Cognitive Neuroscience: Memorable first impressions

Our ability to recall details from a remembered image depends on a single mechanism that is engaged from the very moment the image disappears from view.
  1. Emilio Salinas  Is a corresponding author
  2. Bashirul I Sheikh
  1. Department of Translational Neuroscience, Wake Forest University School of Medicine, United States
  2. Neuroscience Graduate Program, Wake Forest University School of Medicine, United States

Look out the window and see what stands out. Perhaps you notice some red and pink azaleas in full bloom. Now close your eyes and picture that scene in your mind. Initially, the colors and silhouettes linger vividly, but the details wither rapidly, leaving only a faded version of the image. As time passes, the accuracy with which an image can be recalled drops abruptly.

Memory is a critical, wonderful, multifaceted mental capacity that relies on many structures and mechanisms throughout the brain (Baddeley, 2003; Squire and Wixted, 2011; Schacter et al., 2012). This is not surprising, given the diversity of timescales and data types – such as images, words, facts and motor skills – that we can remember. Studies have shown that our visual memories are strongest immediately after an image disappears, remaining reliable for about half a second. This has traditionally been attributed to ‘iconic’ memory, which is thought to rely on a direct readout of stimulus-driven activity in visual circuits in the brain. In this case, the memory remains vivid because, after the stimulus (i.e., the image) has been removed, the visual activity takes some time to decay (Sperling, 1960; Pratte, 2018; Teeuwen et al., 2021).

In contrast, recalling an image a second or so after it has disappeared engages a different type of memory – visual working memory – that relies on information stored in different circuits in the frontal lobe (Pasternak and Greenlee, 2005; D’Esposito and Postle, 2015). Although not as vivid, the stored image remains stable for much longer. This is because, despite being more robust, the storage capacity for visual working memory is more limited: fewer items and less detail can be recalled from a remembered image. Together, these findings led to the idea that there are two distinct short-term memory mechanisms. Now, in eLife, Ivan Tomić and Paul Bays report strong evidence indicating that iconic memory and visual working memory are part of the same recall mechanism (Tomić and Bays, 2023).

Tomić and Bays – who are based at the University of Zagreb and the University of Cambridge – first constructed a detailed computational model to describe how sensory information is passed to a visual working memory circuit for storage and later recall (Figure 1). In this model, visual neurons respond to the presentation of an image containing a few items. This stimulus causes sensory activity to rise smoothly while the input lasts, and to decay once the stimulus ceases, consistent with previous experiments (Teeuwen et al., 2021). This sensory response then drives a population of visual working memory neurons that can sustain their activity in the absence of a stimulus, although this activity will eventually be corrupted due to noise (Wimmer et al., 2014; DePasquale et al., 2018). An important feature of the model is that each remembered item is allocated an equal fraction of the maximum possible working memory activity.

Timeline of events during stimulus presentation and storage.

A visual stimulus (grey box containing pattern) with N items is presented for a period of time (pale blue region). Sensory activity increases to a maximum value during this period, and then decays when the stimulus disappears. For each item, VWM activity also increases towards an effective saturation limit, which is the maximum possible value divided by the number of items presented: here N=2, so the effective saturation limit is half the maximum possible value. When the target item is cued (black arrow; top) at a later time (yellow region), the non-target item(s) are removed from memory (grey trace), and activity associated with the target item (green trace) increases towards the maximum possible value. The level of activity (and hence the accuracy of memory recall) will vary more and more over time due to noise. VWM: visual working memory.

Image credit: Adapted from Figure 2 in the paper by Tomić and Bays, 2023.

The model constructed by Tomić and Bays can make specific testable predictions. For example, it predicts that if an item is cued for later recall while the sensory signal is still present, the working memory activity associated with the non-targets will decay rapidly, freeing up resources and thus increasing the working memory activity associated with the cued item. This leads to more accurate recall of the item. In contrast, if an item is cued for later recall once the sensory signal has approached zero, this ‘boost’ does not happen, and the item is not recalled as accurately. In addition, the working memory activity should increase with longer exposure to the stimulus and should decrease as the number of remembered items increases.

These predictions were confirmed through experiments with humans. Participants were shown visual stimuli while several factors were varied, including the number of items to be remembered, the duration of the stimulus, the time at which the item to be recalled was identified, and the time of the actual recall. The results of these experiments are consistent with the notion that, during recall, visual information is always read out from the same population of neurons.

The findings of Tomić and Bays are satisfying for their simplicity; what seemed to require two separate mechanisms is explained by a single framework aligned with many prior studies. However, models always require simplifications and shortcuts. For instance, much evidence indicates that both frontal lobe circuits and sensory areas contribute to the self-sustained maintenance of activity that underlies the short-term memory of sensory events (Pasternak and Greenlee, 2005). Therefore, visual working memory is likely the result of continuous recurrent dynamics across areas (DePasquale et al., 2018; Stroud et al., 2024). Furthermore, there is still debate about the degree to which visual working memory implies equal sharing of resources, as opposed to some items receiving larger or smaller shares (Ma et al., 2014; Pratte, 2018). Nevertheless, the proposed model is certainly an important advance that future studies can build upon.

References

Article and author information

Author details

  1. Emilio Salinas

    Emilio Salinas is in the Department of Translational Neuroscience, Wake Forest University School of Medicine, Winston-Salem, United States

    For correspondence
    esalinas@wakehealth.edu
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0001-7411-5693
  2. Bashirul I Sheikh

    Bashirul I Sheikh is in the Neuroscience Graduate Program, Wake Forest University School of Medicine, Winston-Salem, United States

    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0002-3987-3891

Publication history

  1. Version of Record published: May 3, 2024 (version 1)

Copyright

© 2024, Salinas and Sheikh

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.

Metrics

  • 506
    views
  • 42
    downloads
  • 0
    citations

Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Emilio Salinas
  2. Bashirul I Sheikh
(2024)
Cognitive Neuroscience: Memorable first impressions
eLife 13:e98274.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.98274

Further reading

    1. Neuroscience
    Sandra P Cárdenas-García, Sundas Ijaz, Alberto E Pereda
    Research Article

    Most nervous systems combine both transmitter-mediated and direct cell-cell communication, known as 'chemical' and 'electrical' synapses, respectively. Chemical synapses can be identified by their multiple structural components. Electrical synapses are, on the other hand, generally defined by the presence of a 'gap junction' (a cluster of intercellular channels) between two neuronal processes. However, while gap junctions provide the communicating mechanism, it is unknown whether electrical transmission requires the contribution of additional cellular structures. We investigated this question at identifiable single synaptic contacts on the zebrafish Mauthner cells, at which gap junctions coexist with specializations for neurotransmitter release and where the contact unequivocally defines the anatomical limits of a synapse. Expansion microscopy of these single contacts revealed a detailed map of the incidence and spatial distribution of proteins pertaining to various synaptic structures. Multiple gap junctions of variable size were identified by the presence of their molecular components. Remarkably, most of the synaptic contact's surface was occupied by interleaving gap junctions and components of adherens junctions, suggesting a close functional association between these two structures. In contrast, glutamate receptors were confined to small peripheral portions of the contact, indicating that most of the synaptic area functions as an electrical synapse. Thus, our results revealed the overarching organization of an electrical synapse that operates with not one, but multiple gap junctions, in close association with structural and signaling molecules known to be components of adherens junctions. The relationship between these intercellular structures will aid in establishing the boundaries of electrical synapses found throughout animal connectomes and provide insight into the structural organization and functional diversity of electrical synapses.

    1. Neuroscience
    Alexandra L Jellinger, Rebecca L Suthard ... Steve Ramirez
    Research Article

    Negative memories engage a brain and body-wide stress response in humans that can alter cognition and behavior. Prolonged stress responses induce maladaptive cellular, circuit, and systems-level changes that can lead to pathological brain states and corresponding disorders in which mood and memory are affected. However, it is unclear if repeated activation of cells processing negative memories induces similar phenotypes in mice. In this study, we used an activity-dependent tagging method to access neuronal ensembles and assess their molecular characteristics. Sequencing memory engrams in mice revealed that positive (male-to-female exposure) and negative (foot shock) cells upregulated genes linked to anti- and pro-inflammatory responses, respectively. To investigate the impact of persistent activation of negative engrams, we chemogenetically activated them in the ventral hippocampus over 3 months and conducted anxiety and memory-related tests. Negative engram activation increased anxiety behaviors in both 6- and 14-month-old mice, reduced spatial working memory in older mice, impaired fear extinction in younger mice, and heightened fear generalization in both age groups. Immunohistochemistry revealed changes in microglial and astrocytic structure and number in the hippocampus. In summary, repeated activation of negative memories induces lasting cellular and behavioral abnormalities in mice, offering insights into the negative effects of chronic negative thinking-like behaviors on human health.