Psychophysics: Time is of the essence for auditory scene analysis

  1. Andrew R Dykstra  Is a corresponding author
  2. Alexander Gutschalk  Is a corresponding author
  1. Ruprecht-Karls-Universität Heidelberg, Germany

On a busy street corner or in a crowded bar, sounds from many different sources mix together before entering the ear canal. However, despite possessing just two ears, humans and other animals are remarkably adept at sorting out which sounds belong to which source. This process, known as auditory scene analysis (Bregman, 1990), is thought to underlie our ability to selectively listen to a single auditory ‘stream’ amidst competing streams: the so-called ‘cocktail party problem’ (Cherry, 1953; Broadbent, 1954). The loss of this ability is one of the most significant difficulties faced by individuals with hearing loss or damage to the auditory system, and may also be affected by the normal ageing process.

In contrast to the complexity of the acoustic environments we encounter on a daily basis, the vast majority of laboratory investigations into auditory scene analysis have used quite simple signals, often consisting of only a few elements (Figure 1A). Such stimuli have been used in an extensive body of research, including behavioural studies, neuroimaging experiments, and direct neuronal recordings. This research has told us a lot about the fundamental ways in which humans process sound, but some have questioned how relevant such simple stimuli are in understanding how we appreciate music or perceive speech at a cocktail party. Now, in eLife, Timothy Griffiths and co-workers—including Sundeep Teki and Maria Chait as joint first authors—report how they have used a new stimulus that more closely approximates natural acoustic signals to demonstrate that temporal coherence (that is, the coincidence of sound elements in and across time) is fundamental to auditory scene analysis in humans (Teki et al., 2013).

Representations of the relationship between time and frequency in three different types of stimuli that have been used to study auditory scene analysis.

(A) The galloping ABA_ paradigm introduced by Van Noorden (1975): when subjects are played two tones that differ little in frequency (lower panel), they report hearing a single, galloping stream. Conversely, when the difference in frequency is large and the low and high tones are out of synch (upper panel), listeners report hearing two regular streams simultaneously. (B) The jittered informational masking paradigm introduced by Gutschalk et al. (2008): although the blue target tones are easy to discriminate visually from the multi-tone background, listeners do not always hear them. (C) The stochastic figure-ground stimuli introduced by Teki et al. (2011) (blue bars) contain elements of different frequencies, making them more like the sounds we encounter in everyday life than A and B.

The first models of our ability to segregate sound sources were based on data from behavioural, neurophysiological and imaging experiments in which subjects listened to various acoustic stimuli similar to those in Figure 1A and were asked to report whether they heard one or two streams of sound. The results of many such experiments are consistent with a model of auditory scene analysis in which the perception of a stream of sound is associated with the activity of a particular population of neurons, which can be readily distinguished from the activity of other populations (for a review see Micheyl et al., 2007). However, recent work has shown that sounds that clearly activate distinct neuronal populations can, when synchronous, result in the percept of a single stream (Elhilali et al., 2009). This led to the proposal that, subsequent to the auditory input being broken down into features such as pitch or spatial location, the sound from a single source is bound back together by temporal coherence between the neuronal populations representing its constituent features (Elhilali et al., 2009; Shamma et al., 2011).

Teki, Chait and co-workers—who are based at University College London, Newcastle University and the University of Maryland—extend previous work by devising a new ‘stochastic figure-ground’ stimulus (Figure 1C) that requires listeners to integrate information across time and frequency in order to perceive the blue ‘figure’ as separate from the background. They find that human listeners are quite sensitive to such figures. Furthermore, using computational modelling, they demonstrate that temporal coherence can at least qualitatively account for the results of behavioural experiments—which models based purely on the activation of separate populations struggle to explain. Because competing streams of speech also overlap in time and frequency, the data obtained with these stimuli further suggest that the brain could use this approach to solve the cocktail-party problem.

Although the current work is a substantial advance, and indicates that the human auditory system likely performs temporal coherence analysis, several questions remain unanswered. We know little about how or where this analysis might be performed in the brain, or how the results of such an analysis might be utilized by other brain regions. An earlier fMRI study revealed that activity in a region of the brain called the intraparietal sulcus increased when these new stimuli were perceived (Teki et al., 2011). They therefore propose that the intraparietal sulcus either carries out temporal coherence computations or represents their output. This leaves open the possibility that these stimuli, and auditory streams generally, are segregated at a relatively early stage of processing, perhaps in auditory cortex. This would be consistent with recent research using other types of stimuli (Figure 1B) (Gutschalk et al., 2008; Dykstra, 2011) as well as updated versions of the temporal-coherence model (Shamma et al., 2013).

Moreover, there are several phenomena that indicate that mechanisms other than, or in addition to, temporal coherence are required to fully explain how we perceptually organize sound. Bistable perception—whereby identical stimuli can give rise to two or more distinct percepts—is a particularly relevant example. On its own, temporal coherence cannot account for the fact that the same stimulus in the classical streaming paradigm (Figure 1A) can be heard as either one or two streams, or that the targets in an informational-masking stimulus (Figure 1B) are only sometimes perceived. The complex relationship between these sounds and the percepts they generate likely depends on additional mechanisms, acting both before and after the brain computes temporal coherence. However, this model provides a new framework within which to examine such questions, and should spark exciting new avenues of research in auditory scene analysis.

References

  1. Book
    1. Bregman AS
    (1990)
    Auditory scene analysis: the perceptual organization of sound
    Cambridge: MIT Press.
  2. Book
    1. Dykstra AR
    (2011)
    Neural correlates of auditory perceptual organization measured with direct cortical recordings in humans [Thesis]
    Massachusetts Institute of Technology.
  3. Book
    1. Shamma S
    2. Elhilali M
    3. Ma L
    4. Micheyl C
    5. Oxenham AJ
    6. Pressnitzer D
    et al. (2013)
    Temporal coherence and the streaming of complex sounds
    In: Moore BCJ, Carlyon RP, Patterson RD, Gockel HE, Winter IM, editors. Basic aspects of hearing: physiology and perception. New York: Springer. pp. 535–544.
  4. Book
    1. Van Noorden L
    (1975)
    Temporal coherence in the perception of tone sequences [Thesis]
    Eindhoven, The Netherlands: Technical University Eindhoven.

Article and author information

Author details

  1. Andrew R Dykstra

    Auditory Cognition Lab, Department of Neurology, Ruprecht-Karls-Universität Heidelberg, Heidelberg, Germany
    For correspondence
    andrew.dykstra@med.uni-heidelberg.de
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  2. Alexander Gutschalk

    Auditory Cognition Lab, Department of Neurology, Ruprecht-Karls-Universität Heidelberg, Heidelberg, Germany
    For correspondence
    alexander.gutschalk@med.uni-heidelberg.de
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.

Publication history

  1. Version of Record published:

Copyright

© 2013, Dykstra and Gutschalk

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.

Metrics

  • 729
    views
  • 79
    downloads
  • 3
    citations

Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Andrew R Dykstra
  2. Alexander Gutschalk
(2013)
Psychophysics: Time is of the essence for auditory scene analysis
eLife 2:e01136.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.01136
  1. Further reading

Further reading

  1. Neuroscientists are getting closer to understanding how people can distinguish where different sounds come from in noisy environments.

    1. Neuroscience
    Rossella Conti, Céline Auger
    Research Article

    Granule cells of the cerebellum make up to 175,000 excitatory synapses on a single Purkinje cell, encoding the wide variety of information from the mossy fibre inputs into the cerebellar cortex. The granule cell axon is made of an ascending portion and a long parallel fibre extending at right angles, an architecture suggesting that synapses formed by the two segments of the axon could encode different information. There are controversial indications that ascending axon (AA) and parallel fibre (PF) synapse properties and modalities of plasticity are different. We tested the hypothesis that AA and PF synapses encode different information, and that the association of these distinct inputs to Purkinje cells might be relevant to the circuit and trigger plasticity, similar to the coincident activation of PF and climbing fibre inputs. Here, by recording synaptic currents in Purkinje cells from either proximal or distal granule cells (mostly AA and PF synapses, respectively), we describe a new form of associative plasticity between these two distinct granule cell inputs. We show for the first time that synchronous AA and PF repetitive train stimulation, with inhibition intact, triggers long-term potentiation (LTP) at AA synapses specifically. Furthermore, the timing of the presentation of the two inputs controls the outcome of plasticity and induction requires NMDAR and mGluR1 activation. The long length of the PFs allows us to preferentially activate the two inputs independently, and despite a lack of morphological reconstruction of the connections, these observations reinforce the suggestion that AA and PF synapses have different coding capabilities and plasticity that is associative, enabling effective association of information transmitted via granule cells.