Chemogenetic inhibition of the medial prefrontal cortex reverses the effects of REM sleep loss on sucrose consumption
Abstract
Rapid eye movement (REM) sleep loss is associated with increased consumption of weight-promoting foods. The prefrontal cortex (PFC) is thought to mediate reward anticipation. However, the precise role of the PFC in mediating reward responses to highly palatable foods (HPF) after REM sleep deprivation is unclear. We selectively reduced REM sleep in mice over a 25–48 hr period and chemogenetically inhibited the medial PFC (mPFC) by using an altered glutamate-gated and ivermectin-gated chloride channel that facilitated neuronal inhibition through hyperpolarizing infected neurons. HPF consumption was measured while the mPFC was inactivated and REM sleep loss was induced. We found that REM sleep loss increased HPF consumption compared to control animals. However, mPFC inactivation reversed the effect of REM sleep loss on sucrose consumption without affecting fat consumption. Our findings provide, for the first time, a causal link between REM sleep, mPFC function and HPF consumption.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.20269.001Introduction
A strong link exists between insufficient sleep and weight gain. A study of more than 30,000 participants found that those who slept less than 6 hr per night were more likely to gain weight over a 1-year period compared to people who slept 7 to 8 hr per night (Watanabe et al., 2010). Moreover, when healthy adults obtain insufficient sleep (i.e., 5 hr per night) they are more likely to consume weight-promoting foods compared to persons who obtain sufficient sleep (i.e., 9 hr per night; Markwald et al., 2013). Rapid eye movement (REM) sleep loss itself may be sufficient to increase consumption of weight promoting, highly palatable foods (HPF). Shechter et al. (2012) found that REM sleep loss in humans, over a period of 5 d, was inversely associated with hunger ratings and fat consumption. Indeed, increased intake of weight promoting, highly palatable foods may, over time, lead to adverse health outcomes such as obesity, diabetes and cardiovascular diseases.
Several studies have attempted to uncover the underlying neural mechanisms responsible for mediating an increased desire to consume HPF in response to sleep deprivation. Healthy adults who were subjected to one night of total sleep loss reported an increased desire to consume high-calorie foods compared to non-sleep deprived controls (Greer et al., 2013). In addition, sleep deprivation decreased activation, assessed by fMRI scans, in areas of the brain thought to mediate reward anticipation and inhibitory control, namely the anterior cingulate and orbitofrontal cortex, respectively (Greer et al., 2013; Liu et al., 2011; Stuss, 2011). However, the relationship between brain function and sleep loss are not consistent. For example, presentation of HPF to sleep deprived healthy adults was not associated with anterior cingulate activation (St. Onge et al., 2014) while in other instances a positive association was found between appetite for HPF and anterior cingulate function (Benedict et al., 2012).
Therefore, the current investigation sought to determine the role of the medial prefrontal cortex (medial PFC, mPFC) in mediating appetite for highly palatable foods in mice experiencing REM sleep loss. To achieve this aim, we used a well-validated chemogenetic method (Lerchner et al., 2007) to produce a modified invertebrate chloride channel (GluClαβ) that when activated by the drug ivermectin (IVM) facilitates neuronal inhibition in the mPFC over several days. We then examined the effect of mPFC inactivation and REM sleep loss on appetite for HPF in mice.
Results
Wire-mesh-grid device exposure produced REM sleep loss
During the dark period, mice exposed to a wire-mesh-grid device (WMGD) in their housing cage (Figure 1A) over a 72-hr period showed significantly less REM sleep after device introduction compared to baseline polygraphic recordings obtained without the device (F3,6 = 23.835, p = 0.001; Figure 1B and C, McEown et al., 2016). There was a significant REM sleep reduction of 58% observed between baseline and the 25–48 hr dark time period (p = 0.026) and a 56% REM sleep reduction observed between baseline and the 49–72 hr dark time period (p = 0.020). In addition, a 46% REM sleep reduction was found when comparing the 0–24 hr and 25–48 hr dark time periods (p = 0.009). Moreover, we did not observe any rebound in REM sleep during the first 3 hr of the 25–48 hr light period (all comparisons p > 0.05; Figure 1—figure supplement 1). A significant increase in wakefulness was also produced by the device during the dark period (F3,6 = 6.886, p = 0.023). Specifically, a 23% increase in wakefulness during the dark period was observed between baseline and the 49–72 hr time period (p = 0.042). No differences were observed between time periods, during the dark phase, for slow-wave sleep (SWS)—also known as non-REM sleep—(F3,6 = 4.605, p = 0.053). In addition, differences between time periods, during the light phase, for wake (F3,6 = 1.608, p = 0.284), REM (F3,6 = 1.103, 0.418) and SWS (F3,6 = 1.434, p = 0.323) were also non-significant.
Exposing mice to the WMGD significantly affected sleep episode numbers during the 25–48 hr light and dark periods (Figure 2A). Specifically, WMGD exposure increased the number of SWS episodes during the light period, lasting 120 to 239 s, by 44% compared to baseline measures taken without the WMGD (t2 = −18.898, p = 0.003). Furthermore, WMGD exposure decreased the number of REM episodes during the dark period, lasting 60 to 119 s, by 69% compared to baseline (t2 = 9.430, p=0.011). On the other hand, WMGD exposure increased the number of wake episodes during the light period, lasting 30 to 59 s, by 42% compared to baseline (t2 = −4.355, p = 0.049). The mean duration of SWS, REM sleep and wake episodes was not significantly different after WMGD exposure (Figure 2B).
SWS electroencephalogram (EEG) power density was also affected by WMGD exposure during the 25–48 hr light and dark periods (Figure 2C). That is, we observed increases in SWS EEG power density, of 12% to 14%, after exposure to the WMGD during the light period at 2.5 Hz (t2 = −51.786, p = 0.000), 22 Hz (t2 = −49.829, p = 0.000) and 24 Hz (t2 = −31.982, p = 0.001) compared to baseline. We also observed increases in SWS EEG power density, of 12% to 14%, after WMGD exposure during the dark period at 2.5 Hz (t2 = −42.819, p = 0.001), 3 Hz (t2 = −27.978, p = 0.001), 10.5 Hz (t2 = −121.636, p = 0.000) and 11 Hz (t2 = −46.198, p = 0.000) compared to baseline. On the other hand, we did not observe significant differences in REM EEG power density during the 25–48 hr light or dark period (all comparisons p > 0.05; Figure 2, Panel C).
These results suggest that REM sleep is specifically reduced during the dark period in mice after WMGD exposure for the 25–48 hr time period, or longer, and thus the WMGD may be useful to elucidate the role of REM sleep loss on HPF consumption.
GluClαβ-mPFC neuronal inhibition reverses the effect REM sleep loss on HPF consumption
Next, we investigated the effect of REM sleep loss on HPF consumption and whether changes in the latter one depend on the mPFC. Therefore, we used a chemogenetic system that involves a Caenorhabditis elegans glutamate-gated and IVM-gated chloride channel that was mutagenically modified to abolish sensitivity to glutamate while retaining sensitivity to IVM (Lerchner et al., 2007). The altered heteromeric channel, which is made up of an α-subunit and β-subunit, prevents the firing of action potentials by hyperpolarizing the cell membrane for several days after administration of IVM.
Two adeno-associated viruses (AAV) carrying the α-subunit or β-subunit of the IVM-gated GluClαβ channel (AAV-GluClα-IVM and AAV-GluClβ-IVM) were stereotaxically co-injected bilaterally into the mPFC of wild-type mice (Figure 3A and B). To detect expression of the GluClαβ channel, we labeled neurons with anti-green fluorescent protein (GFP) antibodies to stain the fusion proteins of the GluClα-subunit and GluClβ-subunit with yellow and cyan fluorescent proteins, respectively. Heat maps depicting AAV location and spread within the mPFC are displayed in Figures 3B and 4A. The virus successfully targeted the mPFC, for mice used in all experiments, including the cingulate, prelimbic and infralimbic cortices (Figure 3B). IVM-injected mice had markedly attenuated Fos expression in the mPFC compared to mice injected with propylene glycol, indicating that the GluClαβ successfully inhibited neuronal activity in the mPFC (Figure 3C). SWS, REM and wakefulness amounts were non-significant when comparing the light and dark phases during the 25–48 hr time period for mPFC inactivated mice to the light and dark phases of the baseline day before the IVM injection (Figure 3D; all comparisons p>0.05).
We then examined the effect of REM sleep loss on sucrose consumption and whether changes in sucrose consumption depend on the mPFC (F3,16 = 3.206, p = 0.051; see Figure 4, Panel B). We compared non-sleep deprived mPFC inactivated mice to non-sleep deprived control mice and found no difference between these groups in sucrose (propylene glycol injected mice: p = 0.391; saline injected mice: p = 0.350) or fat consumption (propylene glycol injected mice: p = 0.575; saline injected mice: p = 0.434); indicating that mPFC inactivation, alone, did not affect sucrose or fat consumption in mice (Figure 4—figure supplement 1, Panel A). Moreover, we found that REM sleep loss increased sucrose consumption by 27% compared to non-sleep deprived saline injected controls (p = 0.039) and 35% compared to non-sleep deprived propylene glycol injected controls (p = 0.033). On the other hand, mPFC neuronal inhibition reversed the effect of REM sleep loss on sucrose consumption. Specifically, mPFC inactivated, REM sleep deprived mice consumed 49% less sucrose compared to REM sleep deprived controls (p = 0.004). Whereas sucrose consumption was non-significant between mPFC inactivated, REM sleep deprived mice and non-sleep deprived propylene glycol injected (p = 0.205) and saline injected controls (p=0.060).
Furthermore, the effect of REM sleep loss on fat consumption was also examined and whether changes in fat consumption depend on the mPFC (F3,16 = 7.778, p = 0.002; see Figure 4, Panel B). We found that REM sleep loss increased fat consumption by 26% compared to non-sleep deprived saline injected controls (p = 0.001) and 35% compared to non-sleep deprived propylene glycol injected controls (p = 0.000). However, mPFC inhibition paired with REM sleep loss did not affect fat consumption compared to sleep deprived controls (p = 0.199).
Baseline and follow-up weights of mice in the non-sleep deprived saline injected, non-sleep deprived propylene glycol injected, REM sleep reduced and mPFC inactivated groups were non-significant (Baseline: F3,16 = 0.414, p = 0.745; Follow-up: F3,16 = 0.538, p = 0.663). In addition, body weight did not significantly change between baseline and follow-up time points for the non-sleep deprived saline injected mice (t8 = 0.468, p = 0.652), non-sleep deprived propylene glycol injected mice (t2 = 0.756, p = 0.529), REM sleep reduced (t3 = 0.157, p = 0.885) or PFC inactivated groups (t3 = 0.001, p = 0.999). Finally, the amounts of sucrose (r = −0.175, p = 0.400) and fat (r = 0.246, p =0.240) consumed were not associated with baseline body weight (Figure 4—figure supplement 1, Panel B).
Discussion
We were successfully able to mitigate the increased consumption of sucrose that resulted from REM sleep loss. We achieved this aim by chemogenetically inhibiting mPFC function. These results are the first to demonstrate a clear, causative link between mPFC function, REM sleep and HPF consumption. Specifically, REM sleep loss increased consumption of two highly palatable foods, namely sucrose and fat. Whereas, mPFC inhibition completely reversed increased sucrose intake by attenuating the effect of REM sleep loss on sucrose consumption. On the other hand, mPFC inhibition did not attenuate the effect of REM sleep loss on fat consumption.
The PFC appears to play a role in mediating goal directed behavior toward highly appetitive food stimuli. Brain circuits responsible for judging the palatability of foods such as taste, texture and smell are mediated by areas of the PFC including the orbitofrontal and anterior cingulate cortex (Rolls, 2015). More specifically, the anterior cingulate appears to mediate taste for glucose in primates as feeding fruit juice (i.e., a high-glucose food) to satiety decreased neuronal firing in the anterior cingulate cortex using single cell recordings, whereas initial juice feeding increased neuronal firing in these animals (Rolls, 2008). Chocolate flavor also increases activation, as measured by fMRI, in the anterior cingulate cortex in humans (Rolls and McCabe, 2007). In addition, obese patients display increased PFC activity in response to high-calorie visual food stimuli; specifically, increased activity in the orbitofrontal and anterior cingulate cortex (Stoeckel et al., 2008; Rothemund et al., 2007). Alterations in PFC brain activity may, over time, result in the inability of these persons to inhibit salient food cues leading to excess high calorie food consumption and weight gain. Our results support these data by directly manipulating mPFC function in mice, which resulted in a substantial decrease in sucrose consumption, but had no effect on fat consumption. On the other hand, it is unknown whether consumption of other highly palatable foods may be effected by mPFC inactivation and/or REM disruption (e.g., foods high in salt content). Future research is needed to explore this possibility. The mPFC contains heterogeneous neurons that include some inhibitory GABAergic interneurons (approximately 20%) and excitatory pyramidal neurons (approximately 80%; (Riga et al., 2014). Moreover, mPFC neurons expressing dopamine D1 receptors mediate food consumption (Land et al., 2014); however, it is currently unknown what role this neuronal subtype may play in controlling highly palatable food consumption in response to REM sleep disruption.
Furthermore, in humans REM sleep loss adversely affects dietary behavior which may lead to weight gain. For example, REM sleep loss in humans enhances the desire to consume food and increases food consumption (Gonnissen et al., 2013; Shechter et al., 2012). In addition, REM sleep loss in children and adolescents, of 1 hr per night measured over three nights, was associated with a marked increase in the likelihood of these children being overweight (Liu et al., 2008). In agreement with these findings, we observed that reducing REM sleep, over a 25–48 hr period, during the dark phase resulted in increased HPF consumption. On the other hand, it should be noted that, in the current investigation, REM sleep was not effected during the light phase. It is unknown why our observed reduction in REM sleep was restricted to the dark phase and not during the light phase. Indeed, sleep pressure is greater during the light phase compared to the dark phase in mice; therefore, it is possible that our device was only able to produce REM reduction during the dark phase when sleep pressure was weaker. In addition, the REM sleep disruption observed in the current investigation did not occur during the first 24 hr after WMGD introduction. A plausible explanation is that the sleep reducing effect of our device requires time to build-up and thus produce disturbances in sleep.
In conclusion, our results demonstrated that mPFC inhibition reduced sucrose consumption in REM sleep deprived mice. Our results clearly indicate that increased HPF consumption, resulting from REM sleep loss, is mediated by mPFC function thereby providing clear findings that elucidate the role of the mPFC and REM sleep loss in mediating unhealthy dietary behavior, which may lead to adverse health outcomes.
Materials and methods
Subjects and chemicals
Request a detailed protocolA male mouse line on a C57BL/6 background, which was maintained at the International Institute of Integrative Sleep Medicine and weighed between 23 and 34 g (9–10 weeks old) at the beginning of the experiments, was used for all experiments. The animals were individually housed in insulated sound-proof chambers maintained at an ambient room temperature of 22°C ± 1°C with a relative humidity of 60 ± 2% in an automatically controlled 12 hr light/dark cycle (lights on at 8:00, lights off at 20:00). Food and water were available ad libitum. No method of blinding or randomization was used for any of the experiments. This study was performed in strict accordance with the recommendations in the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals of the National Institutes of Health. All animals were handled according to approved institutional animal care and use committee (IACUC) protocols (#16–359) of the University of Tsukuba. All protocols aimed to reduce the number of animals used for each experiment and to minimize pain or discomfort. IVM was obtained from Merial Company.
AAV vector generation
Request a detailed protocolThe AAV of serotype rh10 for AAV10-α-GluCl-IVM-EYFP (AAV-GluClα-IVM) and AAV10-β-GluCl-IVM-ECFP (AAV-GluClβ-IVM) were generated by tripartite transfection (AAV-rep2/caprh10 expression plasmid, adenovirus helper plasmid, and pAAV-GluClα-IVM or pAAV-GluClβ-IVM plasmid) into 293A cells. After 3 days, 293A cells were resuspended in artificial cerebrospinal fluid (aCSF), freeze-thawed four times, and treated with benzonase nuclease (Millipore) to degrade all forms of DNA and RNA. Subsequently, the cell debris was removed by centrifugation and the virus titre in the supernatant was determined using an AAVpro Titration Kit for Real Time PCR (Takara) (See Figure 3, Panel A).
Stereotaxic AAV injection and placement of EEG/EMG electrodes
Request a detailed protocolSurgeries for AAV injections were conducted under pentobarbital anaesthesia (50 mg/kg, intraperitoneal [i.p.]). Using aseptic techniques, mice were injected stereotaxically into the mPFC (n = 14) with recombinant GluClα-AAV and GluClβ-AAV (250 nl/injection, 1.1 × 1011 particles/ml), with a glass micropipette and an air pressure injector system (Chamberlin et al., 1998). The following coordinates were used for injections into the mPFC of C57BL/6 mice according to the atlas of Paxinos and Franklin (2001). Bilateral injections were made at 1.7 mm anterior and 0.4 mm lateral to bregma, and 2.0 mm below the dural surface.
Mice were also chronically implanted with EEG and electromyogram (EMG) electrodes for polysomnography (n = 6) as previously outlined (Oishi et al., 2016). Briefly, the implant comprised two stainless steel screws (1 mm diameter) serving as EEG electrodes, one of which was placed epidurally over the right frontal cortex (1.0 mm anterior and 1.5 mm lateral to bregma) and the other over the right parietal cortex (1.0 mm anterior and 1.5 mm lateral to lambda). Two insulated Teflon-coated, silver wires (0.2 mm in diameter), which were placed bilaterally into the trapezius muscles, served as EMG electrodes. Both EEG and EMG electrodes were connected to a microconnector, and the whole assembly was then fixed to the skull with self-curing dental acrylic resin. Animals were allowed one week for postoperative recovery before being placed in experimental housing cages (Figure 1, Panel A) for a 24 hr habituation period and connected with recording leads.
EEG/EMG recording
Request a detailed protocolAfter the 24-hr habituation period, mice were injected with vehicle (propylene glycol, i.p.) at 16:30. Baseline polygraphic recording commenced at 17:00 and terminated at 16:00 the following day. After baseline recordings were obtained, mice (n = 3) were injected with IVM (10 mg/kg; i.p.) at 16:30 on the following day to initiate virus activation. Polygraphic recordings subsequently commenced at 17:00 for a period of 72 hr.
A separate group of EEG/EMG implanted mice (n = 3) were tested using the preceding methods with the exception that mice did not receive AAV infusions and did not receive i.p. injections. Instead, a wire-mesh-grid cage bottom (purchased from Oriental Yeast Company, Tokyo, Japan) was introduced, at 16:30 the day after baseline polygraphic recording, for a period of 72 hr. The wire-mesh-grid cage bottom was constructed from stainless steel and measured: 22 cm (length) × 19.5 cm (width) × 2.5 cm (height) with the grid consisting of 1 cm2 openings (see Figure 1, Panel A).
Vigilance state assessment based on EEG/EMG/locomotor activity recordings
Request a detailed protocolThe EEG/EMG signals were amplified and filtered (EEG: 0.5–64 Hz, EMG: 16–64 Hz), then digitised at a sampling rate of 128 Hz, and recorded using SLEEPSIGN software (Kohtoh et al., 2008) (Kissei Comtec). In addition, locomotor activity was recorded with an infrared photocell sensor (Biotex). The vigilance states were scored offline by 10 s epochs into three stages, including waking, SWS, and REM sleep, according to standard criteria (Oishi et al., 2016). As a final step, defined vigilance stages were examined visually, and corrected when necessary.
HPF data collection
Request a detailed protocolHPF consumption measurements were obtained from mice that did not receive EEG/EMG electrode implantation (n = 24). After AAV infusion, mice were singly placed into testing cages without the wire-mesh-grid-cage bottoms for 7 days. During these seven days mice had free access to laboratory chow. On day 8, mice were injected with IVM (10 mg/kg, i.p.; n = 4) or vehicle (propylene glycol; n = 4) at 16:30 and singly placed back into their housing cages with the addition of the wire-mesh-grid cage bottoms at 17:00 (Figure 1, Panel A). The highly palatable foods were introduced on day 8 and food consumption was subsequently measured every 24 hr at 17:00 over a total period of 72 hr. In addition, three controls groups were used in which one group of mice did not receive AAV infusions and were injected with saline (n = 9), one group of mice received AAV infusions and were injected with propylene glycol (n = 3), and one group of mice received AAV infusions and were injected with IVM (n = 4). All three groups were not exposed to the wire-mesh-grid cage bottom (i.e., the non-sleep deprived groups). During the 72 hr test period, all mice had free access to white chocolate (DARS brand, Morinaga Company, Tokyo, Japan), a high-fat diet (HFD-60, Oriental Yeast Company, Tokyo, Japan) and standard laboratory chow (see Supplementary file 1 for the composition of each highly palatable diet). Total amounts of sucrose and fat consumption were calculated from each food type (i.e., sucrose and fat amounts from the white chocolate and from the high-fat diet) 25–48 hr after initial food exposure.
Immunohistochemistry for GFP and Fos immunoreactive cell expression
Request a detailed protocolWhen data collection, outlined above, was complete mice were deeply anesthetized using chloral hydrate (500 mg/kg, i.p.) and perfused through the left ventricle of the heart with saline and subsequently perfused with neutral buffered 10% formalin solution. Brains were then removed and placed into vials containing 10% formalin solution for 1 week. Brains were subsequently transferred to vials containing 20% sucrose in phosphate-buffered saline (PBS) for 24 hr at 4°C to reduce freezing artifacts. Brains were then frozen using dry ice, placed on a freezing microtome and sectioned at 40 μm. Immunohistochemistry was undertaken on free floating brain sections as previously described (Lazarus et al., 2011). In brief, sections were rinsed in PBS, incubated in 0.3% hydrogen peroxide in 0.25% Triton X-100 in PBS (PBT) for 30 min at room temperature, rinsed in PBS and incubated, overnight at room temperature, in rabbit GFP primary antibody (Molecular Probes, RRID:AB_221569, lot number 1293114) at a dilution of 1:20,000 or rabbit Fos primary antibody (Millipore, RRID:AB_2106755, lot number D00058535) at a dilution of 1:20,000 in PBT with 0.02% sodium azide. After overnight incubation, brain sections were rinsed in PBS for 1 hr and then incubated for 90 min in biotinylated anti-rabbit antibody (Jackson ImmunoResearch) at a dilution of 1:1000. These sections were then treated with avidin-biotin complex (1:1000; Vectastain ABC Elite kit, Vector Labs) for 1 hr and immunoreactive cells were visualised by reaction with 3,3'-diaminobenzidine and 0.01% hydrogen peroxide. Tissue sections mounted on glass slides were scanned with a Hamamatsu NanoZoomer-XR Digital slide scanner (Hamamatsu Photonics), and digital photomicrographs were analyzed with Hamamatsu NDPView software v2.4.26.
AAV location was assessed by visualizing GFP expression in brain sections using Hamamatsu NDPView software v2.4.26. Virus location figures (see Figure 3, Panel B and Figure 4, Panel A) were produced by using the following procedure: (a) observing virus location and spread within brain sections; (b) transforming digital photomicrographs images to color-scale images; and (c) superimposing these color-scale images to obtain a summation of AAV location for all mice.
Furthermore, Fos immunoreactive cell expression was measured using two AAV infused mice that were injected with either propylene glycol or IVM, exposed to the WMGD for 48 hr and fed a diet of white chocolate, HFD and chow. After WMGD exposure, mice were sacrificed during the dark period (i.e. at 21:00). Immunohistochemistry procedures were then undertaken as outlined above. Fos immunoreactive cell imaging, at 20 times magnification, was produced using Hamamatsu NDPView software v2.4.26 (see Panel C, Figure 3) by focusing on an area of interest located within the mPFC at 1.7 mm anterior and 0.4 mm lateral to bregma, and 2.0 mm below the dural surface.
Statistical analyses
Request a detailed protocolMeans and ± S.E.M. are expressed for all statistical comparisons. For comparisons between two groups, two-tailed paired samples Student’s t-tests were used followed by Bonferroni corrections. In addition, Pearson correlations were used to compare associations between two groups. Repeated measures ANOVA or one-way ANOVA were used when comparing groups of three or more followed by Fisher’s Probable Least-Squares Difference (PLSD) post hoc tests. For comparisons between independent samples, the Levene’s test was used to assess homogeneity of variance and for paired sample comparisons the Mauchly’s test was used to assess sphericity. All comparisons were considered statistically significant at p < 0.05. Statistics for all data are reported in Supplementary file 2.
Data availability
-
Data from: Chemogenetic inhibition of the medial prefrontal cortex reverses the effects of REM sleep loss on sucrose consumptionAvailable at Dryad Digital Repository under a CC0 Public Domain Dedication.
References
-
Acute sleep deprivation enhances the brain's response to hedonic food stimuli: an fMRI studyThe Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism 97:E443–E447.https://doi.org/10.1210/jc.2011-2759
-
Effects of sleep fragmentation on appetite and related hormone concentrations over 24 h in healthy menBritish Journal of Nutrition 109:748–756.https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007114512001894
-
The impact of sleep deprivation on food desire in the human brainNature Communications 4:1–7.https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms3259
-
Medial prefrontal D1 dopamine neurons control food intakeNature Neuroscience 17:248–253.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3625
-
Arousal effect of caffeine depends on adenosine A2A receptors in the shell of the nucleus accumbensJournal of Neuroscience 31:10067–10075.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6730-10.2011
-
Rapid eye movement sleep in relation to overweight in children and adolescentsArchives of General Psychiatry 65:924–932.https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.65.8.924
-
Common and distinct networks underlying reward valence and processing stages: a meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studiesNeuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 35:1219–1236.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.12.012
-
Polygraphic recording procedure for measuring sleep in miceJournal of Visualized Experiments 107:e53678.https://doi.org/10.3791/53678
-
Optogenetic dissection of medial prefrontal cortex circuitryFrontiers in Systems Neuroscience 8:1–19.https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2014.00230
-
Enhanced affective brain representations of chocolate in cravers vs. non-craversEuropean Journal of Neuroscience 26:1067–1076.https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05724.x
-
Functions of the orbitofrontal and pregenual cingulate cortex in taste, olfaction, appetite and emotionActa Physiologica Hungarica 95:131–164.https://doi.org/10.1556/APhysiol.95.2008.2.1
-
Taste, olfactory, and food reward value processing in the brainProgress in Neurobiology 127-128:64–90.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pneurobio.2015.03.002
-
Alterations in sleep architecture in response to experimental sleep curtailment are associated with signs of positive energy balanceAJP: Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology 303:R883–R889.https://doi.org/10.1152/ajpregu.00222.2012
-
Sleep restriction increases the neuronal response to unhealthy food in normal-weight individualsInternational Journal of Obesity 38:411–416.https://doi.org/10.1038/ijo.2013.114
-
Functions of the frontal lobes: relation to executive functionsJournal of the International Neuropsychological Society 17:759–765.https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617711000695
-
Association of short sleep duration with weight gain and obesity at 1-year follow-up: a large-scale prospective studySleep 33:161–167.
Article and author information
Author details
Funding
Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (P14762)
- Kristopher McEown
Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (2604762)
- Michael Lazarus
Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science, and Technology (16H01629)
- Michael Lazarus
Japan Science and Technology Agency (16814578)
- Michael Lazarus
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by a Japan Society for the Promotion of Science KAKENHI grant (2604762, to ML) and postdoctoral fellowship (P14762, to KM); a Takeda Science Foundation postdoctoral fellowship (to KM); a grant from the Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology (MEXT) of Japan (Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research on Innovative Areas ‘Living in Space’, 16H01629, to ML); a CREST grant from the Japan Science and Technology Agency (to ML) and the World Premier International Research Center Initiative (WPI) from MEXT (to ML, YT, YC, NN and KA).
Ethics
Animal experimentation: This study was performed in strict accordance with the recommendations in the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals of the National Institutes of Health. All of the animals were handled according to approved institutional animal care and use committee (IACUC) protocols (#16-359) of the University of Tsukuba. All protocols aimed to reduce the number of animals used for each experiment and to minimize pain or discomfort.
Copyright
© 2016, McEown et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 4,372
- views
-
- 642
- downloads
-
- 12
- citations
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Neuroscience
Motivation depends on dopamine, but might be modulated by acetylcholine which influences dopamine release in the striatum, and amplifies motivation in animal studies. A corresponding effect in humans would be important clinically, since anticholinergic drugs are frequently used in Parkinson’s disease, a condition that can also disrupt motivation. Reward and dopamine make us more ready to respond, as indexed by reaction times (RT), and move faster, sometimes termed vigour. These effects may be controlled by preparatory processes that can be tracked using electroencephalography (EEG). We measured vigour in a placebo-controlled, double-blinded study of trihexyphenidyl (THP), a muscarinic antagonist, with an incentivised eye movement task and EEG. Participants responded faster and with greater vigour when incentives were high, but THP blunted these motivational effects, suggesting that muscarinic receptors facilitate invigoration by reward. Preparatory EEG build-up (contingent negative variation [CNV]) was strengthened by high incentives and by muscarinic blockade, although THP reduced the incentive effect. The amplitude of preparatory activity predicted both vigour and RT, although over distinct scalp regions; frontal activity predicted vigour, whereas a larger, earlier, central component predicted RT. The incentivisation of RT was partly mediated by the CNV, though vigour was not. Moreover, the CNV mediated the drug’s effect on dampening incentives, suggesting that muscarinic receptors underlie the motivational influence on this preparatory activity. Taken together, these findings show that a muscarinic blocker impairs motivated action in healthy people, and that medial frontal preparatory neural activity mediates this for RT.
-
- Neuroscience
Gamma oscillations in brain activity (30–150 Hz) have been studied for over 80 years. Although in the past three decades significant progress has been made to try to understand their functional role, a definitive answer regarding their causal implication in perception, cognition, and behavior still lies ahead of us. Here, we first review the basic neural mechanisms that give rise to gamma oscillations and then focus on two main pillars of exploration. The first pillar examines the major theories regarding their functional role in information processing in the brain, also highlighting critical viewpoints. The second pillar reviews a novel research direction that proposes a therapeutic role for gamma oscillations, namely the gamma entrainment using sensory stimulation (GENUS). We extensively discuss both the positive findings and the issues regarding reproducibility of GENUS. Going beyond the functional and therapeutic role of gamma, we propose a third pillar of exploration, where gamma, generated endogenously by cortical circuits, is essential for maintenance of healthy circuit function. We propose that four classes of interneurons, namely those expressing parvalbumin (PV), vasointestinal peptide (VIP), somatostatin (SST), and nitric oxide synthase (NOS) take advantage of endogenous gamma to perform active vasomotor control that maintains homeostasis in the neuronal tissue. According to this hypothesis, which we call GAMER (GAmma MEdiated ciRcuit maintenance), gamma oscillations act as a ‘servicing’ rhythm that enables efficient translation of neural activity into vascular responses that are essential for optimal neurometabolic processes. GAMER is an extension of GENUS, where endogenous rather than entrained gamma plays a fundamental role. Finally, we propose several critical experiments to test the GAMER hypothesis.