A–B, red symbols), including both cases of over-dispersion and under-dispersion. On the other hand, the simpler IP mixture (Figure 2A–B, blue symbols) cannot accommodate under-dispersion due to its mathematical limits, and demonstrated limited ability to model over-dispersion due to the coupling between the mean and variance (Equation 6).
To understand how the CB parameters allow the CB mixture to overcome the limits of the IP mixture, we plot a histogram of the CB parameters for both fits (Figure 2C–D). If the CB parameter of a given CoM-Poisson distribution is , , or , then the CoM-Poisson distribution is under-dispersed, over-dispersed, or Poisson-distributed, respectively. When a CB mixture is fit to the awake data (Figure 2C), we see that it learns a range of values for the CB parameters around −1, to accommodate the variety of Fano factors observed in the awake data (Figure 2A). On the anaesthetized data, even though IP mixtures can capture over-dispersion, the IP mixture underestimates the dispersion of neurons due to the coupling between the mean and variance (Figure 2B). The CB mixture thus uses the CB parameters to further disperse its model neurons (Figure 2D).
In contrast with individual variability, we found that both mixture models were flexible enough to qualitatively capture pairwise noise correlation structure in both awake and anaesthetized animals (Figure 3A–B), and that the distributions of modelled neural correlations were broadly similar when compared to the data (Figure 3C–D). In Appendix 1, we rigorously compare IP mixtures, CB mixtures, and FA on our datasets, and show that although FA is better than our mixture models at capturing second-order statistics in training data, IP mixtures and CB mixtures achieve comparable predictive performance as FA when evaluated on held-out data.
Extended Poisson mixture models capture stimulus-dependent response statistics
So far, we have introduced the exponential family theory of IP and CB mixtures, and shown how they capture response variability and covariability for a fixed stimulus. To allow us to study stimulus encoding and decoding, we further extend our mixtures by inducing a dependency of the model parameters on a stimulus. When there are a finite number of stimulus conditions and sufficient data, we may define a stimulus-dependent model with a lookup table, and fit it by fitting a distinct model at each stimulus condition. However, this is inefficient when the amount of data at each stimulus-condition is limited and the stimulus-dependent statistics have structure that is shared across conditions. A notable feature of the exponential family parameterizations in Equations 1 and 2 is that the baseline parameters influence both the index probabilities and all the component distributions of the model. This suggests that by restricting stimulus-dependence to the baseline parameters, we might model rich stimulus-dependent response structure, while bounding the complexity of the model.
In general, we refer to any finite mixture with stimulus-dependent parameters as a conditional mixture (CM), and depending on whether the CM is based on Equations 1 and 2 and, we refer to it as an IP- or CB-CM, respectively. Although there are many ways we might induce stimulus-dependence, in this paper we consider two forms of CM: (i) a maximal CM, which we implement as a lookup table, such that all the parameters in Equations 1 and 2 and depend on the stimulus, and (ii) a minimal CM, for which we restrict stimulus-dependence to the baseline parameters . This results in the CB-CM
where is the stimulus, and are the stimulus-dependent baseline parameters, and we recover a minimal, IP-CM by setting .
The IP-CM again affords an intuitive interpretation: The first component of an IP-CM has stimulus-dependent natural parameters , and thus the stimulus-dependent firing rate, or tuning curve, of the th neuron given is , where is the th element of . The natural parameters of the th component for are then the sum of and column of . As such, given , the tuning curve of the th neuron is a ‘gain-modulated’ version of , where the gain is the exponential function of element of column of (see Equation 13, Materials and methods). For a CB-CM this interpretation no longer holds exactly, but still serves as an approximate description of the behaviour of its components (see Equation 16 and the accompanying discussions).
Towards understanding the expressive power of CMs, we study a minimal, CB-CM with neurons, mixture components, and randomly chosen parameters (see Materials and methods). Moreover, we assume that the stimulus is periodic (e.g. the orientation of a grating), and that the tuning curves of the component distributions have a von Mises shape, which is a widely applied model of neural tuning to periodic stimuli (Herz et al., 2017). We may achieve such a shape by defining the stimulus-dependent baseline parameters as , where and are parameters, and . Figure 4A shows that the tuning curves of the CB-CM neurons are approximately bell-shaped, yet many also exhibit significant deviations.
We also study if CMs can be effectively fit to datasets comparable to those obtained in typical neurophysiology experiments. We generated 200 responses from the CB-CM described above — the ground truth CB-CM — to each of 10 orientations spread evenly over the half-circle, for a total of 2000 stimulus-response sample points. We then used this data to fit a CB-CM with the same number of components. Toward this aim, we derive an approximate EM algorithm to optimize model parameters (see Materials and methods). Figure 4B shows that the tuning curves of the learned CB-CM are nearly indistinguishable from those of the ground truth CB-CM (Figure 4B, coefficient of determination ).
To reveal the orientation-dependent latent structure of the model, in Figure 4C we plot the index probability for every as a function of the orientation . In Figure 4D we show that the orientation-dependent index probabilities of the learned CB-CM qualitatively match the true index probabilities in Figure 4C. We also note that although the learned CB-CM does not correctly identify the indices themselves, this has no effect on the performance of the CB-CM.
The orientation-dependent index-probabilities provide a high-level picture of how the complexity and structure of model correlations varies with the orientation. The vertical dashed lines in Figure 4C–D denote two orientations that yield substantially different index probabilities . When a large number of index-probabilities are non-zero, the correlation-matrices of the CB-CM can exhibit complex correlations with both negative and positive values (Figure 4E). However, when one index dominates, the correlation structure largely disappears (Figure 4F). In Figure 4G we show that the FFs also depend on stimulus orientation. Lastly, we find that both the FF and the correlation-matrices of the learned CB-CM are nearly indistinguishable from the ground-truth CB-CM (Figure 4E–G).
In summary, our analyses show that minimal CB-CMs can express complex, stimulus-dependent response statistics, and that we can recover the structure of a ground truth CB-CM from realistic amounts of synthetic data with EM. In the following sections, we rigorously evaluate the performance of CMs on our awake and anaesthetized datasets.
Conditional mixtures effectively model neural responses in macaque V1
A variety of models may be defined within the CM framework delineated by Equations 1, 2 and 3. Towards understanding how effectively CMs can model real data, we compare different variants by their cross-validated log-likelihood on both our awake and anaesthetized datasets; this is the same data used in Figures 2 and 3 but now including all stimulus-conditions. We consider both IP and CB variants of each of the following conditional mixtures: (i) maximal CMs where we learn a distinct mixture for each of stimulus conditions, (ii) minimal CMs with von Mises-tuned components, and (iii) minimal CMs with discrete-tuned components given by , where is the Kronecker delta vector with elements, and is the index of the stimulus. In contrast with the von Mises CM, the discrete CM makes no assumptions about the form of component tuning. In Table 1 we detail the number of parameters for all forms of CM.
To provide an interpretable measure of the relative performance of each CM variant, we define the ‘information gain’ as the difference between the estimated log-likelihood (base ) of the given CM and the log-likelihood of a von Mises-tuned, independent Poisson model, which is a standard model of uncorrelated neural responses to oriented stimuli (Herz et al., 2017). We then evaluate the predictive performance of our models with 10-fold cross-validation of the information gain.
Table 2 shows that the CM variants considered achieve comparable performance, and perform substantially better than the independent Poisson lower bound on both the awake and anaesthetized data. Figure 5 shows that a performance peak emerges smoothly as the model complexity (number of parameters) is increased. In all cases, the CB models outperform their IP counterparts, and typically with fewer parameters. The discrete CB-CMs achieve high performance on both datasets. In contrast, von Mises CMs perform well on the anaesthetized data but more poorly on the awake data, and maximal CMs exhibit the opposite trend. Nevertheless, von Mises CMs solve a more difficult statistical problem as they also interpolate between stimulus conditions, and so may still prove relevant even where performance is limited. On the other hand, even though maximal CMs achieve high performance, they simply do so by replicating the high performance of stimulus-independent mixtures (Figures 2 and 3) at each stimulus condition, and require more parameters than minimal CMs to maximize performance.
Conditional mixtures facilitate accurate and efficient decoding of neural responses
To demonstrate that CMs model the neural code, we must show that CMs not only capture the features of neural responses, but that these features also encode stimulus-information. Given an encoding model and a response from the model , we may optimally decode the information in the response about the stimulus by applying Bayes’ rule , where is the posterior distribution (the decoded information), and represents our prior assumptions about the stimulus (Zemel et al., 1998). When we do not know the true encoding model, and rather fit a statistical model to stimulus-response data, using the statistical model for Bayesian decoding and analyzing its performance can tell us how well it captures the features of the neural code.
We analyze the performance of Bayesian decoders based on CMs by quantifying their decoding performance, and comparing the results to other common approaches to decoding. We evaluate decoding performance with the 10-fold cross-validation log-posterior probability (base ) of the true stimulus value , for both our awake and anaesthetized V1 datasets. With regard to choosing the number of components , we analyze the decoding performance of CMs that achieved the best encoding performance based as indicated in Table 2 and depicted Figure 5. We do this to demonstrate how well a single model can simultaneously perform at both encoding and decoding, instead of applying distinct procedures for selecting CMs based on decoding performance (see Materials and methods for a summary of trade-offs when choosing ).
In our comparisons we focus on minimal, discrete CMs as overall they achieved high performance on both datasets (Figure 5). To characterize the importance of neural correlations to Bayesian decoding, we compare our CMs to the decoding performance of independent Poisson models with discrete tuning (Non-mixed IP). To characterize the optimality of our Bayesian decoders, we also evaluate the performance of linear multiclass decoders (Linear), as well nonlinear multiclass decoders defined as artificial neural networks (ANNs) with two hidden layers and a cross-validated number of hidden units (for details on the training and model selection procedure, see Materials and methods).
Table 3 shows that on the awake data, the performance of the CMs is statistically indistinguishable from the ANN, and the CMs and the ANN significantly exceed the performance of both the Linear and Non-mixed IP models. On the anaesthetized data, the minimal CM approaches the performance of the ANN, and the minimal CMs and ANN models again exceed the performance of the Non-mixed IP and Linear models. Yet in this case, the Linear model is much more competitive, whereas the Non-mixed IP model performs very poorly, possibly because of the larger magnitude of noise correlations in this data. In Appendix 2, we also report that a Bayesian decoder based on a factor analysis (FA) encoding model performed inconsistently, and poorly relative to CMs, as it would occasionally assign numerically 0 probability to the true stimulus, and thus score an average log-posterior of negative infinity. In Appendix 2, we present preliminary evidence that this is because CMs capture higher order structure that FA cannot.
On both the awake and anaesthetized data the ANN requires two orders of magnitude more parameters than the CMs to achieve its performance gains. In addition, the CB-CM achieves marginally better performance with fewer parameters than the IP-CM, indicating that although modelling individual variability is not essential for effective Bayesian decoding, doing so still results in a more parsimonious model of the neural code. In Appendix 3, we report a sample complexity analysis of CM encoding and decoding performance. We found that whereas our anaesthetized V1 dataset (sample size ) was large enough to saturate the performance of our models, a larger awake V1 dataset () could yield further improvements to decoding performance.
We also consider widely used alternative measures of decoding performance, namely the Fisher information (FI), which is an upper bound on the average precision (inverse variance) of the posterior (Brunel and Nadal, 1998), as well as the linear Fisher information (LFI), which is a linear approximation of the FI (Seriès et al., 2004) corresponding to the accuracy of the optimal, unbiased linear decoder of the stimulus (Kanitscheider et al., 2015a). The FI is especially helpful when the posterior cannot be evaluated directly (such as when it is continuous), and is widely adopted in theoretical (Abbott and Dayan, 1999; Beck et al., 2011b; Ecker et al., 2014; Moreno-Bote et al., 2014; Kohn et al., 2016) and experimental (Ecker et al., 2011; Kafashan et al., 2021; Rumyantsev et al., 2020) studies of neural coding. As with other models based on exponential family theory (Ma et al., 2006; Beck et al., 2011b; Ecker et al., 2016), the FI of a minimal CM may be expressed in closed-form, and is equal to its LFI (see Materials and methods), and therefore minimal CMs can be used to study FI analytically and obtain model-based estimates of FI from data.
To study how well CMs capture FI, we defined 40 random subpopulations of neurons from both our V1 datasets, fit von Mises IP-CMs to the responses of each subpopulation, and used these learned models as ground-truth populations. We then generated 50 responses at each of 10 evenly spaced orientations from each ground truth IP-CM, for a total of responses per ground-truth model. We then fit a new IP-CM to each set of 500 responses, and compared the FI of the re-fit CM to the FI of the ground-truth CM at 50 evenly spaced orientations. Pooled over all populations and orientations, the relative error of the estimated FI was on the awake data and on the anaesthetized data, suggesting that IP-CMs can recover and even interpolate approximate FIs of ground-truth populations from modest amounts of data.
To summarize, CMs support accurate Bayesian decoding in awake and anaesthetized macaque V1 recordings, and are competitive with nonlinear decoders with two orders of magnitude more parameters. Moreover, CMs afford closed-form expressions of FI and can interpolate good estimates of FI from modest amounts of data, and thereby support analyses of neural data based on this widely applied theoretical tool.
Constrained conditional mixtures support linear probabilistic population coding
Having shown that minimal CMs can both capture the statistics of neural encoding and facilitate accurate Bayesian decoding, we now aim to show how they relate to an influential theory of neural coding known as probabilistic population codes (PPCs), which describes how neural circuits process information in terms of encoding and Bayesian decoding (Zemel et al., 1998). In particular, linear probabilistic population codes (LPPCs) are PPCs with a restricted encoding model, that explain numerous features of neural coding in the brain (Ma et al., 2006; Beck et al., 2008; Beck et al., 2011a).
In general, an exponential family of distributions that depend on some stimulus may be expressed as , where is the sufficient statistic, μ is the base measure, and is known as the log-partition function (in Equations 1-3 we used the proportionality symbol ∝ to avoid writing the log-partition functions explicitly). A PPC is an LPPC when its encoding model is in the so-called exponential family with linear sufficient statistics (EFLSS), which has the form for some functions and (Beck et al., 2011a). If we equate the two expressions we see that an EFLSS is a stimulus-dependent exponential family that satisfies two constraints: that the sufficient statistic is linear, and that the log-partition function does not depend on the stimulus, so that .
As presented, the EFLSS is a mathematical model that does not have fittable parameters. We wish to express CMs as a form of EFLSS in order to show how a fittable model could be compatible with LPPC theory. If we return to the general expression for a minimal CM (Equation 3) and assume that the log-partition function is given by the constant α, then we may write
where , such that the given CM is in the EFLSS. Observe that this equation only holds due to the specific structure of minimal CMs: if the parameters , , or would depend on the stimulus, then it would not be possible to absorb them into the function .
Ultimately, this equivalence between constrained CMs and EFLSSs allows LPPC theory to be applied to constrained CMs, and provides theorists working on neural coding with an effective statistical tool that can help validate their hypotheses.
Minimal conditional mixtures capture information-limiting correlations
Our last aim is to demonstrate that CMs can approximately represent a central phenomenon in neural coding known as information-limiting correlations, which are neural correlations that fundamentally limit stimulus-information in neural circuits (Moreno-Bote et al., 2014; Montijn et al., 2019; Bartolo et al., 2020; Kafashan et al., 2021; Rumyantsev et al., 2020). To illustrate this, we generate population responses with limited information, and then fit an IP-CM to these responses and study the learned latent representation. In particular, we consider a source population of 200 independent Poisson neurons with homogeneous, von Mises tuning curves responding to a noisy stimulus-orientation , where the noise follows a von Mises distribution centred at the true stimulus-orientation (see Materials and methods). In Figure 6A we show that, as expected, the average FI in the source population about the noisy orientation grows linearly with the size of randomized subpopulations, although the FI about the true orientation is theoretically bounded by the precision (inverse variance) of the sensory noise.
Even though the neurons in the source model are uncorrelated, sensory noise ensures that the encoding model contains information-limiting correlations that bound the FI about (Moreno-Bote et al., 2014; Kanitscheider et al., 2015b). Information-limiting correlations can be small and difficult to capture, and to understand how CMs learn in the presence of information-limiting noise correlations, we fit a von Mises IP-CM with mixture components to responses from the information-limited model . Figure 6A (purple) shows that the FI of the learned CM appears to saturate near the precision of the sensory noise, indicating that the learned CM approximates the information-limiting correlations present in .
To understand how the learned CM approximates these information-limiting correlations, we study the relation between the latent structure of the model and how it generates population activity. For an IP-CM, the orientation-dependent index-probabilities may be expressed as , where is the tuning curve of the th neuron under component . In Figure 6B, we plot the sum of the tuning curves for each component as a function of orientation, and we see that each component concentrates the tuning of the population around a particular orientation. This encourages the probability of each component to also concentrate around a particular orientation, and in Figure 6C we see that, given the true orientation , there are three components with probabilities substantially greater than 0.
Because there are essentially three components that are relevant to the responses of the IP-CM to the true orientation , generating a response from the CM approximately reduces to generating a response from one of the three possible component IP distributions. In Figure 6D–F, we depict a response to from each of the three component IP distributions, as well as the optimal posterior based on the learned IP-CM (purple lines), and a suboptimal posterior based on the source model (i.e. ignoring noise correlations; green lines). We observe that the trial-to-trial variability of the learned IP-CM results in random shifts of the peak neural activity away from the true orientation, thus limiting information. Furthermore, when the response of the population is concentrated at the true orientation (Figure 6E), the suboptimal posterior assigns a high probability to the true orientation, whereas when the responses are biased away from the true orientation (Figure 6D and F) the suboptimal posterior assigns nearly 0 probability to the true orientation. This is in contrast to the optimal posterior, which always assigns a significant probability to the true orientation.
In summary, CMs can effectively approximate information-limiting correlations, and the simple latent structure of CMs could help reveal the presence of information-limiting correlations in data.
Discussion
We introduced a latent variable exponential family formulation of Poisson mixtures. We showed how this formulation allows us to effectively extend Poisson mixtures both to capture sub-Poisson variability, and to incorporate stimulus dependence using conditional mixtures. Our analyses and simulations showed that these conditional mixtures (CMs) can be fit efficiently and recover ground truth models in synthetic data, capture a wide range of V1 response statistics in real data, and can be easily inverted to obtain accurate Bayesian decoding that is competitive with nonlinear decoders, while using orders of magnitude less parameters. In addition, we illustrated how the latent structure of CMs can represent a fundamental feature of the neural code, namely information-limiting correlations.
Our framework is particularly relevant for probabilistic theories of neural coding based on the theory of exponential families (Beck et al., 2007), which include theories that address the linearity of Bayesian inference in neural circuits (Ma et al., 2006), the role of phenomena such as divisive normalization in neural computation (Beck et al., 2011a), Bayesian inference about dynamic stimuli (Makin et al., 2015; Sokoloski, 2017), and the metabolic efficiency of neural coding (Ganguli and Simoncelli, 2014; Yerxa et al., 2020). These theories have proven difficult to validate quantitatively with neural data due to a lack of statistical models which are both compatible with their exponential family formulation (see Equation 4), and can model correlated activity in recordings of large neural populations. Our work suggests that CMs can overcome these difficulties, and help connect this rich mathematical theory of neural coding with the state-of-the-art in parallel recording technologies.
CMs are not limited to modelling neural responses to stimuli and can model how arbitrary experimental variables modulate neural variability and covariability. Examples of experimental variables that have measurable effects on neural covariability include the spatial and temporal context around a stimulus (Snyder et al., 2014; Snow et al., 2016; Snow et al., 2017; Festa et al., 2020), as well as task-variables and the attentional state of the animal (Cohen and Maunsell, 2009; Mitchell et al., 2009; Ruff and Cohen, 2014; Maunsell, 2015; Rabinowitz et al., 2015; Verhoef and Maunsell, 2017; Bondy et al., 2018). Each of these variables could be incorporated into a CM by either replacing the stimulus-variable in our equations with the variable of interest, or combining it with the stimulus-variable to construct a CM with multivariate dependence. This would allow researchers to explore how the stimulus and the experimental variables mutually interact to shape variability and covariability in large populations of neurons.
To understand how this variability and covariability effects neural coding, latent variable models such as CMs are often applied to extract interpretable features of the neural code from data (Whiteway and Butts, 2019). The latent states of a CM provide a soft classification of neural activity, and we may apply CMs to model how an experimental variable modulates the class membership of population activity. In the studies on experimental variables listed above, models of neural activity yielded predictions of perceptual and behavioural performance. Because CMs support Bayesian decoding, a CM can also make predictions about how a particular class of neurons is likely to modulate perception and behaviour, and we may then test these predictions with experimental interventions on the neurons themselves (Panzeri et al., 2017). In this manner, we believe CMs could form a critical part of a rigorous, Bayesian framework for ‘cracking the neural code’ in large populations of neurons.
Outside of the framework of mixture models, there are broader possibilities for designing conditional, latent-variable models which have the minimal, exponential family structure of Equation 3, yet for which the latent variable is not a finite index. We make use of finite mixture models in this paper primarily because mixture models are analytically tractable, even when mixing Poisson distributions. In contrast, models with Gaussian latent variables are analytically tractable when the observations are also Gaussian, but not in general. Nevertheless, if the relevant formulae and computations can be effectively approximated, then many of the advantages of CMs could be preserved even when using continuous latent variables. For example, if the expectation step in our EM algorithm does not have a closed-form expression, it might be possible to approximate it with contrastive divergence (Hinton, 2002).
In our applications, we considered one-dimensional stimuli and implemented the stimulus-dependence of the CM parameters with linearly parameterized functions. Nevertheless, this stimulus dependence can be implemented by arbitrary parametric functions of high-dimensional variables such as deep neural networks, and CMs can also incorporate history-dependence via recurrent connectivity (see Appendix 4). As such, CMs have the potential to integrate encoding models of higher cortical areas (Yamins et al., 2014) with models of the temporal features of the neural code (Pillow et al., 2008; Park et al., 2014; Runyan et al., 2017), towards analyzing the neural code in dynamic, correlated neural populations in higher cortex. Finally, outside of neuroscience, high-dimensional count data exists in many fields such as corpus linguistics and genomics (Inouye et al., 2017), and researchers who aim to understand how this data depends on history or additional variables could benefit from our techniques.
Materials and methods
Notation
Request a detailed protocolWe use capital, bold letters (e.g. ) to indicate matrices; small, bold letters (e.g. ) to indicate vectors; and regular letters (e.g. ) to indicate scalars. We use subscript capital letters to indicate the role of a given variable, so that, in Equation 1 for example, are the natural parameters that bias the index-probabilities, are the baseline natural parameters of the neural firing rates, and is the matrix of parameters through which the indices and rates interact.
We denote the th element of a vector by , or e.g. of the vector by . We denote the th row or th column of by or , respectively, and always state whether we are considering a row or column of the given matrix. When referring to the th element of a vector indexed by , we write . Finally, when indexing data points from a sample, or parameters that are tied to individual data points, we use parenthesized, superscript letters, e.g. , or .
Poisson mixtures and their moments
Request a detailed protocolThe following derivations were presented in a more general form in Karlis and Meligkotsidou, 2007, but we present the simpler case here for completeness. A Poisson distribution has the form , where is the count and λ is the rate (in our case, spike count and firing rate, respectively). We may use a Poisson model to define a distribution over spike counts by supposing that the neurons generate spikes independently of one another, leading to the independent Poisson model with firing rates . Finally, if we consider the rate vectors , and weights , where for all , and , we then define a mixture of Poisson distributions as a latent variable model , where , and .
The mean of the th neuron of a mixture of independent Poisson distributions is
The variance of neuron is
where is the variance of the th neuron under the th component distribution, that is the variance of , and where , and both follow from the fact that a distribution’s variance equals the difference between its second moment and squared first moment.
The covariance between spike-counts ni and nj for is then
Observe that if , then is simply the sample covariance between and , where the sample is composed of the rate components of the th and th neurons. Equation 7 thus implies that Poisson mixtures can model arbitrary covariances. Nevertheless, Equation 6 shows that the variance of individual neurons is restricted to being larger than their means.
Exponential family mixture models
Request a detailed protocolIn this section, we show that the latent variable form for Poisson mixtures we introduced above is a member of the class of models known as exponential families. An exponential family distribution over some data has the form , where are the so-called natural parameters, is a vector-valued function of the data called the sufficient statistic, is a scalar-valued function called the base measure, and is the log-partition function (Wainwright and Jordan, 2008). In the context of Poisson mixture models, we note that an independent Poisson model is an exponential family, with natural parameters given by , base measure and sufficient statistic , and log-partition function . Moreover, the distribution of component indices (also known as a categorical distribution) also has an exponential family form, with natural parameters for , sufficient statistic , base measure , and log-partition function . Note that in both cases, the exponential parameters are well-defined only if the rates and weights are strictly greater than 0 — in practice, however, this is not a significant limitation.
We claim that the joint distribution of a multivariate Poisson mixture model can be reparameterized in the exponential family form
where is the log-partition function of . To show this, we show how to express the natural parameters , and as (invertible) functions of the component rate vectors , and the weights . In particular, we set
where is applied element-wise. Then, for , we set the th row of to
and the th element of to
This reparameterization may then be checked by substituting Equations 9, 10, and 11 into Equation 8 to recover the joint distribution of the mixture model ; for a more explicit derivation see Sokoloski, 2019.
The equation for ensures that the index-probabilities are given by
Consequently, the component distributions in exponential family form are given by
Observe that is a multivariate Poisson distribution with parameters , so that for , the parameters are the sum of and row of . Because the exponential family parameters are the logarithms of the firing rates of , each row of modulates the firing rates of multiplicatively. When depends on a stimulus and we consider the component distributions , each row of then scales the tuning curves of the baseline population (i.e. for ); in the neuroscience literature, such scaling factors are typically referred to as gain modulations.
The exponential family form has many advantages. However, it has a less intuitive relationship with the statistics of the model such as the mean and covariance. The most straightforward method to compute these statistics given a model in exponential family form is to first reparameterize it in terms of the weights and component rates, and then evaluate Equations 5, 6, and 7.
CoM-Poisson distributions and their mixtures
Request a detailed protocolConway-Maxwell (CoM) Poisson distributions decouple the location and shape of count distributions (Shmueli et al., 2005; Stevenson, 2016; Chanialidis et al., 2018). A CoM Poisson model has the form . The floor function of the location parameter λ is the mode of the given distribution. With regards to the shape parameter ν, is a Poisson distribution with rate λ when , and is under- or over-dispersed when or , respectively. A CoM-Poisson model is also an exponential family, with natural parameters , sufficient statistic , and base measure . The log-partition function does not have a closed-form expression, but it can be effectively approximated by truncating the series (Shmueli et al., 2005). More generally, when we consider a product of independent CoM-Poisson distributions, we denote its log-partition function by , where are the parameters of the th CoM-Poisson distribution. In this case we can also approximate the log-partition function by truncating the constituent series in parallel.
We define a multivariate CoM-based (CB) mixture as
where is the vector of log-factorials of the individual spike-counts, and is the log-partition function. This form ensures that the index-probabilities satisfy
and consequently that each component distribution is a product of independent CoM Poisson distributions given by
Observe that, whereas the parameters of depend on the index , the parameters of are independent of the index and act exclusively as biases. Therefore, realizing different indices has the effect increasing or decreasing the location parameters, and thus the modes of the corresponding CoM-Poisson distributions. As such, although the different components of a CB mixture are not simply rescaled versions of the first component , in practice they behave approximately in this manner.
The moments of a CoM-Poisson distribution are not available in closed-form, yet they can also be effectively approximated through truncation. We begin by computing approximate means and variances of through truncation, and then the mean of ni is , and its variance is
where . Similarly to Equation 7, the covariance between ni and nj is .
By comparing Equations 6 and 17, we see that the CB mixture may address the limitations on the variances of the IP mixture by setting the average variance of the components in Equation 17 to be small, while holding the value of the means fixed, and ensuring that the means of the components cover a wide range of values to achieve the desired values of and . Solving the parameters of a CB mixture for a desired covariance matrix is unfortunately not possible since we lack closed-form expressions for the means and variances. Nevertheless, we may justify the effectiveness of the CB strategy by considering the approximations of the components means and variances and , which hold when neither or are too small (Chanialidis et al., 2018). Based on these approximations, observe that when is large, is small, whereas is more or less unaffected. Therefore, in the regime where these approximations hold, a small value for can be achieved by reducing the parameters , without significantly restricting the values of or .
Fisher information of a minimal CM
Request a detailed protocolThe Fisher information (FI) of an encoding model with respect to is (Cover and Thomas, 2006). With regard to the FI of a minimal CM,
where follows from the chain rule and properties of the log-partition function (Wainwright and Jordan, 2008). Therefore
where is the covariance matrix of . Moreover, because (Wainwright and Jordan, 2008), the FI of a minimal CM may also be expressed as , which is the linear Fisher information (Beck et al., 2011b).
Note that when calculating the FI or other quantities based on the covariance matrix, IP-CMs have the advantage that their covariance matrices tend to have large diagonal elements and are thus inherently well-conditioned. Because decoding performance is not significantly different between IP- and CB-CMs (see Table 3), IP-CMs may be preferable when well-conditioned covariance matrices are critical. Nevertheless, the covariance matrices of CB mixtures can be made well-conditioned by applying standard techniques.
Expectation-maximization for CMs
Request a detailed protocolExpectation-maximization (EM) is an algorithm that maximizes the likelihood of a latent variable model given data by iterating two steps: generating model-based expectations of the latent variables, and maximizing the complete log-likelihood of the model given the data and latent expectations. Although the maximization step optimizes the complete log-likelihood, each iteration of EM is guaranteed to not decrease the data log-likelihood as well (Neal and Hinton, 1998).
EM is arguably the most widely applied algorithm for fitting finite mixture models (McLachlan et al., 2019). As a form of latent variable exponential family, the expectation step for a finite mixture model reduces to computing average sufficient statistics, and the maximization step is a convex optimization problem (Wainwright and Jordan, 2008). In general, the average sufficient statistics, or mean parameters, correspond to (are dual to) the natural parameters of an exponential family, and where we denote natural parameters with θ, we denote their corresponding mean parameters with η.
Suppose we are given a dataset of neural spike-counts, and a CB mixture with natural parameters , , , and (see Equation 14). The expectation step for this model reduces to computing the data-dependent mean parameters given by
for all . The mean parameters are the averages of the sufficient statistic under the distribution , and are what we use to complete the log-likelihood since we do not observe .
Given , the maximization step of a CB mixture thus reduces to maximizing the complete log-likelihood , where we substitute into the place of in Equation 14, such that
This objective may be maximized in closed-form for an IP mixture (Karlis and Meligkotsidou, 2007), but this is not the case when the model has CoM-Poisson shape parameters or depends on the stimulus. Nevertheless, solving the resulting maximization step is still a convex optimization problem (Wainwright and Jordan, 2008), and may be approximately solved with gradient ascent. Doing so requires that we first compute the mean parameters , , , and that are dual to , , , and , respectively.
We compute the mean parameters by evaluating
where is the th element of , is the th element of , is the th element of , and is the th element of the th column of . Note as well that we truncate the series and to approximate and . Given these mean parameters, we may then express the gradients of as
where ⊗ is the outer product operator, and where the second term in each equation follows from the fact that the derivative of with respect to , , , or yields the dual parameters , , , and , respectively. By ascending the gradients of until convergence, we approximate a single iteration of the EM algorithm for a CB mixture.
Finally, if our dataset includes stimuli , and the parameters depend on the stimulus, then the gradients of the parameters of must also be computed. For a von Mises CM where , the gradients are given by
where is the output of at . Although in this paper we restrict our applications to Von Mises or discrete tuning curves for one-dimensional stimuli, this formalism can be readily extended to the case where the baseline parameters are a generic nonlinear function of the stimulus, represented by a deep neural network. Then, the gradients of the parameters of can be computed through backpropagation, and is the error that must be backpropagated through the network to compute the gradients.
If we ignore stimulus dependence, the single most computationally intensive operation in each gradient ascent step is the computation of the outer product when evaluating , which has a time complexity of . As such, the training algorithm scales linearly in the number of neurons, and CMs could realistically be applied to populations of tens to hundreds of thousands of neurons. That being said, larger values of will typically be required to maximize performance in larger populations, and fitting the model to larger populations typically requires larger datasets and more EM iterations.
CM initialization and training procedures
Request a detailed protocolTo fit a CM to a dataset , we first initialize the CM and then optimize its parameters with our previously described EM algorithm. Naturally, initialization depends on exactly which form of CM we consider, but in general we first initialize the baseline parameters , then add the categorical parameters and mixture component parameters . When training CB-CMs we always first train an IP-CM, and so the initialization procedure remains the same for IP and CB models.
To initialize a von Mises CM with neurons, we first fit independent, von Mises-tuned neurons by maximizing the log-likelihood of . This is a convex optimization problem and so can be easily solved by gradient ascent, in particular by following the gradients
to convergence. For both discrete and maximal CMs, where there are distinct stimuli, we initialize by computing the average rate vector at each stimulus-condition and creating a lookup table for these rate vectors. Formally, where xl is the th stimulus value for , we may express the th rate vector as , where is one when , and 0 otherwise. We then construct a lookup table for these rate vectors in exponential family form by setting , and by setting the th row of to .
In general, we initialize the parameters by sampling the weights of a categorical distribution from a Dirichlet distribution with a constant concentration of 2, and converting the weights into the natural parameters of a categorical distribution . For discrete and maximal CMs, we initialize the modulations by generating each element of from a uniform distribution over the range . For von Mises CMs we initialize each row of as shifted sinusoidal functions of the preferred stimuli of the independent von Mises neurons. That is, given and , we compute the preferred stimulus of the th neuron given by , where is the th row of . We then set the th element of to . Initializing von Mises CMs in this way ensures that each modulation has a unique peak as a function of preferred stimuli, which helps differentiate the modulations from each other, and in our experience improves training speed.
With regard to training, the expectation step in our EM algorithm may be computed directly, and so the only challenge is solving the maximization step. Although the optimal solution strategy depends on the details of the model and data in question, in the context of this paper we settled on a strategy that is sufficient for all simulations we perform. For each model we perform a total of EM iterations, and for each maximization step we take gradient ascent steps with the Adam gradient ascent algorithm (Kingma and Ba, 2014) with the default momentum parameters (see Kingma and Ba, 2014). We restart the Adam algorithm at each iteration of EM and gradually reduce the learning rate. Where and are the initial and final learning rates, we set the learning rate at EM iteration to
where we assume starts at 0 and ends at .
Because we must evaluate large numbers of truncated series when working with CB-CMs, training times are typically one to two orders of magnitude greater. To minimize training time of CB-CMs over the EM iterations, we therefore first train a IP-CM for iterations. We then equate the parameters , , and of the IP-CM (see Equation 8) with a CB-CM (see Equation 14) and set , which ensures that resulting CB model has the same density function as the original IP model. We then train the CB-CM for iterations. We found this strategy results in practically no performance loss, while greatly reducing training time.
Strategies for choosing the CM form and latent structure
Request a detailed protocolThere are a few choices with regards to the form of the model than one must make when applying a CM: The form of the dependence, whether or not to use the CoM-based (CB) extension, and the number of components . The form of the dependence is very open-ended, yet should be fairly clear from the problem context: one should use a minimal model if one wishes to make use of its mathematical features, and otherwise a maximal model may provide better performance. If one wishes to interpolate between stimulus conditions, or the number of stimulus-conditions in the data is high, then a continuous stimulus-dependence model (e.g. von Mises tuning curves) should be used, otherwise discrete tuning curves may provide better performance. Finally, if one wishes to model correlations in a complex neural circuit, one may use for example a deep neural network, and induce correlations in the output layer with the theory of CMs.
Similarly, CB-CMs have clear advantages for modelling individual variability, and as we show in Appendix 2, this includes higher-order variability. Nevertheless, from the perspective of decoding performance, IP-CMs and CB-CMs perform more-or-less equally well, and training CB-CMs is more computationally intensive. As such, IP-CMs may often be the better choice.
The number of components provides a fine-grained method of adjusting model performance. If the goal is to maximize predictive encoding performance, then the standard way to do this is to choose a that maximizes the cross-validated log-likelihood, as we demonstrated in Figure 5. Nevertheless, one may rather aim to maximize decoding performance, in which case maximizing the cross-validated log-posterior may be a more appropriate objective. In both cases, for very large populations of neurons, choosing a solely to maximize performance may be prohibitively, computationally expensive. As demonstrated in Figure 5 and Appendix 3, a small can achieve a large fraction of the performance gain of the optimal , and choosing a modest that achieves qualitatively acceptable performance may prove to be the most productive strategy.
CM parameter selection for simulations
Request a detailed protocolIn the section Extended Poisson mixture models capture stimulus-dependent response statistics and the section Conditional mixtures facilitate accurate and efficient decoding of neural responses we considered minimal CB-CMs with randomized parameters , , , and , which for simplicity we refer to as models 1 and 2, respectively. We construct randomized CMs piece by piece, in a similar fashion to our initialization procedure.
Firstly, where is the number of neurons, we tile their preferred stimuli over the circle such that . We then generate the concentration and gain of the th neuron by sampling from normal distributions in log-space, such that , and . Finally, for von Mises baseline parameters , we set each row of to , and each element of to , where is the logarithm of the modified Bessel function of order 0, which is the log-partition function of the von Mises distribution.
We then set , and generated each element of the modulation matrix in the same matter as the gains, such that . Finally, to generate random CB parameters we generate each element of from a uniform distribution, such that .
Model two entails two more steps. Firstly, when sampling from larger populations of neurons, single modulations often dominate the model activity around certain stimulus values. To suppress this we consider the natural parameters of (see Equation 15), and compute the maximum value of these natural parameters over the range of stimuli . We then set each element of the parameters of the CM to , where , which helps ensure that multiple modulations are active at any given . Finally, since model two is a discrete CM, we replace the von Mises baseline parameters with discrete baseline parameters, by evaluating at each of the valid stimulus-conditions, and assemble the resulting collection of natural parameters into a lookup table in the manner we described in our initialization procedures.
Decoding models
Request a detailed protocolWhen constructing a Bayesian decoder for discrete stimuli, we first estimate the prior by computing the relative frequency of stimulus presentations in the training data. For the given encoding model, we then evaluate at each stimulus condition, and then compute the posterior by brute-force normalization of . When training the encoding model used for our Bayesian encoders, we only trained them to maximize encoding performance as previously described, and not to maximize decoding performance.
We considered two decoding models, namely the linear network and the artificial neural network (ANN) with sigmoid activation functions. In both cases, the input of the network was a neural response vector, and the output the natural parameters of a categorical distribution. The form of the linear network was , and is otherwise fully determined by the structure of the data. For the ANN on the other hand, we had to choose both the number of hidden layers, and the number of neurons per hidden layer. We cross-validated the performance of both 1 and 2 hidden layer models, over a range of sizes from 100 to 2000 neurons. We found the performance of the networks with two hidden layers generally exceeded that of those with one hidden layer, and that 700 and 600 hidden neurons was optimal for the awake and anaesthetized networks, respectively.
Given a dataset , we optimized the linear network and the ANN by maximizing via stochastic gradient ascent. We again used the Adam optimizer with default momentum parameters, and used a fixed learning rate of 0.0003, and randomly divided the dataset into minibatches of 500 data points. We also used early stopping, where for each fold of our 10-fold cross-validation simulation, we partitioned the dataset into 80% training data, 10% test data, and 10% validation data, and stopped the simulation when performance on the test data declined from epoch to epoch.
Experimental design
Request a detailed protocolThroughout this paper, we demonstrate our methods on two sets of parallel response recordings in macaque primary visual cortex (V1). The stimuli were drifting full contrast gratings at nine distinct orientations spread evenly over the half-circle from 0° to 180° (2° diameter, two cycles per degree, 2.5 Hz drift rate). Stimuli were generated with custom software (EXPO by P. Lennie) and displayed on a cathode ray tube monitor (Hewlett Packard p1230; 1024 × 768 pixels, with ~ 40 cd/m2 mean luminance and 100 Hz frame rate) viewed at a distance of 110 cm (for anaesthetized dataset) or 60 cm (for awake dataset). Grating orientations were randomly interleaved, each presented for 70 ms (for anaesthetized dataset) or 150 ms (for awake dataset), separated by a uniform gray screen (blank stimulus) for the same duration. Stimuli were centered in the aggregate spatial receptive field of the recorded units.
Neural activity from the superficial layers of V1 was recorded from a 96 channel microelectrode array (400 μm spacing, 1 mm length). (400 μm spacing, 1 mm length). Standard methods for waveform extraction and pre-processing were applied (see Aschner et al., 2018). We computed spike counts in a fixed window with length equal to the stimulus duration, shifted by 50 ms after stimulus onset to account for onset latency. We excluded from further analyses all neurons that were not driven by any stimulus above baseline + 3std.
In the first dataset, the monkey was awake and performed a fixation task. Methods and protocols are as described in Festa et al., 2020. There were trials of the responses of neurons in the dataset. We refer to this dataset as the awake V1 dataset.
In the second dataset, the monkey was anaesthetized and there were trials of the responses of neurons; we refer to this dataset as the anaesthetized V1 dataset. The protocol and general methods employed for the anaesthetized experiment have been described previously (Smith and Kohn, 2008).
All procedures were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of the Albert Einstein College of Medicine, and were in compliance with the guidelines set forth in the National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals under protocols 20180308 and 20180309 for the awake and anaesthetized macaque recordings, respectively.
Code
Request a detailed protocolAll code used to run the simulations and generate the figures, as well as the awake and anaesthetized datasets, are available at the Git repository https://gitlab.com/sacha-sokoloski/neural-mixtures (Sokoloski, 2021; copy archived at swh:1:rev:8e82799f8934c47961ea02c5b7c25bd952abb961). Instructions are provided for installation, and scripts are provided that may be run on alternative datasets with a similar structure to what we have considered in this manuscript without modifying the code.
Appendix 1
Comparing conditional mixtures with factor analysis
In Conditional mixtures effectively model neural responses in macaque V1, we assess encoding performance with the cross-validated, average log-likelihood of the given conditional mixture (CM) on the given dataset. However, in some cases, one might only be concerned with how well a model captures particular statistics of a dataset. In particular, response models based on Gaussian distributions treat spike-counts as continuous values, and assign positive probability to both negative and non-integer values. Although their log-likelihood performance consequently tends to suffer relative to spike-count models, they can still prove highly effective at capturing the mean and covariance of data.
Here, we compare CMs with factor analysis (FA), which is widely applied to modelling neural responses (Santhanam et al., 2009; Cowley et al., 2016; Semedo et al., 2019). FAs model data as Gaussian distributed, and have a latent structure that facilitates both interpretability and predictive performance. The easiest way to design an encoding model based on FA is with a simple lookup-table, so that we fit an independent FA model at each stimulus condition. This is also how we define maximal CMs, and so to keep our comparison straightforward we compare FA encoding models with maximal CMs. In particular, we compare FA to both independent Poisson (IP) and CoM-Based (CB) maximal CMs on how well they capture response statistics on the two datasets from the article (anaesthetized and awake macaque V1). In general, we trained the CMs with expectation-maximization (EM) as described in Materials and methods, and the FA model with standard EM.
In Appendix 1—figure 1A and B, we depict scatter plots that compare the data noise correlations from our awake and anaesthetized datasets at the stimulus orientation , to the noise correlations learned by CB and IP mixtures, and FA trained on the complete datasets. Each model was defined with latent states/dimensions. We also state the coefficient of determination for each model, and see that although all models perform comparably on the anaesthetized data, FA has a clear advantage over the mixture models on the awake data. To see if this performance advantage holds on held-out data, in Appendix 1—figure 1C and D we depict the results of 10-fold cross-validation of the coefficient of determination between the data noise correlations and the various model noise correlations over all nine stimulus orientations, as a function of the number of latant states/dimensions . We see that the predictive performance advantage of FA on the awake data is small, and that CB-CMs exceed the performance of FA on anaesthetized data. At the same time, FA achieves peak performance on both datasets with a smaller number of parameters. Nevertheless, FA is designed precisely to capture second-order correlations, and that our mixture models achieve comparable performance speaks favourably to the overall strengths of the mixture model approach.
In Appendix 1—figure 2, we depict scatter plots between the data Fano factors (FFs) and learned FFs of our models at stimulus orientation , and find that both the CB mixture and FA almost perfectly capture the data FFs. In Appendix 1—figure 2C–D, we see that the CB mixture and FA also achieve good cross-validated scores on FFs. Unsurprisingly, however, the IP mixture struggles to effectively capture FFs.
Appendix 2
Higher order moments and neural decoding
FA-based encoding models are highly effective at capturing the first- and second-order statistics of neural responses, yet in our simulations we found that Bayesian decoders based on FA encoding models perform poorly when compared to the other decoding models considered in Conditional mixtures facilitate accurate and efficient decoding of neural responses. There we evaluate decoding performance by fitting a candidate model to training data, and computing the mean and standard error of the log-posterior at the true stimulus on held-out data. On the awake data FA scores , which is comparable to an optimal linear decoder, yet still significantly worse than a nonlinear decoder, or a Bayesian decoder based on a CM. On the anaesthetized data FA scored , as it would occasionally assign numerically 0 posterior probability to the true stimulus; when we filtered out values from the average, the FA encoder still only achieved performance of .
Normal distributions — and the FA observable distribution by extension — are essentially defined by their first- and second-order statistics, which suggests that there are higher-order statistics that are important for decoding that FA cannot capture. The third and fourth order statistics known as the skewness and excess kurtosis measure the asymmetry and heavy-tailedness of a given distribution, respectively. Normal distributions have a skewness and excess kurtosis of 0. Here, we study how well this normality assumption is reflected in our neural recordings, and how well our mixture models capture these higher order statistics.
In Figure 1, we present scatter plots of the empirical skewness and kurtosis of all the neurons in our datasets at orientation , and the model skewness and kurtosis learned by our mixture models. Exactly quantifying the non-normality of higher order moments in multivariate distributions is a complicated and evolving subject (Mardia and El-atoum, 1976; Cain et al., 2017), nevertheless in Figure 1 the empirical skewness and kurtosis of the recorded neurons appear to qualitatively deviate from zero. On the awake data, both the CB and IP mixture achieve high when compared with the data skewness (Figure 1A) and kurtosis (Figure 1B), although the CB mixtures achieves notably better performance. On the anaesthetized data (Figure 1C and D), the CB mixture continues to achieve a high , but the IP mixture performs extremely poorly; although the disparity in is not immediately apparent in the scatter plots, this is because some of the model skewness of the IP mixtures are outside the plot bounds.
When fit to the complete datasets, and averaged over all stimulus conditions, the CB mixtures achieved a skewness average and standard error of and , and a kurtosis average and standard error of and on the awake and anaesthetized data, respectively; in contrast, the presence of outliers caused the average scores for the IP mixture to be dramatically negative in all cases. These results suggest that the CoM-based parameters of the CB mixture provide important degrees of freedom for capturing individual variability. That being said, when we cross-validated the performance on the higher-order moments, the results were not significantly higher than 0 for the CB mixture, and as such, accurately estimating higher order moments requires larger datasets than what we have considered here.
Nevertheless, in spite of the inability to capture predictive performance on these moments, we speculate that when combined across higher order moments and cross-moments, the ability of mixture models to capture higher order structure in the data is necessary for maximizing decoding performance, and that these moments might play an important role in neural coding. As the complexity of neural datasets increases, a careful study of such higher-order statistics would become both feasible and warranted, and our mixture model approach could prove to be a useful tool in such work.
Appendix 3
Sample complexity of conditional mixtures
To develop a sense of the sample complexity of CMs, we repeated the cross-validation simulations with discrete CMs on subsets of our two datasets (see Conditional mixtures effectively model neural responses in macaque V1 and Conditional mixtures facilitate accurate and efficient decoding of neural responses). In particular, we ran a 10-fold cross-validation simulation on a single subsample of 25%, 50%, 75%, and 100%, of each of our datasets. On our anaesthetized dataset this occasionally resulted in some neurons recording 0 spikes in a giving training set, which tends to cause our training algorithm to diverge, and so we filtered out neurons with low firing rates, leaving 50 neurons in our anaesthetized dataset.
We present the results of our simulations in Figure 1. In the left column (Figure 1A–D), we present the cross-validated log-likelihood of the vanilla and CoM-models, on the awake and anaesthetized data, respectively, and we see that, as we would expect, models with fewer components maximize their performance on smaller datasets. Even with large amounts of data, however, the performance difference between models with more than 10 components is nearly statistically indistinguishable. In the right column (Figure 1E–H), we present the cross-validated log-posterior performance of the models, and the results mirror those of the log-likelihood simulations, except the benefits of larger models becomes even more marginal.
Appendix 4
Conditional mixtures and generalized linear models
CMs are closely related to generalized linear models (GLMs), which are widely applied in neuroscience. The application of GLMs to modelling stimulus-driven spike-trains was pioneered in Pillow et al., 2008, in which the authors develop a Poisson encoding model , where is the number of recorded neurons, nt is the spike-count of the modelled neuron in timebin , is the stimulus (here the stimulus is an image and represented as a vector), and where each is the spike-count history of the th recorded neuron up to time . The log-rate of the modelled neuron at time depends linearly on the stimulus and the spike-history, and is given by
where and are vectors; in Pillow et al., 2008 both and are represented by basis functions with a manageable number of fittable parameters.
This model may be trivially combined with a CM in order to extend the GLM formulation with a latent source of shared-variability that affords analytic expressions for various quantities of interest. The definition of a CB-CM is , and we may simply replace the variable with all the independent variables in the GLM formulation, namely and , and define the baseline log-firing rates as copies of the function defined by Equation 18, each with its own independent parameters. In principle, the expectation-maximization framework we have presented for training CMs can be directly applied to a model with this structure. That being said, choosing the right parameterization of and would pose a unique challenge in a combined GLM-CM model, and whether such a model would be practically useful is an empirical question that is beyond the scope of this paper.
Here, we also clarify that although a CM is closely related to both mixture models and GLMs, it should not be confused with the models known as ‘mixtures of GLMs’ (Grün and Leisch, 2008). A mixture of GLMs has the form , where is the number of GLMs to be mixed, wk are the weight parameters, and is a GLM with parameters . This model differs from a CM in many subtle ways, and the easiest to note is that the weights wk do not depend on the stimulus as they do in a CM, which, as shown in Figures 4 and 6 of the main paper, is critical to how CMs represent data.
Data availability
All data used in this study is available at the Git repository (https://gitlab.com/sacha-sokoloski/neural-mixtures; copy archived at https://archive.softwareheritage.org/swh:1:rev:8e82799f8934c47961ea02c5b7c25bd952abb961). This includes experimental data used for model validation, and code for running analyses and simulations.
References
-
BookLow-Dimensional Models of Neural Population Activity in Sensory Cortical CircuitsIn: Ghahramani Z, Welling M, Cortes C, Lawrence N. D, Weinberger K. Q, editors. Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems. Curran Associates, Inc. pp. 343–351.
-
Temporal contingencies determine whether adaptation strengthens or weakens normalizationThe Journal of Neuroscience 38:10129–10142.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1131-18.2018
-
Information-Limiting correlations in large neural populationsThe Journal of Neuroscience 40:1668–1678.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2072-19.2019
-
BookComputational Neuroscience: Theoretical Insights into Brain FunctionIn: Cisek P, Drew T, Kalaska J. F, editors. Progress in Brain Research. Elsevier. pp. 509–519.
-
Marginalization in neural circuits with divisive normalizationJournal of Neuroscience 31:15310–15319.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1706-11.2011
-
Mutual information, Fisher information, and population codingNeural Computation 10:1731–1757.https://doi.org/10.1162/089976698300017115
-
Efficient bayesian inference for COM-Poisson regression modelsStatistics and Computing 28:595–608.https://doi.org/10.1007/s11222-017-9750-x
-
Attention improves performance primarily by reducing interneuronal correlationsNature Neuroscience 12:1594–1600.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2439
-
Stimulus-Driven population activity patterns in macaque primary visual cortexPLOS Computational Biology 12:e1005185.https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005185
-
Dimensionality reduction for large-scale neural recordingsNature Neuroscience 17:1500–1509.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3776
-
BookTheoretical Neuroscience: Computational and Mathematical Modeling of Neural SystemsMassachusetts Institute of Technology Press.
-
The effect of noise correlations in populations of diversely tuned neuronsJournal of Neuroscience 31:14272–14283.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2539-11.2011
-
On the structure of neuronal population activity under fluctuations in attentional stateThe Journal of Neuroscience 36:1775–1789.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2044-15.2016
-
Efficient sensory encoding and bayesian inference with heterogeneous neural populationsNeural Computation 26:2103–2134.https://doi.org/10.1162/NECO_a_00638
-
Decoding the activity of neuronal populations in macaque primary visual cortexNature Neuroscience 14:239–245.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2733
-
Stimulus-dependent maximum entropy models of neural population codesPLOS Computational Biology 9:e1002922.https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002922
-
BookFinite Mixtures of Generalized Linear Regression ModelsIn: Shalabh HC, editors. Recent Advances in Linear Models and Related Areas: Essays in Honour of Helge Toutenburg Heidelberg. Berlin, Germany: Springer. pp. 205–230.https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-7908-2064-5
-
Training products of experts by minimizing contrastive divergenceNeural Computation 14:1771–1800.https://doi.org/10.1162/089976602760128018
-
A review of multivariate distributions for count data derived from the poisson distributionWIREs Computational Statistics 9:e1398.https://doi.org/10.1002/wics.1398
-
Measuring Fisher information accurately in correlated neural populationsPLOS Computational Biology 11:e1004218.https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004218
-
Finite mixtures of multivariate poisson distributions with applicationJournal of Statistical Planning and Inference 137:1942–1960.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jspi.2006.07.001
-
Correlations and neuronal population informationAnnual Review of Neuroscience 39:237–256.https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-070815-013851
-
Cognitive computational neuroscienceNature Neuroscience 21:1148–1160.https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-018-0210-5
-
Bayesian inference with probabilistic population codesNature Neuroscience 9:1432–1438.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1790
-
BookEmpirical Models of Spiking in Neural PopulationsIn: Shawe-Taylor J, Zemel R. S, Bartlett P. L, Pereira F, Weinberger K. Q, editors. Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems. Curran Associates, Inc. pp. 1350–1358.
-
BookHow Biased Are Maximum Entropy Models?In: Shawe-Taylor J, Zemel R. S, Bartlett P. L, Pereira F, Weinberger K. Q, editors. Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems. Curran Associates, Inc. pp. 2034–2042.
-
Learning to estimate dynamical state with probabilistic population codesPLOS Computational Biology 11:e1004554.https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004554
-
Neuronal mechanisms of visual attentionAnnual Review of Vision Science 1:373–391.https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-vision-082114-035431
-
Finite mixture modelsAnnual Review of Statistics and Its Application 6:355–378.https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-statistics-031017-100325
-
Encoding and decoding in parietal cortex during sensorimotor decision-makingNature Neuroscience 17:1395–1403.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3800
-
Probabilistic brains: knowns and unknownsNature Neuroscience 16:1170–1178.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3495
-
Ignoring correlated activity causes a failure of retinal population codesNature Communications 11:4605.https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-18436-2
-
Attention can either increase or decrease spike count correlations in visual cortexNature Neuroscience 17:1591–1597.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3835
-
Factor-analysis methods for higher-performance neural prosthesesJournal of Neurophysiology 102:1315–1330.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00097.2009
-
Towards the design principles of neural population codesCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology 37:133–140.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2016.03.001
-
A useful distribution for fitting discrete data: revival of the Conway–Maxwell–Poisson distributionJournal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series C 54:127–142.https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9876.2005.00474.x
-
Spatial and temporal scales of neuronal correlation in primary visual cortexJournal of Neuroscience 28:12591–12603.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2929-08.2008
-
Correlations in V1 are reduced by stimulation outside the receptive fieldJournal of Neuroscience 34:11222–11227.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0762-14.2014
-
Implementing a Bayes filter in a neural circuit: the case of unknown stimulus dynamicsNeural Computation 29:2450–2490.https://doi.org/10.1162/neco_a_00991
-
BookImplementing Bayesian Inference with Neural NetworksDissertation: University of Leipzig.
-
Population coding in neuronal systems with correlated noisePhysical Review E 64:051904.https://doi.org/10.1103/PhysRevE.64.051904
-
Flexible models for spike count data with both over- and under- dispersionJournal of Computational Neuroscience 41:29–43.https://doi.org/10.1007/s10827-016-0603-y
-
Modeling bimodal discrete data using Conway-Maxwell-Poisson mixture modelsJournal of Business & Economic Statistics 33:352–365.https://doi.org/10.1080/07350015.2014.949343
-
Testing the odds of inherent vs. observed overdispersion in neural spike countsJournal of Neurophysiology 115:434–444.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00194.2015
-
The simplest maximum entropy model for collective behavior in a neural networkJournal of Statistical Mechanics: Theory and Experiment 2013:P03011.https://doi.org/10.1088/1742-5468/2013/03/P03011
-
Modeling the impact of common noise inputs on the network activity of retinal ganglion cellsJournal of Computational Neuroscience 33:97–121.https://doi.org/10.1007/s10827-011-0376-2
-
Graphical models, exponential families, and variational inferenceFoundations and Trends 1:1–305.https://doi.org/10.1561/2200000001
-
A neural basis of probabilistic computation in visual cortexNature Neuroscience 23:122–129.https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-019-0554-5
-
A bayesian observer model constrained by efficient coding can explain 'anti-Bayesian' perceptsNature Neuroscience 18:1509–1517.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4105
-
The quest for interpretable models of neural population activityCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology 58:86–93.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2019.07.004
-
Decoding spike trains instant by instant using order statistics and the mixture-of-Poissons modelThe Journal of Neuroscience 23:2394–2406.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-06-02394.2003
-
Efficient sensory coding of multidimensional stimuliPLOS Computational Biology 16:e1008146.https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1008146
-
Gaussian-Process factor analysis for Low-Dimensional Single-Trial analysis of neural population activityJournal of Neurophysiology 102:614–635.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.90941.2008
-
Probabilistic interpretation of population codesNeural Computation 10:403–430.https://doi.org/10.1162/089976698300017818
Article and author information
Author details
Funding
National Institutes of Health (EY030578)
- Ruben Coen-Cagli
National Institutes of Health (EY02826)
- Sacha Sokoloski
- Amir Aschner
National Institutes of Health (EY016774)
- Amir Aschner
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Acknowledgements
We thank all the members of the labs of Ruben Coen-Cagli and Adam Kohn for their regular feedback and support.
Ethics
Animal experimentation: All procedures were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of the Albert Einstein College of Medicine, and were in compliance with the guidelines set forth in the National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals under protocols 20180308 and 20180309 for the awake and anaesthetized macaque recordings, respectively.
Copyright
© 2021, Sokoloski et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 2,040
- views
-
- 317
- downloads
-
- 9
- citations
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Neuroscience
Specialized chemosensory signals elicit innate social behaviors in individuals of several vertebrate species, a process that is mediated via the accessory olfactory system (AOS). The AOS comprising the peripheral sensory vomeronasal organ has evolved elaborate molecular and cellular mechanisms to detect chemo signals. To gain insight into the cell types, developmental gene expression patterns, and functional differences amongst neurons, we performed single-cell transcriptomics of the mouse vomeronasal sensory epithelium. Our analysis reveals diverse cell types with gene expression patterns specific to each, which we made available as a searchable web resource accessed from https://www.scvnoexplorer.com. Pseudo-time developmental analysis indicates that neurons originating from common progenitors diverge in their gene expression during maturation with transient and persistent transcription factor expression at critical branch points. Comparative analysis across two of the major neuronal subtypes that express divergent GPCR families and the G-protein subunits Gnai2 or Gnao1, reveals significantly higher expression of endoplasmic reticulum (ER) associated genes within Gnao1 neurons. In addition, differences in ER content and prevalence of cubic membrane ER ultrastructure revealed by electron microscopy, indicate fundamental differences in ER function.
-
- Medicine
- Neuroscience
Pain after surgery causes significant suffering. Opioid analgesics cause severe side effects and accidental death. Therefore, there is an urgent need to develop non-opioid therapies for managing post-surgical pain. Local application of Clarix Flo (FLO), a human amniotic membrane (AM) product, attenuated established post-surgical pain hypersensitivity without exhibiting known side effects of opioid use in mice. This effect was achieved through direct inhibition of nociceptive dorsal root ganglion (DRG) neurons via CD44-dependent pathways. We further purified the major matrix component, the heavy chain-hyaluronic acid/pentraxin 3 (HC-HA/PTX3) from human AM that has greater purity and water solubility than FLO. HC-HA/PTX3 replicated FLO-induced neuronal and pain inhibition. Mechanistically, HC-HA/PTX3-induced cytoskeleton rearrangements to inhibit sodium current and high-voltage activated calcium current on nociceptive DRG neurons, suggesting it is a key bioactive component mediating pain relief. Collectively, our findings highlight the potential of naturally derived biologics from human birth tissues as an effective non-opioid treatment for post-surgical pain. Moreover, we unravel the underlying neuronal mechanisms of pain inhibition induced by FLO and HC-HA/PTX3.