Evolution: Kin selection spreads
Charles Darwin considered the evolution of altruism to be a puzzle (Darwin, 1871). Altruistic acts benefit the recipient but may cost the actor, making it difficult to grasp how they could have evolved in a world where organisms compete to survive. Since individuals who act selfishly should fare better than those with altruistic tendencies, shouldn’t altruism be quickly selected against?
An elegant solution to this problem emerged in the 1930s, when mathematical arguments were laid out to show how genes that enable altruistic behaviors could spread through a population (Fisher, 1930; Haldane, 1932). These ideas were further developed in the 1960s by W.D. ‘Bill’ Hamilton, and later re-derived by George Price (Hamilton, 1964a; Hamilton, 1964b; Price, 1972).
This work showed that if the individual(s) receiving altruistic acts are genetically related to the altruistic actor, and if an altruistic act is sufficiently beneficial to the receivers relative to the costs to the actor, then altruistic behaviors can spread. This process was named ‘kin selection’ (Smith, 1964). A swathe of additional terms were then coined, including ‘indirect fitness’ (the benefits associated with promoting the offspring of relatives) and ‘inclusive fitness’ (the sum total of direct and indirect fitness). These theoretical developments set the stage for thousands of empirical studies of social behavior. Now, in eLife, Marie Charpentier and colleagues in France, Germany and Gabon – including joint first author Clémence Poirotte – report intriguing new evidence which deepens our understanding of how kin selection may take place within complex societies (Charpentier et al., 2022).
The study took place in a free-ranging population of mandrills living in Gabon. These large and colorful ground-dwelling monkeys live in societies structured around groups of closely related females who share a maternal ancestor, with mature males joining the group during the mating season to sire offspring. As for other social mammals, the health and success of an individual is often shaped by their family connections and how well they are integrated in to the community.
Building on their previous work, Charpentier et al. used machine learning to train a human face recognition algorithm on a large database of mandrill face pictures. The program was then used on the study population to show that infants with the same father look alike – more so than unrelated individuals or maternal half-siblings. Long-term behavioral observations then revealed that mothers spend more time close to infants whose faces resemble their own offspring’s. Such patterns may result in paternal half-siblings interacting with each other more frequently, and potentially may lead to the establishment of social relationships between paternally related infants. Lastly, Charpentier et al. build a theoretical model which demonstrates that mothers would gain fitness benefits by promoting altruistic acts among offspring sired by the same father.
Several factors could explain how increased associations may emerge between siblings who share a father. For example, these related infants may have more similar nutritional needs or digestive processes, and hence find themselves in the same feeding areas more often. Charpentier et al. address one such alternative hypothesis: that fathers may be driving these interactions, for example by spending more time with the mothers they have mated with, or with the babies they have sired. They show that this mechanism is unlikely to explain their results, in part because paternal care is thought to be rare or absent, and because many males are not present within the group outside of the mating season.
As the movement of mandrill infants is primarily determined by their mother, Charpentier et al. reason that it is most likely to be maternal behavior that drives interactions between similar-looking infants. While classic kin selection posits that a mother might increase her fitness by interacting with genetically related individuals, the team argues that female mandrills may ensure that their offspring has better survival outcomes by fostering relationships with non-kin individuals (their infant’s paternal half siblings). They term this process ‘second-order kin selection’.
The work by Charpentier et al. features a particularly rich and diverse set of methods, combining digital images, genetics, machine learning, long-term behavioral observations, and theoretical modeling. Together, these approaches result in an impressive analysis and argument which provoke new questions on how indirect fitness effects might manifest, and the kinds of social behaviors that might be explained by kin selection. Overall, this study adds an intriguing new layer to the mechanisms by which kin selection might operate, and to our understanding of the complexity of evolved social behaviors.
References
-
BookThe Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to SexNew York: John Murray.https://doi.org/10.5962/bhl.title.24784
-
BookThe genetical theory of natural selectionClarendon Press.https://doi.org/10.5962/bhl.title.27468
-
Fisher’s “fundamental theorem” made clearAnnals of Human Genetics 36:129–140.https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-1809.1972.tb00764.x
Article and author information
Author details
Publication history
Copyright
© 2022, Higham
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 484
- views
-
- 43
- downloads
-
- 0
- citations
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Ecology
- Microbiology and Infectious Disease
Predicting how species diversity changes along environmental gradients is an enduring problem in ecology. In microbes current theories tend to invoke energy availability and enzyme kinetics as the main drivers of temperature-richness relationships. Here we derive a general empirically-grounded theory that can explain this phenomenon by linking microbial species richness in competitive communities to variation in the temperature-dependence of their interaction and growth rates. Specifically, the shape of the microbial community temperature-richness relationship depends on how rapidly the strength of effective competition between species pairs changes with temperature relative to the variance of their growth rates. Furthermore, it predicts that a thermal specialist-generalist tradeoff in growth rates alters coexistence by shifting this balance, causing richness to peak at relatively higher temperatures. Finally, we show that the observed patterns of variation in thermal performance curves of metabolic traits across extant bacterial taxa is indeed sufficient to generate the variety of community-level temperature-richness responses observed in the real world. Our results provide a new and general mechanism that can help explain temperature-diversity gradients in microbial communities, and provide a quantitative framework for interlinking variation in the thermal physiology of microbial species to their community-level diversity.
-
- Ecology
Bumblebees (Bombus terrestris) have been shown to engage in string-pulling behavior to access rewards. The objective of this study was to elucidate whether bumblebees display means-end comprehension in a string-pulling task. We presented bumblebees with two options: one where a string was connected to an artificial flower containing a reward and the other presenting an interrupted string. Bumblebees displayed a consistent preference for pulling connected strings over interrupted ones after training with a stepwise pulling technique. When exposed to novel string colors, bees continued to exhibit a bias towards pulling the connected string. This suggests that bumblebees engage in featural generalization of the visual display of the string connected to the flower in this task. If the view of the string connected to the flower was restricted during the training phase, the proportion of bumblebees choosing the connected strings significantly decreased. Similarly, when the bumblebees were confronted with coiled connected strings during the testing phase, they failed to identify and reject the interrupted strings. This finding underscores the significance of visual consistency in enabling the bumblebees to perform the task successfully. Our results suggest that bumblebees’ ability to distinguish between continuous strings and interrupted strings relies on a combination of image matching and associative learning, rather than means-end understanding. These insights contribute to a deeper understanding of the cognitive processes employed by bumblebees when tackling complex spatial tasks.