A stochastic model of hippocampal synaptic plasticity with geometrical readout of enzyme dynamics
Abstract
Discovering the rules of synaptic plasticity is an important step for understanding brain learning. Existing plasticity models are either (1) top-down and interpretable, but not flexible enough to account for experimental data, or (2) bottom-up and biologically realistic, but too intricate to interpret and hard to fit to data. To avoid the shortcomings of these approaches, we present a new plasticity rule based on a geometrical readout mechanism that flexibly maps synaptic enzyme dynamics to predict plasticity outcomes. We apply this readout to a multi-timescale model of hippocampal synaptic plasticity induction that includes electrical dynamics, calcium, CaMKII and calcineurin, and accurate representation of intrinsic noise sources. Using a single set of model parameters, we demonstrate the robustness of this plasticity rule by reproducing nine published ex vivo experiments covering various spike-timing and frequency-dependent plasticity induction protocols, animal ages, and experimental conditions. Our model also predicts that in vivo-like spike timing irregularity strongly shapes plasticity outcome. This geometrical readout modelling approach can be readily applied to other excitatory or inhibitory synapses to discover their synaptic plasticity rules.
Editor's evaluation
Synaptic plasticity is a ubiquitous but also highly complex phenomenon and developing a unifying description has been challenging. This study presents a realistic biophysical model of plasticity induction, with a novel read-out of CaMKII and Calcineurin. It is able to describe a wide range of experimental results and sets a new benchmark for realistic computational models.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.80152.sa0Introduction
To understand how brains learn, we need to identify the rules governing how synapses change their strength in neural circuits. The dominant principle at the basis of current models of synaptic plasticity is the Hebb postulate (Hebb, 1949) which states that neurons with correlated electrical activity strengthen their synaptic connections, while neurons active at different times weaken their connections. In particular, spike-timing-dependent plasticity (STDP) models (Blum and Abbott, 1996; Gerstner et al., 1996; Eurich et al., 1999) were formulated based on experimental observations that precise timing of pre- and post-synaptic spiking determines whether synapses are strengthened or weakened (Debanne et al., 1996; Tsodyks and Markram, 1997; Bi and Poo, 1998; Markram et al., 2011). However, experiments also found that plasticity induction depends on the rate and number of stimuli delivered to the synapse (Dudek and Bear, 1992; Sjöström et al., 2001), and the level of dendritic spine depolarisation (Artola et al., 1990; Magee and Johnston, 1997; Sjöström and Häusser, 2006; Golding et al., 2002; Hardie and Spruston, 2009). The lack of satisfactory plasticity models based solely on neural spiking prompted researchers to consider simple models based on synapse biochemistry (Castellani et al., 2001; Castellani et al., 2005). Following a proposed role for postsynaptic calcium (Ca2+) signalling in synaptic plasticity (Lisman, 1989), previous models assumed that the amplitude of postsynaptic calcium controls long-term alterations in synaptic strength, with moderate levels of calcium causing long-term depression (LTD) and high calcium causing long-term potentiation (LTP) (Shouval et al., 2002; Karmarkar and Buonomano, 2002). However, experimental data suggests that calcium dynamics are also important (Yang et al., 1999; Mizuno et al., 2001; Wang et al., 2005; Nevian and Sakmann, 2006; Tigaret et al., 2016). As a result, subsequent phenomenological models of plasticity incorporated slow variables that integrate the fast synaptic input signals, loosely modelling calcium and its downstream effectors (Abarbanel et al., 2003; Rubin et al., 2005; Rackham et al., 2010; Clopath and Gerstner, 2010; Kumar and Mehta, 2011; Graupner and Brunel, 2012; Honda et al., 2013; Standage et al., 2014; De Pittà and Brunel, 2016). Concurrently, more detailed models tried to explicitly describe the molecular pathways integrating the calcium dynamics and its stochastic nature (Cai et al., 2007; Shouval and Kalantzis, 2005; Miller et al., 2005; Zeng and Holmes, 2010; Yeung et al., 2004). However, even these models do not account for data showing that plasticity is highly sensitive to physiological conditions such as the developmental age of the animal (Dudek and Bear, 1993; Meredith et al., 2003; Cao and Harris, 2012; Cizeron et al., 2020), extracellular calcium and magnesium concentrations (Mulkey and Malenka, 1992; Inglebert et al., 2020) and temperature (Volgushev et al., 2004; Wittenberg and Wang, 2006; Klyachko and Stevens, 2006). This limits the predictive power of this class of plasticity models.
An alternative approach taken by several groups (Bhalla and Iyengar, 1999; Jędrzejewska-Szmek et al., 2017; Blackwell et al., 2019; Chindemi et al., 2022; Zhang et al., 2021) was to model the complex molecular cascade leading to synaptic weight changes. The main benefit is the direct correspondence between the model’s components and biological elements, but at the price of numerous poorly constrained parameters. Additionally, the increased number of nonlinear equations and stochasticity makes fitting to plasticity experiment data difficult (Mäki-Marttunen et al., 2020).
Subtle differences between experimental STDP protocols can produce completely different synaptic plasticity outcomes, indicative of finely tuned synaptic behaviour as detailed above. To tackle this problem, we devised a new plasticity rule based on a bottom-up, data-driven approach by building a biologically-grounded model of plasticity induction at a single rat hippocampal CA3–CA1 synapse. We focused on this synapse type because of the abundant published experimental data that can be used to quantitatively constrain the model parameters. Compared to previous models in the literature, we aimed for an intermediate level of detail: enough biophysical components to capture the key dynamical processes underlying plasticity induction, but not the detailed molecular cascade underlying plasticity expression; much of which is poorly quantified for the various experimental conditions we cover in this study.
Our model is centred on dendritic spine electrical dynamics, calcium signalling and immediate downstream molecules, which we then map to synaptic strength change via a conceptually new dynamical, geometric readout mechanism. It assumes that a compartment-based description of calcium-triggered processes is sufficient to reproduce known properties of LTP and LTD induction. Also, neither spatially-resolved elements (Bartol et al., 2015; Griffith et al., 2016) nor calcium-independent processes are required to predict the observed synaptic change. Crucially, the model also captured intrinsic noise based on the stochastic switching of synaptic receptors and ion channels (Yuste et al., 1999; Ribrault et al., 2011). We report that, with a single set of parameters, the model could account for published data from spike-timing and frequency-dependent plasticity experiments, and variations in physiological parameters influencing plasticity outcomes. We also tested how the model responded to in vivo-like spike timing jitter and spike failures.
Results
A multi-timescale model of synaptic plasticity induction
We built a computational model of plasticity induction at a single CA3-CA1 rat glutamatergic synapse (Figure 1). Our goal was to reproduce results on synaptic plasticity that explored the effects of several experimental parameters: fine timing differences between pre and postsynaptic spiking (Figure 2 and Figure 3); stimulation frequency (Figure 4); animal age (Figure 5); external calcium and magnesium (Figure 6); stochasticity in the firing structure (Figure 7), temperature and experimental conditions variations (Supplemental Information). Where possible, we set parameters to values previously estimated from synaptic physiology and biochemistry experiments, and tuned the remainder within physiologically plausible ranges to reproduce our target plasticity experiments (see Materials and methods).
The model components are schematized in Figure 1 (full details in Materials and methods). For glutamate release, we used a two-pool vesicle depletion and recycling system, which accounts for short-term presynaptic depression and facilitation. When glutamate is released from vesicles, it can bind to the postsynaptic α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic acid and N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors (AMPArs and NMDArs, respectively), depolarizing the spine head by ∼30 mV (Kwon et al., 2017; Jayant et al., 2017; Beaulieu-Laroche and Harnett, 2018). The dendritic spine membrane depolarization causes the activation of voltage-gated calcium channels (VGCCs) and removes magnesium ([Mg2+]o) block from NMDArs. Backpropagating action potentials (BaP) can also depolarize the spine membrane by up to ∼60 mV (Kwon et al., 2017; Jayant et al., 2017). As an inhibitory component, we modelled a gamma-aminobutyric acid receptor (GABAr) synapse on the dendrite shaft (Destexhe et al., 1998). Calcium ions influx through VGCCs and NMDArs can activate SK potassium channels (Adelman et al., 2012; Griffith et al., 2016), which provide a tightly-coupled local negative feedback limiting spine depolarisation. Upon entering the spine, calcium ions also bind to calmodulin (CaM). Calcium-bound CaM in turn activates two major signalling molecules (Fujii et al., 2013): Ca2+/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II (CaMKII) and calcineurin (CaN) phosphatase, also known as PP2B (Saraf et al., 2018). We included these two enzymes because of the overwhelming evidence that CaMKII activation is necessary for Schaffer-collateral LTP (Giese et al., 1998; Chang et al., 2017), while CaN activation is necessary for LTD (O’Connor et al., 2005; Otmakhov et al., 2015). Later, we show how we map the joint activity of CaMKII and CaN to LTP and LTD. Ligand-gated ion channels (ionotropic receptors) and voltage-gated ion channels have an inherent random behavior, stochastically switching between open, closed and internal states (Ribrault et al., 2011). If the number of ion channels is large, then the variability of the total population activity becomes negligible relative to the mean (O’Donnell and van Rossum, 2014). However individual hippocampal synapses contain only small numbers of receptors and ion channels, for example they contain ∼10 NMDArs and <15 VGCCs (Takumi et al., 1999; Sabatini and Svoboda, 2000; Nimchinsky et al., 2004), making their total activation highly stochastic. Therefore, we modelled AMPAr, NMDAr, VGCCs and GABAr as stochastic processes. Presynaptic vesicle release events were also stochastic: glutamate release was an all-or-none event, and the amplitude of each glutamate pulse was drawn randomly, modelling heterogeneity in vesicle size (Liu et al., 1999). The inclusion of stochastic processes to account for an intrinsic noise in synaptic activation (Deperrois and Graupner, 2020) contrasts with most previous models in the literature, which either represent all variables as continuous and deterministic or add an external generic noise source (Bhalla, 2004; Antunes and De Schutter, 2012; Bartol et al., 2015).
The synapse model showed nonlinear dynamics across multiple timescales. For illustration, we stimulated the synapse with single simultaneous glutamate and GABA vesicle releases (Figure 1b). AMPArs and VGCCs open rapidly but close again within a few milliseconds. The dendritic GABAr closes more slowly, on a timescale of ∼10 ms. NMDArs, the major calcium source, closes on timescales of ∼50 and ∼250 ms for the GluN2A and GluN2B subtypes, respectively.
To show the typical responses of the spine head voltage and Ca2+, we stimulated the synapse with a single presynaptic pulse (EPSP) paired 10 ms later with a single BaP (1Pre1Post10; Figure 1c left). For this pairing, the arrival of a BaP at the spine immediately after an EPSP, leads to a large Ca2+ transient aligned with the BaP due to the NMDArs first being bound by glutamate then unblocked by the BaP depolarisation (Figure 1c right).
Single pre or postsynaptic stimulation pulses did not cause depletion of vesicle reserves or substantial activation of the enzymes. To illustrate these slower-timescale processes, we stimulated the synapse with a prolonged protocol: one presynaptic pulse followed by one postsynaptic pulse 10 ms later, repeated 30 times at 5 Hz (Figure 1d–e). The number of vesicles in both the docked and reserve pools decreased substantially over the course of the stimulation train (Figure 1d left), which in turn caused decreased vesicle release probability. Similarly, by the 30th pulse, the dendritic BaP amplitude had attenuated to ∼85% (∼70% BaP efficiency; Figure 1d right) of its initial amplitude, modelling the effects of slow dendritic sodium channel inactivation (Colbert et al., 1997; Golding et al., 2001). Free CaM concentration rose rapidly in response to calcium transients but also decayed back to baseline on a timescale of ∼500 ms (Figure 1e top). In contrast, the concentration of active CaMKII and CaN accumulated over a timescale of seconds, reaching a sustained peak during the stimulation train, then decayed back to baseline on a timescale of ∼10 and ∼120 s respectively, in line with experimental data (Quintana et al., 2005; Fujii et al., 2013; Chang et al., 2017; Figure 1e).
The effects of the stochastic variables can be seen in Figure 1b–d. The synaptic receptors and ion channels open and close randomly (Figure 1b). Even though spine voltage, calcium, and downstream molecules were modelled as continuous and deterministic, they inherited some randomness from the upstream stochastic variables. As a result, there was substantial trial-to-trial variability in the voltage and calcium responses to identical pre and postsynaptic spike trains (grey traces in Figure 1c). This variability was also passed on to the downstream enzymes CaM, CaMKII and CaN, but was filtered and therefore attenuated by the slow dynamics of CaMKII and CaN. In summary, the model contained stochastic nonlinear variables acting over five different orders of magnitude of timescale, from ∼1 ms to ∼1 min, making it sensitive to both fast and slow components of input signals.
Distinguishing between stimulation protocols using the CaMKII and CaN joint response
It has proven difficult for simple models of synaptic plasticity to capture the underlying rules and explain why some stimulation protocols induce plasticity while others do not. We tested the model’s sensitivity by simulating its response to a set of protocols used by Tigaret et al., 2016 in a recent ex vivo experimental study on adult (P50-55) rat hippocampus with blocked GABAr. We schematized the Tigaret et al., 2016 protocols in Figure 2a. Notably, three leading spike-timing and calcium-dependent plasticity models (Song et al., 2000; Pfister and Gerstner, 2006; Graupner and Brunel, 2012) could not fit well these data (Figure 2b–d). Next, we asked if our new model could distinguish between three pairs of protocols (see Figure 2e–m). For each of these pairs, one of the protocols experimentally induced LTP or LTD, while the other subtly different protocol caused no change (NC) in synapse strength.
The first pair of protocols differed in intensity. A protocol which caused no plasticity consisted of 1 presynaptic spike followed 10 ms later by one postsynaptic spike repeated at 5 Hz for 1 min (1Pre1Post10, 300 at 5 Hz). The other protocol induced LTP, but differed only in that it included a postsynaptic doublet instead of a single spike (1Pre2Post10, 300 at 5 Hz), implying a slightly stronger initial BaP amplitude. We first attempted to achieve separability by plotting CaMKII or CaN activities independently. As observed in the plots in Figure 2e, it was not possible to set a single concentration threshold on either CaMKII or CaN that would discriminate between the protocols. This result was expected, at least for CaMKII, as recent experimental data demonstrates a fast saturation of CaMKII concentration in dendritic spines regardless of stimulation frequency (Chang et al., 2017).
To achieve better separability we set out to test a different approach, which was to combine the activity of the two enzymes, by plotting the joint CaMKII and CaN responses against each other on a 2D plane (Figure 2f). This innovative geometric plot is based on the mathematical concept of orbits from dynamical systems theory (Meiss, 2007). In this plot, the trajectories of two protocols can be seen to overlap for the initial part of the transient and then diverge. To quantify trial-to-trial variability, we also calculated contour maps showing the mean fraction of time the trajectories spent in each part of the plane during the stimulation (Figure 2g). Importantly, both the trajectories and contour maps were substantially non-overlapping between the two protocols, implying that they can be separated based on the joint CaN-CaMKII activity. We found that the 1Pre2Post10 protocol leads to a weaker response in both CaMKII and CaN, corresponding to the lower blue traces in Figure 2f. The decreased response to the doublet protocol was due to the stronger attenuation of dendritic BaP amplitude over the course of the simulation (Golding et al., 2001), leading to reduced calcium influx through NMDArs and VGCCs (data not shown).
Using the second pair of protocols, we explored if this combined enzyme activity analysis could distinguish between subtle differences in protocol sequencing. We stimulated our model with one causal paring protocol (EPSP-BaP) involving a single presynaptic spike followed 50 ms later by a doublet of postsynaptic spikes (1Pre2Post50, 300 at 5 Hz), repeated at 5 Hz for one minute, which caused LTP in Tigaret et al., 2016. The other, anticausal, protocol involved the same total number of pre and postsynaptic spikes, but with the pre-post order reversed (2Post1Pre50, 300 at 5 Hz). Experimentally, the anticausal (BaP-EPSP) protocol did not induce plasticity (Tigaret et al., 2016). Notably, the only difference was the sequencing of whether the pre or postsynaptic neuron fired first, over a short time gap of 50 ms. Although the time courses of CaMKII and CaN activities were difficult to distinguish (Figure 2h), the LTP-inducing protocol caused greater CaN activation, compared to the non LTP-inducing protocol. Indeed, this translated to a horizontal offset in both the trajectory and contour map (Figure 2i–j), demonstrating that this pair of protocols can also be separated in the joint CaN-CaMKII plane.
The third pair of protocols differed in both duration and intensity. We thus tested the combined enzyme activity analysis in this configuration. In line with a previous study (Isaac et al., 2009), Tigaret et al., 2016 found that a train of doublets of presynaptic spikes separated by 50 ms repeated at a low frequency of 3 Hz for 5 min (2Pre50, 900 at 3 Hz) induced LTD, while a slightly more intense but shorter duration protocol of presynaptic spike doublets separated by 10 ms repeated at 5 Hz for 1 min (2Pre10, 300 at 5 Hz) did not cause plasticity. When we simulated both protocols in the model (Figure 2k–m), both caused similar initial responses in CaMKII and CaN. In the shorter protocol, this activation decayed to baseline within 100 s of the end of the stimulation. However the slower and longer-lasting 2Pre50 3 Hz 900 p protocol caused an additional sustained, stochastically fluctuating, plateau of activation of both enzymes (Figure 2k). This resulted in the LTD-inducing protocol having a downward and leftward-shifted CaN-CaMKII trajectory and contour plot, relative to the other protocol (Figure 2l–m). These results again showed that the joint CaN-CaMKII activity can be used to predict plasticity changes.
A geometrical readout mapping joint enzymatic activity to plasticity outcomes
The three above examples demonstrated that plotting the combined CaN-CaMKII activities in a 2D plane (geometrical readout which is abstract e.g. not defined within a physical space) allowed us to distinguish between subtly different protocols with correct assignment of plasticity outcome. We found that the simulated CaN-CaMKII trajectories from the two LTP-inducing protocols (Figure 2e–g and Figure 2h–j) spent a large fraction of time near ∼20 µM CaMKII and 7–10 µM CaN. In contrast, protocols that failed to trigger LTP had either lower (Figure 2h–j, k–m), or higher CaMKII and CaN activation (1Pre1Post10, Figure 2e–g). The LTD-inducing protocol, by comparison, spent a longer period in a region of sustained but lower ∼12 µM CaMKII and ∼2 µM CaN and activation Figure 2k–m. The plots in Figure 2g, j and m, show contour maps of histograms of the joint CaMKII-CaN activity, indicating where in the plane the trajectories spent most time. Figure 2g and j indicate that this measure can be used to predict plasticity, because the NC and LTP protocol histograms are largely non-overlapping. In Figure 2g, the NC protocol response ‘overshoots’ (mostly due to higher CaN concentration) the LTP protocol response, whereas in Figure 2j the NC protocol response ‘undershoots’ (mostly due to lower CaN concentration) the LTP protocol response. In contrast, when we compared the response histograms for the LTD and NC protocols, we found a greater overlap (Figure 2m). This suggested that, in this case, the histogram alone was not sufficient to separate the protocols, and that protocol duration is also important. LTD induction (2Pre50) required a more prolonged activation than NC (2Pre10). We thus took advantage of these joint CaMKII-CaN activity maps to design a minimal readout mechanism connecting combined enzyme activity to LTP, LTD or NC. We reasoned that this readout would need three key properties. First, although the figure suggests that the CaMKII-CaN trajectories corresponding to LTP and LTD could be linearly separable, we will demonstrate later (see Figure 3—figure supplement 3) that the readout requires nonlinear boundaries to activate the plasticity inducing component. Second, since LTD requires more prolonged activity than LTP, the readout should be sensitive to the timescale of the input. Third, a mechanism is required to convert the 2D LTP-LTD inducing signals into a synaptic weight change. After iterating through several designs, we satisfied the first property by designing ‘plasticity regions’: polygons in the CaN-CaMKII plane that would detect when trajectories pass through. We satisfied the second property by using two plasticity inducing components with different time constants which low-pass-filter the plasticity region signals. We satisfied the third property by feeding both the opposing LTP and LTD signals into a stochastic Markov chain which accumulated the total synaptic strength change. Overall, this readout mechanism acts as a parsimonious model of the complex signalling cascade linking CaMKII and CaN activation to expression of synaptic plasticity (He et al., 2015). It can be considered as a two-dimensional extension of previous computational studies that applied analogous 1D threshold functions to dendritic spine calcium concentration (Shouval et al., 2002; Karmarkar and Buonomano, 2002; Graupner and Brunel, 2012; Standage et al., 2014).
We now elaborate on the readout design process (see also Figure 21 of Materials and methods). We first drew non-overlapping polygons of LTP and LTD ‘plasticity regions’ in the CaN-CaMKII plane (Figure 3a). We positioned these regions over the parts of the phase space where the enzyme activities corresponding to the LTP- and LTD-inducing protocols were most different (Materials and methods), as shown by trajectories in Figure 2f, i and l. When a trajectory enters in one of these plasticity regions, it activates LTD or LTP indicator variables (Materials and methods) which encode the joint enzyme activities (trajectories in the phase plots) transitions across the LTP and LTD regions over time (Figure 3b). These indicator variables drove transition rates of a plasticity Markov chain used to predict LTP or LTD (Figure 3c), see Materials and methods. Intuitively, this plasticity Markov chain models the competing processes of insertion/deletion of AMPArs to the synapse, although this is not represented in the model. The LTD transition rates were slower than the LTP transition rates, to reflect studies showing that LTD requires sustained synaptic stimulation (Yang et al., 1999; Mizuno et al., 2001; Wang et al., 2005). The parameters for this plasticity Markov chain (Materials and methods) were fit to the plasticity induction outcomes from different protocols (Appendix 1—table 1). At the beginning of the simulation, the plasticity Markov chain starts with 100 processes (Destexhe et al., 1998) in the NC state, with each variable representing 1% weight change, an abstract measure of synaptic strength that can be either EPSP, EPSC, or field EPSP slope depending on the experiment. Each process can transit stochastically between NC, LTP and LTD states. At the end of the protocol, the plasticity outcome is given by the difference between the number of processes in the LTP and the LTD states (Materials and methods).
In Figure 3d, we plot the model’s responses to seven different plasticity protocols used by Tigaret et al., 2016 by overlaying example CaMKII-CaN trajectories for each protocol with the LTP and LTD regions. The corresponding region indicators are plotted as function of time in Figure 3e, and long-term alterations in the synaptic strength are plotted as function of time in Figure 3f. The three protocols that induced LTP in the Tigaret et al., 2016 experiments spent substantial time in the LTP region, and so triggered potentiation. In contrast, the combined response (CamKII, CaN) to 1Pre1Post10 overshoots both regions, crossing them only briefly on its return to baseline, and so resulted in little weight change. The protocol that induced LTD (2Pre50, purple trace) is five times longer than other protocols, spending sufficient time inside the LTD region (Figure 3f). In contrast, two other protocols that spent time in the same LTD region of the CaN-CaMKII plane (2Post1Pre50 and 2Pre10) were too brief to induce LTD. These protocols were also not strong enough to reach the LTP region, so resulted in no net plasticity, again consistent with Tigaret et al., 2016 experiments.
We observed run-to-run variability in the amplitude of the predicted plasticity, due to the inherent stochasticity in the model. To ensure that stochastic components are necessary for adequate model behaviour, we compared stochastic and deterministic versions of the model with and without discrete presynaptic release and found that adding stochastic components indeed modified the model’s behaviour (Figure 3—figure supplement 1). Also, we confirmed that VGCCs are necessary for accurate modelling of Tigaret et al., 2016 data as blocking these channels reproduced the data obtained in VGCC blockers by Tigaret that is no potentiation could be elicited (Figure 3—figure supplement 2). Finally, we stress in Figure 3—figure supplement 3 that the horizontal boundaries (related to CaMKII activity) are indeed necessary.
In Figure 3g, we plot the distributions of the simulation outcomes, along with the experimental data, for the protocols in Tigaret et al., 2016. We find a very good correspondence between the model and experiments. Of note, data fitting of the experiments in Tigaret et al., 2016 (Figure 3g) was more accurate with our model than the fitting obtained with existing leading spike-or calcium-based STDP models (Song et al., 2000; Pfister and Gerstner, 2006; Graupner and Brunel, 2012), see Figure 2b–d.
Experimentally, LTP can be induced by few pulses while LTD usually requires stimulation protocols of longer duration (Yang et al., 1999; Mizuno et al., 2001; Wang et al., 2005). We incorporated this effect into the geometrical readout model by letting LTP have faster transition rates than LTD (Figure 3c). Tigaret et al., 2016 found that 300 repetitions of anticausal post-before-pre pairings did not cause LTD, in contrast to the canonical spike-timing-dependent plasticity curve (Bi and Poo, 1998). We hypothesized that LTD might indeed appear with the anticausal protocol (Appendix 1—table 1) if stimulation duration was increased. To explore this possibility in our model, we systematically Alabi and Tsien, 2012 varied the number of paired repetitions from 100 to 1200, and also co-varied the pre-post delay from –100 to 100 ms. Figure 3h shows a contour plot of the predicted mean synaptic strength change across for the 1Pre2Post(delay) stimulation protocol for different numbers of pairing repetitions. In Figure 3h and a LTD window appears after ∼500 pairing repetitions for some anticausal pairings, in line with our expectation. The magnitude of LTP also increases with pulse number, for causal positive pairings. For either 100 or 300 pairing repetitions, only LTP or NC is induced (Figure 3i). The model also made other plasticity predictions by varying Tigaret et al., 2016 experimental conditions (Figure 3—figure supplement 4). In summary, our geometrical readout mechanism suggests that the direction and magnitude of the change in synaptic strength can be predicted from the joint CaMKII-CaN activity in the LTP and LTD regions.
Frequency-dependent plasticity
The stimulation protocols used by Tigaret et al., 2016 explored how subtle variations in pre and postsynaptic spike timing influenced the direction and magnitude of plasticity (see Appendix 1—table 1 for experimental differences). In contrast, traditional synaptic plasticity protocols exploring the role of presynaptic stimulation frequency did not measure the timing of co-occurring postsynaptic spikes (Dudek and Bear, 1992; Wang and Wagner, 1999; Kealy and Commins, 2010). These studies found that long-duration low-frequency stimulation (LFS) induces LTD, whereas short-duration high-frequency stimulation induces LTP, with a cross-over point of zero change at intermediate stimulation frequencies. In addition to allowing us to explore frequency-dependent plasticity (FDP), this stimulation paradigm also gave us further constraints to define the LTD polygon region in the model since in Tigaret et al., 2016, only one LTD case was available. For FDP, we focused on modelling the experiments from Dudek and Bear, 1992, who stimulated Schaffer collateral projections to pyramidal CA1 neurons with 900 pulses in frequencies ranging from 1 to 50 Hz. In addition to presynaptic stimulation patterns, the experimental conditions differed from Tigaret et al., 2016 in two other aspects: animal age and control of postsynaptic spiking activity (see Appendix 1—table 1 legend). We incorporated both age-dependence and EPSP-evoked-BaPs in our model (Materials and methods). Importantly, the geometrical readout mechanism mapping joint CaMKII-CaN activity to plasticity remained identical for all experiments in this work.
Figure 4a shows the joint CaMKII-CaN activity when we stimulated the model with 900 presynaptic spikes at 1, 3, 5, 10, and 50 Hz (Dudek and Bear, 1992). Higher stimulation frequencies drove stronger responses in both CaN and CaMKII activities (Figure 4a). Figure 4b and c show the corresponding plasticity region indicator for the LTP/LTD region threshold crossings and the synaptic strength change. From this set of five protocols, only the 50 Hz stimulation drove a response strong enough to reach the LTP region of the plane (Figure 4a and d). Although the remaining four protocols drove responses primarily in the LTD region, only the 3 and 5 Hz stimulations resulted in substantial LTD. The 1 and 10 Hz stimulations resulted in negligible LTD, but for two distinct reasons. Although the 10 Hz protocol’s joint CaMKII-CaN activity passed through the LTD region of the plane (Figure 4a and d), it was too brief to activate the slow LTD mechanism built into the readout (Materials and methods). The 1 Hz stimulation, on the other hand, was prolonged, but its response was too weak to reach the LTD region, crossing the threshold only intermittently (Figure 4b, bottom trace). Overall, the model matched well the mean plasticity response found by Dudek and Bear, 1992, see Figure 4e, following a classic BCM-like curve as function of stimulation frequency (Abraham et al., 2001; Bienenstock et al., 1982).
We then used the model to explore the stimulation space in more detail by varying the stimulation frequency from 0.5 to 40 Hz, and varying the number of presynaptic pulses from 50 to 1200. Figure 4f shows a contour map of the mean synaptic strength change (%) in this 2D frequency–pulse number space. Dudek and Bear, 1992 experimental conditions, we found that LTD induction required at least ∼300 pulses, at frequencies between 1 and 3 Hz. In contrast, LTP could be induced using ∼50 pulses at ∼20 Hz or greater. The contour map also showed that increasing the number of pulses (vertical axis in Figure 4f) increases the magnitude of both LTP and LTD. This was accompanied by a widening of the LTD frequency range, whereas the LTP frequency threshold remained around ∼20 Hz, independent of pulse number. This general effect, that increasing pulse number tends to increase the magnitude of plasticity, was also observed in simulation of Tigaret et al., 2016 (see Figure 3h). Ex vivo experiments in Dudek and Bear, 1992 were done at 35°C. However, lower temperatures are more widely usetd for ex vivo experiments because they extend brain slice viability.
At this point, having fully described the model, we show the importance of the stochasticity of the different components of the model. We simulated three protocols of Dudek and Bear, 1992 with deterministic equations in Figure 4—figure supplement 2. We show that, for the different protocols, if some of the channels are modelled with deterministic equations, the net effect on synapse weight differs from the expected outcome provided by the original model. The relative contributions of each source of noise differed, depending on the plasticity protocol. We can conclude that all noise sources we introduced in our model are important.
Variations in plasticity induction with developmental age
The rules for induction of LTP and LTD change during development (Dudek and Bear, 1993; Cao and Harris, 2012), so a given plasticity protocol can produce different outcomes when delivered to synapses from young animals versus mature animals. For example, when Dudek and Bear, 1993 tested the effects of low-frequency stimulation (1 Hz) on CA3-CA1 synapses from rats of different ages, they found that the magnitude of LTD decreases steeply with age from P7 until becoming minimal in mature animals >P35 (Figure 5a, circles). Across the same age range, they found that a theta burst stimulation (TBS) protocol induced progressively greater LTP magnitude with developmental age (Figure 5b, circles). Multiple properties of neurons change during development: the NMDAr switches its dominant subunit expression from GluN2B to GluN2A (Sheng et al., 1994; Popescu et al., 2004; Iacobucci and Popescu, 2017), the reversal potential of the receptor (GABAr) switches from depolarising to hyperpolarizing (Rivera et al., 1999; Meredith et al., 2003; Rinetti-Vargas et al., 2017), and the action potential backpropagates more efficiently with age (Buchanan and Mellor, 2007). These mechanisms have been proposed to underlie the developmental changes in synaptic plasticity rules because they are key regulators of synaptic calcium signalling (Meredith et al., 2003; Buchanan and Mellor, 2007). However, their sufficiency and individual contributions to the age-related plasticity changes are unclear and have not been taken into account in any previous model. We incorporated these mechanisms in the model (Materials and methods) by parametrizing each of the three components to vary with the animal’s postnatal age, to test if they could account for the age-dependent plasticity data.
We found that elaborating the model with age-dependent changes in NMDAr composition, GABAr reversal potential, and BaP efficiency, while keeping the same plasticity readout parameters, was sufficient to account for the developmental changes in LTD and LTP observed by Dudek and Bear, 1993 (Figure 5a and b). We then explored the model’s response to protocols of various stimulation frequencies, from 0.5 to 40 Hz, across ages from P5 to P80 (Figure 5c and e). Figure 5c shows the synaptic strength change as function of stimulation frequency for ages P15, P25, P35, and P45. The magnitude of LTD decreases with age, while the magnitude of LTP increases with age. Figure 5e shows a contour plot of the same result, covering the age-frequency space.
The 1 Hz presynaptic stimulation protocol in Dudek and Bear, 1993 did not induce LTD in adult animals (Dudek and Bear, 1992). We found that the joint CaN-CaMKII activity trajectories for this stimulation protocol underwent an age-dependent leftward shift beyond the LTD region (Figure 5f). This implies that LTD is not induced in mature animals by this conventional LFS protocol due to insufficient activation of enzymes. In contrast, Tigaret et al., 2016 and Isaac et al., 2009 were able to induce LTD in adult rat tissue by combining LFS with presynaptic spike pairs repeated 900 times at 3 Hz. Given these empirical findings and our modelling results, we observe that LTD induction in adult animals requires that the stimulation protocol: (1) causes CaMKII and CaN activity to stay more in the LTD region than the LTP region and (2) is sufficiently long to activate the LTD readout mechanism. With experimental parameters used by Dudek and Bear, 1993, this may be as short as 300 pulses when multi-spike presynaptic protocols are used since the joint CaMKII-CaN activity can reach the LTD region more quickly than for single spike protocols. We simulated two such potential protocols as predictions: doublet and quadruplet spike groups delivered 300 times at 1 Hz, with 50 ms between each pair of spikes in the group (Figure 5d). The model predicts that both these protocols induce LTD in adults, whereas as shown above, the single pulse protocol did not cause LTD. These simulations suggest that the temporal requirements for inducing LTD may not be as prolonged as previously assumed, since they can be reduced by varying stimulation intensity. See Figure 5—figure supplement 1 for frequency versus age maps for presynaptic bursts.
Dudek and Bear, 1993 also performed theta burst stimulation (Appendix 1—table 1) at different developmental ages, and found that LTP is not easily induced in young rats (Cao and Harris, 2012), as depicted in Figure 5b. The model qualitatively matches this trend, and also predicts that TBS induces maximal LTP around P21, before declining further during development (Figure 5b, green curve). Similarly, the model predicts that high-frequency stimulation induces LTP only for ages >P15, peaks at P35, then gradually declines at older ages (Figure 5e). Note that in Figure 5b, we used six epochs instead of four used by Dudek and Bear, 1993 to increase LTP outcome which is known to washout after one hour for young rats (Cao and Harris, 2012).
In contrast to Dudek and Bear, 1993 findings, other studies have found that LTP can be induced in hippocampus in young animals (<P15) with STDP. For example, Meredith et al., 2003 found that, at room temperature, 1Pre1Post10 induces LTP in young rats, whereas 1Pre2Post10 induces NC. This relationship was inverted for adults, with 1Pre1Post inducing no plasticity and 1Pre2Post10 inducing LTP (as captured by our model in Figure 5—figure supplement 1).
Together, these results suggest that not only do the requirements for LTP/LTD change with age, but also that these age-dependencies are different for different stimulation patterns. Finally, we explore which mechanisms are responsible for plasticity induction changes across development in the FDP protocol (Figure 5—figure supplement 1) by fixing each parameter to young or adult values for the FDP paradigm. Our model analysis suggests that the NMDAr switch (Iacobucci and Popescu, 2017) is a dominant factor affecting LTD induction, but the maturation of BaP (Buchanan and Mellor, 2007) is the dominant factor affecting LTP induction, with GABAr shift having only a weak influence on LTD induction for Dudek and Bear, 1993 FDP.
Plasticity requirements during development do not necessarily follow the profile in Dudek and Bear, 1993 as shown by Meredith et al., 2003 STDP experiment. Our model suggests that multiple developmental profiles are possible when experimental conditions vary within the same stimulation paradigm. This is illustrated in Figure 6—figure supplement 2a–c by varying the age of STDP experiments done in different conditions. We fitted well the data from Wittenberg and Wang, 2006 by adapting the model with appropriate age and temperature.
Effects of extracellular calcium and magnesium concentrations on plasticity outcome
The canonical STDP rule (Bi and Poo, 1998), measured in cultured neurons with high extracellular calcium ([Ca2+]o) and at room temperature, was recently found not to be reproducible at physiological [Ca2+]o in CA1 brain slices (Inglebert et al., 2020). Instead, by varying the [Ca2+]o and [Mg2+]o, Inglebert et al., 2020 found a spectrum of STDP rules with either no plasticity or full-LTD for physiological [Ca2+]o conditions ([Ca2+]o< 1.8 mM) and a bidirectional rule for high [Ca2+]o ([Ca2+]o> 2.5 mM), shown in Figure 6a-c.
We attempted to reproduce Inglebert et al., 2020 findings by varying [Ca2+]o and [Mg2+]o with the following consequences for the model mechanisms (Materials and methods). On the presynaptic side, [Ca2+]o modulates vesicle release probability. On the postsynaptic side, high [Ca2+]o reduces NMDAr conductance (Maki and Popescu, 2014), whereas [Mg2+]o affects the NMDAr Mg2+ block (Jahr and Stevens, 1990). Furthermore, spine calcium influx activates SK channels, which hyperpolarize the membrane and indirectly modulate NMDAr activity (Ngo-Anh et al., 2005; Griffith et al., 2016).
Figure 6a–c compares our model to Inglebert et al., 2020 STDP data at different [Ca2+]o and [Mg2+]o. Note that Inglebert et al., 2020 used 150 pairing repetitions for the anti-causal stimuli and 100 pairing repetitions for the causal stimuli both delivered at 0.3 Hz. At [Ca2+]o=1.3 mM, Figure 6a shows that the STDP rule induced weak LTD for brief causal delays. At [Ca2+]o = 1.8 mM, in Figure 6b, the model predicted a full-LTD window. At [Ca2+]o = 3 mM, in Figure 6c, it predicts a bidirectional rule with a second LTD window for long causal pairings, previously theorized by Rubin et al., 2005.
Figure 6d illustrates the time spent by the joint CaN-CaMKII activity for 1Pre1Post10 using Inglebert et al., 2020 experimental conditions. Each density plot corresponds to a specific Ca/Mg ratio as in Figure 6a–c. The response under low [Ca2+]o spent most time inside the LTD region, but high [Ca2+]o shifts the trajectory to the LTP region. Figure 6—figure supplement 1a presents density plots for the anti-causal protocols.
Inglebert et al., 2020 fixed the Ca/Mg ratio at 1.5, although aCSF formulations in the literature differ (see Appendix 1—table 1). Figure 6—figure supplement 1d shows that varying the Ca/Mg ratio and [Ca2+]o for Inglebert et al., 2020 experiments restrict LTP to Ca/Mg >1.5 and [Ca2+]o>1.8 mM.
Figure 6e shows a map of plasticity as function of pre-post delay and Ca/Mg concentrations and the parameters where LTP is induced for the 1Pre1Post10 protocol. Since plasticity rises steeply at around [Ca2+]o = 2.2 mM (see Figure 6—figure supplement 1b), small fluctuations in [Ca2+]o near this boundary could cause qualitative transitions in plasticity outcomes. For anti-causal pairings, increasing [Ca2+]o increases the magnitude of LTD (Figure 6—figure supplement 1b illustrates this with Inglebert et al., 2020 data). Our model can identify the transitions between LTD and LTP as a function of the ratio between [Ca2+]o and [Mg2+]o, see Figure 6—figure supplement 1.
Inglebert et al., 2020 also found that increasing the pairing frequency to 5 or 10 Hz results in a transition from LTD to LTP for 1Pre1Post10 at [Ca2+]o = 1.8 mM (Figure 6—figure supplement 1c), similar frequency-STDP behaviour has been reported in the cortex (Sjöström et al., 2001). In Figure 6f, we varied both the pairing frequencies and [Ca2+]o and we observe similar transitions to Inglebert et al., 2020. However, the model’s transition for [Ca2+]o = 1.8 mM was centred around 0.5 Hz, which was untested by Inglebert et al., 2020. The model predicts no plasticity at higher frequencies, unlike the data, that shows scattered LTP and LTD (see Figure 6—figure supplement 1c). Another frequency dependent comparison, Figure 3—figure supplement 4c and Figure 6—figure supplement 1h, show that Tigaret et al., 2016 burst-STDP and Inglebert et al., 2020 STDP share a similar transition structure, different from Dudek and Bear, 1992 FDP.
In contrast to Inglebert et al., 2020 results, the model predicts that setting low [Ca2+]o for Tigaret et al., 2016 burst-STDP abolishes LTP, and does not induce strong LTD (Figure 3—figure supplement 4d). For Dudek and Bear, 1992 experiment, Figure 4—figure supplement 1a [Mg2+]o controls a sliding threshold between LTD and LTP but not [Ca2+]o (Figure 4—figure supplement 1b). For another direct stimulation experiment, Figure 6—figure supplement 1f shows that in an Mg-free medium, LTP expression requires fewer pulses (Mizuno et al., 2001).
Despite exploring physiological [Ca2+]o and [Mg2+]o Inglebert (Inglebert et al., 2020) use a non-physiological temperature () which extends T-type VGCC closing times and modifies the CaN-CaMKII baseline (Figure 6—figure supplement 2f). In summary, our model predicts that temperature can change STDP rules in a similar fashion to [Ca2+]o (Figure 6—figure supplement 1a and b). Overall, we confirm that plasticity is highly sensitive to variations in extracellular calcium, magnesium, and temperature (Figure 3—figure supplement 4a, Figure 6—figure supplement 2d–f).
In vivo-like spike variability affects plasticity
In the above sections, we used highly regular and stereotypical stimulation protocols to replicate typical ex vivo plasticity experiments. In contrast, neural spiking in hippocampus in vivo is irregular and variable (Fenton and Muller, 1998; Isaac et al., 2009). Previous studies that asked how natural firing variability affects the rules of plasticity induction used simpler synapse models (Rackham et al., 2010; Graupner et al., 2016; Cui et al., 2018). We explored this question in our synapse model using simulations with three distinct types of additional variability: (1) spike time jitter, (2) failures induced by dropping spikes, (3) independent pre and postsynaptic Poisson spike trains (Graupner et al., 2016).
We introduced spike timing jitter by adding zero-mean Gaussian noise (s.d. ) to pre and postsynaptic spikes, changing spike pairs inter-stimulus interval (ISI). In Figure 7a, we plot the LTP magnitude as function of jitter magnitude (controlled by ) for protocols taken from Tigaret et al., 2016. With no jitter, , these protocols have different LTP magnitudes (corresponding to Figure 3) and become similar once increases. The three protocols with a postsynaptic spike doublet gave identical plasticity for ms.
To understand the effects of jittering, we plotted the trajectories of joint CaN-CaMKII activity (Figure 7c). 2Post1Pre50 which ‘undershoots’ the LTP region shifted into the LTP region for jitter ms. In contrast, 1Pre1Post10 which ‘overshoots’ (mostly smaller CaN concentration) the LTP region shifted to the opposite direction towards the LTP region.
Why does jitter cause different spike timing protocols to yield similar plasticity magnitudes? Increasing jitter causes a fraction of pairings to invert causality. Therefore, the jittered protocols became a mixture of causal and anticausal pairings (Figure 7c). This situation occurs for all paired protocols. So any protocol with the same number spikes will produce a similar outcome if the jitter is large enough. Note that despite noise the mean frequency was conserved at 5 ±13.5 Hz (see Figure 7e).
Next, we studied the effect of spike removal. In the previous sections, synaptic release probability was ∼60% (for [Ca2+]o = 2.5 mM) or lower, depending on the availability of docked vesicles (Materials and methods). However, baseline presynaptic vesicle release probability is heterogeneous across CA3-CA1 synapses, ranging from % (Dobrunz et al., 1997; Enoki et al., 2009) and likely lower on average in vivo (Froemke and Dan, 2002; Borst, 2010). BaPs are also heterogeneous with random attenuation profiles (Golding et al., 2001) and spike failures (Short et al., 2017). To test the effects of pre and postsynaptic failures on plasticity induction, we performed simulations where we randomly removed spikes, altering the regular attenuation observed in Tigaret et al., 2016 protocols.
In Figure 7b, we plot the plasticity magnitude as function of sparsity (percentage of removed spikes). The sparsity had different specific effects for each protocol. 1Pre2Post10 and 1Pre2Post50 which originally produced substantial LTP were robust to spike removal until ∼60% sparsity. In contrast, the plasticity magnitude from both 1Pre1Post10 and 2Post1Pre50 showed a non-monotonic dependence on sparsity, first increasing then decreasing, with maximal LTP at ∼40% sparsity.
To understand how sparsity causes this non-monotonic effect on plasticity magnitude, we plotted the histograms of time spent in the CaN-CaMKII plane for 2Post1Pre50 for three levels of sparsity: 0%, 30%, and 80% (Figure 7d). For 0% sparsity, the activation spent most time at the border between the LTP and LTD regions, resulting in no change. Increasing sparsity to 30% caused the activation to shift rightward into the LTP region because there was less attenuation of pre and postsynaptic resources. In contrast, at 80% sparsity, the activation moved into the LTD region because there were not enough events to substantially activate CaMKII and CaN. Since LTD is a slow process and the protocol duration is short (60 s), there was no net plasticity. Therefore for this protocol, high and low sparsity caused no plasticity for distinct reasons, whereas intermediate sparsity enabled LTP by balancing resource depletion with enzyme activation.
Next we tested the interaction of jitter and spike removal. Figure 7f shows a contour map of weight change as a function of jitter and sparsity for the 2Post1Pre50 protocol, which originally induced no plasticity (Figure 3). Increasing spike jitter enlarged the range of sparsity inducing LTP. In summary, these simulations (Figure 7a, b, f and h) show that different STDP protocols have different degrees of sensitivity to noise in the firing structure, suggesting that simple plasticity rules derived from regular ex vivo experiments may not predict plasticity in vivo.
How does random spike timing affect rate-dependent plasticity? We stimulated the model with pre and postsynaptic Poisson spike trains for 10 s, under Dudek and Bear, 1992 experimental conditions. We systematically varied both the pre and postsynaptic rates (Figure 7h). The 10 s stimulation protocols induced only LTP, since LTD requires a prolonged stimulation (Mizuno et al., 2001). LTP magnitude monotonically increased with the presynaptic rate (Figure 7g and h). In contrast, LTP magnitude varied non-monotonically as a function of postsynaptic rate, initially increasing until a peak at 10 Hz, then decreasing with higher stimulation frequencies. This non-monotonic dependence on post-synaptic rate is inconsistent with classic rate-based models of Hebbian plasticity. From this analysis, we can make the prediction that firing variability can alter the rules of plasticity, in the sense that it is possible to add noise to cause LTP for protocols that did not otherwise induce plasticity. For example, we show that protocols inducing LTP can be hindered by jittering, e.g. 1Pre2Post10 in Figure 7a. Also, protocols that are just outside the LTP plasticity region may turn into LTP if jitter is applied, e.g. 2Post1Pre50 and 1Pre1Post10 in Figure 7a. We also investigated how this plasticity dependence on pre- and postsynaptic Poisson firing rates varies with developmental age (Figure 4—figure supplement 1g–i). We found that at P5 no plasticity is induced, at P15 a LTP region appears at around 1 Hz postsynaptic rate, and at P20 plasticity becomes similar to the mature age, with a peak in LTP magnitude at 10 Hz postsynaptic rate.
Discussion
We built a model of a rat CA3-CA1 hippocampal synapse, including key electrical and biochemical components underlying synaptic plasticity induction (Figure 1). We developed a novel geometric readout of combined CaN-CaMKII dynamics (Figures 2—4) to predict the outcomes from a range of plasticity experiments with heterogeneous conditions: animal developmental age (Figure 5), aCSF composition (Figure 6), temperature (Supplemental files), and in vivo-like firing variability (Figure 7). This readout provides a simple and intuitive window into the dynamics of the synapse during plasticity. Our model is thus based on the joint activity of these two key postsynaptic enzymes at both fast and slow time scales and considers the stochastic dynamics of their activities dictated by the upstream calcium-dependent components at both the pre- and postsynapse. On this basis alone, our model is akin to biological processes where the outcome is jointly determined by several stochastic signalling components and a combination of multiple enzyme activities, that is, are multi-dimensional. The principal assumption underlying the proposed ‘geometric readout’ mechanism is that all information determining the induction of LTP vs. LTD is contained in the time-dependent spine Ca2+/calmodulin-bound CaN and CaMKII concentrations, and that no extra elements are required. Further, since both CaN and CaMKII concentrations are uniquely determined by the time course of postsynaptic Ca2+ concentration, the model implicitly assumes that the LTP/LTD induction depends solely on spine Ca2+ concentration time course, as in previously compared simplified models (see Introduction).
In addition to providing a new model of CA3-CA1 synapse biophysics, the main contribution of this work is the novel readout mechanism mapping synaptic enzymes to plasticity outcomes. This readout was built based on the concept that the full temporal activity of CaN-CaMKII over the minutes-timescale stimulus duration, and not their instantaneous levels, is responsible for changes in synaptic efficacy (Fujii et al., 2013). We instantiated this concept by analysing the joint CaN-CaMKII activity in the two-dimensional plane and designing polygonal plasticity readout regions (Figure 3a). Here, we used only a two-dimensional readout, but anticipate a straightforward generalisation to higher dimensions. The central discovery is that these trajectories, despite being stochastic, can be separated in the plane as a function of the stimulus (Figure 3). This is the basis of our new synaptic plasticity rule. We generalised previous work with plasticity induction based on single threshold and a slow variable (Badoual et al., 2006; Rubin et al., 2005; Clopath and Gerstner, 2010; Graupner and Brunel, 2012). In contrast, previous models assume that plasticity is explainable in terms of synaptic calcium or enzyme response to single BAP-EPSP pairings (Shouval et al., 2002; Karmarkar and Buonomano, 2002). We expect that future studies using high temporal resolution measurements such as those provided by recent FRET tools available for CaMKII (Chang et al., 2017; Chang et al., 2019) will bring refinements to our model with the possibility to further test our readout predictions.
Let us describe the intuition behind our model more concisely. First, we abstracted away the sophisticated cascade of plasticity expression. Second, the plasticity regions, crossed by the trajectories, are described with a minimal set of parameters. Importantly, their tuning is quite straightforward and done only once, even when the joint activity is stochastic. The tuning of the model is possible thanks to the decoupling of the plasticity process from the spine biophysics which acts as a feedforward input to the plasticity Markov chain and from the distributions of the different trajectories, which are well separated. The separability afforded by the geometrical readout, along with the model flexibility via fitting the plasticity regions, enabled us to reproduce data from nine different experiments using a single fixed set of model parameters. In contrast, we found that classic spike-timing (Song et al., 2000; Pfister and Gerstner, 2006) or calcium-threshold (Graupner and Brunel, 2012) models could not reproduce the range of protocols from Tigaret et al., 2016 (Figure 2b–d). More complicated molecular-cascade models have been shown to account for individual plasticity experiments (Antunes et al., 2016; Jędrzejewska-Szmek et al., 2017; Mäki-Marttunen et al., 2020; Bhalla, 2017), but have not been demonstrated to reproduce the wide range of protocols presented here while considering experimental heterogeneity.
For some protocols, the CaMKII-CaN trajectories overshot the plasticity regions (e.g. Figure 3d). Although abnormally high and prolonged calcium influx to cells can trigger cell death (Zhivotovsky and Orrenius, 2011), the effects of high calcium concentrations at single synapses are poorly understood. Notably, a few studies have reported evidence consistent with an overshoot, where strong synaptic calcium influx does not induce LTP (Yang et al., 1999; Tigaret et al., 2016; Pousinha et al., 2017).
Our model included critical components for plasticity induction at CA3-CA1 synapses: those affecting dendritic spine voltage, calcium signalling, and enzymatic activation. We were able to use our model to make quantitative predictions, because its variables and parameters corresponded to biological components. This property allowed us to incorporate the model components’ dependence on developmental age, external Ca/Mg levels, and temperature to replicate datasets across a range of experimental conditions. The model is relatively fast to simulate, taking ∼1 min of CPU time to run 1 min of biological time. These practical benefits should enable future studies to make experimental predictions on dendritic integration of multiple synaptic inputs (Blackwell et al., 2019; Oliveira et al., 2012; Ebner et al., 2019) and on the effects of synaptic molecular alterations in pathological conditions. In contrast, abstract models based on spike timing (Song et al., 2000; Pfister and Gerstner, 2006; Clopath and Gerstner, 2010) or simplified calcium dynamics (Shouval et al., 2002; Graupner and Brunel, 2012) must rely on ad hoc adjustment of parameters with less biological interpretability.
Intrinsic noise is an essential component of the model. How can the synapse reliably express plasticity but be noisy at the same time (Yuste et al., 1999; Ribrault et al., 2011)? Noise can be reduced either by redundancy or by averaging across time, also called ergodicity (Sterling and Laughlin, 2015). However, redundancy requires manufacturing and maintaining more components, and therefore costs energy. We propose that, instead, plasticity induction is robust due to temporal averaging by slow-timescale signalling and adaptation processes. These slow variables display reduced noise levels by averaging the faster timescale stochastic variables. This may be a reason why CaMKII uses auto-phosphorylation to sustain its activity and slow its decay time (Chang et al., 2017; Chang et al., 2019). In summary, this suggests that the temporal averaging by slow variables, combined with the separability afforded by the multidimensional readout, allows synapses to tolerate noise while remaining energy-efficient.
A uniqueness of our model is that it simultaneously incorporates biological variables such as electrical components at pre and postsynaptic sites, some with adaptive functions such as attenuation, age and temperature, stochastic noise and fast and slow timescales. Some of these variables have been modelled by other groups, for example stochasticity, BaP attenuation or pre-synaptic plasticity (Cai et al., 2007; Shouval and Kalantzis, 2005; Zeng and Holmes, 2010; Miller et al., 2005; Yeung et al., 2004; Shah et al., 2006; Deperrois and Graupner, 2020; Costa et al., 2015), but generally independently from each other. To position the uniqueness of our model in this broader context, we also provide a direct comparison of our model with some of the most recent leading models of excitatory synapse plasticity and the experimental work they reproduce (Appendix 1—table 2 and Appendix 1—table 3).
We identified some limitations of the model. First, we modelled only a single postsynaptic spine attached to a two-compartment neuron (soma and dendrite). Second, the model abstracted the complicated process of synaptic plasticity expression. Indeed, even if this replicated the early phase of LTP/LTD expression in the first 30–60 min after induction, we did not take into account slower protein-synthesis-dependent processes, maintenance processes, and synaptic pruning proceed at later timescales (Bailey et al., 2015). Third, like most biophysical models, ours contained many parameters. Although we set these to physiologically plausible values and then tuned to match the plasticity data, other combinations of parameters may fit the data equally well (Marder and Taylor, 2011; Mäki-Marttunen et al., 2020) due to the ubiquitous phenomenon of redundancy in biochemical and neural systems (Gutenkunst et al., 2007; Marder, 2011). Indeed synapses are quite heterogeneous in receptor and ion channel counts (Takumi et al., 1999; Sabatini and Svoboda, 2000; Racca et al., 2000; Nimchinsky et al., 2004), protein abundances (Shepherd and Harris, 1998; Sugiyama et al., 2005), and spine morphologies (Bartol et al., 2015; Harris and Stevens, 1989), even within the subpopulation of CA1 pyramidal neuron synapses that we modelled here. Fourth, the activation of clustered synapses could influence the plasticity outcome, and the number of synapses activated during plasticity induction can be difficult to control experimentally. Our model concerns plasticity at a single synapse, which is also important during synaptic cluster activation (Ujfalussy and Makara, 2020). We drew from data in Tigaret et al., 2016 where there is little indication of simultaneous clustered synaptic activation. Furthermore, our simulations are in good agreement with plasticity experiments using local field potential recordings (Dudek and Bear, 1993) where the number of activated synapses is uncertain. This indicates that the model proposed here can account for this aspect of synaptic plasticity heterogeneity. Finally, our readout model does not correspond to a specific molecular cascade beyond CaN and CaMKII activations. However, we anticipate that the same mapping could be implemented by simple biochemical reaction networks, with for example, transition rates based on Hill functions for the plasticity boundaries.
Since the model respected the stochasticity of vesicle release (Rizzoli and Betz, 2005; Alabi and Tsien, 2012), NMDAr (Nimchinsky et al., 2004; Popescu et al., 2004; Iacobucci and Popescu, 2017; Sinclair et al., 2016), and VGCC opening (Magee and Johnston, 1995; Sabatini and Svoboda, 2000; Iftinca et al., 2006), the magnitude of plasticity varied from simulation trial to trial. This suggests that the rules of plasticity are inherently stochastic (Bhalla, 2004; Antunes et al., 2016) and that the variability observed in these experiments (Inglebert et al., 2020; Tigaret et al., 2016; Dudek and Bear, 1992; Dudek and Bear, 1993; Mizuno et al., 2001; Meredith et al., 2003; Wittenberg and Wang, 2006) is partly due to stochastic signalling. With our current model, we have been able to reproduce nine experiments with a single set of parameters. By running extensive simulations over the space of protocols beyond those tested experimentally (Figure 3h and i; Figure 4f; Figure 5c, e and f; Figure 6e and f), we made testable predictions for plasticity outcomes that should therefore be of a high level of confidence. For example, Tigaret et al., 2016 did not find LTD when using classic post-before-pre stimulation protocols, but the model predicted that LTD could be induced if the number of pairing repetitions was extended (Figure 3h and i). The model also predicts that the lack of LTD induced by FDP in adults can be recovered using doublets, triplets or quadruplets of spikes in the protocols (Figure 5d). We tested the model’s sensitivity to spike time jitter and spike failure in the stimulation protocols (Figure 7). Our simulations predict that this firing variability can alter the rules of plasticity, in the sense that it is possible to add noise to cause LTP for protocols that did not otherwise induce plasticity.
What do these results imply about the rules of plasticity in vivo? First, we noticed that successful LTP or LTD induction required a balance between two types of slow variables: those that attenuate, such as presynaptic vesicle pools and dendritic BaP, versus those that accumulate, such as slow enzymatic integration (Cai et al., 2007; Mizusaki et al., 2022; Deperrois and Graupner, 2020). This balance is reflected in the inverted-U shaped magnitude of LTP seen as a function of post-synaptic firing rate (Figure 7h). Second, although spike timing on millisecond timescales can in certain circumstances affect the direction and magnitude of plasticity (Figure 3), in order to drive sufficient activity of synaptic enzymes, these patterns would need to be repeated for several seconds. However, if these repetitions are subject to jitter or failures, as observed in hippocampal spike trains in vivo (Fenton and Muller, 1998; Wierzynski et al., 2009), then the millisecond-timescale information will be destroyed as it gets averaged out across repetitions by the slow integration processes of CaMKII and CaN (Figure 7a–d). The net implication is that millisecond-timescale structure of individual spike pairs is unlikely to play an important role in determining hippocampal synaptic plasticity in vivo (Froemke and Dan, 2002; Sadowski et al., 2016; Graupner et al., 2016).
In summary, we presented a new type of biophysical model for plasticity induction at the rat CA3-CA1 glutamatergic synapse. Although the model itself is specific to this synapse type, the study’s insights may generalise to other synapse types, enabling a deeper understanding of the rules of synaptic plasticity and brain learning.
Materials and methods
Experimental datasets
Request a detailed protocolThe datasets at the basis of our model were obtained directly from the authors Tigaret et al., 2016 or extracted from graphs in the references in Appendix 1—table 2 using WebPlotDigitizer v 4.6 software (Rohatgi, A.). The dataset from Tigaret et al., 2016 is freely available upon request.
Code availability
Request a detailed protocolAll simulations were performed in the Julia programming language (version 1.4.2). This choice was dictated by simplicity and speed (Perkel, 2019). The code for the Markov chains is mostly automatically generated from reactions using the Julia package Catalyst.jl (Loman et al., 2022), and could be exported to an SBML representation for porting to other languages. The model is available on GitHub at SynapseElife (copy archived at Veltz, 2023).
Simulating the synapse model is equivalent to sampling a piecewise deterministic Markov process, and this relies on the thoroughly tested Julia package PiecewiseDeterministicMarkovProcesses.jl. These simulations are event-based, and no approximation is made beyond the ones required to integrate the ordinary differential equations by the LSODA method (Livermore Solver for Ordinary Differential Equations). We ran the parallel simulations in the Nef cluster operated by Inria.
Notation
Request a detailed protocolWe write for the indicator of a set , meaning that if belongs to and zero otherwise.
Vesicle release and recycling
Request a detailed protocolThe detailed exocytosis model we implemented was motivated by taking into account the following minimal requirements: synaptic failures (which impact STDP protocols), vesicle depletion (for frequency/pulse number repetition dependent protocols), external calcium (motivated by Inglebert et al., 2020). This is best modeled by counting the released vesicles, hence our choice of a stochastic model. In biological synapses, an action potential arriving at the presynaptic terminal can trigger the release of a neurotransmitter–filled vesicle, which activates postsynaptic receptors. We derived a vesicle–release Markov chain model based on a deterministic approach described in Sterratt et al., 2011.
Vesicles can be in two states, either belonging to the docked pool (with cardinal ) with fast emptying, or to the reserve pool (with cardinal ) which replenishes (Rizzoli and Betz, 2005). Initially the docked and reserve pools have D0 and R0 vesicles, respectively. The docked pool loses one vesicle each time a release occurs (Rudolph et al., 2015), with transition (Figure 8). The reserve pool replenishes the docked pool with reversible transition . Finally, the reserve pool is replenished towards initial value over a timescale via the transition .
In addition to the stochastic dynamics in Table 1, each spike triggers a vesicle release with probability :
which is a function of presynaptic calcium and extracellular calcium concentration, [Ca2+]o, through the threshold . The function was fitted in Figure 8e against data from Tigaret et al., 2016; Hardingham et al., 2006. To decide whether a vesicle is released for a presynaptic spike, we use a phenomenological model of (see Figure 8a) based on a resource-use function (Tsodyks and Markram, 1997):
Upon arrival of the presynaptic spikes, , we update according to the deterministic jump:
Finally, after has been updated, a vesicle may be released with probability (Figure 8b).
Parameters for the vesicle release model are given in Table 2. We constrained these parameters against the data reported by Hardingham et al., 2006 and Tigaret et al., 2016. Because [Ca2+]o modifies the release probability dynamics (King et al., 2001), we fixed an initial release probability to 68% for [Ca2+]o = 2.5 mM as reported by Tigaret et al., 2016 (initial value in Figure 8b and d). Additionally, Hardingham et al., 2006 reports a 38% reduction in the initial release probability when changing [Ca2+]o from 2.5 mM to 1 mM. Taking these into account, the decreasing sigmoid function in the Figure 8e depicts our [Ca2+]o-dependent release probability model ().
Figure 8e shows that our function is in good agreement with a previous analytical model suggesting that (King et al., 2001). Our model also qualitatively reproduces the decrease of calcium dye fluorescence levels after 20 s of theta trains from Tigaret et al., 2016. We interpret their fluorescence measurements as an effect of short-term depression (see Figure 8b).
Despite these agreements between our model and previous work, it is a simplified presynaptic model that does not encompass the highly heterogeneous nature of vesicle release. Vesicle release dynamics are known to be sensitive to various experimental conditions such as temperature (Fernández-Alfonso and Ryan, 2004), the age for some brain regions (Rudolph et al., 2015) or magnesium concentration (Hardingham et al., 2006). Although we did not consider this for the presynaptic model, note that we do incorporate such experimental parameters (age, temperature, [Ca2+]o, [Mg2+]o) to our model of NMDArs, which is the main postsynaptic calcium source. Furthermore, since our model of vesicle dynamics is simple, in Equation 2 has two roles: to delay the recovery caused by inactivation (enforced by in Equation 2) and to prevent vesicle release after HFS-induced depression (King et al., 2001; Rizzoli and Betz, 2005). Our presynaptic model for is purely phenomenological as, in principle, the jump parameter should depend on , but replacing the model with a more physiological model would not affect the results, since the measured dependence of release probability on is already satisfied by this phenomenological model. Also, multi-vesicular release (MVR) at SC-CA1 synapses was shown to be prominent after manipulations that increase release probability, for example during the facilitation seen with paired-pulse stimulations (Christie and Jahr, 2006; Oertner et al., 2002). Yet, we chose not to incorporate this mechanism in the pre-synaptic model because we do not hold enough information on how MVR participates to plasticity outcomes of the different protocols we used in this study.
Model compartments
Request a detailed protocolOur model has three compartments. 1) a spherical dendritic spine linked by the neck to 2) a cylindrical dendrite connected to 3) a spherical soma. The membrane potential of these compartments satisfy the equations below (parameters in Table 3). Since the dendrite is a single compartment, the precise spine location is undefined. For more detailed morphological simulations to predict plasticity, see Ebner et al., 2019, Chindemi et al., 2022 and Jędrzejewska-Szmek et al., 2017. The distance from the soma to the spine functionally mimics the BaP attenuation as shown in Golding et al., 2001, and it is set to 200 µm for all simulations, except in Figure 3—figure supplement 4e and Figure 4—figure supplement 1c. In these panels, we modified this distance parameter as described in the graph y-axis obtaining results similar to Ebner et al., 2019. The different currents in the soma, dendrite and spine are described in Equations 3–5.
Membrane potential and currents
Request a detailed protocolThe membrane potential of these compartments satisfy the equations below (parameters in Table 3). The different currents are described in the following sections.
Action-potential backpropagation (BaP)
Postsynaptic currents
Request a detailed protocolThe postsynaptic currents are generated in the soma, backpropagated to the dendritic spine via a passive dendrite which filters them. The soma generates BaPs using a version of the Na+ and K+ channel models developed by Migliore et al., 1999. The related parameters are described in Table 4 (the voltage unit is mV). We used the following the units: and .
To trigger a BaP, an external current is injected in the soma at times (postsynaptic input times) for a chosen duration with amplitude . Taking as the Heaviside function, this is expressed as:
The currents underlying the BaP in the soma are filtered in a distance-dependent manner by the dendrite before reaching the dendritic spine. Biologically, BaP adaptation is caused by the inactivation of sodium channels and variations in sodium and potassium channel expression along the dendrite (Jung et al., 1997; Golding et al., 2001). We used a phenomenological model, implementing distant-dependent BaP amplitude attenuation by modifying the axial resistance (see Equation 4 and Equation 5) between the dendrite and the soma as follows (Figure 9c top):
where is the distance of the spine from the soma and where the factor is dynamically regulated based on a resource-use equation from Tsodyks and Markram, 1997 with a dampening factor changing the size of the attenuation step :
The BaP attenuation model is based on Golding et al., 2001 data for strongly attenuating neurons. Therefore, the second type of attenuation (weakly attenuating) in neurons is not considered (dichotomy in Figure 9a). Figure 9a compares Golding’s data to our model and illustrates the effect of BaP attenuation in the upper panels of Figure 9a, b.
Table 4 shows the BaP attenuation parameters. The plasticity outcomes as function of the dendritic spine distance from the soma are shown in Figure 3—figure supplement 4e and Figure 4—figure supplement 1c.
Age-dependent BaP adaptation
Request a detailed protocolNeuronal bursting properties are altered during development through development through the maturation and expression of potassium and sodium channels (Gymnopoulos et al., 2014), which change the interaction of hyperpolarizing and depolarizing currents (see Figure 9b; Grewe et al., 2010; Jung et al., 1997). We fitted the data of the age dependent somatic attenuation profiles from Buchanan and Mellor, 2007 (Figure 9b) with our model by including an age-dependent BaP amplitude attenuation factor. We define the attenuation factor (Figure 9c bottom), as follows.
In Equation 5, the age effects are introduced by multiplying the sodium and the external currents by the attenuation factor .
AP evoked by EPSP
Request a detailed protocolBiologically, a presynaptic stimulation triggers a BaP if sufficient depolarization is caused by the EPSPs reaching the soma (Stuart et al., 2016). To model this effect for some LFP recordings protocols, we included an option to choose whether an EPSP can evoke an AP using an event generator resembling the previous presynaptic release probability model , as in Equation 1. Like , the BaPs evoked by EPSPs are calculated offline, before the postsynaptic simulation. We use a variable which is incremented by 1 at each presynaptic time and has exponential decay:
Since the BaPs evoked by EPSPs are triggered by the afferent synapses and are limited by their respective docked pools (), we use the previous to define the probability of an AP to occur. We test the ratio of successful releases from 25 synapses to decide if a BaP is evoked by an EPSP, setting a test threshold of 80%. Therefore, we express the probability of evoking an AP, , with the following test:
In real neurons, the EPSP summation dynamics in the soma and dendrites depend on the complex neuron morphology (Etherington et al., 2010; Ebner et al., 2019) which was not implemented by our model. Instead our ‘AP evoked by EPSP test’ is a simplified way to produce BaPs, similar to an integrate-and-fire model (Sterratt et al., 2011).
Previous work (Mayr and Partzsch, 2010) suggests that low-frequency stimulation, as used in Dudek and Bear, 1992 ([Ca2+]o = 2.5 mM), can evoke BaPs with a ∼5% probability. Our model accounts for this, but also allows the probability of eliciting an AP to increase with stimulation frequency (Etherington et al., 2010). This is captured by as shown in Figure 9f. Figure 9d, e show how a 5 Hz stimulation can evoke APs. The delay between the EPSP and the evoked AP is set to , similar to the EPSP-spike latency reported for CA1 pyramidal neurons (Fricker and Miles, 2000).
AMPAr
Markov chain
Request a detailed protocolThe AMPArs are modeled as a Markov chain (Figure 10) described by Robert and Howe, 2003 and Coombs et al., 2017 and adapted to temperature changes according to Postlethwaite et al., 2007. Here, we introduce the additional parameters to cover AMPAr temperature-sensitive kinetics (Postlethwaite et al., 2007). The corresponding parameters are given in Table 5.
The AMPAr conductance is given as the sum of the occupancies of the three subconductance states , and of the Markov chain in Figure 10. The AMPAr current is then:
The adaptation of the Markov chain from Robert and Howe, 2003 is made by changing the forward and backward rates in a temperature-dependent manner matching the decay time reported by Postlethwaite et al., 2007:
The effects of temperature change on AMPAr dynamics are presented in Figure 11, which also shows that the desensitisation is not altered by temperature changes (Figure 11b and c). The recovery time from desensitisation is the same as at room temperature (Robert and Howe, 2003). Desensitisation measurements are required to account for a temperature-dependent change in the rates of the ‘vertical’ transitions in Figure 10, see Postlethwaite et al., 2007. This could be relevant for presynaptic bursts as indicated in Figure 11b–c.
Postsynaptic Ca2+ influx
Request a detailed protocolThe effects of experimental conditions on the calcium dynamics are due to receptors, ion channels and enzymes. A leaky term models the calcium resting concentration in the Equation 9. The calcium fluxes from NMDAr and VGCCs (T, R, L types) are given in Equation 10. The diffusion term through the spine neck is expressed in Equation 11. Finally, the buffer, the optional dye and the enzymatic reactions are given in Equation 12 (parameter values given at the Table 6):
Despite the driving force to the resting concentration, , the tonic opening of T-type channels causes calcium to fluctuate making its mean baseline value dependent on temperature, extracellular calcium, and voltage. The effects of this tonic opening in various experimental conditions are shown in Figure 6—figure supplement 2f. To avoid modelling dendritic calcium sources, we use a dampening term as one-third of the calcium level since calcium imaging comparing dendrite and spine fluorescence have shown this trend (Segal and Korkotian, 2014). Equation 11 implements the diffusion of calcium from the spine to the dendrite through the neck. The time constant for the diffusion coefficient , is estimated as described in Holcman et al., 2005. The calcium buffer and the optional dye are described as a two-state reaction system (Sabatini et al., 2002):
Tigaret et al., 2016 experiments used the synthetic calcium-indicator dye Fluo-5f, which is well-modelled by a single Ca2+-dye binding reaction (Maravall et al., 2000; Bartol et al., 2015). Although we include a detailed model of Calmodulin, which is a major endogenous calcium buffer, the other types are poorly quantified experimentally. Instead, we used a parsimonious generic buffer model that represents an aggregate of these largely unknown endogenous buffers. Future iterations of the model could include more detailed versions of these endogenous buffers, for example calbindin (Bartol et al., 2015).
Unlike other calcium-based plasticity models (Graupner and Brunel, 2012) using the dye fluorescence decay as an approximation to calcium decay, our model is based on receptor and ion channel kinetics. Additionally, our model can simulate the dye kinetics as a buffer using (Equation 13) when appropriate. Figure 12 highlights differences between calcium and dye dynamics which is affected by the laser-induced temperature increase (Wells et al., 2007; Deng et al., 2014). We estimated the calcium reversal potential for the calcium fluxes using the Goldman–Hodgkin–Katz (GHK) flux equation described in Hille, 1978. The calcium ion permeability, , was used as a free parameter adjusting a single EPSP to produce a calcium amplitude of ∼3 µM (Chang et al., 2017). This free scaling is needed to compensate for the fact that that GHK equation is derived for a model that assumes ionic currents pass through the membrane as a distributed and continuous flux, rather that the ion channels we modelled as having discrete conductance levels. Although this adaptation implies that we are using the magnitude of the GHK flux in a phenomenological way, it nevertheless captures the nonlinear dependence of relative calcium flux on extracellular calcium concentration.
where is used to determine the calcium influx through NMDAr and VGCC in the Equation 15, Equation 16, Equation 17 and Equation 18 using the spine membrane voltage and calcium internal concentration . Note that for simplicity the calcium external concentration was kept fixed during the simulation and only altered by experimental conditions given by the aCSF composition.
NMDAr - GluN2A and GluN2B
Markov chain
Request a detailed protocolIn hippocampus, NMDArs are principally heteromers composed of the obligatory subunit GluN1 and either the GluN2A or GluN2B subunits. These N2 subunits determine the kinetics of these receptors, with the GluN1/GLUN2B heteromers displaying slow kinetics (∼250 ms) and the GluN1/GluN2A heteromers displaying faster kinetics (∼50 ms). We modeled both NMDA subtypes. The NMDAr containing GluN2A is modeled with the following Markov chain (Popescu et al., 2004):
where we have introduced the additional parameters to account for temperature dependence (below).
The NMDAr containing GluN2B is modeled with a Markov chain based on the above GluN2A scheme. We decreased the rates by ∼75% in order to match the GluN2B decay at 25°C as published in Iacobucci and Popescu, 2018.
The different rates are given in Table 7.
NMDAr and age switch
Request a detailed protocolThe age-dependent expression ratio of the subtypes GluN2A and GluN2B () was obtained from experimental data of mouse hippocampus (Sinclair et al., 2016). We added noise to this ratio causing ∼1 NMDAr subunit to flip towards GluN2A or GluN2B (see Figure 13e). The population of 15 total NMDArs is divided in the two subtypes according to the ratio plotted in Figure 13b, as a function of age. The ratio to define the number NMDAr subtypes as function of age reads:
The round term in the two previous equations ensures that we have an integer value for the NMDAr subtypes, making the stair shaped curve seen in Figure 13e.
NMDAr and temperature
Request a detailed protocolWe adjusted the GluN2A and GluN2B forward and backward rates to follow the temperature effects on NMDAr-mediated EPSPs (Korinek et al., 2010), see Figure 13a and d. Because GluN2B dominates the NMDAr-mediated EPSP, we fit the GluN2B decay time to data on the NMDAr-mediated EPSP,w as function of temperature as reported by Korinek et al., 2010 using logistic functions and . The decay time comparison is shown in Figure 13a. Then, we applied the same temperature factor and for GluN2A. The decay times of GluN2A and GluN2B are similar to those reported by Iacobucci and Popescu, 2018. The forward and backward factors are described as follows:
NMDAr current and Ca2+-dependent conductance
Request a detailed protocolNMDAr conductance is modulated by external calcium and is modelled according to the next equations using NMDAr subconductances and (GluN2A), and and (GluN2B).
We modified the conductance as a function of extracellular calcium from that reported by Maki and Popescu, 2014. The reported NMDAr conductance at [Ca2+]o = 1.8 mM is . Here, we used the higher conductance for NMDAr (for both subtypes) at [Ca2+]o = 1.8 mM to compensate for the small number of NMDArs reported by Nimchinsky et al., 2004. Hence, we adjusted the Maki and Popescu, 2014 data to take into account this constraint: this caused a rightward-shift in the NMDA-conductance curve (Figure 13c). The calcium influx is modulated by the GHK factor, Equation 14, as a function of the internal and external calcium concentrations and the spine voltage:
The combined effect of extracellular Magnesium (Jahr and Stevens, 1990) and Calcium concentration are displayed in Figure 13f.
GABA(A) receptor
Since the precise delay of GABA release relative to glutamate is not known, we assumed GABA and glutamate release are synchronized for simplicity (see Table 5). We used the GABA(A) receptor Markov chain (Figure 14) presented in Busch and Sakmann, 1990; Destexhe et al., 1998 and we estimated temperature adaptations using the measurements reported by Otis and Mody, 1992. Table 8 presents the GABAr model parameters.
GABA(A)r and temperature
Request a detailed protocolBecause the amplitude of GABA(A) current is altered by the GABAr reversal potential shift during development (Rinetti-Vargas et al., 2017), we applied temperature changes only to the closing rates using a logistic function for , estimated by fitting to the measurements from Otis and Mody, 1992 (data comparison in the Figure 15b and e).
GABA(A)r current and age switch
Request a detailed protocolThe GABA(A)r-driven current changes during development (Meredith et al., 2003) passing from depolarizing (excitatory) to hyperpolarizing (inhibitory) (Chamma et al., 2012). The reversal potential of chloride ions permeating GABA(A)r shifts from above the membrane resting potential (inward driving force - excitatory) to below the membrane resting potential (outward driving force - inhibitory; Rinetti-Vargas et al., 2017). This effect mediated is associated with the KCC2 pump (K Cl co-transporter) which becomes efficient in extruding chloride ions during maturation (Rinetti-Vargas et al., 2017). To account for the GABA(A)r age-dependent shift, we fit a function for the chloride reversal potential () to the data published by Rinetti-Vargas et al., 2017 (Figure 15c):
VGCC - T, R, and L type
Markov chain
Request a detailed protocolA stochastic VGCC model was devised using the channel gating measurements from rat CA1 (2–8 weeks) pyramidal neurons by Magee and Johnston, 1995 at room temperature. Our model has three different VGCC subtypes described by the Markov chains in Figure 16: the T-type (low-voltage), the R-type (medium-to-high-voltage) and the L-type (high-voltage).
The VGCC Markov chain models were derived from voltage activation and inactivation profiles reported in Magee and Johnston, 1995. The T- (Figure 17a and d) and R-type (Figure 17b and e) models are composed of independent activation (m) and inactivation (h) gating variables, while the L-type (Figure 17c) model has one closed state but two open states, to capture the two timescales of channel closing kinetics reported by Magee and Johnston, 1995. The VGCC model equations are given below. We used the following the units: and .
VGCC and temperature
Request a detailed protocolWe used the same temperature factor for every VGCC subtype, respectively and (see Figure 17f), as follows:
The VGCC subtypes have different sensitivities to temperature, with temperature factors for decay times ranging from 2 (Iftinca et al., 2006) to 50-fold (Peloquin et al., 2008). T-type isoforms further complicate the issue: different isoforms can have temperature factors that either speed up or slow down the kinetics. For instance, when passing from room to physiological temperatures, the Ca3.3 isoform has a closing rate ∼50% faster (Iftinca et al., 2006), but the Ca3.1 isoform becomes ∼15% slower. For simplicity in our model, the same temperature factor was adopted for all VGCC subtypes.
VGCC currents
Request a detailed protocolThe VGCC currents are estimated using the GHK (Equation 14), as follows:
Table 9 presents the parameters to model the VGCC channels. VGCC rates and temperature factors are shown in Figure 17.
SK channel
Request a detailed protocolThe small potassium (SK) channel produces hyperpolarizing currents which are enhanced in the presence of intracellular calcium elevations. We included SK channels to incorporate a key negative feedback loop between spine calcium and voltage due to the tight coupling that exists between SK channels and NMDArs (Adelman et al., 2012; Griffith et al., 2016). Although there are a few publications on the single channel recording of SK channels (Hirschberg et al., 1998; Hirschberg et al., 1999) and at least one stochastic model of SK channel (Stanley et al., 2011), we chose to model SK channels deterministically. In tests, we found that this assumption had only a negligible impact on the outcomes of plasticity protocols (data not shown). Although SK channels can additionally be regulated by metabotropic glutamate receptors and muscarinic receptors (Tigaret et al., 2016), we did not include these regulatory steps in the model. The SK channel current was based on the description from Griffith et al., 2016 as follows:
There is little information on how temperature effects SK channel function, but van Herck et al., 2018 suggests a left-ward shift in the SK half-activation when changing from 37°C () to 25°C (); that is a 65% decrease. Thus, to mimic temperature dependence of SK, we decrease the decay time of the SK hyperpolarizing current by a factor of two when passing from physiological to room temperature.
Table 10 presents the parameters to model the SK channel.
Enzymes - CaM, CaN, and CaMKII
To model enzyme dynamics, we adapted a monomeric CaM-CaMKII Markov chain from Chang et al., 2019 which was built on the model by Pepke et al., 2010. Our adaptation incorporates a simplified CaN reaction which only binds to fully saturated CaM, i.e. CaM bound to four calcium ions on its N and C terminals (see Markov chain in Figure 18). A consequence of the Pepke coarse-grained model is that calcium binds and unbinds simultaneously from the CaM terminals (N,C). We assumed a lack of dephosphorylation reaction between CaMKII and CaN since Otmakhov et al., 2015 experimentally suggested that no known phosphatase affects CaMKII decay time which is probably caused only by CaM untrapping (Otmakhov et al., 2015). This was previously theorized in the Michalski’s model Michalski, 2013, and it is reflected in Chang data (Chang et al., 2019; Chang et al., 2017). The structure of the corresponding Markov chain is shown in Figure 18.
Chang et al., 2019 data provides a high-temporal resolution fluorescence measurements for CaMKII in dendritic spines of rat CA1 pyramidal neurons and advances the description of CaMKII self-phosphorylation (at room temperature). We modified Chang’s model of CaMKII unbinding rates to fit CaMKII dynamics at room/physiological temperature as shown by Chang et al., 2017 supplemental files. Previous modelling of CaMKII Chang et al., 2019; Pepke et al., 2010 used a stereotyped waveform with no adaptation to model calcium. Our contribution to CaMKII modelling was to use calcium dynamics sensitive to the experimental conditions to reproduce CaMKII data, therefore, allowing us to capture physiological temperature measurements from Chang et al., 2017. Note that the CaMKII dynamic has two time scales and we capture only the fastest timescale which ends after stimulation ceases (at 60 s). The slowest dynamic occurs at the end of the stimulus, close to the maximum (Figure 19a). This may be caused by the transient volume increase in the dendritic spine as measured by Chang et al., 2017. Table 11 shows the concentration of the enzymes and Table 12 shows the parameters to model enzymes reactions in shown in Figure 18.
We provide an example of equation describing the binding reaction associated to the state . Note that these equations are automatically generated by the code which implements Table 12.
The CaN concentration was chosen as the total concentration used in a previous model (Stefan et al., 2008) (1.6 μM) scaled by a factor of 12 due to a higher CaN concentration in dendritic spines (Goto et al., 1986; Baumgärtel and Mansuy, 2012) and taking into account the discrepancy between different CaN concentration studies (Kuno et al., 1992; Goto et al., 1986). Kuno et al., 1992 proposes 9.6 μg/mg (7.0+2.6 μg/mg for Aα and Aβ isoforms) for the catalytic subunit A of CaN (CaNA) in the hippocampus, while Goto et al., 1986 proposes 1.45 μg/mg (presumably for both isoforms). There is therefore a lack of consensus on CaN concentration in neurons, which seems to range between 1 and 10 μg/mg. However, models of CaN in spines (Stefan et al., 2008) use low values of CaN concentration (e.g. 1.6 μM), without adjusting for the fact that these values were estimated from measurements from the entire neuropil. There is little information on CaN concentration in spines, but Kuno et al., 1992 note that the concentration of CaN is 50% to 84% higher in synaptosomes than in neuronal nuclei. With this information in mind, we set CaN spine concentration 20 μM in our model. CaN was entirely activated through CaM for the following reason: CaNA is activated by calcium-CaM in a highly cooperative manner (Hill coefficient 2.8–3), whereas the activation of CaN by calcium (via CaNB) is at most 10% of that achieved with CaM (Stemmer and Klee, 1994). In other words, CaNA affinity for CaM is 16 nM to 26 pM (Creamer, 2020), while CaNB affinity for calcium ranges from 15 μM to 24 nM (Kakalis et al., 1995). CaN decay time was modeled using experimental spine CaN activity dynamics measured in Fujii et al., 2013.
The lack of reactions between CaN and CaMKII
Request a detailed protocolThe protein phosphatases responsible for CaMKII dephosphorylation have not been established unequivocally (Lisman, 1989). Our model of CaMKII is based directly on a quantitative model fit to FRET imaging data (Chang et al., 2017; Chang et al., 2019), which implicitly account for the effects of any ‘hidden’ phosphatases, absorbing their contribution into the decay rates of the CaMKII activity. As pointed out by Otmakhov et al., 2015, FRET sensor imaging of CaMKII activity unfortunately does not capture the identity of the phosphatases involved in the dephosphorylation of CaMKII. More specifically, Otmakhov et al., 2015 observed no significant changes in the decay constant of their CaMKII FRET sensor when selectively inhibiting PP1 and PP2A. Given that these two phosphatases are widely used in models to determine plasticity, we believe that our model is more aligned with data of CaMKII activity in vivo.
Yet, our decision to include CaN in the model was determined by the evidence supporting CaN as the strongest candidate for calcium-sensitive protein phosphatase in the brain (Baumgärtel and Mansuy, 2012). Furthermore, the central role of CaN in synaptic plasticity has been demonstrated both pharmacologically and with genetic manipulation (Onuma et al., 1998; Malleret et al., 2001).
Temperature effects on enzymatic activity
Request a detailed protocolWe included temperature factors in the coarse-grained model using Chang’s data (Chang et al., 2019), as shown in Figure 19. For CaMKII, we fit the modified dissociation rates of the phosphorylation states k2, k3, and k5 to match the data on relative amplitude and decay time using the following logistic function:
For CaN, we fit the Fujii et al., 2013 data at 25°C as seen in Figure 20a. However, since CaN-CaM dissociation rates at physiological temperatures were not reported, we set the temperature factor to CaN that fits the outcomes of the protocols that we aimed to reproduce. A reference value from the CaN-AKAP79 complex (Li et al., 2012) showed a , which is nearly the temperature factor used in our model for CaN. Therefore, both the association and dissociation rates are modified using the following logistic functions:
Positioning of the plasticity regions
Request a detailed protocolTigaret et al., 2016 LTP protocols were used to set the LTP region and as a first approximation of the LTD region. See Figure 21 TopDudek and Bear, 1992, Dudek and Bear, 1993 and Inglebert et al., 2020 were used to further define the LTD region. See Figure 21 Middle and Bottom. We highlight further a few points. For simplicity, we positioned the right border of the LTD region at the left border of the LTP region. The bottom part of the geometrical readout, under 4 μM of CaMKII, does not code for any dynamics. Note that some protocols may also enter and leave the plasticity regions multiple times, for example, TBS in Figure 5b, protocols in between LTD/LTP region in Figure 3d and age related LTD in Figure 5f. Because of this, we created an integrate and leak variable instead of using the time spent for predicting plasticity (see next section). This way, only after a certain time spent in a region would the synaptic weights start to change, as in Figure 23h. The coordinates of the plasticity regions are given in the last two rows of Table 13.
Geometrical readout
Request a detailed protocolWe describe here the geometrical readout mechanism which allows for plasticity outcome assignment. First, we define the following variables which are representative of ‘active CaMKII’ and ‘active CaN’:
The first two equations of (20) represent the total activation of K and P components of CaMKII associated with CaM as defined in the code that accompanies Chang et al., 2019. The last equation is from Chang et al., 2019.
Calcium entry in the spine initiates a cascade of events that ultimately leads to long term plasticity changes. Specific concentrations of CaMKII and CaN trigger activation functions and when they belong to one of the two polygonal regions (P and D), termed plasticity regions in the main text:
The variables and act as low pass filters of CaMKII and CaN activities with some memory of previous passages in the respective plasticity regions. To specify the LTP/LTD rates, termed and , we use the activation functions, and , as follows:
The Markov plasticity chain (see Figure 22) starts with initial conditions , and .
Table 13 provides the parameters that define the boundaries of the plasticity regions (see Figure 21 and Figure 23b).
Figure 23 shows how the readout works to predict plasticity for a single orbit. Figure 23a shows the enzyme’s activity alone which is combined to form an orbit as shown in Figure 23b. The region indicator of the respective orbit is shown in Figure 23c. Simultaneously, Figure 23d depicts the leaky activation and , which will define the rate of plasticity induction in Figure 23e and f. The rates in the plasticity Markov chain will not reset to 0 if the orbit leaves the readout. The plasticity Markov chain is shown in Figure 23g with the prediction outcome represented as a weight change (%). Figure 23h shows the rate, and , activation profile. The LTP activation rate is steep, meaning that orbits do not need to spend a long time inside the readout to promote LTP induction, while the LTD region requires five-fold longer activation times.
Appendix 1
Data availability
The datasets at the basis of our model were obtained directly from the authors Tigaret et al., 2016 or extracted from graphs in the references in Appendix 1—table 1 using WebPlotDigitizer v 4.6 software (Rohatgi,A.)The dataset from Tigaret et al., 2016 is freely available upon request. Concerning the dataset Tigaret et al., 2016, Jack Mellor the person to contact. One can contact Cezar M. Tigaret as well. The model is available on GitHub at https://github.com/rveltz/SynapseElife (copy archived at Veltz, 2023).
References
-
Biophysical model of synaptic plasticity dynamicsBiological Cybernetics 89:214–226.https://doi.org/10.1007/s00422-003-0422-x
-
Small-Conductance Ca2+-activated K+ channels: form and functionAnnual Review of Physiology 74:245–269.https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-physiol-020911-153336
-
Probing the interplay between dendritic spine morphology and membrane-bound diffusionBiophysical Journal 113:2261–2270.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpj.2017.06.048
-
Synaptic vesicle pools and dynamicsCold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Biology 4:a013680.https://doi.org/10.1101/cshperspect.a013680
-
A stochastic signaling network mediates the probabilistic induction of cerebellar long-term depressionThe Journal of Neuroscience 32:9288–9300.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5976-11.2012
-
Stochastic induction of long-term potentiation and long-term depressionScientific Reports 6:30899.https://doi.org/10.1038/srep30899
-
Biophysical and phenomenological models of multiple spike interactions in spike-timing dependent plasticityInternational Journal of Neural Systems 16:79–97.https://doi.org/10.1142/S0129065706000524
-
Structural components of synaptic plasticity and memory consolidationCold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Biology 7:a021758.https://doi.org/10.1101/cshperspect.a021758
-
Computational reconstitution of spine calcium transients from individual proteinsFrontiers in Synaptic Neuroscience 7:17.https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsyn.2015.00017
-
Neural functions of calcineurin in synaptic plasticity and memory: Figure 1Learning & Memory 19:375–384.https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.027201.112
-
Molecular mechanisms underlying striatal synaptic plasticity: relevance to chronic alcohol consumption and seekingThe European Journal of Neuroscience 49:768–783.https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.13919
-
A model of spatial MAP formation in the hippocampus of the ratNeural Computation 8:85–93.https://doi.org/10.1162/neco.1996.8.1.85
-
The low synaptic release probability in vivoTrends in Neurosciences 33:259–266.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2010.03.003
-
ConferenceSynaptic transmission in hippocampal neurons: numerical reconstruction of quantal iPSCsCold Spring Harbor Symposia on Quantitative Biology. pp. 69–80.https://doi.org/10.1101/sqb.1990.055.01.009
-
Effect of stochastic synaptic and dendritic dynamics on synaptic plasticity in visual cortex and hippocampusJournal of Neurophysiology 97:375–386.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00895.2006
-
Role of the neuronal K-Cl co-transporter KCC2 in inhibitory and excitatory neurotransmissionFrontiers in Cellular Neuroscience 6:5.https://doi.org/10.3389/fncel.2012.00005
-
Multivesicular release at Schaffer collateral–ca1 hippocampal synapsesThe Journal of Neuroscience 26:210–216.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4307-05.2006
-
Connectivity reflects coding: a model of voltage-based STDP with homeostasisNature Neuroscience 13:344–352.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2479
-
Voltage and spike timing interact in STDP-a unified modelFrontiers in Synaptic Neuroscience 2:25.https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsyn.2010.00025
-
Robustness of STDP to spike timing jitterScientific Reports 8:1–15.https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-26436-y
-
Optogenetics, the intersection between physics and neuroscience: light stimulation of neurons in physiological conditionsAmerican Journal of Physiology. Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology 307:R1292–R1302.https://doi.org/10.1152/ajpregu.00072.2014
-
Short-Term depression and long-term plasticity together tune sensitive range of synaptic plasticityPLOS Computational Biology 16:e1008265.https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1008265
-
Synthesis of models for excitable membranes, synaptic transmission and neuromodulation using a common kinetic formalismJournal of Computational Neuroscience 1:195–230.https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00961734
-
BookKinetic models of synaptic transmissionIn: Koch C, Segev I, editors. Methods in Neuronal Modeling. MIT Press. pp. 1–25.
-
Bidirectional long-term modification of synaptic effectiveness in the adult and immature hippocampusThe Journal of Neuroscience 13:2910–2918.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-07-02910.1993
-
Synaptic integrationEncyclopedia of LifeSciences 1:a0000208.https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470015902.a0000208.pub2
-
Dynamics of self-organized delay adaptationPhysical Review Letters 82:1594–1597.https://doi.org/10.1103/PhysRevLett.82.1594
-
Dynamic modulation of spike timing-dependent calcium influx during corticostriatal upstatesJournal of Neurophysiology 110:1631–1645.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00232.2013
-
Gain control in CA1 pyramidal cells using changes in somatic conductanceThe Journal of Neuroscience 30:230–241.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3995-09.2010
-
Distinct coincidence detectors govern the corticostriatal spike timing-dependent plasticityThe Journal of Physiology 588:3045–3062.https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.2010.188466
-
Dichotomy of action-potential backpropagation in CA1 pyramidal neuron dendritesJournal of Neurophysiology 86:2998–3010.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.2001.86.6.2998
-
Natural firing patterns imply low sensitivity of synaptic plasticity to spike timing compared with firing rateThe Journal of Neuroscience 36:11238–11258.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0104-16.2016
-
Back-Propagation of physiological action potential output in dendrites of slender-tufted l5a pyramidal neuronsFrontiers in Cellular Neuroscience 4:13.https://doi.org/10.3389/fncel.2010.00013
-
Control of Ca2+ influx and calmodulin activation by sk-channels in dendritic spinesPLOS Computational Biology 12:e1004949.https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004949
-
Universally sloppy parameter sensitivities in systems biology modelsPLOS Computational Biology 3:30189.https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.0030189
-
Developmental mapping of small-conductance calcium-activated potassium channel expression in the rat nervous systemThe Journal of Comparative Neurology 522:1072–1101.https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.23466
-
BookThe Organization of Behavior: A Neuropsychological TheoryAmerican Psychological Association.https://doi.org/10.4324/9781410612403
-
Calcium signaling in dendritic spinesCold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Biology 4:a005686.https://doi.org/10.1101/cshperspect.a005686
-
The neuron simulation environmentNeural Computation 9:1179–1209.https://doi.org/10.1162/neco.1997.9.6.1179
-
Gating of recombinant small-conductance Ca-activated K+ channels by calciumThe Journal of General Physiology 111:565–581.https://doi.org/10.1085/jgp.111.4.565
-
Gating properties of single SK channels in hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neuronsBiophysical Journal 77:1905–1913.https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3495(99)77032-3
-
Nmda receptors: linking physiological output to biophysical operationNature Reviews. Neuroscience 18:236–249.https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2017.24
-
Kinetic models for activation and modulation of NMDA receptor subtypesCurrent Opinion in Physiology 2:114–122.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cophys.2018.02.002
-
Hippocampal place cell firing patterns can induce long-term synaptic plasticity in vitroThe Journal of Neuroscience 29:6840–6850.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0731-09.2009
-
A quantitative description of NMDA receptor-channel kinetic behaviorThe Journal of Neuroscience 10:1830–1837.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.10-06-01830.1990
-
Calcium dynamics predict direction of synaptic plasticity in striatal spiny projection neuronsThe European Journal of Neuroscience 45:1044–1056.https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.13287
-
A model of spike-timing dependent plasticity: one or two coincidence detectors?Journal of Neurophysiology 88:507–513.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.2002.88.1.507
-
Extracellular calcium depletion as a mechanism of short-term synaptic depressionJournal of Neurophysiology 85:1952–1959.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.2001.85.5.1952
-
Frequency-Dependent changes in nmdar-dependent synaptic plasticityFrontiers in Computational Neuroscience 5:38.https://doi.org/10.3389/fncom.2011.00038
-
Distinct cellular expression of calcineurin A alpha and a beta in rat brainJournal of Neurochemistry 58:1643–1651.https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-4159.1992.tb10036.x
-
Attenuation of synaptic potentials in dendritic spinesCell Reports 20:1100–1110.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2017.07.012
-
Learning rules for spike timing-dependent plasticity depend on dendritic synapse locationThe Journal of Neuroscience 26:10420–10429.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2650-06.2006
-
Balanced interactions of calcineurin with AKAP79 regulate ca2+–calcineurin–nfat signalingNature Structural & Molecular Biology 19:337–345.https://doi.org/10.1038/nsmb.2238
-
Sk2 channel plasticity contributes to LTP at Schaffer collateral-CA1 synapsesNature Neuroscience 11:170–177.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn2041
-
Kinetic properties of the GABAA receptor main conductance state of mouse spinal cord neurones in cultureThe Journal of Physiology 410:479–499.https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1989.sp017545
-
Extracellular Ca (2+) ions reduce NMDA receptor conductance and gatingThe Journal of General Physiology 144:379–392.https://doi.org/10.1085/jgp.201411244
-
Variability, compensation, and modulation in neurons and circuitsPNAS 108 Suppl 3:15542–15548.https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1010674108
-
Multiple models to capture the variability in biological neurons and networksNature Neuroscience 14:133–138.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2735
-
A history of spike-timing-dependent plasticityFrontiers in Synaptic Neuroscience 3:4.https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsyn.2011.00004
-
Small conductance Ca2+-activated K+ channels and calmodulinThe Journal of Physiology 554:255–261.https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.2003.049072
-
Rate and pulse based plasticity governed by local synaptic state variablesFrontiers in Synaptic Neuroscience 2:33.https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsyn.2010.00033
-
Maturation of long-term potentiation induction rules in rodent hippocampus: role of GABAergic inhibitionThe Journal of Neuroscience 23:11142–11146.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-35-11142.2003
-
The delicate bistability of CaMKIIBiophysical Journal 105:794–806.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpj.2013.06.038
-
Role of an A-type K+ conductance in the back-propagation of action potentials in the dendrites of hippocampal pyramidal neuronsJournal of Computational Neuroscience 7:5–15.https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1008906225285
-
Spine Ca2+ signaling in spike-timing-dependent plasticityThe Journal of Neuroscience 26:11001–11013.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1749-06.2006
-
Sk channels and NMDA receptors form a Ca2+-mediated feedback loop in dendritic spinesNature Neuroscience 8:642–649.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1449
-
The number of glutamate receptors opened by synaptic stimulation in single hippocampal spinesThe Journal of Neuroscience 24:2054–2064.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5066-03.2004
-
Dissection of bidirectional synaptic plasticity into saturable unidirectional processesJournal of Neurophysiology 94:1565–1573.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00047.2005
-
Systematic analysis of the contributions of stochastic voltage gated channels to neuronal noiseFrontiers in Computational Neuroscience 8:105.https://doi.org/10.3389/fncom.2014.00105
-
Facilitation at single synapses probed with optical quantal analysisNature Neuroscience 5:657–664.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn867
-
Subcellular location of PKA controls striatal plasticity: stochastic simulations in spiny dendritesPLOS Computational Biology 8:e1002383.https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002383
-
Dopamine receptor activation is required for corticostriatal spike-timing-dependent plasticityThe Journal of Neuroscience 28:2435–2446.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4402-07.2008
-
Triplets of spikes in a model of spike timing-dependent plasticityThe Journal of Neuroscience 26:9673–9682.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1425-06.2006
-
Electrical behaviour of dendritic spines as revealed by voltage imagingNature Communications 6:8436.https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms9436
-
Kinetics of calmodulin binding to calcineurinBiochemical and Biophysical Research Communications 334:674–680.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbrc.2005.06.152
-
Nmda receptor content of synapses in stratum radiatum of the hippocampal CA1 areaThe Journal of Neuroscience 20:2512–2522.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-07-02512.2000
-
From the stochasticity of molecular processes to the variability of synaptic transmissionNature Reviews. Neuroscience 12:375–387.https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3025
-
How AMPA receptor desensitization depends on receptor occupancyThe Journal of Neuroscience 23:847–858.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-03-00847.2003
-
Spike timing-dependent long-term depression requires presynaptic NMDA receptorsNature Neuroscience 11:744–745.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2125
-
Calcium time course as a signal for spike-timing-dependent plasticityJournal of Neurophysiology 93:2600–2613.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00803.2004
-
The ubiquitous nature of multivesicular releaseTrends in Neurosciences 38:428–438.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2015.05.008
-
A friend or foe: calcineurin across the gamut of neurological disordersACS Central Science 4:805–819.https://doi.org/10.1021/acscentsci.8b00230
-
Endoplasmic reticulum calcium stores in dendritic spinesFrontiers in Neuroanatomy 8:64.https://doi.org/10.3389/fnana.2014.00064
-
A biophysical basis for the inter-spike interaction of spike-timing-dependent plasticityBiological Cybernetics 95:113–121.https://doi.org/10.1007/s00422-006-0071-y
-
The stochastic nature of action potential backpropagation in apical tuft dendritesJournal of Neurophysiology 118:1394–1414.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00800.2016
-
Stochastic properties of synaptic transmission affect the shape of spike time-dependent plasticity curvesJournal of Neurophysiology 93:1069–1073.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00504.2004
-
Effects of sex and DTNBP1 (dysbindin) null gene mutation on the developmental glun2b-glun2a switch in the mouse cortex and hippocampusJournal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders 8:14.https://doi.org/10.1186/s11689-016-9148-7
-
Competitive Hebbian learning through spike-timing-dependent synaptic plasticityNature Neuroscience 3:919–926.https://doi.org/10.1038/78829
-
Dendritic glutamate receptor channels in rat hippocampal CA3 and CA1 pyramidal neuronsThe Journal of Physiology 482:325–352.https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1995.sp020521
-
Stochastic amplification of calcium-activated potassium currents in Ca2+ microdomainsJournal of Computational Neuroscience 31:647–666.https://doi.org/10.1007/s10827-011-0328-x
-
BookPrinciples of Computational Modelling in NeuroscienceCambridge University Press.https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511975899
-
Different modes of expression of AMPA and NMDA receptors in hippocampal synapsesNature Neuroscience 2:618–624.https://doi.org/10.1038/10172
-
Impact of functional synapse clusters on neuronal response selectivityNature Communications 11:1413.https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-15147-6
-
Probability of transmitter release at neocortical synapses at different temperaturesJournal of Neurophysiology 92:212–220.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.01166.2003
-
Priming-Induced shift in synaptic plasticity in the rat hippocampusJournal of Neurophysiology 82:2024–2028.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1999.82.4.2024
-
Coactivation and timing-dependent integration of synaptic potentiation and depressionNature Neuroscience 8:187–193.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1387
-
Location-Dependent synaptic plasticity rules by dendritic spine cooperativityNature Communications 7:11380.https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms11380
-
Biophysical mechanisms of transient optical stimulation of peripheral nerveBiophysical Journal 93:2567–2580.https://doi.org/10.1529/biophysj.107.104786
-
Malleability of spike-timing-dependent plasticity at the CA3-CA1 synapseThe Journal of Neuroscience 26:6610–6617.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5388-05.2006
-
Selective induction of LTP and LTD by postsynaptic [ Ca2+ ] i elevationJournal of Neurophysiology 81:781–787.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1999.81.2.781
-
The effect of noise on CaMKII activation in a dendritic spine during LTP inductionJournal of Neurophysiology 103:1798–1808.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.91235.2008
-
Morphometric analysis of hippocampal pyramidal neurons in situ and in grafts developing in the anterior eye chambers of young and aged Wistar ratsJournal of Neural Transplantation & Plasticity 6:49–57.https://doi.org/10.1155/NP.1997.49
Article and author information
Author details
Funding
Medical Research Council (MR/V034111/1)
- Cezar M Tigaret
University Côte d'Azur (ComputaBrain Idex UCA Jedi)
- Yuri Elias Rodrigues
- Hélène Marie
- Romain Veltz
Medical Research Council (MR/S026630/1)
- Cian O'Donnell
Leverhulme Trust (RPG-2019-229)
- Cian O'Donnell
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Copyright
© 2023, Rodrigues et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 674
- views
-
- 94
- downloads
-
- 6
- citations
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Neuroscience
Complex macro-scale patterns of brain activity that emerge during periods of wakeful rest provide insight into the organisation of neural function, how these differentiate individuals based on their traits, and the neural basis of different types of self-generated thoughts. Although brain activity during wakeful rest is valuable for understanding important features of human cognition, its unconstrained nature makes it difficult to disentangle neural features related to personality traits from those related to the thoughts occurring at rest. Our study builds on recent perspectives from work on ongoing conscious thought that highlight the interactions between three brain networks – ventral and dorsal attention networks, as well as the default mode network. We combined measures of personality with state-of-the-art indices of ongoing thoughts at rest and brain imaging analysis and explored whether this ‘tri-partite’ view can provide a framework within which to understand the contribution of states and traits to observed patterns of neural activity at rest. To capture macro-scale relationships between different brain systems, we calculated cortical gradients to describe brain organisation in a low-dimensional space. Our analysis established that for more introverted individuals, regions of the ventral attention network were functionally more aligned to regions of the somatomotor system and the default mode network. At the same time, a pattern of detailed self-generated thought was associated with a decoupling of regions of dorsal attention from regions in the default mode network. Our study, therefore, establishes that interactions between attention systems and the default mode network are important influences on ongoing thought at rest and highlights the value of integrating contemporary perspectives on conscious experience when understanding patterns of brain activity at rest.
-
- Neuroscience
Gamma oscillations in brain activity (30–150 Hz) have been studied for over 80 years. Although in the past three decades significant progress has been made to try to understand their functional role, a definitive answer regarding their causal implication in perception, cognition, and behavior still lies ahead of us. Here, we first review the basic neural mechanisms that give rise to gamma oscillations and then focus on two main pillars of exploration. The first pillar examines the major theories regarding their functional role in information processing in the brain, also highlighting critical viewpoints. The second pillar reviews a novel research direction that proposes a therapeutic role for gamma oscillations, namely the gamma entrainment using sensory stimulation (GENUS). We extensively discuss both the positive findings and the issues regarding reproducibility of GENUS. Going beyond the functional and therapeutic role of gamma, we propose a third pillar of exploration, where gamma, generated endogenously by cortical circuits, is essential for maintenance of healthy circuit function. We propose that four classes of interneurons, namely those expressing parvalbumin (PV), vasointestinal peptide (VIP), somatostatin (SST), and nitric oxide synthase (NOS) take advantage of endogenous gamma to perform active vasomotor control that maintains homeostasis in the neuronal tissue. According to this hypothesis, which we call GAMER (GAmma MEdiated ciRcuit maintenance), gamma oscillations act as a ‘servicing’ rhythm that enables efficient translation of neural activity into vascular responses that are essential for optimal neurometabolic processes. GAMER is an extension of GENUS, where endogenous rather than entrained gamma plays a fundamental role. Finally, we propose several critical experiments to test the GAMER hypothesis.