Hippocampus: Connecting to the long axis
Few questions in neuroscience generate as much debate and interest as: “what does the hippocampus do?”. This small, cylinder-like structure is embedded in each brain hemisphere, where it participates in spatial navigation, episodic memory, emotional responses and other cognitive functions. Much of what is known about the hippocampus has come from studying the organization of a complex neural circuit along its short, transverse axis (Marr, 1971). However, a promising avenue to understand how the hippocampus can accommodate its multiple functions is to study its long axis, which extends from its head (anterior) to its tail (posterior).
Reports showing the rodent equivalent of the hippocampal head and tail carrying out distinct roles can be traced back over 50 years (Nadel, 1968). Furthermore, differences in neural activity have been observed across the anterior-posterior axis of the human brain at rest and during task-based activities (Vogel et al., 2020; Strange et al., 1999). Recent studies have shown that distinct regions along the long axis of the hippocampus may control specific cognitive behaviours (Strange et al., 2014). Yet, little is known about how this functional organisation is tied to anatomical connections in the human brain – that is, how areas along the anterior-posterior axis connect to the distant cortical regions associated with the relevant cognitive process. Now, in eLife, Marshall Dalton and colleagues from the University of Sydney report new findings that help to answer this question (Dalton et al., 2022).
The team used a quantitative analysis protocol to analyse images of the human brain captured using diffusion-weighted magnetic resonance imaging (Smith et al., 2015). This non-invasive technique provided a biologically meaningful estimate of the number of nerve fibres, or connectivity, between the hippocampus and various areas in the cortex. Dalton et al. also refined the approach to see where fibres from these distant cortical regions preferentially projected to within the hippocampus.
This revealed that the posterior hippocampus has multiple connections with the primary and early visual cortex and the medial parietal cortex, and this connectivity gradually decreases towards the head of the hippocampus. By contrast, the anterior hippocampus was more strongly linked to the temporal pole and lateral temporal cortex, with only a small number of nerve fibres from these cortical areas projecting to the hippocampal tail. Some regions in the hippocampus received inputs from multiple cortical areas. This is in keeping with other models which also show discrete areas of connectivity between the hippocampus and specific brain areas, and other cortical connections which gradually change in density further along the anterior-posterior axis (Strange et al., 2014; Figure 1).
Dalton et al. found that the connection between the hippocampus and the visual system is much stronger in humans than in rodents (the species from which most insights about the anterior-posterior axis have derived). Emerging evidence suggests that, in primates, the homologues of neuronal activity related to spatial self-location in rodents (e.g. place or grid cell activity) may in fact be view or grid-like responses to visual space (Nau et al., 2018; Rolls and Wirth, 2018). Thus, the finding of Dalton et al. that the human visual cortex and hippocampus are more strongly connected than has been appreciated in rodents is an important discovery. Furthermore, connections with the early visual cortex were higher in the posterior hippocampus, a section which, in primates, is thought to have a more prominent role in spatial navigation than the anterior portion. As certain regions in the early visual cortex receive inputs from particular parts of the retina, the findings from Dalton et al. raise the intriguing question of whether this ‘retinotopic’ information is also relayed to the posterior hippocampus.
As with all studies, there are some limitations. For instance, it is possible that the connections between the hippocampus and the visual areas which Dalton et al. report are, at least in part, a fusion of fibres within a well-known tract that links the visual cortex to a structure very close to the body and tail of the hippocampus. Furthermore, some of the results of Dalton et al. differ from non-human primate studies (Insausti and Muñoz, 2001), which could be due to limitations of non-invasive neuronal tracing methods in humans, rather than legitimate differences between species.
Dalton et al. also found that the density of neuron ‘ends’ varied across the transverse axis of the hippocampus. For instance, nerve fibres projecting from the visual and parietal cortex (which process visual and sensory information, respectively) seemed to mostly terminate on the medial region of the posterior hippocampus; whereas, neurons in the temporal lobe (which processes semantic information) tended to connect to the lateral side of the anterior hippocampus. However, it is unclear why this might be and how it impacts cognitive functions – although some clues may emerge from our understanding of the hippocampal transverse axis. It will be important to determine how the hippocampus integrates information from these various cortical areas during cognitive processes, like recalling a memory, given that they project to different places.
Another surprising finding of this study is the relatively low connectivity between the anterior portion of the hippocampus and the medial prefrontal cortex, as this connection is important for a number of brain functions (Barnett et al., 2021), such as forming integrated memories from overlapping experiences (Zeithamova et al., 2012). However, Dalton et al. mention that there is evidence to suggest that the number of nerve fibres directly projecting between the prefrontal cortex and hippocampus is quite sparse in the human brain. Nevertheless, this raises a more fundamental question regarding the extent that anatomically observed connections reflect functional relationships between regions, and whether even a small number of neurons can strongly couple parts of the brain together. Future work using the imaging method adopted by Dalton et al. will be important for answering this question.
References
-
Cortical projections of the non-entorhinal hippocampal formation in the cynomolgus monkey (Macaca fascicularis)The European Journal of Neuroscience 14:435–451.https://doi.org/10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01662.x
-
Simple memory: a theory for archicortexPhilosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences 262:23–81.https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.1971.0078
-
Dorsal and ventral hippocampal lesions and behaviorPhysiology & Behavior 3:891–900.https://doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(68)90174-1
-
Hexadirectional coding of visual space in human entorhinal cortexNature Neuroscience 21:188–190.https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-017-0050-8
-
Functional organization of the hippocampal longitudinal axisNature Reviews 15:655–669.https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3785
Article and author information
Author details
Publication history
Copyright
© 2022, Strange
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 1,383
- views
-
- 154
- downloads
-
- 2
- citations
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Neuroscience
The concept that dimeric protein complexes in synapses can sequentially replace their subunits has been a cornerstone of Francis Crick’s 1984 hypothesis, explaining how long-term memories could be maintained in the face of short protein lifetimes. However, it is unknown whether the subunits of protein complexes that mediate memory are sequentially replaced in the brain and if this process is linked to protein lifetime. We address these issues by focusing on supercomplexes assembled by the abundant postsynaptic scaffolding protein PSD95, which plays a crucial role in memory. We used single-molecule detection, super-resolution microscopy and MINFLUX to probe the molecular composition of PSD95 supercomplexes in mice carrying genetically encoded HaloTags, eGFP, and mEoS2. We found a population of PSD95-containing supercomplexes comprised of two copies of PSD95, with a dominant 12.7 nm separation. Time-stamping of PSD95 subunits in vivo revealed that each PSD95 subunit was sequentially replaced over days and weeks. Comparison of brain regions showed subunit replacement was slowest in the cortex, where PSD95 protein lifetime is longest. Our findings reveal that protein supercomplexes within the postsynaptic density can be maintained by gradual replacement of individual subunits providing a mechanism for stable maintenance of their organization. Moreover, we extend Crick’s model by suggesting that synapses with slow subunit replacement of protein supercomplexes and long-protein lifetimes are specialized for long-term memory storage and that these synapses are highly enriched in superficial layers of the cortex where long-term memories are stored.
-
- Neuroscience
Motivation depends on dopamine, but might be modulated by acetylcholine which influences dopamine release in the striatum, and amplifies motivation in animal studies. A corresponding effect in humans would be important clinically, since anticholinergic drugs are frequently used in Parkinson’s disease, a condition that can also disrupt motivation. Reward and dopamine make us more ready to respond, as indexed by reaction times (RT), and move faster, sometimes termed vigour. These effects may be controlled by preparatory processes that can be tracked using electroencephalography (EEG). We measured vigour in a placebo-controlled, double-blinded study of trihexyphenidyl (THP), a muscarinic antagonist, with an incentivised eye movement task and EEG. Participants responded faster and with greater vigour when incentives were high, but THP blunted these motivational effects, suggesting that muscarinic receptors facilitate invigoration by reward. Preparatory EEG build-up (contingent negative variation [CNV]) was strengthened by high incentives and by muscarinic blockade, although THP reduced the incentive effect. The amplitude of preparatory activity predicted both vigour and RT, although over distinct scalp regions; frontal activity predicted vigour, whereas a larger, earlier, central component predicted RT. The incentivisation of RT was partly mediated by the CNV, though vigour was not. Moreover, the CNV mediated the drug’s effect on dampening incentives, suggesting that muscarinic receptors underlie the motivational influence on this preparatory activity. Taken together, these findings show that a muscarinic blocker impairs motivated action in healthy people, and that medial frontal preparatory neural activity mediates this for RT.