Neural Plasticity: Restoring cerebellar-dependent learning

Behavioral and pharmaceutical interventions reverse defects associated with increased cerebellar long-term depression in a mouse model of Fragile X syndrome.
  1. Jessica L Verpeut  Is a corresponding author
  1. Department of Psychology, Arizona State University, United States

Neurodevelopmental disorders, such as Fragile X syndrome, are often characterized by neurons connecting incorrectly, leading to impairments in learning, memory and cognitive flexibility (Schmitt et al., 2022). Yet the brain can reorganize itself in response to an organism’s changing needs by altering the strength and number of connections (or synapses) between certain neurons. Whether therapeutics could restore behavioral deficits by encouraging nerve cells to 'rewire,' particularly when the brain is at its most malleable, remains a controversial question. In fact, enhancing neural plasticity has been shown to sometimes impair learning (Navakkode et al., 2022; Makin and Krakauer, 2023; Diniz and Crestani, 2023).

Long-term depression (LTD for short) is a type of plasticity that results in weaker synapses. Perhaps counterintuitively, this process is crucial to fine-tune neural responses and enable learning, memory and cognitive flexibility. For example, this is the case in the cerebellum, the brain center best known for controlling movement and learning new motor skills. There, output neurons, known as Purkinje cells, process and integrate the information received from parallel and climbing fibers originating from cells in other parts of the cerebellum or the nervous system. Both types of fibers work together to regulate the strength of the synapses between parallel fibers (PF for short) and Purkinje cells via LTD (Ito, 1989).

Oculomotor learning is a cerebellar mechanism by which the brain can use previous experiences to fine-tune how eye muscles compensate for head movements so that vision can remain steady (a process known as the vestibulo-ocular reflex, or VOR; Nguyen-Vu et al., 2017). Mice with increased LTD at the PF-Purkinje cell synapses have disrupted oculomotor learning, but why this is the case has remained unclear. Now, in eLife, Amin Shakhawat, Jennifer Raymond and colleagues at Stanford University report how this defect can be reverted in a mouse model of Fragile X syndrome (Shakhawat et al., 2023).

Scientists often use the VOR to examine cerebellar function and synaptic plasticity, as animals can be trained to adjust the strength of this reflex (Hirano and Inoshita, 2021; Ito, 1989; Clopath et al., 2014). For instance, mice trying to keep track of rotating black and white stripes while moving on a turntable will make smaller eye movements if this stimulus and the head are moving in the same direction (VOR-decrease learning). Conversely, these eye movements will increase if the stripes and the head travel in opposite directions (VOR-increase learning).

VOR-increase training relies heavily on LTD, yet studies in mice show that such learning is impaired when this type of plasticity is enhanced at PF-Purkinje cells; on the other hand, VOR-decrease learning, which is less dependent on long-term depression, is preserved. This led Shakhawat et al. to propose that enhanced LTD can be too easily triggered by erroneous signals; the cerebellar circuit responds by raising the threshold required to activate this plasticity, which then makes it more difficult for LTD to be recruited during learning.

To test this hypothesis, the team used mice in which Purkinje cells carry the mutation responsible for Fragile X syndrome – a genetic change known to enhance PF-Purkinje cell LTD. First, Shakhawat et al. confirmed that these animals (known as L7-Fmr1 KO mice) showed profound impairment in their VOR-increase learning, while their VOR-decrease learning remained intact. Next, they tested whether two types of interventions that could prevent or reduce a prior increase in activation threshold for LTD might be able to reverse these defects.

The behavioral intervention consisted of both wild-type and L7-Fmr1 KO mice receiving a VOR-decrease ‘pre-training’ session (which can improve LTD at PF-Purkinje cell synapses) before their VOR-increase training. This manipulation did not affect the control group, but it greatly improved VOR-increase learning in the mutated mice.

In the pharmaceutical intervention, Shakhawat et al. treated wild type and L7-Fmr1 KO animals with diazepam prior to VOR as this compound is known to decrease neural firing in the cerebellum (and would therefore prevent incorrect LTD activation prior to testing). Similar to pre-training, diazepam temporarily rescued VOR-increase learning impairments in L7-Fmr1 KO mice if administered 18-24hours before the task. The team further showed that diazepam could restore another type of LTD-dependent learning (known as the optokinetic reflex) for which these mice are also impaired. In fact, receiving this treatment led to the animals outperforming their wild-type counterparts.

Taken together, these results suggest that defects in VOR-increase learning can be rescued through specific pre-training or the use of certain pharmaceuticals (Figure 1). The findings support a model in which prior experiences reshape the activation thresholds of mice with enhanced LTD at their PF-Purkinje cells in such a way that subsequent activation of this plasticity is hindered.

Behavioral and pharmaceutical interventions can rescue vestibulo-ocular reflex increase learning in a mouse model of Fragile X syndrome.

Top: Genetically modified animals (L7-Fmr1 KO mice) whose Purkinje cells carry the mutation that causes Fragile X syndrome show impairments when trained to enhance their vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR-increase learning). This type of learning relies on the cerebellum (red brain structure). VOR-increase learning involves the animals keeping track of a stimulus (black and white stripes, top blue arrow) rotating in the opposite direction as head movement (bottom blue arrow). The defect is linked to the mice displaying increased long-term depression (LTD; thick black arrow) at the synapses between Purkinje cells (PC; black) and parallel fibers (PF; green). This type of neural plasticity is under the control of both parallel and climbing fibers (CF; red). Bottom: When the VOR-increase learning is preceded by VOR-decrease pre-training (whereby the visual stimulus and head rotate in the same direction) or administration of diazepam, PF-Purkinje cell long-term depression is normalized (thin black arrow) and VOR-increase learning is improved for stimuli rotating at high frequencies.

Image credit: Mouse by Ethan Tyler and Lex Kravitz (CC BY 4.0), mouse head by Luigi Petrucco (CC BY 4.0), Purkinje cells and climbing fibers by Akshay Markanday (CC BY 4.0).

Alternative biological mechanisms may also be involved, however. For instance, the interventions rescued VOR-increase learning when the stimuli used during training were rotating at high frequencies (1Hz), but not at low frequencies (0.5Hz) – possibly because the processing of such frequencies does not rely as much on long-term depression. The mutation associated with Fragile X syndrome may lead to cellular changes or disruptions that were not assessed in this study, and further work should examine whether the structure of Purkinje cells in this mouse model remains unchanged. Follow-up research should also review how these local changes may reshape the long-distance connections between Purkinje cells and neurons outside of the cerebellar microcircuit.

Overall, this work has exciting therapeutic implications for Fragile X syndrome while also expanding our understanding of cerebellar long-term depression. Such knowledge may be crucial as the cerebellum has been implicated in several neurodevelopmental and aging disorders, yet specific treatments that target this structure are still lacking (Wang et al., 2014; Arleo et al., 2024; Iskusnykh et al., 2024).

References

  1. Book
    1. Hirano T
    2. Inoshita T
    (2021) Contribution of norepinephrine to cerebellar long-term depression and motor learning
    In: Mizusawa H, Kakei S, editors. Cerebellum as a CNS Hub. Springer International Publishing. pp. 337–348.
    https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-75817-2_16

Article and author information

Author details

  1. Jessica L Verpeut

    Jessica L Verpeut is in the Department of Psychology, Arizona State University, Tempe, United States

    For correspondence
    jverpeut@asu.edu
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0003-2941-7697

Publication history

  1. Version of Record published:

Copyright

© 2024, Verpeut

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.

Metrics

  • 308
    views
  • 41
    downloads
  • 0
    citations

Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Jessica L Verpeut
(2024)
Neural Plasticity: Restoring cerebellar-dependent learning
eLife 13:e100251.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.100251

Further reading

    1. Neuroscience
    Katie Morris, Edita Bulovaite ... Mathew H Horrocks
    Research Article

    The concept that dimeric protein complexes in synapses can sequentially replace their subunits has been a cornerstone of Francis Crick’s 1984 hypothesis, explaining how long-term memories could be maintained in the face of short protein lifetimes. However, it is unknown whether the subunits of protein complexes that mediate memory are sequentially replaced in the brain and if this process is linked to protein lifetime. We address these issues by focusing on supercomplexes assembled by the abundant postsynaptic scaffolding protein PSD95, which plays a crucial role in memory. We used single-molecule detection, super-resolution microscopy and MINFLUX to probe the molecular composition of PSD95 supercomplexes in mice carrying genetically encoded HaloTags, eGFP, and mEoS2. We found a population of PSD95-containing supercomplexes comprised of two copies of PSD95, with a dominant 12.7 nm separation. Time-stamping of PSD95 subunits in vivo revealed that each PSD95 subunit was sequentially replaced over days and weeks. Comparison of brain regions showed subunit replacement was slowest in the cortex, where PSD95 protein lifetime is longest. Our findings reveal that protein supercomplexes within the postsynaptic density can be maintained by gradual replacement of individual subunits providing a mechanism for stable maintenance of their organization. Moreover, we extend Crick’s model by suggesting that synapses with slow subunit replacement of protein supercomplexes and long-protein lifetimes are specialized for long-term memory storage and that these synapses are highly enriched in superficial layers of the cortex where long-term memories are stored.

    1. Neuroscience
    Samyogita Hardikar, Bronte Mckeown ... Jonathan Smallwood
    Research Article

    Complex macro-scale patterns of brain activity that emerge during periods of wakeful rest provide insight into the organisation of neural function, how these differentiate individuals based on their traits, and the neural basis of different types of self-generated thoughts. Although brain activity during wakeful rest is valuable for understanding important features of human cognition, its unconstrained nature makes it difficult to disentangle neural features related to personality traits from those related to the thoughts occurring at rest. Our study builds on recent perspectives from work on ongoing conscious thought that highlight the interactions between three brain networks – ventral and dorsal attention networks, as well as the default mode network. We combined measures of personality with state-of-the-art indices of ongoing thoughts at rest and brain imaging analysis and explored whether this ‘tri-partite’ view can provide a framework within which to understand the contribution of states and traits to observed patterns of neural activity at rest. To capture macro-scale relationships between different brain systems, we calculated cortical gradients to describe brain organisation in a low-dimensional space. Our analysis established that for more introverted individuals, regions of the ventral attention network were functionally more aligned to regions of the somatomotor system and the default mode network. At the same time, a pattern of detailed self-generated thought was associated with a decoupling of regions of dorsal attention from regions in the default mode network. Our study, therefore, establishes that interactions between attention systems and the default mode network are important influences on ongoing thought at rest and highlights the value of integrating contemporary perspectives on conscious experience when understanding patterns of brain activity at rest.