Chronic pain induces generalized enhancement of aversion
Abstract
A hallmark feature of chronic pain is its ability to impact other sensory and affective experiences. It is notably associated with hypersensitivity at the site of tissue injury. It is less clear, however, if chronic pain can also induce a generalized site-nonspecific enhancement in the aversive response to nociceptive inputs. Here, we showed that chronic pain in one limb in rats increased the aversive response to acute pain stimuli in the opposite limb, as assessed by conditioned place aversion. Interestingly, neural activities in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) correlated with noxious intensities, and optogenetic modulation of ACC neurons showed bidirectional control of the aversive response to acute pain. Chronic pain, however, altered acute pain intensity representation in the ACC to increase the aversive response to noxious stimuli at anatomically unrelated sites. Thus, chronic pain can disrupt cortical circuitry to enhance the aversive experience in a generalized anatomically nonspecific manner.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25302.001Introduction
Chronic pain exerts a profound influence over daily life by impacting a range of sensory and affective behaviors. It is associated with enhanced response to noxious stimuli, leading to symptoms of allodynia and hyperalgesia at the site of tissue or nerve injury (Basbaum et al., 2009; Latremoliere and Woolf, 2009). The mechanisms for such sensory and affective hypersensitivity at the site of chronic pain have been well investigated. However, conditions such as fibromyalgia and persistent postoperative pain raise the possibility that chronic pain may also increase the aversive reaction towards noxious stimuli in an anatomically nonspecific distribution (Scudds et al., 1987; Petzke et al., 2003; Kehlet et al., 2006; Kudel et al., 2007; Scott et al., 2010). If confirmed, this generalized form of enhancement in pain aversion, as opposed to anatomically specific hypersensitivity, can greatly expand our understanding of the impact of chronic pain on behavior.
The anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) has a crucial role in the affective-aversive experience of pain (Lubar, 1964; Foltz and White, 1968; Turnbull, 1972; Talbot et al., 1995; Craig et al., 1996; Rainville et al., 1997; Koyama et al., 2000; Johansen et al., 2001; Koyama et al., 2001; LaGraize et al., 2006; Qu et al., 2011). The ACC receives nociceptive inputs from the medial thalamus as well as from other cortical regions (Vogt and Sikes, 2000; Shyu et al., 2010). Individual ACC neurons can respond to noxious stimuli by increasing firing rates (Sikes and Vogt, 1992; Yamamura et al., 1996; Hutchison et al., 1999; Kung et al., 2003; Iwata et al., 2005; Kuo and Yen, 2005; Zhang et al., 2011) to provide evaluation for the intensity of acute pain (Coghill et al., 1999; Büchel et al., 2002). While previous studies have demonstrated that the ACC is necessary and sufficient for the acquisition of stable aversive learning in the chronic pain condition (Johansen et al., 2001; Qu et al., 2011; Barthas et al., 2015; Navratilova et al., 2015), its role in the aversive response to transient acute pain signals is less well characterized. Furthermore, chronic pain has been shown to induce synaptic plasticity in ACC neurons, resulting in hypersensitivity at the site of injury through descending modulation (Wu et al., 2005; Li et al., 2010; Koga et al., 2015). It is unknown, however, if chronic pain can also impair ACC functions to alter the aversive response to noxious stimuli in an anatomically nonspecific manner. Using a multidisciplinary approach by combining optogenetics, in vivo electrophysiology and machine-learning decoding analysis, we found that chronic pain can disrupt acute pain representation in the ACC to induce generalized, anatomically nonspecific enhancement of aversion.
Results
Chronic pain can enhance the aversive response to noxious inputs at anatomically disparate sites
Conditioned place aversion (CPA) and conditioned place preference are well-established assays to test aversive learning as well as the negative reinforcement of analgesia in rodent chronic pain models (Johansen et al., 2001; King et al., 2009; Navratilova et al., 2012; Daou et al., 2013). These tests, however, have rarely been used to assess the aversive value presented by acute pain signals. Based on the concept of quantitative evaluation of aversion developed by standard CPA protocols (Johansen et al., 2001; Johansen and Fields, 2004), we constructed a brief 2-chamber CPA test for rats (Figure 1A). During the baseline (preconditioning) phase (10 min) of the test, rats were allowed free access to both chambers, and the time spent in either chamber was recorded as baseline. Next, during a brief conditioning period (10 min), we paired each chamber with a distinct laser stimulus directed at the hind paw of a freely-moving rat. The stimulus was classified as non-noxious (NS), low-intensity noxious (LS), or high-intensity noxious (HS). The intensity of the stimulus corresponded to the power output of the laser. Higher laser output transferred more heat to induce more intense thermal pain (Figure 1—figure supplement 1). At the behavioral level, NS did not typically elicit withdrawals within 5 s of stimulation (<5%), whereas HS and LS both elicited paw withdrawals 100% of the time. In addition, HS elicited withdrawals with a shorter latency than LS (Figure 1B). After this brief conditioning phase, we immediately tested the rat’s aversive response by measuring the time spent in each of the two treatment chambers during a test (postconditioning) phase (10 min). During the test phase, rats were again allowed free access to both chambers without any peripheral stimulation. We compared the amount of time spent during baseline (preconditioning) and test (postconditioning) phases in each chamber (Johansen et al., 2001; King et al., 2009; Navratilova et al., 2012). A statistically significant reduction in the amount of time spent in a treatment chamber during the test phase when compared with baseline indicates an avoidance of that chamber, which in turn indicates aversion towards the stimulus associated with that chamber. Thus, while similar in principle to the multi-day conditioning protocols using repeated or prolonged exposure to painful stimuli to measure the acquisition of stable aversive memory (Johansen et al., 2001; King et al., 2009; Navratilova et al., 2012), our paradigm allows the assessment of acute aversive reaction.
First, we compared the aversive responses towards HS and NS. During conditioning, one of the chambers was paired with HS, and the other was paired with NS. We found that rats spent less time in the chamber paired with HS treatment during the test phase than at baseline (Figure 1C). Conversely, rats spent more time in the NS chamber during the test phase than at baseline. These results indicate that rats recognized and sought to avoid the aversive value associated with HS. Next, we measured the ability of the rats to distinguish between the aversive values of LS and NS. We conditioned rats by pairing LS with one chamber and NS in the other. After conditioning, rats spent less time in the chamber paired with LS relative to baseline, and more time in the chamber paired with NS (Figure 1D), indicating an ability to recognize the aversive value for LS. Finally, we conditioned rats with HS and LS, and during the test phase rats spent less time in the HS-paired chamber (and more time in the LS-paired chamber) than at baseline, suggesting that these rats displayed a greater aversive response to HS than LS (Figure 1E). Together, these results indicate that rats can detect not only the presence of acute pain, but also the aversive values of higher vs lower-intensity noxious stimuli. This quantitative distinction in aversive response correlates well with changes in nocifensive spinal reflex (Figure 1B).
Next, we tested if this quantifiable aversive response to acute pain is altered by the presence of chronic pain at an anatomically unrelated location. We injected Complete Freund’s Adjuvant (CFA) to induce persistent inflammatory pain in the opposite paw and performed CPA by conditioning with noxious stimulation of the uninjected paw. 10 days post-CFA injections, rats were conditioned with HS and NS to the healthy paw, and they demonstrated a preference for the NS chamber and avoidance of the HS chamber during the test phase compared with baseline (Figure 1F). These results suggest that rats in chronic pain were still able to distinguish between highly noxious from non-noxious stimulations. We then examined the response of rats in chronic pain to the low-intensity noxious stimuli. First, CFA-treated rats were conditioned with LS and NS in separate chambers. During the test phase these rats spent less time in the LS chamber than at baseline, indicating an aversive response to LS (Figure 1G). Interestingly, CFA-treated rats appeared to demonstrate an increased avoidance of the LS-paired chamber than rats that did not have chronic pain (compare Figure 1G with Figure 1D). We then examined the ability for these CFA-treated rats to distinguish between the aversive quality of LS vs HS. We found that CFA-treated rats did not seem able to clearly distinguish between LS and HS, as they failed to avoid the HS chamber after conditioning (Figure 1H), a striking difference from the avoidance of the HS chamber exhibited by rats that did not experience chronic pain (Figure 1E). These comparisons suggest qualitatively that while rats in chronic pain were still able to distinguish between non-noxious and noxious stimuli, they seemed to have developed a heightened aversive response to the low-intensity stimuli even at an anatomically distinct site.
To provide a quantitative analysis for the above findings, we calculated a CPA score (or aversion score) to measure the aversive value of peripheral stimulations. This CPA score was computed by subtracting the amount of time rats spent in a chamber paired with a specific noxious stimulus during the test phase from the time they spent in that chamber at baseline (Johansen et al., 2001; Johansen and Fields, 2004; De Felice et al., 2013). A higher CPA score (a greater difference in the time spent in the associated chamber between baseline and test phase) indicates a greater aversive value for that stimulus. We first calculated the CPA score for LS, after we conditioned rats with LS in one chamber and NS in the other. We found that this CPA score was significantly higher for CFA-treated rats than saline-treated (control) rats (Figure 1I). This quantitative analysis confirms that CFA-treated rats avoided the LS chamber more than control rats, and thus rats in chronic pain developed an increased aversive response to acute low-intensity noxious stimulations even at an anatomically distinct site. Next, we quantified the ability for the rats to distinguish between HS and LS. This time, we conditioned rats with HS and LS, and we computed the CPA score for HS, by subtracting the amount of time rats spent in the HS chamber during the test phase from the time they spent at baseline. In this case, a higher CPA score indicates a stronger aversive response to HS, and hence a greater ability to distinguish between HS and LS. Here, we found that CFA-treated rats demonstrated a dramatic reduction in their CPA score (Figure 1J), indicating that rats in chronic pain lost the ability to distinguish between the aversive values of HS and LS. Results from Figure 1I together indicate that after chronic pain, rats perceived both low-intensity and high-intensity noxious stimuli as highly aversive. Thus, the presence of chronic pain induced an alteration in the aversive evaluation of acute pain signals even at an anatomically distinct site. There have been reports suggesting that unilateral noxious stimulations can in some cases cause bilateral changes in spinal circuits regulating mechanical sensitivity (Gao and Ji, 2010, Gao et al., 2010). We tested whether the healthy paws in our study displayed any behavioral signs of spinal or peripheral hypersensitivity as the result of CFA injection in the opposite paws, but we did not find any abnormality in the withdrawal reflex to noxious stimulation or the presence of mechanical allodynia (Figure 1K). Thus, the effect of chronic pain on acute pain responses at anatomically unrelated sites is likely specific for the aversive component, sparing the sensory component. We termed this anatomically nonspecific increase in the aversive response to acute pain ‘generalized enhancement of pain aversion.’
Chronic pain disrupts the ACC representation of acute pain signals
Having established this phenomenon of generalized enhancement of pain aversion, we investigated whether a disruption in the ACC representation of acute pain contributes to the neural substrate for such behavior. We conducted extracellular recordings in the ACC in freely moving rats before, during and after peripheral stimulation by NS, LS or HS, and analyzed the firing rates of individual neurons (Figure 2A). We found that a number of neurons in the ACC increased their firing rates after an acute pain stimulus (see Materials and methods; Figure 2C), similar to reports from human studies (Hutchison et al., 1999). We also found a significant number of neurons that responded to noxious stimulation and at the same time showed increased firing rate after HS than LS (Figure 2D,E). The identification of such neurons that responded to noxious intensity is also compatible with previous reports (Sikes and Vogt, 1992; Yamamura et al., 1996; Hutchison et al., 1999; Kung et al., 2003; Iwata et al., 2005; Kuo and Yen, 2005; Zhang et al., 2011). In order to make an unbiased assessment of the contribution of individual ACC neurons in pain representation, we then applied a population-decoding analysis using a support vector machine (SVM) classifier (see Materials and methods). We analyzed groups of neurons across multiple recording sessions, each session comprising of >30 trials. We used some of the trials in training and the remaining trials for testing. Both pain-responsive and non-responsive neurons were used in unbiased decoding analysis, but the firing activity from pain-responsive neurons would contribute to a higher weight (i.e., being the ‘support vector’; see Materials and methods). Our decoding analysis yielded high accuracy in distinguishing between NS and HS (85% accuracy) or between LS and HS (76% accuracy) (Figure 2F, Figure 2—figure supplement 1). Therefore, our unbiased decoding analysis supported a critical role of ACC neurons in the representation of pain intensity.
Our neurophysiological recordings were performed in freely moving animals, and thus baseline movement was unlikely to affect our data interpretation. However, noxious stimulation also induced paw withdrawals, and these movements are spinal reflexes in nature (Vardeh et al., 2016). To ensure that these spinal reflexes did not influence our neural findings, we quantitatively analyzed these motor responses. We found that while rats did not withdraw their paws in response to NS (<5%), the percentages of withdrawal responses to LS and HS were equal (~100%). To quantify the motor function of paw withdrawals, we then calculated the withdrawal velocity after LS and HS. We did not find any statistical difference in the velocity of withdrawals (Figure 2—figure supplement 2). These results indicate that there was no significant difference in the gross motor responses to LS and HS, in contrast to the dramatic difference in neural responses in the ACC seen in Figure 2. Thus, the neural responses we observed were less likely to be related to stimulus-induced movement. Such responses in the ACC more likely represented nociceptive processing, compatible with previous reports (Sikes and Vogt, 1992; Hutchison et al., 1999; Wang et al., 2003; Kuo and Yen, 2005; Zhang et al., 2011).
Next, we examined how the presence of chronic pain in the opposite limb affected ACC representation of acute pain (Figure 2G). After CFA injection in the opposite foot, we continued to find neurons in the ACC that responded to pain stimuli (Figure 2H). In addition, our SVM decoding algorithm continued to distinguish HS from NS (80% accuracy) and HS from LS (67% accuracy) (Figure 2J) – as compared to the chance level of 50% (Figure 2—figure supplement 3). We then examined the ‘tuning curves’ of all ACC neurons that showed increased peak firing rates after HS than LS. Such tuning curves demonstrate the degree of neural responsiveness to different intensities of noxious stimulation. In the chronic pain condition, however, neurons showed a flatter tuning curve, as indicated by a significant decrease in the slope of linear fit (Figure 2K). This suggests a decreased ability to distinguish between LS and HS at the level of individual neurons. Furthermore, our unbiased machine learning analysis also indicated that chronic pain caused a statistically significant decrease in the ability to decode the difference between LS and HS signals (control vs. CFA: 76% vs 67% accuracy, Figure 2L). Thus, our neural data indicate a disruption of ACC representation of acute pain intensity in the chronic pain state. This impaired distinction between LS and HS by ACC neurons correlates very well with behavioral findings that showed increases in the aversive valuation of LS in the chronic pain state (Figure 1G–J), suggesting that the ACC likely plays a role in the impact of chronic pain on the acute pain experience.
Chronic pain impairs the bidirectional regulation of acute pain by the ACC
To define the function of ACC in generalized enhancement of pain aversion, we temporally paired noxious stimuli with optogenetic control of ACC neurons (Figure 3A). First, we used channelrhodopsin (ChR2) to activate ACC pyramidal neurons, by expressing ChR2 linked to a CAMKII promotor in an AAV vector in the ACC region (Figure 3B; Figure 3—figure supplement 1). We did not observe any changes in paw withdrawal latency with optogenetic stimulation (Figure 3C), suggesting that brief ACC activation did not strongly modulate the acute nocifensive reflex. Next, we conditioned rats by coupling ACC activation with LS in one of the CPA chambers, and LS alone in the opposite chamber during the CPA test (Figure 3A). We found that ACC activation during the presentation of LS increased the aversive response to LS, as shown by an avoidance of the chamber associated with optogenetic activation during the test phase compared with baseline (Figure 3D). This pain-enhancing effect of ACC activation was not observed when the animal was presented with NS stimulation (Figure 3E), indicating that while transient ACC activation does not turn an acute non-noxious stimulus into a noxious one, it can significantly enhance the aversive quality of a noxious stimulus. Meanwhile, ACC activation during the presentation of HS increased the aversive response to HS on the CPA, but this effect was not statistically significant (Figure 3F). There are two possible interpretations for this apparent lack of effect of ACC activation during HS. First, our CPA test serves as a pain-scale report for rats, and with highly noxious stimulation (HS), there may be a maximal aversive response expressed by rats, similar to a maximal pain score experienced by humans. A second possible interpretation is that there may be a limit (ceiling effect) on the aversive response that could be elicited with our CPA assay, and the further aversive effect of ACC activation on HS was limited by such constraints. Nevertheless, our results clearly indicate that ACC activation during the presentation of a low intensity noxious input is capable of elevating the aversive value of that input, demonstrating that the ACC can indeed regulate the behavioral response to acute pain signals.
To ensure that activation of the ACC did not impact movement of the animals, we performed a locomotion test and found no changes in locomotor activity after ACC activation (Figure 3—figure supplement 2). Furthermore, as expected, shining light in the ACC injected with a YFP-only vector without opsin expression did not result in any changes in pain aversion phenotypes (Figure 3—figure supplement 3). These control experiments support the specific role of ACC neurons in the aversive response to pain.
To further understand the role of the ACC in generalized enhancement of pain aversion, we compared the effect of ACC activation with CFA treatment on the aversive evaluation of noxious stimulations. First, we conditioned rats by coupling ACC activation with LS in one chamber, and NS without ACC modulation in the opposite chamber. This experiment allowed us to assess the aversive value of LS during the activation of ACC relative to a non-noxious condition. When we compared the avoidance of the LS chamber in these rats that received ACC activation (Figure 3G) with naïve rats (Figure 1D), we noticed that ACC activation likely caused an additional increase in the aversive value of LS. To quantify this increase, we measured the CPA score for LS in this experiment by subtracting the amount of time rats spent during the test phase from baseline in the chamber paired with LS and ACC activation. This CPA score represents the aversive value for the LS in the presence of ACC activation. Next, we computed the aversive value for LS in the chronic pain state by calculating the CPA score in the experiment where we conditioned CFA-treated rats with LS vs NS without optogenetic modulation of the ACC. This second CPA score indicates the aversive value for the LS in the presence of chronic pain. We found that these two CPA scores were nearly identical (Figure 3H). Thus, the amplitude of the additional aversive effect provided by ACC activation during the presentation of LS is similar to the effect of chronic pain. This similarity supports the role for ACC activation in the generalized enhancement of pain aversion.
To further confirm the role of the ACC in pain regulation, we tested whether inhibition of ACC neurons could decrease the aversive response to noxious stimuli. We used halorhodopsin (NpHR) to inhibit ACC neurons during the presentation of noxious stimulation (Figure 4A; Figure 4—figure supplement 1). We conditioned rats by coupling ACC inhibition with a peripheral stimulus in one of the CPA chambers, and that stimulus alone in the opposite chamber (Figure 4A). We found that ACC inhibition decreased the aversive response to LS and HS, as shown by increased amounts of time spent in the chamber associated with LS or HS coupled with ACC inhibition during the test phase compared with baseline (Figure 4C,D). Inhibition of the ACC did not impact free movements of the animals (Figure 4—figure supplement 2). Furthermore, as expected, ACC inhibition had no effect on the aversive response to NS, which was not noxious (Figure 4—figure supplement 3). Next, we wanted to know if ACC inhibition could remove the aversive response to a noxious stimulus altogether. We paired one chamber with ACC inhibition and LS, and another chamber with NS. After conditioning, we found that ACC inhibition coupled with LS stimulation removed the avoidance of the LS-paired chamber (Figure 4E, p>0.05). Similarly, ACC inhibition during HS removed the higher aversive value of HS compared with LS, by eliminating the avoidance of HS-paired chamber (Figure 4F, p>0.05). These results indicate that ACC inhibition during the presentation of a more noxious stimulus can eliminate the greater aversive reaction towards that stimulus. Therefore, the ChR2 and NpHR data together demonstrate that neurons in the ACC can bidirectionally control the aversive response to acute pain.
Finally, we tested the effect of ACC inhibition specifically on the generalized enhancement of pain aversion. We have shown that CFA-treated rats demonstrated an increased aversive response to LS in the uninjected paw (Figure 1G). Here, we found that ACC inhibition during LS eliminated the avoidance of LS-paired chamber in CFA-treated rats when these rats were conditioned with NS (Figure 4G). Thus, ACC inhibition blocked the aversion-amplifying effect of chronic pain. Furthermore, rats in chronic pain have been shown to lose the ability to differentiate between LS and HS on the CPA test (Figure 1H). ACC inhibition during LS stimulations, however, restored the normal aversive scale in CFA-treated rats, by reinstating the avoidance of HS chamber (Figure 4H). To quantitatively confirm these findings, we calculated the CPA score for CFA-treated rats which were conditioned with LS with or without ACC inhibition and NS. This CPA score was computed by subtracting the amount of time rats spent during the test phase from baseline in the chamber paired with LS (Figure 4I). We found that ACC inhibition reduced this CPA score and hence the aversive response for LS even in rats with chronic pain. We also calculated the CPA score for CFA-treated rats which were conditioned with HS and LS in the presence or absence of ACC inhibition, by subtracting the amount of time rats spent during the test phase from baseline in the chamber paired with HS (Figure 4J). We found that ACC inhibition during LS elevated the CPA score for HS and hence reinstated the difference in aversive values between HS and LS in rats with chronic pain. Thus, ACC inhibition restored the normal aversive response to acute pain in those rats with chronic pain. These results strongly indicate that neural activities in the ACC play a vital role in the generalized enhancement of aversion in chronic pain conditions.
Discussion
The key finding in our study is that chronic pain causes a generalized enhancement in pain aversion. Previous studies have revealed the aversive state secondary to nociceptive inputs from the site of chronic pain (Johansen et al., 2001; Johansen and Fields, 2004; King et al., 2009; De Felice et al., 2013). The novel aspect of our results lies in the demonstration that chronic pain at one site in the body can also increase the aversive response to acute pain in a separate location. Interestingly, acute pain responses elicited by lower or intermediate intensity stimulus (such as LS) were more affected by chronic pain than maximal pain stimulus (HS), leading to a distortion of the pain-intensity scale. Epidemiological studies have shown that the presence of chronic pain coincides with increased pain severity and a similar distortion of pain intensity scale in a diffuse anatomic pattern (Scudds et al., 1987; Petzke et al., 2003; Kehlet et al., 2006; Kudel et al., 2007; Scott et al., 2010). Our study confirms these clinical findings by providing a causal link between chronic pain and a generalized enhancement of pain aversion. This generalized enhancement in aversion may be an important mechanism for chronic pain to influence normal sensory and affective processes.
Our study also provides a mechanistic basis for this generalized enhancement of pain aversion. Individual ACC neurons can respond to noxious stimuli by increasing firing rates (Sikes and Vogt, 1992; Yamamura et al., 1996; Hutchison et al., 1999; Kung et al., 2003; Iwata et al., 2005; Kuo and Yen, 2005; Zhang et al., 2011). Our study shows that this neural representation of acute pain intensity is profoundly altered by chronic pain, which causes ACC neurons to display a disproportional response to low-intensity pain stimuli. Previous studies have found that chronic pain can induce maladaptive synaptic plasticity in the ACC (Li et al., 2010; Koga et al., 2015). It is possible that such plasticity increases the response of ACC neurons to acute pain signals, and responses to low-intensity stimuli are disproportionally affected because the neuronal response is possibly saturated at high-intensity stimuli in the absence of chronic pain. At the network level, ACC neurons are known to project to or receive inputs from a number of regions important for pain processing, including the prefrontal cortex, medial thalamus, insular cortex, amygdala, nucleus accumbens, hippocampus, etc. Thus, acute pain likely triggers a concerted neural response in an interconnected network as suggested by fMRI studies (Schweinhardt and Bushnell, 2012; Wager et al., 2013). Chronic pain, however, has the capacity to disturb this network response.
Another important finding in our study is the ability of the ACC to exert bidirectional control of pain aversion. Prior lesion and pharmacological studies have shown that the rostral region of the ACC is required specifically for the acquisition of stable aversive learning induced by chronic pain (Johansen et al., 2001; Johansen and Fields, 2004; Qu et al., 2011). Our study indicates that this brain region is also necessary and sufficient for the temporal regulation of the aversive response to acute pain stimuli. Interestingly, in our study, brief activation of the ACC did not cause an aversive reaction to a non-noxious stimulus, whereas previous studies demonstrate that repeated activation of ACC neurons itself can be an aversive teaching signal (Johansen and Fields, 2004). This difference suggests that repeated and possibly prolonged activation of ACC is required for the acquisition of stable aversive memory, whereas transient activation provides context-specific aversive valuation and response. At the molecular and cellular level, persistent peripheral nociceptive inputs have been shown to trigger opioid signaling and synaptic plasticity in the ACC to regulate sensory and aversive components of chronic pain (Li et al., 2010; Navratilova et al., 2015). Our results here suggest that these mechanisms have the potential to impact ACC regulation of pain aversion in an input- and output-nonspecific fashion to exert a more generalized form of control for acute pain behavior.
In addition to pain regulation, the anterior cingulate cortex is also involved in a number of sensory, affective and cognitive processes (de Araujo et al., 2003; Rolls et al., 2003; Grabenhorst et al., 2008; Rolls et al., 2008). It plays an important role in reward-based learning, such as providing necessary evaluation for rewarding or aversive cues as well as for the interpretation of errors in predicting such cues (Bush et al., 2002). It also plays roles in attention and in conflict monitoring (Braver et al., 2001). While our experiment demonstrated that ACC changes are likely specific to pain instead of motor responses to pain, we cannot absolutely rule out all the potential behavioral covariance associated with noxious stimulation. However, given the complexity and diversity of its functions and anatomic connections, it is perhaps not surprising that plasticity within the ACC as the result of chronic pain can alter the regulation of general aversive responses. It should be noted, in addition, that given its rich functional connectivity, the ACC is likely to be an important node in a complex network of brain structures that regulate this generalized enhancement in pain aversion.
In summary, we have demonstrated that chronic pain can disrupt cortical function to increase the aversive response to acute noxious signals in an anatomically nonspecific manner. This mechanism of generalized enhancement of pain aversion may underpin the pathophysiology of diffuse pain syndromes such as fibromyalgia and chronic postoperative pain. It also raises the possibility that other conditions such as depression and anxiety may exert similar impact on acute pain or other sensory and affective processes in general.
Materials and methods
Animals
All procedures in this study were approved by the New York University School of Medicine (NYUSOM) Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC) as consistent with the National Institute of Health (NIH) Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals to ensure minimal animal use and discomfort. Male Sprague-Dawley rats were purchased from Taconic Farms, Albany, NY and kept at Mispro Biotech Services Facility in the Alexandria Center for Life Science, with controlled humidity, temperature, and 12 hr (6:30 AM to 6:30 PM) light-dark cycle. Food and water were available ad libitum. Animals arrived to the animal facility at 250 to 300 grams and were given on average 10 days to adjust to the new environment prior to the onset of experiments.
Complete Freund's Adjuvant (CFA) administration
Request a detailed protocolTo produce chronic inflammatory pain, 0.1 ml of CFA (mycobacterium tuberculosis, Sigma-Aldrich) was suspended in an oil-saline (1:1) emulsion and injected subcutaneously into the plantar aspect of the hindpaw opposite to the paw that was stimulated by laser. Control rats received equal volume of saline injection.
Virus construction and packaging
Request a detailed protocolRecombinant AAV vectors were serotyped with AAV1 coat proteins and packaged by the viral vector core at the University of Pennsylvania. Viral titers were 5 × 1012 particles/mL for AAV1.CAMKII.ChR2-eYFP.WPRE.hGH and AAV1.CAMKII.NpHR-eYFP.WPRE.hGH.
Stereotaxic optic fiber implantation and intracranial viral injections
Request a detailed protocolAs described previously (Goffer et al., 2013; Lee et al., 2015), rats were anesthetized with isoflurane (1.5% to 2%). In all experiments, virus was delivered to the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) only. Rats were bilaterally injected with 0.5 µL of viral vectors at a rate of 0.1 µL/10 s with a 26-gauge 1 µL Hamilton syringe at anteroposterior (AP) +2.6 mm, mediolateral (ML) ±1.6 mm, and dorsoventral (DV) −2.25 mm, with tips angle 28° toward the midline. The microinjection needles were left in place for 10 min, raised 1 mm and left for another minute to allow for diffusion of virus particles away from injection site while minimizing spread of viral particles along the injection tract. Rats were then implanted with 200 μm optic fibers held in 1.25 mm ferrules (Thorlabs) in the ACC: AP +2.6 mm, ML ±1.6 mm, DV −1.25 mm. Fibers with ferrules were held in place by dental acrylic.
Electrode implantation and surgery
Request a detailed protocolTetrodes were constructed from four twisted 12.7 µm polyimide-coated microwires (Sandvik) and mounted in an eight tetrode VersaDrive (Neuralynx). Electrode tips were plated with gold to reduce electrode impedances to 100–500 kΩ at 1 kHz. Rats were anesthetized with isoflurane (1.5–2%). The skull was exposed and a 2.5-mm-diameter hole was drilled above the target region. A durotomy was performed before tetrodes were slowly lowered unilaterally into the ACC with the stereotaxic apparatus. Coordinates for ACC implants were: AP +2.7 mm, ML 0.8 mm, and DV 1.4 mm, with tetrode tips angled 10° toward the midline. The drive was secured to the skull screws with dental cement.
Following animal sacrifice, brain sections were collected at a thickness of 20 µm using Microm HM525 Cryostat machine, and sections were analyzed for viral expression and optic fiber localization with histological staining. Animals with improper fiber or electrode placements, low viral expression, or viral expression outside the ACC were excluded from the study.
In vivo electrophysiological recordings
Request a detailed protocolBefore stimulation, animals were given a 30 min period to habituate to a recording chamber over a mesh table. Noxious stimulation via a 1000 mW blue diode-pumped solid-state laser (Shanghai Dreams Laser Technology Co., LTD.) was applied 1 mm from the plantar surface of the hind paw contralateral to the brain recording site in freely moving rats (Chen et al., 2017). The laser output intensity could be NS (laser intensity of 50 mW), LS (150 mW) or HS (250 mW) (see below) in a single session with 200 µm core diameter fiber (M83L01, Thorlabs). The laser output power was calibrated by compact power and energy meter console (PM100D, Thorlabs) at the beginning of every recording session. In a single trial, the laser was turned on by a transistor-to-transistor (TTL) pulse generator (Doric) until paw withdrawal was observed (or for a total of 5 s if no withdrawal occurred). All recording sessions consisted of approximately 35 trials with variable inter-trial intervals (approximately 1 min) using one category of stimulation (NS, LS, or HS). A video camera (HC-V550, Panasonic) was used to record the experiment. Long inter-trial intervals between trials and the break between sessions were used to avoid sensitization. We did not identify any sensitization behavior nor physical damage to the paw during our experiment. The withdrawal latency was defined by the time between onset of laser and paw withdrawal.
In a subset of experiments intended for decoding analysis, we performed recordings using two different lasers to provide two different output intensities (NS&HS, or LS&HS). The stimulations were randomly applied to rat's hind paw for a total of approximately 60 trials (of equal number of trials for each stimulation intensity).
Optrode recordings were made when animals were anesthetized with 1% isoflurane and secured to a stereotaxic apparatus, as described previously (Lee et al., 2015). A 32-channel optrode (VersaDrive8 optical, Neuralynx) containing an optical fiber positioned 0.5 mm above the tips of 8 surrounding tetrodes was implanted after virus injection, using the coordinates mentioned above. The optical fiber was connected to a laser, which was connected to a TTL pulse-generating box (OTPG4, Doric Instruments). An extra output on the TTL box and the headstage were then connected to the data acquisition system in order to simultaneously record laser pulses and brain activity.
Data collection and preprocessing
Request a detailed protocolTetrodes were lowered in steps of 120 µm before each day of recording. The neuronal activity and the onset of noxious laser stimulation were simultaneously recorded with an acquisition equipment (Open Ephys) via an RHD2132 amplifier board (Intan Technologies). Signals were monitored and recorded from 32 low-noise amplifier channels at 30 kHz, band-passed filtered (0.3 to 7.5 kHz). To get spike activity, the raw data were high-pass filtered at 300 Hz with subsequent thresholding and offline sorting by commercial software (Offline Sorter, Plexon). The threshold was lower than the 3-Sigma peak heights line and optimized manually based on the signal to noise ratio. The features of three valley electrodes were used for spike sorting. Only clear spike clusters with good tetrode spike waveforms and ISI (inter spike interval) Poisson distribution were selected for analysis. Single units with peak firing rates lower than 1 Hz were excluded. Trials were aligned to the initiation of laser-on to compute the PSTH for each single unit using MATLAB (Mathworks).
Immunohistochemistry
Request a detailed protocolRats were deeply anesthetized with Isoflurane and transcardially perfused with ice-cold PBS followed by 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA) in PBS. Brains were fixed in PFA overnight and then transferred to 30% sucrose in PBS to equilibrate for three days as described (Lee et al., 2015). 20 µm coronal sections were made with a cryostat and washed with PBS for 10 min. Sections were washed in PBS and coverslipped with Vectashield mounting medium. Images containing tetrodes were stained with cresyl violet. These images were acquired using a Nikon eclipse 80i microscope with a DS-U2 camera head. Sections were also made after viral transfer for opsin verification, and these sections were stained with anti-rabbit GFP (1:500, Abcam, Cambridge, MA, #AB290), anti-mouse VGLUT 1/2 (1:200, Millipore, Temecula, CA, #MAB5502/5504), and DAPI (1:200, Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA) antibodies. Secondary antibodies were anti-rabbit IgG conjugated to AlexaFluor 488, and anti-mouse IgG conjugated to AlexaFluor 647 (1:200, Life Technologies, Carlsbad, CA). Images were acquired with a Zeiss LSM 700 Confocal Microscope (Carl Zeiss, Thornwood, NY).
Animal behavioral tests
For optogenetic experiments, optic fibers were connected to a 473 nm (for ChR2) or 589 nm (for NpHR) laser diode (Shanghai Dream Lasers) through a mating sleeve as described previously (Lee et al., 2015). Laser intensity was measured with a power meter (Thorlabs) prior to experiments. Laser was delivered using a TTL pulse-generator (Doric).
Conditioned place aversion (CPA)
View detailed protocolCPA experiments were conducted similar to what has been described previously (Johansen et al., 2001; Johansen and Fields, 2004; King et al., 2009; De Felice et al., 2013; Lee et al., 2015). The movements of rats in each chamber were automatically recorded by a camera and analyzed with the Any-maze software. The CPA protocol included preconditioning (baseline), conditioning, and testing phases (10 min during each phase). Animals spending more than 500 s or less than 100 s of the total time in either main chamber in the preconditioning phase were eliminated from further analysis (approximately 20% of total animals). Immediately following the pre-conditioning phase, the rats underwent conditioning for 10 min. Each of the two chambers was paired with a unique blue laser stimulus, which could be NS, LS or HS. For NS, the laser output power was approximately 50 mW, for LS, it was 150 mW, and for HS, it was 250 mW. Laser intensity was measured with a power meter (Thorlabs) prior to each experiment, and the same intensity was used consistently to generate LS and HS. A stimulus was terminated after paw withdrawal, and this occurred for LS and HS. In the case of NS, withdrawals occurred on less than 5% of stimulations, and in the case of non-withdrawals the stimulus was applied for a total of 5 s. The stimulus was repeated every 10 s. During a subset of the experiments, optogenetic activation was concurrent with laser stimulation in one of the treatment chambers. Laser stimulation, optogenetic stimulation and chamber pairings were counterbalanced. During the test phase, the animals did not receive any treatment and had free access to both compartments for a total of 10 min. Animal movements in each of the chambers were recorded, and the time spent in either of the treatment chambers was analyzed by the AnyMaze software. Decreased time spent in a chamber during the test phase as compared with the baseline indicates avoidance (aversion) for that chamber.
Paw withdrawal latency test
Request a detailed protocolA laser stimulation of 50 mW (NS), 150 mW (LS), or 250 mW (HS) was applied to the hind paw of rats. Less than 5% of the time 50 mW stimulation elicited a withdrawal response within 5 s, whereas 150 mW and 250 mW elicited withdrawal 100% of the time. A video was used to tape the procedure and analyze the withdrawal latency.
Mechanical allodynia test
Request a detailed protocolA Dixon up-down method with von Frey filaments was used to measure mechanical allodynia (Chaplan et al., 1994; Bourquin et al., 2006; Wang et al., 2011). Rats were individually placed into plexiglass chambers over a mesh table and acclimated for 20 min before testing. Beginning with 2.55 g, von Frey filaments in a set with logarithmically incremental stiffness (0.45, 0.75, 1.20, 2.55, 4.40, 6.10, 10.50, 15.10 g) were applied to the paws of rats. 50% withdrawal threshold was calculated as described previously (Wang et al., 2011).
Measurement of the velocity of paw withdrawals
Request a detailed protocolWe used a high speed camera (Sony Handycam FDR-AX53) to record frame by frame the movement of the paws after noxious stimulation with LS or HS. We measured the velocity of paw withdrawal by dividing the height of paw withdrawal by the time it took for the animal to reach this height with its affected paw. An average of 10 measurements were calculated for each rat.
Locomotion test
Request a detailed protocolWe recorded locomotion over 10 min for rats that received optogenetic activation or inhibition of the ACC. Either blue or yellow light was turned on for 3 s every 10 s during the locomotion test. In control rats, no light activation was provided. Total distance travelled was computed based on AnyMaze recordings.
Statistical analysis
Request a detailed protocolThe results of behavioral experiments were given as mean ± S.E.M. For mechanical allodynia, a two-way ANOVA with repeated measures and post hoc multiple pair-wise comparison Bonferroni tests were used to compare the time spent in chamber or 50% withdrawal threshold under various testing conditions. During the CPA test, a paired Student’s t test was used to compare the time spent in each treatment chamber before and after conditioning (i.e. baseline vs test phase for each chamber) (King et al., 2009). Decreased time spent in a chamber during the test phase as compared with the baseline indicates avoidance (aversion) for that chamber. A CPA score was computed by subtracting the time spent in the more noxious chamber during the test phase from the time spent in that chamber at baseline (Johansen et al., 2001; Johansen and Fields, 2004; De Felice et al., 2013). Thus, for rats that were conditioned with LS and NS, CPA for LS was computed by subtracting the time spent in the LS chamber during the test phase from the time spent in that chamber at baseline. Meanwhile, for rats that were conditioned with HS and LS, CPA for HS was computed by subtracting the time spent in the HS chamber during the test phase from the time spent in that chamber at baseline. A two-tailed unpaired Student’s t test was used to compare differences in CPA scores under various testing conditions.
For neuronal spike analysis, to define a neuron that altered its firing rate in response to a peripheral stimulus, we calculated peri-stimulus time histograms (PSTH), using a 5 s range before and after laser stimulus and a bin size of 200 ms. The baseline mean and standard deviation was calculated from the five second interval prior to stimulus. To calculate z-scored firing rate, we used the following equation: Z = (FR – mean of FRb) / standard deviation of FRb, where FR indicates firing rate and FRb indicates baseline firing rate prior to NS, LS or HS. To define a pain responsive neuron, we used the following criteria: (1) The absolute value of the z score firing rate of least two time bins after stimulation must be ≥2.33; and (2) If the first criterion is passed, the lower bound z-score as defined by (Z –ZSEM) at least two bins after stimulation must be greater than 1.645. ZSEM is defined by the following equations: ZSEM (bin) = FRSEM (bin)/standard deviation of FRb (baseline/bin size), and FRSEM (bin) = (standard error of FR over all laser trials)/bin size. For ACC neurons that demonstrated increased firing rates after HS than LS, we also used a robust linear regression model to fit the peak z-scored firing rates in response to HS and LS and to calculate the slope of fit. This provided a ‘tuning curve’ to differentiate between HS and LS. For comparing the slopes of two regression lines, we used a Student’s t-test (Andrade and Estévez-Pérez, 2014).
For all tests, a p value<0.05 was considered statistically significant. All data were analyzed using the GraphPad Prism Version 7 software (GraphPad) and MATLAB (MathWorks).
Population-decoding analysis using machine learning
Request a detailed protocolAfter spike sorting, we obtained population spike trains from simultaneously recorded ACC neurons. For each single neuronal recording, we binned spikes into 100 ms to obtain spike count data in time. To simulate the online decoding, we used a 100 ms moving window to accumulate spike count statistics from laser onset (time 0) until 5 s (i.e., 50 bins). We assessed the decoding accuracy at each time bin based on the cumulative spike count statistics. Therefore, for a total of C neurons, the input dimensionality ranged from C (the first bin) to 50C (all bins). In these experiments where we randomly mixed different laser intensities (NS and HS or LS and HS), we assumed that we have n1 trials under laser intensity 1, and n2 trials under laser intensity 2. We split the total (n1 +n2) trials into two groups, 80% used for training, and 20% used for testing. The goal of population-decoding analysis was to classify the trial labels of different stimulation intensities (e.g., LS vs. HS) based on population spike data. We used a support vector machine (SVM) classifier (Bishop, 2007). The SVM is a discriminative supervised learning model that constructs the classification boundary by a separating hyperplane with maximum margin. Specifically, the SVM can map the input x into high-dimensional feature spaces which allows nonlinear classification.
where denote the class label for the training sample xi (some of which associated with nonzero are called support vectors), b denotes the bias, and K(•,•) denotes the kernel function. We used a polynomial kernel and trained the nonlinear SVM with a sequential minimal optimization algorithm (MATLAB Machine Learning Toolbox: ‘fitcsvm’ function). Finally, the decoding accuracy was assessed by 5-fold cross-validation from 100 Monte Carlo simulations. We report the mean ± S.E.M. decoding accuracy.
As a control, we also computed the chance-level decoding accuracy. We randomly permuted class labels between two classes and repeated the decoding analysis. This shuffling procedure was repeated 500 times, and we reported the chance level by the averaged classification accuracy based on shuffled data with permuted labels. In theory, when the sample sizes from both classes are perfectly balanced, the chance level should be close to 50%. Based on our experimental data, we obtained a chancel-level ~51–53%.
In all population-decoding analyses (pre- and post-CFA), we only used the recording sessions with five or more simultaneously recorded ACC units, independent of the cell firing properties. For NS vs HS, we have 6 and 4 sessions in pre-CFA and post-CFA conditions, respectively. For NS vs LS, we have 5 and 4 sessions in pre-CFA and post-CFA conditions, respectively. For LS vs HS, we have 9 and 15 sessions in pre-CFA and post-CFA conditions, respectively.
References
-
Statistical comparison of the slopes of two regression lines: A tutorialAnalytica Chimica Acta 838:1–12.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aca.2014.04.057
-
The anterior cingulate cortex is a critical hub for pain-induced depressionBiological Psychiatry 77:236–245.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.08.004
-
Dissociable neural responses related to pain intensity, stimulus intensity, and stimulus awareness within the anterior cingulate cortex: a parametric single-trial laser functional magnetic resonance imaging studyJournal of Neuroscience 22:970–976.
-
Quantitative assessment of tactile allodynia in the rat pawJournal of Neuroscience Methods 53:55–63.https://doi.org/10.1016/0165-0270(94)90144-9
-
Deciphering neuronal population codes for acute thermal painJournal of Neural Engineering 14:036023.https://doi.org/10.1088/1741-2552/aa644d
-
Pain intensity processing within the human brain: a bilateral, distributed mechanismJournal of Neurophysiology 82:1934–1943.
-
Remote optogenetic activation and sensitization of pain pathways in freely moving miceJournal of Neuroscience 33:18631–18640.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2424-13.2013
-
Representation of umami taste in the human brainJournal of Neurophysiology 90:313–319.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00669.2002
-
Capturing the aversive state of cephalic pain preclinicallyAnnals of Neurology 74:275–265.https://doi.org/10.1002/ana.23922
-
The role of rostral cingulumotomy in "pain" reliefInternational Journal of Neurology 6:353–373.
-
Pain-related neurons in the human cingulate cortexNature Neuroscience 2:403–405.https://doi.org/10.1038/8065
-
Anterior cingulate cortical neuronal activity during perception of noxious thermal stimuli in monkeysJournal of Neurophysiology 94:1980–1991.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00190.2005
-
Unmasking the tonic-aversive state in neuropathic painNature Neuroscience 12:1364–1366.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2407
-
Anterior cingulate activity during pain-avoidance and reward tasks in monkeysNeuroscience Research 39:421–430.https://doi.org/10.1016/S0168-0102(01)00197-3
-
Predictors and consequences of multiple persistent postmastectomy painsJournal of Pain and Symptom Management 34:619–627.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpainsymman.2007.01.013
-
Activation of corticostriatal circuitry relieves chronic neuropathic painJournal of Neuroscience 35:5247–5259.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3494-14.2015
-
Effect of medial cortical lesions on the avoidance behavior of the catJournal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 58:38–46.https://doi.org/10.1037/h0041014
-
Endogenous opioid activity in the anterior cingulate cortex is required for relief of painJournal of Neuroscience 35:7264–7271.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3862-14.2015
-
Neuroimaging of pain: insights into normal and pathological pain mechanismsNeuroscience Letters 520:129–130.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2012.06.014
-
Predicting dissatisfaction following total knee replacement: a prospective study of 1217 patientsJournal of Bone and Joint Surgery - British Volume 92:1253–1258.https://doi.org/10.1302/0301-620X.92B9.24394
-
Pain perception and personality measures as discriminators in the classification of fibrositisThe Journal of Rheumatology 14:563–569.
-
Nociceptive processing by anterior cingulate pyramidal neuronsJournal of Neurophysiology 103:3287–3301.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00024.2010
-
Nociceptive neurons in area 24 of rabbit cingulate cortexJournal of Neurophysiology 68:1720–1732.
-
Evaluation of pain perception after anterior capsulotomy: a case reportSomatosensory & Motor Research 12:115–126.https://doi.org/10.3109/08990229509101503
-
Bilateral cingulumotomy combined with thalamotomy or mesencephalic tractotomy for painSurgery, Gynecology & Obstetrics 134:958–962.
-
Toward a Mechanism-Based approach to pain diagnosisThe Journal of Pain 17:T50–T69.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2016.03.001
-
The medial pain system, cingulate cortex, and parallel processing of nociceptive informationProgress in Brain Research 122:223–235.https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(08)62141-X
-
An fMRI-based neurologic signature of physical painNew England Journal of Medicine 368:1388–1397.https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMoa1204471
-
Upregulation of forebrain NMDA NR2B receptors contributes to behavioral sensitization after inflammationJournal of Neuroscience 25:11107–11116.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1678-05.2005
Article and author information
Author details
Funding
National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NS100065)
- Jing Wang
- Zhe Chen
National Science Foundation (IIS-1307645)
- Zhe Chen
National Institute of General Medical Sciences (GM102691)
- Jing Wang
National Institute of General Medical Sciences (GM115384)
- Jing Wang
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by the National Institute of General Medical Sciences (GM102691, GM115384, JW), National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NS100065, ZC and JW), (Bethesda, MD, USA), the National Science Foundation (IIS-1307645, Arlington, VA, ZC) and the Anesthesia Research Fund of New York University Department of Anesthesiology (New York, NY, JW).
Ethics
Animal experimentation: All procedures in this study were approved by the New York University School of Medicine Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC) as consistent and in strict accordance with the National Institute of Health (NIH) Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals to ensure minimal animal use and discomfort. The protocol (170315-01) was approved by the ethics committee at New York University School of Medicine. All surgeries were performed under isoflurane anesthesia, and every effort was made to minimize suffering.
Copyright
© 2017, Zhang et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 3,020
- views
-
- 561
- downloads
-
- 74
- citations
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Neuroscience
Brain water homeostasis not only provides a physical protection, but also determines the diffusion of chemical molecules key for information processing and metabolic stability. As a major type of glia in brain parenchyma, astrocytes are the dominant cell type expressing aquaporin water channel. How astrocyte aquaporin contributes to brain water homeostasis in basal physiology remains to be understood. We report that astrocyte aquaporin 4 (AQP4) mediates a tonic water efflux in basal conditions. Acute inhibition of astrocyte AQP4 leads to intracellular water accumulation as optically resolved by fluorescence-translated imaging in acute brain slices, and in vivo by fiber photometry in mobile mice. We then show that aquaporin-mediated constant water efflux maintains astrocyte volume and osmotic equilibrium, astrocyte and neuron Ca2+ signaling, and extracellular space remodeling during optogenetically induced cortical spreading depression. Using diffusion-weighted magnetic resonance imaging (DW-MRI), we observed that in vivo inhibition of AQP4 water efflux heterogeneously disturbs brain water homeostasis in a region-dependent manner. Our data suggest that astrocyte aquaporin, though bidirectional in nature, mediates a tonic water outflow to sustain cellular and environmental equilibrium in brain parenchyma.
-
- Neuroscience
Neuromodulatory inputs to the hippocampus play pivotal roles in modulating synaptic plasticity, shaping neuronal activity, and influencing learning and memory. Recently, it has been shown that the main sources of catecholamines to the hippocampus, ventral tegmental area (VTA) and locus coeruleus (LC), may have overlapping release of neurotransmitters and effects on the hippocampus. Therefore, to dissect the impacts of both VTA and LC circuits on hippocampal function, a thorough examination of how these pathways might differentially operate during behavior and learning is necessary. We therefore utilized two-photon microscopy to functionally image the activity of VTA and LC axons within the CA1 region of the dorsal hippocampus in head-fixed male mice navigating linear paths within virtual reality (VR) environments. We found that within familiar environments some VTA axons and the vast majority of LC axons showed a correlation with the animals’ running speed. However, as mice approached previously learned rewarded locations, a large majority of VTA axons exhibited a gradual ramping-up of activity, peaking at the reward location. In contrast, LC axons displayed a pre-movement signal predictive of the animal’s transition from immobility to movement. Interestingly, a marked divergence emerged following a switch from the familiar to novel VR environments. Many LC axons showed large increases in activity that remained elevated for over a minute, while the previously observed VTA axon ramping-to-reward dynamics disappeared during the same period. In conclusion, these findings highlight distinct roles of VTA and LC catecholaminergic inputs in the dorsal CA1 hippocampal region. These inputs encode unique information, with reward information in VTA inputs and novelty and kinematic information in LC inputs, likely contributing to differential modulation of hippocampal activity during behavior and learning.