Evolution: Kin selection spreads

By spending more time around infants which physically resemble their own, mandrill mothers may increase how frequently their offspring interact with their paternal half siblings.
  1. James P Higham  Is a corresponding author
  1. Department of Anthropology, New York University, United States

Charles Darwin considered the evolution of altruism to be a puzzle (Darwin, 1871). Altruistic acts benefit the recipient but may cost the actor, making it difficult to grasp how they could have evolved in a world where organisms compete to survive. Since individuals who act selfishly should fare better than those with altruistic tendencies, shouldn’t altruism be quickly selected against?

An elegant solution to this problem emerged in the 1930s, when mathematical arguments were laid out to show how genes that enable altruistic behaviors could spread through a population (Fisher, 1930; Haldane, 1932). These ideas were further developed in the 1960s by W.D. ‘Bill’ Hamilton, and later re-derived by George Price (Hamilton, 1964a; Hamilton, 1964b; Price, 1972).

This work showed that if the individual(s) receiving altruistic acts are genetically related to the altruistic actor, and if an altruistic act is sufficiently beneficial to the receivers relative to the costs to the actor, then altruistic behaviors can spread. This process was named ‘kin selection’ (Smith, 1964). A swathe of additional terms were then coined, including ‘indirect fitness’ (the benefits associated with promoting the offspring of relatives) and ‘inclusive fitness’ (the sum total of direct and indirect fitness). These theoretical developments set the stage for thousands of empirical studies of social behavior. Now, in eLife, Marie Charpentier and colleagues in France, Germany and Gabon – including joint first author Clémence Poirotte – report intriguing new evidence which deepens our understanding of how kin selection may take place within complex societies (Charpentier et al., 2022).

The study took place in a free-ranging population of mandrills living in Gabon. These large and colorful ground-dwelling monkeys live in societies structured around groups of closely related females who share a maternal ancestor, with mature males joining the group during the mating season to sire offspring. As for other social mammals, the health and success of an individual is often shaped by their family connections and how well they are integrated in to the community.

Building on their previous work, Charpentier et al. used machine learning to train a human face recognition algorithm on a large database of mandrill face pictures. The program was then used on the study population to show that infants with the same father look alike – more so than unrelated individuals or maternal half-siblings. Long-term behavioral observations then revealed that mothers spend more time close to infants whose faces resemble their own offspring’s. Such patterns may result in paternal half-siblings interacting with each other more frequently, and potentially may lead to the establishment of social relationships between paternally related infants. Lastly, Charpentier et al. build a theoretical model which demonstrates that mothers would gain fitness benefits by promoting altruistic acts among offspring sired by the same father.

Several factors could explain how increased associations may emerge between siblings who share a father. For example, these related infants may have more similar nutritional needs or digestive processes, and hence find themselves in the same feeding areas more often. Charpentier et al. address one such alternative hypothesis: that fathers may be driving these interactions, for example by spending more time with the mothers they have mated with, or with the babies they have sired. They show that this mechanism is unlikely to explain their results, in part because paternal care is thought to be rare or absent, and because many males are not present within the group outside of the mating season.

As the movement of mandrill infants is primarily determined by their mother, Charpentier et al. reason that it is most likely to be maternal behavior that drives interactions between similar-looking infants. While classic kin selection posits that a mother might increase her fitness by interacting with genetically related individuals, the team argues that female mandrills may ensure that their offspring has better survival outcomes by fostering relationships with non-kin individuals (their infant’s paternal half siblings). They term this process ‘second-order kin selection’.

The work by Charpentier et al. features a particularly rich and diverse set of methods, combining digital images, genetics, machine learning, long-term behavioral observations, and theoretical modeling. Together, these approaches result in an impressive analysis and argument which provoke new questions on how indirect fitness effects might manifest, and the kinds of social behaviors that might be explained by kin selection. Overall, this study adds an intriguing new layer to the mechanisms by which kin selection might operate, and to our understanding of the complexity of evolved social behaviors.

References

  1. Book
    1. Haldane JBS
    (1932)
    The causes of evolution
    London: Longmans, Green & Co.
    1. Hamilton WD
    (1964a)
    The evolution of social behavior I
    Journal of Theoretical Biology 7:1–16.
    1. Hamilton WD
    (1964b)
    The evolution of social behavior II
    Journal of Theoretical Biology 7:17–52.

Article and author information

Author details

  1. James P Higham

    James P Higham is in the Department of Anthropology, New York University, New York, United States

    For correspondence
    jhigham@nyu.edu
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0002-1133-2030

Publication history

  1. Version of Record published: November 24, 2022 (version 1)

Copyright

© 2022, Higham

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.

Metrics

  • 436
    Page views
  • 40
    Downloads
  • 0
    Citations

Article citation count generated by polling the highest count across the following sources: Crossref, PubMed Central, Scopus.

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. James P Higham
(2022)
Evolution: Kin selection spreads
eLife 11:e84142.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.84142
  1. Further reading

Further reading

    1. Ecology
    Yongzhi Yan, Scott Jarvie, Qing Zhang
    Research Article

    Habitat loss and fragmentation per se have been shown to be a major threat to global biodiversity and ecosystem function. However, little is known about how habitat loss and fragmentation per se alters the relationship between biodiversity and ecosystem function (BEF relationship) in the natural landscape context. Based on 130 landscapes identified by a stratified random sampling in the agro-pastoral ecotone of northern China, we investigated the effects of landscape context (habitat loss and fragmentation per se) on plant richness, above-ground biomass, and the relationship between them in grassland communities using a structural equation model. We found that habitat loss directly decreased plant richness and hence decreased above-ground biomass, while fragmentation per se directly increased plant richness and hence increased above-ground biomass. Fragmentation per se also directly decreased soil water content and hence decreased above-ground biomass. Meanwhile, habitat loss decreased the magnitude of the positive relationship between plant richness and above-ground biomass by reducing the percentage of grassland specialists in the community, while fragmentation per se had no significant modulating effect on this relationship. These results demonstrate that habitat loss and fragmentation per se have inconsistent effects on BEF, with the BEF relationship being modulated by landscape context. Our findings emphasise that habitat loss rather than fragmentation per se can weaken the positive BEF relationship by decreasing the degree of habitat specialisation of the community.

    1. Ecology
    Anna L Erdei, Aneth B David ... Teun Dekker
    Research Article

    Over two decades ago, an intercropping strategy was developed that received critical acclaim for synergizing food security with ecosystem resilience in smallholder farming. The push-pull strategy reportedly suppresses lepidopteran pests in maize through a combination of a repellent intercrop (push), commonly Desmodium spp., and an attractive, border crop (pull). Key in the system is the intercrop's constitutive release of volatile terpenoids that repel herbivores. However, the earlier described volatiles were not detectable in the headspace of Desmodium, and only minimally upon herbivory. This was independent of soil type, microbiome composition, and whether collections were made in the laboratory or in the field. Further, in oviposition choice tests in a wind tunnel, maize with or without an odor background of Desmodium was equally attractive for the invasive pest Spodoptera frugiperda. In search of an alternative mechanism, we found that neonate larvae strongly preferred Desmodium over maize. However, their development stagnated and no larva survived. In addition, older larvae were frequently seen impaled and immobilized by the dense network of silica-fortified, non-glandular trichomes. Thus, our data suggest that Desmodium may act through intercepting and decimating dispersing larval offspring rather than adult deterrence. As a hallmark of sustainable pest control, maize-Desmodium push-pull intercropping has inspired countless efforts to emulate stimulo-deterrent diversion in other cropping systems. However, detailed knowledge of the actual mechanisms is required to rationally improve the strategy, and translate the concept to other cropping systems.