Biobank-wide association scan identifies risk factors for late-onset Alzheimer’s disease and endophenotypes

  1. Donghui Yan
  2. Bowen Hu
  3. Burcu F Darst
  4. Shubhabrata Mukherjee
  5. Brian W Kunkle
  6. Yuetiva Deming
  7. Logan Dumitrescu
  8. Yunling Wang
  9. Adam Naj
  10. Amanda Kuzma
  11. Yi Zhao
  12. Hyunseung Kang
  13. Sterling C Johnson
  14. Cruchaga Carlos
  15. Timothy J Hohman
  16. Paul K Crane
  17. Corinne D Engelman
  18. Alzheimer’s Disease Genetics Consortium (ADGC)
  19. Qiongshi Lu  Is a corresponding author
  1. University of Wisconsin-Madison, United States
  2. Department of Statistics, University of Wisconsin-Madison, United States
  3. Department of Population Health Sciences, University of Wisconsin-Madison, United States
  4. Division of General Internal Medicine, Department of Medicine, University of Washington, United States
  5. University of Miami Miller School of Medicine, United States
  6. Vanderbilt Memory and Alzheimer’s Center, Vanderbilt University Medical Center, Vanderbilt University School of Medicine, United States
  7. School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, United States
  8. Wisconsin Alzheimer’s Institute, University of Wisconsin School of Medicine and Public Health, United States
  9. Geriatric Research Education and Clinical Center, Wm. S. Middleton Memorial VA Hospital, United States
  10. Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center, University of Wisconsin School of Medicine and Public Health, United States
  11. Department of Psychiatry, Washington University in St. Louis, United States
  12. Department of Biostatistics and Medical Informatics, University of Wisconsin-Madison, United States

eLife assessment

In the last 15 years, large-scale association studies (GWAS) have served to estimate the association between genome-wide common variants and a large number of disparate traits and diseases in humans. This valuable method provides a new way to find correlations between the genetic component of a phenotype of interest, and all this wealth of genetic information. This software adds as a new tool to investigate genetic correlation between traits, and to generate new mechanistic hypotheses and dissect the role of the observed associations in disease heterogeneity. The results of the application of their method are solid and generally agree with what others have seen using similar AD and UKB data.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.91360.2.sa0

Abstract

Rich data from large biobanks, coupled with increasingly accessible association statistics from genome-wide association studies (GWAS), provide great opportunities to dissect the complex relationships among human traits and diseases. We introduce BADGERS, a powerful method to perform polygenic score-based biobank-wide association scans. Compared to traditional approaches, BADGERS uses GWAS summary statistics as input and does not require multiple traits to be measured in the same cohort. We applied BADGERS to two independent datasets for late-onset Alzheimer’s disease (AD; n=61,212). Among 1738 traits in the UK biobank, we identified 48 significant associations for AD. Family history, high cholesterol, and numerous traits related to intelligence and education showed strong and independent associations with AD. Furthermore, we identified 41 significant associations for a variety of AD endophenotypes. While family history and high cholesterol were strongly associated with AD subgroups and pathologies, only intelligence and education-related traits predicted pre-clinical cognitive phenotypes. These results provide novel insights into the distinct biological processes underlying various risk factors for AD.

Introduction

Late-onset AD is a prevalent, complex, and devastating neurodegenerative disease without a current cure. Millions of people are currently living with AD worldwide, and the number is expected to grow rapidly as the population continues to age (Prince et al., 2013; Reitz and Mayeux, 2014). With the failure of numerous drug trials, it is of great interest to identify modifiable risk factors that can be potential targets in the therapeutics development for AD (Østergaard et al., 2015; Larsson et al., 2017; Norton et al., 2014). Epidemiological studies that directly test associations between measured risk factors and AD are difficult to conduct and interpret because identified associations are, in many cases, affected by confounding and reverse causality. Despite being ubiquitous challenges in risk factor studies for complex diseases, these issues are particularly critical for AD due to its extended pre-clinical stage – irreversible pathologic changes have already occurred in the decade or two prior to clinical symptoms (Jack et al., 2013). On the other hand, Mendelian randomization methods (Sleiman and Grant, 2010; Davey Smith and Hemani, 2014; Zhu et al., 2018) have been developed to identify causal risk factors for disease using data from GWAS. Despite the favorable theoretical properties in identifying causal relationships, these methods have limited statistical power, thereby not suitable for hypothesis-free screening of risk factors.

Motivated by transcriptome-wide association study – an analysis strategy that identifies genes whose genetically regulated expression values are associated with disease (Gamazon et al., 2015; Gusev et al., 2016; Hu et al., 2018), we seek a systematic and statistically powerful approach to identify risk factors using summary association statistics from large-scale GWAS. GWAS for late-onset AD has been successful, and dozens of associated loci have been identified to date (Lambert et al., 2013; Harold et al., 2009; Hollingworth et al., 2011; Naj et al., 2011; Seshadri et al., 2010; Jun et al., 2017). Although direct information on risk factors is limited in these studies, dense genotype data on a large number of samples, in conjunction with independent reference datasets for thousands of complex human traits such as the UK biobank (Bycroft et al., 2017), make it possible to genetically impute potential risk factors and test their associations with AD. This strategy allows researchers to study risk factors that are not directly measured in AD studies. Furthermore, it reduces the reverse causality because the imputation models are trained on independent, younger, and mostly dementia-free reference cohorts, thereby improving the interpretability of findings.

Here, we introduce BADGERS (Biobank-wide Association Discovery using GEnetic Risk Scores), a statistically powerful and computationally efficient method to identify associations between a disease of interest and a large number of genetically imputed complex traits using GWAS summary statistics. We applied BADGERS to identify associated risk factors for AD from 1738 heritable traits in the UK biobank and replicated our findings in independent samples. Furthermore, we performed multivariate conditional analysis, Mendelian randomization, and follow-up association analysis with a variety of AD biomarkers, pathologies, and pre-clinical cognitive phenotypes to provide mechanistic insights into our findings.

Results

Method overview

Here, we briefly introduce the BADGERS model. The workflow of BADGERS is shown in Figure 1. A brief flowchart including all the analyses we contained in the manuscript was shown in the supplementary material (Figure 1—figure supplement 1). Complete statistical details are discussed in the Methods section. BADGERS is a two-stage method to test associations between traits. First, polygenic risk scores (PRS) are trained to impute complex traits using genetic data. Next, we test the association between a disease or trait of interest and various genetically-imputed traits. Given a PRS model, the imputed trait can be denoted as

T^=XW
Figure 1 with 1 supplement see all
Biobank-wide Association Discovery using GEnetic Risk Scores (BADGERS) Workflow.

BADGERS takes (a) Alzheimer’s disease genome-wide association studies (GWAS), (b) linkage disequilibrium (LD) reference panel, and (c) Human traits GWAS from the UK biobank as input. The generated result will be the (d) Association between Alzheimer’s disease and human traits. In graph (d), each triangle represents one human trait, and different colors represent different trait categories.

where XN×M is the genotype matrix for N individuals in a GWAS, and WM×1 is the Mx1 matrix denotes pre-calculated weight values on SNPs in the PRS model. Then, we test the association between measured trait Y and imputed trait T^ via a univariate linear model.

Y=α+T^γ+δ

The test statistic for γ can be expressed as:

Z=γ^se(γ^)WTΓZ

where Z~ is the vector of SNP-level association z-scores for trait Y, and Γ is a diagonal matrix with the jth diagonal element being the ratio between the standard deviation of the jth SNP and that of T^ .

This model can be further generalized to perform multivariate analysis. If K imputed traits are included in the analysis, we use a similar notation as in univariate analysis:

T^=XW

Here, WM×K* is a matrix and each column of W* is the pre-calculated weight values on SNPs for each imputed trait. Then, the associations between Y and K imputed traits T^i(1iK) are tested via a multivariate linear model.

Y=α+T^γ+δ

where γ*=(γ1,,γK)T is the vector of regression coefficients. The z-score for γi(1iK) can be denoted as:

Zi=γ^ise(γ^i)1UiiIiTU(W)TΘZ

where U is the inverse variance-covariance matrix of T^ ; Ii is the K×1 vector with the ith element being 1 and all other elements equal to 0; is a M×M diagonal matrix with the ith diagonal element being varXi ; and Z~ is defined the same as the univariate case as the vector of SNP-level association z-scores for trait Y.

Simulations

We used real genotype data from the Genetic Epidemiology Research on Adult Health and Aging (GERA) to conduct simulation analyses (Methods). First, we evaluated the performance of our method on data simulated under the null hypothesis. We tested the associations between randomly simulated traits and 1738 PRS from the UK biobank and did not observe inflation in type-I error (Supplementary file 1). Similar results were also observed when we simulated traits that are heritable but not directly associated with any PRS. Since BADGERS only uses summary association statistics and externally estimated linkage disequilibrium (LD) as input, we also compared effect estimates in BADGERS with those of traditional regression analysis based on individual-level data. Regression coefficient estimates and association p-values from these two methods were highly consistent in both simulation settings (Figure 2A and Figure 2—figure supplements 13), showing minimal information loss in summary statistics compared to individual-level data indicating highly consistent performance compared to methods based on individual-level data. To evaluate the statistical power of BADGERS, we simulated traits by combining effects from randomly selected PRS and a polygenic background (Methods). We set the effect size of PRS to be 0.02, 0.015, 0.01, 0.008, and 0.005. BADGERS showed comparable statistical power to the regression analysis based on individual-level genotype and phenotype data (Figure 2B, Supplementary file 1). Overall, our results suggest that using summary association statistics and externally estimated LD as a proxy for individual-level genotype and phenotype data does not inflate type-I error rate or decrease power. The performance of BADGERS is comparable to regression analysis based on individual-level data. We also studied if more sophisticated polygenic risk prediction methods could potentially lead to higher statistical power in downstream association tests. We compared the performance of PRS based on marginal effect sizes with that of LDpred, a more sophisticated PRS model that jointly estimates SNP effects via a Bayesian framework (Vilhjálmsson et al., 2015). Imputation models based on multivariate analysis indeed improved the results. When using marginal PRS to impute traits, the correlation between γ^i and γi was 0.79. This correlation improved to 0.91 when using LDpred PRS (Figure 2—figure supplement 4). However, such improvement did not substantially affect the statistical power in association testing. Using marginal PRS, our analysis achieved a statistical power of 86% to identify associations at a type-I error rate of 0.05, and the power was 88% when using multivariate effect estimates to calculate PRS. These results suggest that while more sophisticated PRS methods can improve the results in BADGERS, simple PRS based on marginal effects also shows reasonably good performance.

Figure 2 with 4 supplements see all
Simulation results.

Biobank-wide Association Discovery using GEnetic Risk Scores (BADGERS) and regression analysis based on individual-level data showed (A) highly consistent effect size estimates for 1738 polygenic risk scores (PRS) in simulation and (B) comparable statistical power (setting 3).

Identify risk factors for late-onset AD among 1738 heritable traits in the UK biobank

We applied BADGERS to conduct a biobank-wide association scan (BWAS) for AD risk factors from 1738 heritable traits (p<0.05; Methods) in the UK biobank. We repeated the analysis on two independent GWAS datasets for AD and further combined the statistical evidence via meta-analysis (Figure 3—figure supplement 1). We used stage-I association statistics from the International Genomics of Alzheimer’s Project (IGAP; n=54,162) as the discovery phase, then replicated the findings using 7050 independent samples from the Alzheimer’s Disease Genetics Consortium (ADGC). We identified 50 significant trait-AD associations in the discovery phase after correcting for multiple testing, among which 14 had p<0.05 in the replication analysis. Despite the considerably smaller sample size in the replication phase, top traits identified in the discovery stage showed strong enrichment for p<0.05 in the replication analysis (enrichment = 2.5, p=2.2e-5; hypergeometric test). In the meta-analysis, a total of 48 traits reached Bonferroni-corrected statistical significance and showed consistent effect directions in the discovery and replication analyses (Figure 3 and Supplementary file 2).

Figure 3 with 2 supplements see all
polygenic risk score (PR)S-based biobank-wide association scan (BWAS) identifies risk factors for Alzheimer’s disease (AD).

Meta-analysis p-values for 1738 heritable traits in the UK biobank are shown in the figure. p-values are truncated at 1e-15 for visualization purposes. The horizontal line marks the Bonferroni-corrected significance threshold (i.e. p=0.05/1738). Positive associations point upward, and negative associations point downward.

Unsurprisingly, many identified associations were related to dementia and cognition. Family history of AD and dementia showed the most significant associations with AD (p=3.7e-77 and 5.2e-28 for illnesses of mother and father, respectively). Having any dementia diagnosis is also strongly and positively associated (p=8.5e-11). In addition, we observed consistent and negative associations between better performance in cognition test and AD risk. These traits include fluid intelligence score (p=2.4e-14), time to complete round in cognition test (p=2.8e-9), correct final attempt (p=9.1e-11), and many others. Consistently, education attainment showed strong associations with AD. Age completed full time education (p=2.5e-7) was associated with lower AD risk. Four out of seven traits based on a survey about education and qualifications were significantly associated with AD (Figure 3—figure supplement 2). Higher education such as having a university degree (p=4.4e-12), A levels/AS levels or equivalent (p=6.9e-9), and professional qualifications (p=7.1e-6) were associated with lower AD risk. In contrast, choosing ‘none of the above’ in this survey was associated with a higher risk (p=1.6e-11). Other notable strong associations include high cholesterol (p=2.5e-15; positive), lifestyle traits such as cheese intake (p=2.5e-10; negative), occupation traits such as job involving heavy physical work (p=2.7e-10; positive), anthropometric traits including height (p=5.3e-7; negative), and traits related to pulmonary function, e.g., forced expiratory volume in 1 s (FEV1; p=1.9e-6; negative). Detailed information on all associations is summarized in Supplementary file 2.

Multivariate conditional analysis identifies independently associated risk factors

Of note, associations identified in the marginal analysis are not guaranteed to be independent. We observed clear correlational structures among the identified traits (Figure 4). For example, PRS of various intelligence and cognition-related traits are strongly correlated, and consumption of cholesterol-lowering medication is correlated with self-reported high cholesterol. To account for the correlations among traits and identify risk factors that are independently associated with AD, we performed multivariate conditional analysis using GWAS summary statistics (Methods). First, we applied hierarchical clustering to the 48 traits we identified in marginal association analysis and divided these traits into 15 representative clusters. The traits showing the most significant marginal association in each cluster were included in the multivariate analysis (Figure 4—figure supplement 1). Similar to the marginal analysis, we analyzed IGAP and ADGC data separately and combined the results using meta-analysis (Supplementary file 2). All 15 representative traits remained nominally significant (p<0.05) and showed consistent effect directions between marginal and conditional analyses (Supplementary file 1). However, several traits showed substantially reduced effect estimates and inflated p-values in multivariate analysis, including fluid intelligence score, mother still alive, unable to work because of sickness or disability, duration of moderate activity, and intake of cholesterol-lowering spread. Interestingly, major trait categories that showed the strongest marginal associations with AD (i.e. family history, high cholesterol, and education/cognition) were independent from each other. Paternal and maternal family history also showed independent associations with AD, consistent with the low correlation between their genetic risk scores (correlation = 0.052).

Figure 4 with 1 supplement see all
Polygenic risk score (PRS) correlation matrix for the 48 traits identified in marginal association analysis.

Trait categories and association directions with Alzheimer’s disease (AD) are annotated. The dendrogram indicates the results of hierarchical clustering. We used 1000 genome samples with European ancestry to calculate PRS and evaluate their correlations. Label ‘irnt’ means that trait values were standardized using rank-based inverse normal transformation in the genome-wide association study (GWAS) analysis.

Influence of the APOE region on identified associations

Furthermore, we evaluated the impact of APOE on identified associations. We removed the extended APOE region (chr19: 45,147,340–45,594,595; hg19) from summary statistics of the 48 traits showing significant marginal associations with AD and repeated the analysis. We observed a substantial drop in the significance level of many traits, especially family history of AD, dementia diagnosis, and high cholesterol (Figure 5, Figure 5—figure supplement 1, and Supplementary file 2). 38 out of 48 traits remained significant under stringent Bonferroni correction after APOE removal. Interestingly, the associations between AD and almost all cognition/intelligence traits were virtually unchanged, suggesting a limited role of APOE in these associations.

Figure 5 with 1 supplement see all
Influence of the APOE region on trait-Alzheimer’s disease (AD) associations.

The horizontal and vertical axes denote association p-values before and after removal of the APOE region, respectively. Original p-values (i.e. the x-axis) were truncated at 1e-20 for visualization purposes.

Causal inference via Mendelian randomization

Next, we investigated the evidence for causality among identified associations. We performed Mendelian randomization (MR-IVW; Methods) in IGAP and ADGC datasets separately and meta-analyzed the results on the complete set of 1738 heritable traits from the UK biobank. A total of 48 traits reached Bonferroni-corrected statistical significance and showed consistent effect directions in the discovery and replication analyses using BADGERS. In contrast, MR-IVW only identified nine traits with Bonferroni-corrected statistical significance. Among these nine traits, seven were also identified by BADGERS (Supplementary file 1). The signs of all significant causal effects identified by MR-IVW were consistent with results from BADGERS. The most significant effect was family history (p=1.1e-233 and 1.7e-69 for maternal and paternal history, respectively). Dementia diagnosis (p=9.1e-7), high cholesterol (p=4.1e-6), A levels/AS levels education (p=1.7e-4), and time spent watching television (p=2.4e-4) were also among the top significant effects. Of note, the fluid intelligence score, one of the most significant associations identified by BADGERS, did not reach statistical significance in MR (p=0.06), which may be explained by its polygenic genetic architecture. It is also worth noting that if we scan all 1738 traits using BADGERS and then apply MR-IVW on the 48 Bonferroni-corrected significant traits, 23 could reach nominal significance (p<0.05) in MR, and seven could reach significance under Bonferroni correction (p<0.05/48; Supplementary file 1).

We also compared BADGERS with another more recent method GSMR (Zhu et al., 2018). Due to the smaller sample size in the ADGC dataset, we only applied GSMR to the IGAP summary statistics. In total, 18 traits reached statistical significance under Bonferroni correction (Supplementary file 1). However, these results showed only moderate consistency with MR-IVW and BADGERS. Among the 18 significant traits, only 1 trait, maternal family history of Alzheimer’s disease and dementia, overlapped with significant traits identified by MR-IVW. Six out of 18 traits overlapped with significant traits identified by BADGERS. Among the 18 significant traits, eight are related to body fat mass and two are related to educational attainment. The most significant effect was illnesses of mother (p=2.4e-294). College or University degree (p=4.84e-6), education; none of the above (p=3.6e-4), A levels/AS levels education (p=3.8e-6), and time spent watching television (p=4.0e-3) were also among top significant effects. Notably, GSMR failed to identify paternal family history or high cholesterol as risk factors for Alzheimer’s disease. If we only consider the 48 significant traits identified by BADGERS, 11 were nominally significant (p<0.05). However, 23 traits did not have enough significant SNPs to perform the GSMR analysis (at least 10 SNPs are required). The signs of all significant causal effects identified by GSMR were consistent between association effects in BADGERS.

Additionally, we included GSMR analysis results after removing APOE region from the 48 identified traits. Only maternal family history reached Bonferroni-corrected statistical significance, further demonstrating the lack of statistical power in MR when performing biobank-wide scans (Supplementary file 1).

Associations with AD subgroups, biomarkers, and pathologies

To further investigate the mechanistic pathways for the identified risk factors, we applied BADGERS to a variety of AD subgroups, biomarkers, and neuropathologic features (Supplementary file 1). Overall, 29 significant associations were identified under a false discovery rate (FDR) cutoff of 0.05, and these endophenotypes showed distinct association patterns with AD risk factors (Figure 6; Figure 6—figure supplement 1). First, we tested the associations between the 48 AD-associated traits and five AD subgroups defined in the Executive Prominent Alzheimer’s Disease (EPAD) study, i.e., memory, language, visuospatial, none, and mix (Methods) (Mukherjee et al., 2018; Crane et al., 2017). Maternal family history of AD and dementia was strongly and consistently associated with all five EPAD subgroups (Supplementary file 2), with memory subgroup showing the strongest association (p=3.3e-16), which is consistent with the higher frequency of APOE ε4 in this subgroup (Mukherjee et al., 2018). Paternal family history was not strongly associated with any subgroups, but the effect directions were consistent. Interestingly, intelligence and cognition-related traits such as correct final attempt in cognitive test (p=2.7e-5) and fluid intelligence score (p=6.8e-5) were specifically associated with the ‘none’ subgroup – AD samples without relative impairment in any of the four cognitive domains. High cholesterol and related traits were associated with language, memory, and mix (i.e. AD samples with relative impairment in two or more domains) subgroups but showed weaker associations with the visuospatial and none subgroups.

Figure 6 with 2 supplements see all
Associations between identified Alzheimer’s disease (AD) risk factors and various AD subgroups, cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) biomarkers, and neuropathologic features.

Asterisks denote significant associations based on an false discovery rate (FDR) cutoff of 0.05. p-values are truncated at 1e-5 for visualization purposes.

Next, we extended our analysis to three biomarkers of AD in cerebrospinal fluid (CSF): amyloid beta (Aβ42), tau, and phosphorylated tau (ptau181) (Deming et al., 2017). Somewhat surprisingly, AD risk factors did not show strong associations with Aβ42 and tau (Supplementary file 2). Maternal family history of AD and dementia was associated with ptau181 (p=4.2e-4), but associations were absent for Aβ42 and tau. It has been recently suggested that CSF biomarkers have a sex-specific genetic architecture (Deming et al., 2018). However, no association passed an FDR cutoff of 0.05 in our sex-stratified analyses (Supplementary file 2).

Furthermore, we applied BADGERS to a variety of neuropathologic features of AD and related dementias (Methods), including neuritic plaques (NPs), neurofibrillary tangles (NFTs), cerebral amyloid angiopathy (CAA), lewy body disease (LBD), hippocampal sclerosis (HS), and vascular brain injury (VBI) (Beecham et al., 2014). Family history of AD/dementia (p=3.8e-8, maternal; p=1.4e-5, paternal) and high cholesterol (p=2.1e-5) were strongly associated with NFT Braak stages (Supplementary file 2). NP also showed very similar association patterns with these traits (p=2.7e-19, maternal family history; p=2.6e-7, paternal family history; p=0.001, high cholesterol). The other neuropathologic features did not show strong associations. Of note, despite not being statistically significant, family history of AD/dementia was negatively associated with VBI, and multiple intelligence traits were positively associated with LBD, showing distinct patterns with other pathologies (Figure 6—figure supplement 2). We also note that various versions of the same pathologies all showed consistent associations in our analyses (Figure 6—figure supplement 2). The complete association results for all the endophenotypes and all the traits are summarized in Supplementary file 2. We further identified the influence of the APOE region in these results. The association results for all the endophenotypes with APOE Region being removed are summarized in Supplementary file 2.

Associations with cognitive traits in a pre-clinical cohort

Finally, we studied the associations between AD risk factors and pre-clinical cognitive phenotypes using 1198 samples from the Wisconsin Registry for Alzheimer’s Prevention (WRAP), a longitudinal study of initially dementia-free middle-aged adults (Johnson et al., 2018). Assessed phenotypes include mild cognitive impairment (MCI) status and three cognitive composite scores for executive function, delayed recall, and learning (Methods). A total of 12 significant associations reached an FDR cutoff of 0.05 (Supplementary file 2). Somewhat surprisingly, parental history and high cholesterol, the risk factors that showed the strongest associations with various AD endophenotypes, were not associated with MCI or cognitive composite scores in WRAP. Instead, education and intelligence-related traits strongly predicted pre-clinical cognition (Figure 7). A-levels education and no education both showed highly significant associations with delayed recall (p=4.0e-5 and 7.7e-7) and learning (p=7.6e-6 and 5.0e-8). No education was also associated with higher risk of MCI (p=2.5e-4). Additionally, fluid intelligence score was positively associated with the learning composite score (p=7.5e-4), and time to complete round in cognition test was negatively associated with the executive function (p=1.1e-5).

Associations between six traits and pre-clinical cognitive phenotypes in Wisconsin Registry for Alzheimer's Prevention (WRAP).

Error bars denote the standard error of effect estimates. N=1,198.

Discussion

In this work, we introduced BADGERS, a new method to perform association scans at the biobank scale using genetic risk scores and GWAS association statistics. Through simulations, we demonstrated that our method provides consistent effect estimates and similar statistical power compared to regression analysis based on individual-level data. Additionally, we applied BADGERS to two large and independent GWAS datasets for late-onset AD. In our analyses, we used GWAS summary statistics from the UK biobank, one of the largest genetic cohort in the world, to generate PRS for complex traits. We estimated heritability for all available traits in the UK biobank and only included traits with nominally significant heritability (p<0.05) in our analyses. The GWAS summary statistics for Alzheimer’s disease were also obtained from the largest available study – International Genomics of Alzheimer’s Project (IGAP) and we further sought replication using a large, independent dataset from the Alzheimer’s Disease Genetics Consortium (ADGC). Overall, we are confident that these quality control procedures largely controlled the false findings in our study. Among 1738 heritable traits in the UK biobank, we identified 48 traits showing statistically significant associations with AD. These traits covered a variety of categories, including family history, cholesterol, intelligence, education, occupation, and lifestyle. Although many of the identified traits are genetically correlated, multivariate conditional analysis confirmed multiple strong and independent associations for AD. Family history showing strong associations with AD is not a surprise, and many other associations are supported by the literature as well. The protective effect of higher educational and occupational attainment on the risk and onset of dementia is well studied (Valenzuela and Sachdev, 2006; Stern, 2012). Cholesterol buildup is also known to associate with β-amyloid plaques in the brain and higher AD risk (Reed et al., 2014; Djelti et al., 2015; Simons et al., 2001).

More interestingly, these identified traits had distinct association patterns with various AD subgroups, biomarkers, pathologies, and pre-clinical cognitive traits. Five cognitively-defined AD subgroups were consistently associated with maternal family history, but only the group without substantial relative impairment in any domain (i.e. EPAD_none) was associated with intelligence and education. In addition, family history and high cholesterol were strongly associated with classic AD neuropathologies, including NP and NFT, while intelligence and educational attainment predicted pre-clinical cognitive scores and MCI. These results suggest that various AD risk factors may affect the disease course at different time points and via distinct biological processes, and genetically predicted risk factors for clinical AD include at least two separate components. While some risk factors (e.g. high cholesterol and APOE) may directly contribute to the accumulation of pathologies, other factors (e.g. intelligence and education) may buffer the adverse effect of brain pathology on cognition (Stern, 2012). One possible scenario is that family history and high cholesterol are the fundamental causes of AD while education level and intelligence are the parameters of such factors. While if one didn’t have such a factor in the first stage, they are protected from getting AD, if someone with such factor and also has high score in education attainment or intelligence, they can also get rid of the possibility of getting AD. We also investigated the influence of APOE on the identified associations. Effects of family history and high cholesterol were substantially reduced after APOE removal. In contrast, associations with cognition and education were virtually unchanged. These results suggest that various AD risk factors may affect the disease course at different time points and via distinct biological processes. While some risk factors (e.g. high cholesterol and APOE) may directly contribute to the accumulation of pathologies, other factors (e.g. intelligence and education) reduce the adverse effect of brain pathology on cognition (Stern, 2012).

Furthermore, we note that the association results in BADGERS need to be interpreted with caution. Although PRS-based association analysis is sometimes treated as causal inference in the literature (Paternoster et al., 2017), we do not see BADGERS as a tool to identify causal factors. Key assumptions in causal inference are in many cases, violated when analyzing complex, highly polygenic traits, which may lead to complications when interpreting results. In our analysis, BADGERS showed superior statistical power than MR-IVW – among 1738 heritable traits, 48 reached Bonferroni significance in BADGERS, 9 and 18 traits reached Bonferroni significance in MR-IVW and GSMR, respectively. Among the 48 traits identified by BADGERS, 23 reached nominal statistical significance in MR-IVW and 11 were nominally significant in GSMR. BADGERS is a statistically powerful and computationally efficient method for identifying associations between a disease of interest and genetically imputed complex traits. Due to the capability of utilizing PRS with a large number of SNPs to impute complex traits, BADGERS has substantially improved statistical power compared to MR methods. And because of this, it can serve as a hypothesis-free method to screen for candidate risk factors from biobank-scale datasets with an overwhelming number of traits. After a list of candidate risk factors is identified using BADGERS, MR methods can be applied to carefully demonstrate causality. We envision BADGERS as a tool to prioritize associations among a large number of candidate risk factors so that robust causal inference methods can be applied to carefully assess causal effects. In addition, BADGERS requires a reference panel to provide LD estimates as a summary statistics-based method. If the population in the reference panel does not match that of the GWAS, it may create bias in the analysis. Our simulation results suggest that 1000 Genomes European samples is sufficient for our analysis when the GWAS was also conducted on European samples. Our implemented BADGERS software is flexible on the choice of LD reference panel. It allows users to change the reference dataset when they see fit.

What’s more, environmental factors may play a big role in the identified associations. There is little doubt that the environment could influence many complex traits, including the ones highlighted by the reviewer. However, this does not necessarily mean that these traits cannot also have a genetic component (or be genetically heritable). we summarized the heritability estimates for the 48 traits identified in our BADGERS meta-analysis of two independent datasets for Alzheimer’s disease (Supplementary file 1), and all of them have nominally significant heritability estimates (p<0.05) based on our selection criteria. Nevertheless, we do acknowledge that the high heritability of these traits is influenced by correlations with other traits. For example, job involving heavy manual or physical work is genetically correlated with educational attainment (Figure 3), which indicates that the association between this trait and Alzheimer’s disease may not be direct. Therefore, it is important to note that association results from BADGERS analysis need to be interpreted with caution.

Limited sample size in AD endophenotypes is another limitation in our study. We have used data from the largest available GWAS for CSF biomarkers and neuropathologies. Still, the small sample size made it challenging to assess the effects of traits that were weakly associated with AD. When an independent validation dataset is available, it would be of interest to assess the prediction accuracy of PRS on each trait. However, external validation datasets rarely exist in real applications. In that case, the users may choose to use heritability estimates to filter traits with a substantial genetic component. Furthermore, in the BADGERS framework, PRS are independent variables in the regression analysis. If the PRS has limited predictive power, such noise is similar to measurement errors in standard regression analysis. This may decrease the statistical power in association tests but does not inflate the type-I error rate. Finally, emerging evidence has highlighted the sex-specific genetic architecture of AD (Deming et al., 2018; Hohman et al., 2018). In our analysis, maternal family history of AD showed stronger associations with various phenotypes than paternal family history. However, we note that this may be explained by the sample size difference in the UK biobank (Ncase = 28,507 and 15,022 for samples with maternal and paternal family history, respectively). We also performed sex-stratified analyses for CSF biomarkers but identified limited associations, possibly due to the small sample size. Overall, sex-specific effects of risk factors remain to be investigated in the future using larger datasets. In total, BADGERS requires the training data for genetic prediction models and the downstream disease GWAS to be independent but of similar genetic ancestry. Development of methods that are more robust to sample overlap and diverse genetic ancestry remains an open problem for future research.

In conclusion, BADGERS is a statistically powerful method to identify associated risk factors for complex diseases. Large-scale biobanks continue to provide rich data on various human traits that may be of interest in disease research. Our method uses GWAS to bridge large biobanks with studies on specific diseases, lessens the limitation of insufficient disease cases in biobanks and lack of risk factor measurements in disease studies, and provides a statistically justified approach to identifying risk factors for disease. We have demonstrated the effectiveness of BADGERS through extensive simulations, a two-stage BWAS for late-onset AD, and various follow-up analyses on identified risk factors. Our results provided new insights into the genetic basis of AD, and revealed distinct mechanisms for the involvement of risk factors in AD etiologies. The ever-growing sample size in GWAS and biobanks, in conjunction with increasingly accessible summary association statistics, makes BADGERS a powerful and valuable tool in human genetics research.

Methods

BADGERS framework

The goal of this method is to study the association between Y, a measured trait in the study, and T^ , a trait imputed from genetic data via a linear prediction model:

T^=XW

Here, XN×M is the genotype matrix for N individuals in a study of trait Y. WM×1 is the pre-calculated weight values on SNPs in the imputation model. M denotes the number of SNPs. We use Y, a N×1 vector, to denote the trait values measured on the same group of individuals. We test the association between Y and T^ via a linear model.

Y=α+T^γ+δ

where α is the intercept, δ is the term for random noise, and regression coefficient γ is the parameter of interest. The ordinary least squares (OLS) estimator for γ can be denoted as,

γ^=cov(T^,Y)var(T^)=cov(XW,Y)var(T^)=1var(T^)WT(cov(X1,Y)cov(XM,Y))

Here, Xj is the jth column of X. Additionally, we derive the formula for the standard error of γ^ :

se(γ^)=var(δ)N×var(T^)var(Y)N×var(T^)

The approximation in this formula is based on the assumption that trait Y has complex etiology and imputed trait T^ only explains a small proportion of its phenotypic variance. When an accurate estimate of var(δ) is difficult to obtain, this approximation approach provides conservative results and controls type-I error in the analysis.

In practice, individual-level genotype (i.e. X) and phenotype data (i.e. Y) may not be accessible due to policy and privacy concerns. Therefore, it is of practical interest to perform the aforementioned association analysis using summary association statistics. Standard genetic association analysis tests the association between trait Y and each SNP via the following linear model:

Y=μj+Xjβj+εj(1jM)

The OLS estimator for βj and its standard error have the following forms.

β^j=cov(Xj,Y)var(Xj)
se(β^j)=var(εj)N×var(Xj)var(Y)N×var(Xj)

Again, the approximation is based on the empirical observation in complex trait genetics – each SNP explains little variability of Y (Manolio et al., 2009).

Next, we derive the test statistic (i.e. z-score) for γ:

Z=γ^se(γ^)Nvar(Y)×var(T^)WT(cov(X1,Y)cov(XM,Y))1var(T^)WT(var(X1)β1^se(β1^)var(XM)βM^se(βM^))=WTΓZ

where Γ is a diagonal matrix with the jth diagonal element being

Γjj=var(Xj)var(T^)

and Z is the vector of SNP-level z-scores obtained from the GWAS of trait Y, i.e.,

Zj=β^jse(β^j)

Without access to individual-level genotype data, var(Xj) and var(T^) need to be estimated using an external panel with a similar ancestry background. We use X~ to denote the genotype matrix from an external cohort, then var(Xj) can be approximated using the sample variance of X~j . Variance of T^ can be approximated as follows

var(T^)WTDW

where D~ is the variance-covariance matrix of all SNPs estimated using X~ . However, when the number of SNPs is large in the imputation model for trait T, calculation of D~ is computationally intractable. Instead, we use an equivalent but computationally more efficient approach. We first impute trait T in the external panel using the same imputation model

T=XW

Then, var(T^) can be approximated by sample variance var(T) .

Thus, we can test the association between Y and T^ without having access to individual-level genotype and phenotype data from the GWAS. The required input variables for BADGERS include a linear imputation model for trait T, SNP-level summary statistics from a GWAS of trait Y, and an external panel of genotype data. With these, the association test can be performed.

Multivariate analysis in BADGERS

To adjust for potential confounding effects, it may be of interest to include multiple imputed traits in the same BADGERS model. We still use Y to denote the measured trait of interest. The goal is to perform a multiple regression analysis using K imputed traits (i.e. T^1 ,..., T^K) as predictor variables:

Y=T^γ+δ

Here, we use T^=(T^1,,T^K) to denote a N×K matrix for K imputed traits. Regression coefficients γ*=(γ1,,γK)T are the parameters of interest. To simplify algebra, we also assume trait Y and all SNPs in the genotype matrix X are centered so there is no intercept term in the model, but the conclusions apply to the general setting. Similar to univariate analysis, traits T^1,,T^K are imputed from genetic data via linear prediction models:

T^=XW

where WM×K* are imputation weights assigned to SNPs. The ith column of W denotes the imputation model for trait Ti . Then, the OLS estimator γ^ and its variance-covariance matrix can be denoted as follows:

γ^=((T^)TT^)1(T^)TY
cov(γ^)var(Y)((T^)TT^)1

The approximation is based on the assumption that imputed traits T^1,,T^K collectively explain little variance in Y, which is reasonable in complex trait genetics if K is not too large. We further denote:

U:=N((T^)TT^)1=(var(T^1)cov(T^1,T^K)cov(T^K,T^1)var(T^K))1

All elements in matrix U can be approximated using a reference panel X~ (Dudbridge, 2013):

cov(T^i,T^j)cov(Ti,Tj)

Therefore, the z-score for γk(1kK) is

Zk=γk^se(γk^)=IkTU(W)TXTYNUkkvar(Y)=1UkkIkTU(W)TΘZ

where Ik is the K×1 vector with the kth element being 1 and all other elements equal to 0, is a M×M diagonal matrix with the ith diagonal element being var(Xi) , and similar to the notation in univariate analysis, Z~ is the vector of SNP-level z-scores from the GWAS of trait Y. Given imputation models for K traits (i.e. W*), GWAS summary statistics for trait Y (i.e. Z~), and an external genetic dataset to estimate U and , multivariate association analysis can be performed without genotype and phenotype data from the GWAS.

Genetic prediction

Any linear prediction model can be used in the BADGERS framework. With access to individual-level genotype and phenotype data, the users can train their preferred statistical learning models, e.g., penalized regression or linear mixed model. When only GWAS summary statistics are available for risk factors (i.e. T), PRS can be used for imputation. We used PRS to impute complex traits in all analyses throughout the paper. Of note, more advanced PRS methods that explicitly model LD (Vilhjálmsson et al., 2015) and functional annotations (Hu et al., 2017) to improve prediction accuracy have been developed. However, additional independent datasets may be needed if there are tuning parameters in PRS. In general, higher imputation accuracy will improve statistical power in association testing (Hu et al., 2018). The BADGERS software allows users to choose their preferred imputation model.

Simulation settings

We simulated quantitative traits using genotype data of 62,313 individuals from the GERA cohort (dbGap accession: phs000674). Summary association statistics were generated using PLINK (Purcell et al., 2007). We ran BADGERS on summary statistics based on the simulated traits and PRS of 1738 traits in the UK biobank. To compare BADGERS with the traditional approach that uses individual-level data as input, we also directly regressed simulated traits on the PRS of UK biobank traits to estimate association effects.

Setting 1

We simulated quantitative trait values as i.i.d. samples from normal distribution with mean 0 and variance 1. In this setting, simulated trait values were independent from genotype data.

Setting 2

We simulated quantitative trait values based on an additive random effect model commonly used in heritability estimation (Yang et al., 2015). We fixed heritability to be 0.1. In this setting, the simulated trait is associated with SNPs, but is not directly related to PRS of UK biobank traits.

Setting 3

We selected 100 traits from 1738 UK-Biobank traits to calculate PRS on GERA data. For each of these 100 PRS, we simulated a quantitative trait by summing up the effect of PRS, a polygenic genetic background, and a noise term.

Y=Xβ+ρP+ε

Here, X denotes the genotype of samples; β is the effect size of each variant; P is the PRS of one of the selected traits; ρ is the effect size of PRS; and ε is the error term following a standard normal distribution. The polygenic background and random noise (i.e. Xβ+ε) were simulated using the same model described in setting 2. This term and the PRS were normalized separately. The standardized effect size (i.e. ρ) was set as 0.02, 0.015, 0.01, 0.008, and 0.005 in our simulations. In this setting, simulated traits are directly associated with SNPs and PRS. For each value of ρ, statistical power was calculated as the proportion of significant results (p<0.05) out of 100 traits.

Setting 4

We simulated 100 quantitative traits T1,,T100 based on an additive random effect model commonly used with heritability fixed as 0.1. And the response traits Y1,,Y100 were simulated by adding a noise term to T.

Yi=γiTi+εi

Where γiN(0,2) , and εiN(0,Var(Ti)). The dataset was split into two subsets, one with 31,162 (subset 1) and another with 31,163 samples (subset 2). Marginal summary statistics correspond to Ti ’s and Yi ’s were derived using subset 1 and subset 2, respectively. We applied LDpred to jointly estimate all SNPs’ effects using marginal summary statistics from subset 1. Then, we ran BADGERS to identify associations between 100 pairs of Yi and Ti using two methods to impute Ti ’s (i.e. marginal PRS and LDpred).

GWAS datasets

Summary statistics for 4357 UK biobank traits were generated by Dr. Benjamin Neale’s group and were downloaded from (http://www.nealelab.is/uk-biobank). AD summary statistics from the IGAP stage-I analysis were downloaded from the IGAP website (http://web.pasteur-lille.fr/en/recherche/u744/igap/igap_download.php). ADGC phase 2 summary statistics were generated by first analyzing individual datasets using logistic regression adjusting for age, sex, and the first three principal components in the program SNPTest v2 (Marchini et al., 2007). Meta-analysis of the individual dataset results was then performed using the inverse-variance weighted approach (Willer et al., 2010).

GWAS summary statistics for neuropathologic features of AD and related dementias were obtained from the ADGC. Details on these data have been previously reported (Beecham et al., 2014). We analyzed a total of 13 neuropathologic features, including four NP traits, two traits for NFT Braak stages, three traits for LBD, CAA, HS, and two VBI traits. Among different versions of the same pathology, we picked one dataset for each pathologic feature to show in our primary analyses. Six AD subgroups were defined in the recent EPAD paper (Mukherjee et al., 2018) on the basis of relative performance in memory, executive functioning, visuospatial functioning, and language at the time of Alzheimer’s diagnosis. Four subgroups include AD samples with an isolated substantial relative impairment in one of four domains; the ‘none’ subgroup includes samples without substantial relative impairment; the ‘mix’ subgroup includes samples with relative impairment in multiple domains. Each domain was compared with healthy controls in case-control association analyses. We did not include the executive functioning subgroup in our analysis due to its small sample size in cases. Detailed information about the design of CSF biomarker GWAS and the recent sex-stratified analysis has been described previously (Deming et al., 2017; Deming et al., 2018). Details on the association statistics for AD subgroups, CSF biomarkers, and neuropathological features are summarized in Supplementary file 2.

Analysis of GWAS summary statistics

We applied LD score regression implemented in the LDSC software (Bulik-Sullivan et al., 2015) to estimate the heritability of each trait. Among 4357 traits, we selected 1738 with nominally significant heritability (p<0.05) to include in our analyses. We removed SNPs with association p-values greater than 0.01 from each of the 1738 summary statistics files, clumped the remaining SNPs using a LD cutoff of 0.1 and a radius of 1 Mb in PLINK (Purcell et al., 2007), and built PRS for each trait using the effect size estimates of remaining SNPs.

Throughout the paper, we used samples of European ancestry in the 1000 Genomes Project as a reference panel to estimate LD (Abecasis et al., 2012). In univariate analyses, we tested marginal associations between each PRS and AD using the IGAP stage-I dataset and replicated the findings using the ADGC summary statistics. Association results in two stages were combined using an inverse variance-weighted meta-analysis (Willer et al., 2010). A stringent Bonferroni-corrected significance threshold was used to identify AD-associated risk factors. For associations between identified risk factors and AD endophenotypes, we used an FDR cutoff of 0.05 to claim statistical significance. We applied hierarchical clustering to the covariance of 48 traits we identified from marginal association analysis, then divided the result into 15 clusters and selected one most significant trait from each cluster and used them to perform multivariate conditional analysis. We analyzed IGAP and ADGC datasets separately, and combined the results using meta-analysis.

We used MR-IVW approach (Burgess et al., 2013) implemented in the Mendelian Randomization R package (Yavorska and Burgess, 2017) to study the causal effects of 48 risk factors identified by BADGERS. For each trait, we selected instrumental SNP variables as the top 30 most significant SNPs after clumping all SNPs using a LD cutoff of 0.1.

Analysis of WRAP data

WRAP is a longitudinal study of initially dementia-free middle-aged adults that allows for the enrollment of siblings and is enriched for a parental history of AD. Details of the study design and methods used have been previously described (Johnson et al., 2018; Sager et al., 2005). After quality control, a total of 1198 participants whose genetic ancestry was primarily of European descent were included in our analysis. On average, participants were 53.7 years of age (SD = 6.6) at baseline and had a bachelor’s degree, and 69.8% (n=836) were female. Participants had two to six longitudinal study visits, with an average of 4.3 visits, leading to a total of 5184 observations available for analysis.

DNA samples were genotyped using the Illumina Multi-Ethnic Genotyping Array at the University of Wisconsin Biotechnology Center. Thirty-six blinded duplicate samples were used to calculate a concordance rate of 99.99%, and discordant genotypes were set to missing. Imputation was performed with the Michigan Imputation Server v1.0.3 (Das et al., 2016), using the Haplotype Reference Consortium (HRC) v. r1.1 2016 (McCarthy et al., 2016) as the reference panel and Eagle2 v2.3 (Loh et al., 2016) for phasing. Variants with a quality score R2 <0.80, MAF <0.001, or that were out of HWE were excluded, leading to 10,499,994 imputed and genotyped variants for analyses. Data cleaning and file preparation were completed using PLINK v1.9 (Chang et al., 2015) and VCFtools v0.1.14 (Danecek et al., 2011). Coordinates are based on the hg19 genome build. Due to the sibling relationships present in the WRAP cohort, genetic ancestry was assessed and confirmed using Principal Components Analysis in Related Samples (PC-AiR), a method that makes robust inferences about population structure in the presence of relatedness (Conomos et al., 2015).

Composite scores were calculated for executive function, delayed recall, and learning based on a previous analysis (Clark et al., 2016). Each composite score was calculated from three neuropsychological tests, which were each converted to z-scores using baseline means and standard deviations. These z-scores were then averaged to derive executive function and delayed recall composite scores at each visit for each individual. Cognitive impairment status was determined based on a consensus review by a panel of dementia experts. Resulting cognitive statuses included cognitively normal, early MCI, clinical MCI, impairment that was not MCI, or dementia, as previously defined (Koscik et al., 2016). Participants were considered cognitively impaired if their worst consensus conference diagnosis was early MCI, clinical MCI, or dementia (n=387). Participants were considered cognitively stable if their consensus conference diagnosis was cognitively normal across all visits (n=803).

The 48 PRSs were developed within the WRAP cohort using PLINK v1.9 (Chang et al., 2015) and tested for associations with the three composite scores (i.e. executive function, delayed recall, and learning) and cognitive impairment statuses. MCI status was tested using logistic regression models in R, while all other associations, which utilized multiple study visits, were tested using linear mixed regression models implemented in the lme4 package in R (Bates et al., 2015). All models included fixed effects for age and sex, and cognitive composite scores additionally included a fixed effect for practice effect (using visit number). Mixed models included random intercepts for within-subject correlations due to repeated measures and within-family correlations due to the enrollment of siblings.

Software availability

The BADGERS software is freely available at https://github.com/qlu-lab/BADGERS, copy archived at qlu-lab, 2024.

Data availability

The current manuscript is a computational study, so no data have been generated for this manuscript. The modeling code is available at https://github.com/qlu-lab/BADGERS, copy archived at qlu-lab, 2024.

The following previously published data sets were used
    1. Lambert JC
    (2013) NIAGADS database
    ID ng00036. NG00036 - IGAP Summary Statistics- Lambert et al. (2013).

References

    1. Hollingworth P
    2. Harold D
    3. Sims R
    4. Gerrish A
    5. Lambert JC
    6. Carrasquillo MM
    7. Abraham R
    8. Hamshere ML
    9. Pahwa JS
    10. Moskvina V
    11. Dowzell K
    12. Jones N
    13. Stretton A
    14. Thomas C
    15. Richards A
    16. Ivanov D
    17. Widdowson C
    18. Chapman J
    19. Lovestone S
    20. Powell J
    21. Proitsi P
    22. Lupton MK
    23. Brayne C
    24. Rubinsztein DC
    25. Gill M
    26. Lawlor B
    27. Lynch A
    28. Brown KS
    29. Passmore PA
    30. Craig D
    31. McGuinness B
    32. Todd S
    33. Holmes C
    34. Mann D
    35. Smith AD
    36. Beaumont H
    37. Warden D
    38. Wilcock G
    39. Love S
    40. Kehoe PG
    41. Hooper NM
    42. Vardy E
    43. Hardy J
    44. Mead S
    45. Fox NC
    46. Rossor M
    47. Collinge J
    48. Maier W
    49. Jessen F
    50. Rüther E
    51. Schürmann B
    52. Heun R
    53. Kölsch H
    54. van den Bussche H
    55. Heuser I
    56. Kornhuber J
    57. Wiltfang J
    58. Dichgans M
    59. Frölich L
    60. Hampel H
    61. Gallacher J
    62. Hüll M
    63. Rujescu D
    64. Giegling I
    65. Goate AM
    66. Kauwe JSK
    67. Cruchaga C
    68. Nowotny P
    69. Morris JC
    70. Mayo K
    71. Sleegers K
    72. Bettens K
    73. Engelborghs S
    74. De Deyn PP
    75. Van Broeckhoven C
    76. Livingston G
    77. Bass NJ
    78. Gurling H
    79. McQuillin A
    80. Gwilliam R
    81. Deloukas P
    82. Al-Chalabi A
    83. Shaw CE
    84. Tsolaki M
    85. Singleton AB
    86. Guerreiro R
    87. Mühleisen TW
    88. Nöthen MM
    89. Moebus S
    90. Jöckel KH
    91. Klopp N
    92. Wichmann HE
    93. Pankratz VS
    94. Sando SB
    95. Aasly JO
    96. Barcikowska M
    97. Wszolek ZK
    98. Dickson DW
    99. Graff-Radford NR
    100. Petersen RC
    101. van Duijn CM
    102. Breteler MMB
    103. Ikram MA
    104. DeStefano AL
    105. Fitzpatrick AL
    106. Lopez O
    107. Launer LJ
    108. Seshadri S
    109. CHARGE consortium
    110. Berr C
    111. Campion D
    112. Epelbaum J
    113. Dartigues JF
    114. Tzourio C
    115. Alpérovitch A
    116. Lathrop M
    117. EADI1 consortium
    118. Feulner TM
    119. Friedrich P
    120. Riehle C
    121. Krawczak M
    122. Schreiber S
    123. Mayhaus M
    124. Nicolhaus S
    125. Wagenpfeil S
    126. Steinberg S
    127. Stefansson H
    128. Stefansson K
    129. Snaedal J
    130. Björnsson S
    131. Jonsson PV
    132. Chouraki V
    133. Genier-Boley B
    134. Hiltunen M
    135. Soininen H
    136. Combarros O
    137. Zelenika D
    138. Delepine M
    139. Bullido MJ
    140. Pasquier F
    141. Mateo I
    142. Frank-Garcia A
    143. Porcellini E
    144. Hanon O
    145. Coto E
    146. Alvarez V
    147. Bosco P
    148. Siciliano G
    149. Mancuso M
    150. Panza F
    151. Solfrizzi V
    152. Nacmias B
    153. Sorbi S
    154. Bossù P
    155. Piccardi P
    156. Arosio B
    157. Annoni G
    158. Seripa D
    159. Pilotto A
    160. Scarpini E
    161. Galimberti D
    162. Brice A
    163. Hannequin D
    164. Licastro F
    165. Jones L
    166. Holmans PA
    167. Jonsson T
    168. Riemenschneider M
    169. Morgan K
    170. Younkin SG
    171. Owen MJ
    172. O’Donovan M
    173. Amouyel P
    174. Williams J
    (2011) Common variants at ABCA7, MS4A6A/MS4A4E, EPHA1, CD33 and CD2AP are associated with Alzheimer’s disease
    Nature Genetics 43:429–435.
    https://doi.org/10.1038/ng.803
    1. Lambert JC
    2. Ibrahim-Verbaas CA
    3. Harold D
    4. Naj AC
    5. Sims R
    6. Bellenguez C
    7. DeStafano AL
    8. Bis JC
    9. Beecham GW
    10. Grenier-Boley B
    11. Russo G
    12. Thorton-Wells TA
    13. Jones N
    14. Smith AV
    15. Chouraki V
    16. Thomas C
    17. Ikram MA
    18. Zelenika D
    19. Vardarajan BN
    20. Kamatani Y
    21. Lin CF
    22. Gerrish A
    23. Schmidt H
    24. Kunkle B
    25. Dunstan ML
    26. Ruiz A
    27. Bihoreau MT
    28. Choi SH
    29. Reitz C
    30. Pasquier F
    31. Cruchaga C
    32. Craig D
    33. Amin N
    34. Berr C
    35. Lopez OL
    36. De Jager PL
    37. Deramecourt V
    38. Johnston JA
    39. Evans D
    40. Lovestone S
    41. Letenneur L
    42. Morón FJ
    43. Rubinsztein DC
    44. Eiriksdottir G
    45. Sleegers K
    46. Goate AM
    47. Fiévet N
    48. Huentelman MW
    49. Gill M
    50. Brown K
    51. Kamboh MI
    52. Keller L
    53. Barberger-Gateau P
    54. McGuiness B
    55. Larson EB
    56. Green R
    57. Myers AJ
    58. Dufouil C
    59. Todd S
    60. Wallon D
    61. Love S
    62. Rogaeva E
    63. Gallacher J
    64. St George-Hyslop P
    65. Clarimon J
    66. Lleo A
    67. Bayer A
    68. Tsuang DW
    69. Yu L
    70. Tsolaki M
    71. Bossù P
    72. Spalletta G
    73. Proitsi P
    74. Collinge J
    75. Sorbi S
    76. Sanchez-Garcia F
    77. Fox NC
    78. Hardy J
    79. Deniz Naranjo MC
    80. Bosco P
    81. Clarke R
    82. Brayne C
    83. Galimberti D
    84. Mancuso M
    85. Matthews F
    86. Cohorts for Heart and Aging Research in Genomic Epidemiology
    87. Moebus S
    88. Mecocci P
    89. Del Zompo M
    90. Maier W
    91. Hampel H
    92. Pilotto A
    93. Bullido M
    94. Panza F
    95. Caffarra P
    96. Nacmias B
    97. Gilbert JR
    98. Mayhaus M
    99. Lannefelt L
    100. Hakonarson H
    101. Pichler S
    102. Carrasquillo MM
    103. Ingelsson M
    104. Beekly D
    105. Alvarez V
    106. Zou F
    107. Valladares O
    108. Younkin SG
    109. Coto E
    110. Hamilton-Nelson KL
    111. Gu W
    112. Razquin C
    113. Pastor P
    114. Mateo I
    115. Owen MJ
    116. Faber KM
    117. Jonsson PV
    118. Combarros O
    119. O’Donovan MC
    120. Cantwell LB
    121. Soininen H
    122. Blacker D
    123. Mead S
    124. Mosley TH
    125. Bennett DA
    126. Harris TB
    127. Fratiglioni L
    128. Holmes C
    129. de Bruijn RF
    130. Passmore P
    131. Montine TJ
    132. Bettens K
    133. Rotter JI
    134. Brice A
    135. Morgan K
    136. Foroud TM
    137. Kukull WA
    138. Hannequin D
    139. Powell JF
    140. Nalls MA
    141. Ritchie K
    142. Lunetta KL
    143. Kauwe JS
    144. Boerwinkle E
    145. Riemenschneider M
    146. Boada M
    147. Hiltuenen M
    148. Martin ER
    149. Schmidt R
    150. Rujescu D
    151. Wang LS
    152. Dartigues JF
    153. Mayeux R
    154. Tzourio C
    155. Hofman A
    156. Nöthen MM
    157. Graff C
    158. Psaty BM
    159. Jones L
    160. Haines JL
    161. Holmans PA
    162. Lathrop M
    163. Pericak-Vance MA
    164. Launer LJ
    165. Farrer LA
    166. van Duijn CM
    167. Van Broeckhoven C
    168. Moskvina V
    169. Seshadri S
    170. Williams J
    171. Schellenberg GD
    172. Amouyel P
    (2013) Meta-analysis of 74,046 individuals identifies 11 new susceptibility loci for Alzheimer’s disease
    Nature Genetics 45:1452–1458.
    https://doi.org/10.1038/ng.2802
    1. Naj AC
    2. Jun G
    3. Beecham GW
    4. Wang L-S
    5. Vardarajan BN
    6. Buros J
    7. Gallins PJ
    8. Buxbaum JD
    9. Jarvik GP
    10. Crane PK
    11. Larson EB
    12. Bird TD
    13. Boeve BF
    14. Graff-Radford NR
    15. De Jager PL
    16. Evans D
    17. Schneider JA
    18. Carrasquillo MM
    19. Ertekin-Taner N
    20. Younkin SG
    21. Cruchaga C
    22. Kauwe JSK
    23. Nowotny P
    24. Kramer P
    25. Hardy J
    26. Huentelman MJ
    27. Myers AJ
    28. Barmada MM
    29. Demirci FY
    30. Baldwin CT
    31. Green RC
    32. Rogaeva E
    33. St George-Hyslop P
    34. Arnold SE
    35. Barber R
    36. Beach T
    37. Bigio EH
    38. Bowen JD
    39. Boxer A
    40. Burke JR
    41. Cairns NJ
    42. Carlson CS
    43. Carney RM
    44. Carroll SL
    45. Chui HC
    46. Clark DG
    47. Corneveaux J
    48. Cotman CW
    49. Cummings JL
    50. DeCarli C
    51. DeKosky ST
    52. Diaz-Arrastia R
    53. Dick M
    54. Dickson DW
    55. Ellis WG
    56. Faber KM
    57. Fallon KB
    58. Farlow MR
    59. Ferris S
    60. Frosch MP
    61. Galasko DR
    62. Ganguli M
    63. Gearing M
    64. Geschwind DH
    65. Ghetti B
    66. Gilbert JR
    67. Gilman S
    68. Giordani B
    69. Glass JD
    70. Growdon JH
    71. Hamilton RL
    72. Harrell LE
    73. Head E
    74. Honig LS
    75. Hulette CM
    76. Hyman BT
    77. Jicha GA
    78. Jin L-W
    79. Johnson N
    80. Karlawish J
    81. Karydas A
    82. Kaye JA
    83. Kim R
    84. Koo EH
    85. Kowall NW
    86. Lah JJ
    87. Levey AI
    88. Lieberman AP
    89. Lopez OL
    90. Mack WJ
    91. Marson DC
    92. Martiniuk F
    93. Mash DC
    94. Masliah E
    95. McCormick WC
    96. McCurry SM
    97. McDavid AN
    98. McKee AC
    99. Mesulam M
    100. Miller BL
    101. Miller CA
    102. Miller JW
    103. Parisi JE
    104. Perl DP
    105. Peskind E
    106. Petersen RC
    107. Poon WW
    108. Quinn JF
    109. Rajbhandary RA
    110. Raskind M
    111. Reisberg B
    112. Ringman JM
    113. Roberson ED
    114. Rosenberg RN
    115. Sano M
    116. Schneider LS
    117. Seeley W
    118. Shelanski ML
    119. Slifer MA
    120. Smith CD
    121. Sonnen JA
    122. Spina S
    123. Stern RA
    124. Tanzi RE
    125. Trojanowski JQ
    126. Troncoso JC
    127. Van Deerlin VM
    128. Vinters HV
    129. Vonsattel JP
    130. Weintraub S
    131. Welsh-Bohmer KA
    132. Williamson J
    133. Woltjer RL
    134. Cantwell LB
    135. Dombroski BA
    136. Beekly D
    137. Lunetta KL
    138. Martin ER
    139. Kamboh MI
    140. Saykin AJ
    141. Reiman EM
    142. Bennett DA
    143. Morris JC
    144. Montine TJ
    145. Goate AM
    146. Blacker D
    147. Tsuang DW
    148. Hakonarson H
    149. Kukull WA
    150. Foroud TM
    151. Haines JL
    152. Mayeux R
    153. Pericak-Vance MA
    154. Farrer LA
    155. Schellenberg GD
    (2011) Common variants at MS4A4/MS4A6E, CD2AP, CD33 and EPHA1 are associated with late-onset Alzheimer’s disease
    Nature Genetics 43:436–441.
    https://doi.org/10.1038/ng.801

Article and author information

Author details

  1. Donghui Yan

    University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, United States
    Contribution
    Data curation, Software, Formal analysis, Validation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review and editing
    Contributed equally with
    Bowen Hu
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0002-5433-0764
  2. Bowen Hu

    Department of Statistics, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, United States
    Contribution
    Data curation, Formal analysis, Validation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review and editing
    Contributed equally with
    Donghui Yan
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
  3. Burcu F Darst

    Department of Population Health Sciences, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, United States
    Contribution
    Data curation, Writing – review and editing
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
  4. Shubhabrata Mukherjee

    Division of General Internal Medicine, Department of Medicine, University of Washington, Seattle, United States
    Contribution
    Data curation, Writing – review and editing
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
  5. Brian W Kunkle

    University of Miami Miller School of Medicine, Miami, United States
    Contribution
    Data curation, Writing – review and editing
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
  6. Yuetiva Deming

    Department of Population Health Sciences, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, United States
    Contribution
    Data curation, Writing – review and editing
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0001-7512-5703
  7. Logan Dumitrescu

    Vanderbilt Memory and Alzheimer’s Center, Vanderbilt University Medical Center, Vanderbilt University School of Medicine, Nashville, United States
    Contribution
    Data curation, Writing – review and editing
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
  8. Yunling Wang

    University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, United States
    Contribution
    Data curation, Formal analysis, Writing – review and editing
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
  9. Adam Naj

    School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, United States
    Contribution
    Data curation, Writing – review and editing
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
  10. Amanda Kuzma

    School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, United States
    Contribution
    Data curation, Writing – review and editing
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
  11. Yi Zhao

    School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, United States
    Contribution
    Data curation, Writing – review and editing
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
  12. Hyunseung Kang

    Department of Statistics, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, United States
    Contribution
    Data curation, Writing – review and editing
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
  13. Sterling C Johnson

    1. Wisconsin Alzheimer’s Institute, University of Wisconsin School of Medicine and Public Health, Madison, United States
    2. Geriatric Research Education and Clinical Center, Wm. S. Middleton Memorial VA Hospital, Madison, United States
    3. Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center, University of Wisconsin School of Medicine and Public Health, Madison, United States
    Contribution
    Data curation, Writing – review and editing
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
  14. Cruchaga Carlos

    Department of Psychiatry, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, United States
    Contribution
    Data curation, Writing – review and editing
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
  15. Timothy J Hohman

    Vanderbilt Memory and Alzheimer’s Center, Vanderbilt University Medical Center, Vanderbilt University School of Medicine, Nashville, United States
    Contribution
    Data curation, Writing – review and editing
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
  16. Paul K Crane

    Division of General Internal Medicine, Department of Medicine, University of Washington, Seattle, United States
    Contribution
    Data curation, Writing – review and editing
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
  17. Corinne D Engelman

    1. Department of Population Health Sciences, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, United States
    2. Wisconsin Alzheimer’s Institute, University of Wisconsin School of Medicine and Public Health, Madison, United States
    3. Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center, University of Wisconsin School of Medicine and Public Health, Madison, United States
    Contribution
    Data curation, Writing – review and editing
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
  18. Alzheimer’s Disease Genetics Consortium (ADGC)

    Contribution
    Data curation
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
  19. Qiongshi Lu

    1. Department of Statistics, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, United States
    2. Department of Biostatistics and Medical Informatics, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, United States
    Contribution
    Conceptualization, Resources, Data curation, Formal analysis, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Project administration, Writing – review and editing
    For correspondence
    qlu@biostat.wisc.edu
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0002-4514-0969

Funding

National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences (Clinical and Translational Science Award (CTSA) program, UL1TR000427)

  • Bowen Hu
  • Yunling Wang
  • Qiongshi Lu
  • Donghui Yan

U.S. National Library of Medicine (Computation and Informatics in Biology and Medicine Training Program, NLM 5T15LM007359)

  • Burcu F Darst

National Institutes of Health (R01AG054047)

  • Corinne D Engelman
  • Qiongshi Lu

National Institutes of Health (R01AG27161)

  • Sterling C Johnson

National Institutes of Health (UL1TR000427)

  • Burcu F Darst

National Institutes of Health (P2C HD047873)

  • Corinne D Engelman

NIH/NIA (U01 AG032984)

  • Alzheimer’s Disease Genetics Consortium (ADGC)

NIH/NIA (U01 AG016976)

  • Alzheimer’s Disease Genetics Consortium (ADGC)

NIH/NIA (U01 AG016976)

  • Alzheimer’s Disease Genetics Consortium (ADGC)

Alzheimer’s Association (ADGC–10–196,728)

  • Alzheimer’s Disease Genetics Consortium (ADGC)

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Acknowledgements

This project was supported by the Clinical and Translational Science Award (CTSA) program, through the NIH National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences (NCATS), grant UL1TR000427. Support for this research was also provided by the University of Wisconsin-Madison Office of the Chancellor and the Vice Chancellor for Research and Graduate Education with funding from the Wisconsin Alumni Research Foundation. BFD was supported by an NLM training grant to the Computation and Informatics in Biology and Medicine Training Program [NLM 5T15LM007359]. This research was also supported by the NIH [grants R01AG054047, R01AG27161, UL1TR000427, and P2C HD047873], Helen Bader Foundation, Northwestern Mutual Foundation, Extendicare Foundation, and the State of Wisconsin. The authors thank the University of Wisconsin Madison Biotechnology Center Gene Expression Center for providing Illumina Infinium genotyping services. We thank the International Genomics of Alzheimer’s Project (IGAP) for providing summary results data for these analyses. The investigators within IGAP contributed to the design and implementation of IGAP and/or provided data but did not participate in analysis or writing of this report. IGAP was made possible by the generous participation of the subjects and their families. The i-Select chips were funded by the French National Foundation on Alzheimer’s disease and related disorders. EADI was supported by the LABEX (laboratory of excellence program investment for the future) DISTALZ grant, Inserm, Institut Pasteur de Lille, Université de Lille 2, and the Lille University Hospital. GERAD was supported by the Medical Research Council (Grant n° 503480), Alzheimer’s Research UK (Grant n° 503176), the Wellcome Trust (Grant n° 082604/2/07/Z), and German Federal Ministry of Education and Research (BMBF): Competence Network Dementia (CND) grant n° 01GI0102, 01GI0711, 01GI0420. CHARGE was partly supported by the NIH/NIA grant R01 AG033193 and the NIA AG081220 and AGES contract N01–AG–12100, the NHLBI grant R01 HL105756, the Icelandic Heart Association, and the Erasmus Medical Center and Erasmus University. ADGC was supported by the NIH/NIA grants: U01 AG032984, U24 AG021886, U01 AG016976, and the Alzheimer’s Association grant ADGC–10–196,728. We thank contributors who collected samples used in this study, as well as patients and their families, whose help and participation made this work possible; Data for this study were prepared, archived, and distributed by the National Institute on Aging Alzheimer’s Disease Data Storage Site (NIAGADS) at the University of Pennsylvania (U24-AG041689-01). We are also grateful for ADGC and its investigators for providing GWAS summary statistics for various AD phenotypes. The full acknowledgement to ADGC is included in the Supplementary file 1.

Version history

  1. Sent for peer review:
  2. Preprint posted:
  3. Reviewed Preprint version 1:
  4. Version of Record published:

Cite all versions

You can cite all versions using the DOI https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.91360. This DOI represents all versions, and will always resolve to the latest one.

Copyright

© 2023, Yan, Hu et al.

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.

Metrics

  • 571
    views
  • 44
    downloads
  • 8
    citations

Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Donghui Yan
  2. Bowen Hu
  3. Burcu F Darst
  4. Shubhabrata Mukherjee
  5. Brian W Kunkle
  6. Yuetiva Deming
  7. Logan Dumitrescu
  8. Yunling Wang
  9. Adam Naj
  10. Amanda Kuzma
  11. Yi Zhao
  12. Hyunseung Kang
  13. Sterling C Johnson
  14. Cruchaga Carlos
  15. Timothy J Hohman
  16. Paul K Crane
  17. Corinne D Engelman
  18. Alzheimer’s Disease Genetics Consortium (ADGC)
  19. Qiongshi Lu
(2024)
Biobank-wide association scan identifies risk factors for late-onset Alzheimer’s disease and endophenotypes
eLife 12:RP91360.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.91360.2

Share this article

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.91360

Further reading

    1. Epidemiology and Global Health
    2. Genetics and Genomics
    Tianyu Zhao, Hui Li ... Li Chen
    Research Article

    Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is a complex degenerative disease of the central nervous system, and elucidating its pathogenesis remains challenging. In this study, we used the inverse-variance weighted (IVW) model as the major analysis method to perform hypothesis-free Mendelian randomization (MR) analysis on the data from MRC IEU OpenGWAS (18,097 exposure traits and 16 AD outcome traits), and conducted sensitivity analysis with six models, to assess the robustness of the IVW results, to identify various classes of risk or protective factors for AD, early-onset AD, and late-onset AD. We generated 400,274 data entries in total, among which the major analysis method of the IVW model consists of 73,129 records with 4840 exposure traits, which fall into 10 categories: Disease, Medical laboratory science, Imaging, Anthropometric, Treatment, Molecular trait, Gut microbiota, Past history, Family history, and Lifestyle trait. More importantly, a freely accessed online platform called MRAD (https://gwasmrad.com/mrad/) has been developed using the Shiny package with MR analysis results. Additionally, novel potential AD therapeutic targets (CD33, TBCA, VPS29, GNAI3, PSME1) are identified, among which CD33 was positively associated with the main outcome traits of AD, as well as with both EOAD and LOAD. TBCA and VPS29 were negatively associated with the main outcome traits of AD, as well as with both EOAD and LOAD. GNAI3 and PSME1 were negatively associated with the main outcome traits of AD, as well as with LOAD, but had no significant causal association with EOAD. The findings of our research advance our understanding of the etiology of AD.

    1. Chromosomes and Gene Expression
    2. Genetics and Genomics
    Hasiba Asma, Ellen Tieke ... Marc S Halfon
    Tools and Resources

    Annotation of newly sequenced genomes frequently includes genes, but rarely covers important non-coding genomic features such as the cis-regulatory modules—e.g., enhancers and silencers—that regulate gene expression. Here, we begin to remedy this situation by developing a workflow for rapid initial annotation of insect regulatory sequences, and provide a searchable database resource with enhancer predictions for 33 genomes. Using our previously developed SCRMshaw computational enhancer prediction method, we predict over 2.8 million regulatory sequences along with the tissues where they are expected to be active, in a set of insect species ranging over 360 million years of evolution. Extensive analysis and validation of the data provides several lines of evidence suggesting that we achieve a high true-positive rate for enhancer prediction. One, we show that our predictions target specific loci, rather than random genomic locations. Two, we predict enhancers in orthologous loci across a diverged set of species to a significantly higher degree than random expectation would allow. Three, we demonstrate that our predictions are highly enriched for regions of accessible chromatin. Four, we achieve a validation rate in excess of 70% using in vivo reporter gene assays. As we continue to annotate both new tissues and new species, our regulatory annotation resource will provide a rich source of data for the research community and will have utility for both small-scale (single gene, single species) and large-scale (many genes, many species) studies of gene regulation. In particular, the ability to search for functionally related regulatory elements in orthologous loci should greatly facilitate studies of enhancer evolution even among distantly related species.