Decision Making: Unraveling the enigmatic role of the subthalamic nucleus
Pondering whether “to be or not to be”, or impulsively “tilting at windmills”, decision-making comes in many forms, as so eloquently demonstrated in many great works of literature. But what causes us to sometimes come to quick conclusions while we struggle to make up our minds at other times?
Making decisions involves a complex interplay between numerous neurons located in various regions of the brain. The neural circuitry involved in this process remains, however, enigmatic. To study the cognitive processes implicated in making simple two-choice decisions, researchers often employ the Drift Diffusion Model. This mathematical framework assumes that information accumulates over time until a decision threshold is reached, and has three main parameters: the drift rate (the rate information accumulates), the decision threshold (the amount of information needed to make a decision), and the non-decision time (the time taken for processes not directly related to decision-making, such as stimulus encoding and response execution; Ratcliff, 1978; Ratcliff and McKoon, 2008).
Two of the most critical brain areas involved in decision-making are the prefrontal cortex and the hippocampus. More recently, it has been proposed that a region known as the subthalamic nucleus, which is part of the basal ganglia – and is therefore involved in motor control and integration – also has an integral role. Now, in eLife, Kathryn Rogers, Joshua Gold and Long Ding from the University of Pennsylvania report new insights into how the subthalamic nucleus contributes to decision-making (Rogers et al., 2024).
Previous computational models propose three roles for the subthalamic nucleus in decision-making and how it interacts with other parts of the brain (Figure 1): the subthalamic nucleus works with the medial prefrontal cortex to set thresholds for decision-making. These thresholds determine when enough information has been gathered to make a decision, helping control impulsivity (Hypothesis 1). Through its interaction with the external segment of the globus pallidus (a component of the basal ganglia), the subthalamic nucleus helps to calibrate how different options are evaluated, making sure the choices are assessed properly (Hypothesis 2). It helps to implement step-like, all-or-none nonlinear computations to improve the basal ganglia’s efficacy in adjusting decision bounds (Hypothesis 3; Frank, 2006; Cavanagh et al., 2011; Ratcliff and Frank, 2012; Bogacz and Gurney, 2007; Coulthard et al., 2012; Green et al., 2013; Lo and Wang, 2006; Wei et al., 2015).
![](https://iiif.elifesciences.org/lax:100598%2Felife-100598-fig1-v1.tif/full/617,/0/default.jpg)
The Drift Diffusion Model illustrated by Don Quixote’s decision-making.
Over time (t), Don Quixote (black mask) accumulates information that allows him to decide whether he is looking at a giant (option 1) or a windmill (option 2), and act accordingly. The subthalamic nucleus is thought to influence this decision-making process in three different ways, per the Drift Diffusion Model. First (H1), by adjusting the amount of information needed to make a decision, known as the decision threshold (represented as the gap between the two red unbroken lines increasing to the gap between the two dashed lines). Second (H2), by calibrating different pieces of evidence so they can be used to evaluate the two possibilities (whether an object is a windmill or a giant). This enables the evidence accumulation process to shift from weighting unreliable perceptual evidence too heavily (represented by the solid black line) to favoring more reliable evidence later in the decision process (represented by the dashed black line). Third (H3), by helping neural pathways sent from the basal ganglia to precisely adjust the boundaries of the decision threshold.
Image credit: Qianli Yang (CC BY 4.0).
Don Quixote’s mistaking of windmills for giants can be explained by all three hypotheses. His decision threshold may have been too low, causing him to react based on initial false evidence that the windmills’ rotating blades are the giants' flailing arms (Hypothesis 1). Similarly, he may have failed to adjust his decision threshold according to his peaceful surroundings (and thus failing to realize that he was surrounded by windmills, rather than giants (Hypothesis 3)), or to properly weigh his unreliable perception of the environment around him, which favored seeing an angry giant over a harmless windmill (Hypothesis 2).
To determine which hypothesis best explains the role of the subthalamic nucleus in decision-making, Rogers et al. recorded the activity of single neurons in the subthalamic nucleus of monkeys during a visual-saccadic decision task. In the experiment, the monkeys undertook a direction-discrimination task, during which they reported the perceived motion direction of a random dot stimulus by making a rapid eye movement (saccade) towards the corresponding choice target.
This revealed three distinct neuronal subpopulations in the subthalamic nucleus, each corresponding to one of the hypothetical models: The first subpopulation showed choice- and coherence-dependent activity that ramped up during motion viewing, consistent with the theory that these neurons pool and normalize evidence-related signals (Hypothesis 2). The second subpopulation exhibited an early, sharp rise in activity that was independent of choice and coherence during motion viewing, and gradually decreased toward saccade onset. This matches the prediction that the neurons provide an early signal to suppress immature choices (Hypothesis 1). The third subpopulation showed choice- and coherence-dependent ramping activity during motion viewing and a short burst of activity for one choice just before saccade onset. This aligns with the prediction that the subthalamic nucleus balances evidence-related signals until decision time (Hypothesis 3). This heterogeneity suggests that the subthalamic nucleus influences perceptual decision-making (where sensory information is used to guide behavior) in multiple ways.
Rogers et al. then applied electrical microstimulation (weak electric currents that affect neurons near the electrode) to the subthalamic nucleus during the task to perturb its activity. This caused the biases influencing the monkeys’ choice biases to change, reduced the influence of motion strength (i.e., the speed of the moving dot) on choices, and decreased response times. Fitting the data with a Drift Diffusion Model revealed that microstimulation of the subthalamic nucleus affected the decision threshold, evidence accumulation, as well as processes not involved in decision-making.
This groundbreaking study provides new insights into the causal roles of the subthalamic nucleus in perceptual decision-making, highlighting its involvement in modulating various aspects of the decision process through distinct neural subpopulations. These findings advance our understanding of how the basal ganglia contribute to decision-making and cognitive function.
However, it remains unclear if the subthalamic nucleus interacts with other brain regions, and if potential anatomical and/or functional alterations to this region could be implicated in neurological and psychiatric disorders. Future research could explore these issues and investigate therapy avenues that target the subthalamic nucleus for conditions involving impaired decision-making, potentially offering hope for individuals like Don Quixote.
References
-
Subthalamic nucleus stimulation reverses mediofrontal influence over decision thresholdNature Neuroscience 14:1462–1467.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2925
-
A theory of memory retrievalPsychological Review 85:59–108.https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.85.2.59
-
Role of the indirect pathway of the basal ganglia in perceptual decision makingThe Journal of Neuroscience 35:4052–4064.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3611-14.2015
Article and author information
Author details
Publication history
Copyright
© 2024, Yang
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 508
- views
-
- 54
- downloads
-
- 0
- citations
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Neuroscience
Recent studies suggest that calcitonin gene-related peptide (CGRP) neurons in the parabrachial nucleus (PBN) represent aversive information and signal a general alarm to the forebrain. If CGRP neurons serve as a true general alarm, their activation would modulate both passive nad active defensive behaviors depending on the magnitude and context of the threat. However, most prior research has focused on the role of CGRP neurons in passive freezing responses, with limited exploration of their involvement in active defensive behaviors. To address this, we examined the role of CGRP neurons in active defensive behavior using a predator-like robot programmed to chase mice. Our electrophysiological results revealed that CGRP neurons encode the intensity of aversive stimuli through variations in firing durations and amplitudes. Optogenetic activation of CGRP neurons during robot chasing elevated flight responses in both conditioning and retention tests, presumably by amplifying the perception of the threat as more imminent and dangerous. In contrast, animals with inactivated CGRP neurons exhibited reduced flight responses, even when the robot was programmed to appear highly threatening during conditioning. These findings expand the understanding of CGRP neurons in the PBN as a critical alarm system, capable of dynamically regulating active defensive behaviors by amplifying threat perception, and ensuring adaptive responses to varying levels of danger.
-
- Neuroscience
When observing others’ behaviors, we continuously integrate their movements with the corresponding sounds to enhance perception and develop adaptive responses. However, how the human brain integrates these complex audiovisual cues based on their natural temporal correspondence remains unclear. Using electroencephalogram (EEG), we demonstrated that rhythmic cortical activity tracked the hierarchical rhythmic structures in audiovisually congruent human walking movements and footstep sounds. Remarkably, the cortical tracking effects exhibit distinct multisensory integration modes at two temporal scales: an additive mode in a lower-order, narrower temporal integration window (step cycle) and a super-additive enhancement in a higher-order, broader temporal window (gait cycle). Furthermore, while neural responses at the lower-order timescale reflect a domain-general audiovisual integration process, cortical tracking at the higher-order timescale is exclusively engaged in the integration of biological motion cues. In addition, only this higher-order, domain-specific cortical tracking effect correlates with individuals’ autistic traits, highlighting its potential as a neural marker for autism spectrum disorder. These findings unveil the multifaceted mechanism whereby rhythmic cortical activity supports the multisensory integration of human motion, shedding light on how neural coding of hierarchical temporal structures orchestrates the processing of complex, natural stimuli across multiple timescales.