Theta-burst microstimulation in the human entorhinal area improves memory specificity
Abstract
The hippocampus is critical for episodic memory, and synaptic changes induced by long-term potentiation (LTP) are thought to underlie memory formation. In rodents, hippocampal LTP may be induced through electrical stimulation of the perforant path. To test whether similar techniques could improve episodic memory in humans, we implemented a microstimulation technique that allowed delivery of low-current electrical stimulation via 100 μm-diameter microelectrodes. As thirteen neurosurgical patients performed a person recognition task, microstimulation was applied in a theta-burst pattern, shown to optimally induce LTP. Microstimulation in the right entorhinal area during learning significantly improved subsequent memory specificity for novel portraits; participants were able both to recognize previously-viewed photos and reject similar lures. These results suggest that microstimulation with physiologic level currents—a radical departure from commonly used deep brain stimulation protocols—is sufficient to modulate human behavior and provides an avenue for refined interrogation of the circuits involved in human memory.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.29515.001Introduction
The hippocampus has been implicated in episodic and autobiographical memory formation in animal models (Devito and Eichenbaum, 2011; Ergorul and Eichenbaum, 2004; Morris et al., 1982; Squire, 1992) and humans (Squire and Zola-Morgan, 1991; Tulving, 2002). Using electrical stimulation to modulate hippocampal activity to restore memory function has recently become a focus of considerable interest. This idea is motivated to an extent by a long history of research on long-term potentiation (LTP), which is accepted as a neural substrate of learning and memory (Bliss and Lomo, 1973; Kandel and Schwartz, 1982) and can be induced by electrical stimulation to hippocampal afferents, such as the perforant path or Schaffer collaterals (Bliss and Lomo, 1973; Cooke and Bliss, 2006; Larson et al., 1986; Nguyen and Kandel, 1997; Staubli and Lynch, 1987).
Deep brain stimulation (DBS) of hippocampal-related targets, such as the fornix (Hamani et al., 2008; Miller et al., 2015) and the entorhinal region (Suthana et al., 2012), have shown promise for modulating human cognition. However, DBS is commonly applied through pairs of large clinical electrodes (e.g., 1.27 mm diameter), which lack target specificity and, hence, may affect wide and heterogeneous neuronal assemblies in nearby brain regions. More spatially-focused stimulation in the medial temporal lobe (MTL) could be achieved using smaller electrodes (for example, 100 μm diameter microelectrodes), allowing higher anatomical specificity and physiological modulation of brain circuits critical for learning and memory. Moreover, targeting afferent projections may be a more specific and efficient strategy to drive downstream neurons (Riva-Posse et al., 2014), as suggested by recent optogenetic studies (Gradinaru et al., 2009; Rajasethupathy et al., 2016).
Microstimulation mimics effects that drive neurons naturally (Fetsch et al., 2014) and has been shown in animal studies to modulate a variety of specific brain functions, depending on the site of stimulation (Hamani et al., 2008; Histed et al., 2009; Logothetis et al., 2010). For example, in nonhuman primates, stimulation using microelectrodes improved face categorization (Afraz et al., 2006), modulated motion perception with great specificity (Fetsch et al., 2014), and increased the learning rate during a reinforcement learning task (Williams and Eskandar, 2006). In humans, microstimulation of primary visual cortex has been shown to induce phosphenes (Schmidt et al., 1996), and microstimulation of the substantia nigra can influence reinforcement learning (Ramayya et al., 2014). Further evidence of the high specificity that this method of stimulation can bring to human brain modulation has been provided by intraoperative investigations (Histed et al., 2013; Logothetis et al., 2010). Stimulation using microwires presents a novel opportunity to improve selective and natural activation of neuronal circuits in the human brain.
Here, we asked whether application of microstimulation targeted to the entorhinal afferents into hippocampus could enhance declarative memory function in humans. We postulated that afferent stimulation would most directly affect the downstream hippocampal fields, and thus memory function as well. Importantly, LTP induction by high-frequency microstimulation of the perforant path in vivo has been shown to cause reorganization of wide hippocampal and cortical networks in rats (Canals et al., 2009). A theta-burst protocol has been shown to be optimal for inducing LTP (Larson et al., 1986). We therefore hypothesized that theta-burst microstimulation, targeted to brain regions containing afferent fibers to the hippocampus, would improve episodic memory performance in humans.
One crucial aspect of episodic memory is pattern separation, the ability to retrieve the specifics of past events without generalizing to similar or partially overlapping events (Yassa and Stark, 2011). During a task in which subjects studied photos of novel people, microstimulation was applied in a theta-burst pattern. We show that theta-burst microstimulation of the right entorhinal area, applied prior to stimulus onset, enhanced memory specificity for these photographs. That is, during a test phase, subjects performed better at accepting photographs from the encoding stage as previously viewed, concomitant with rejecting similar (lure) photographs.
Results
Participants were thirteen neurosurgical patients with pharmacoresistant epilepsy, implanted with intracranial depth electrodes, who performed a person recognition task (Figure 1, Figure 1—source data 1). During the encoding phase, subjects viewed novel portraits of people. On half of the trials, randomly selected for each participant, electrical stimulation was applied during the fixation period prior to stimulus onset (see Microstimulation Protocol section in Materials and methods) (Figure 1A–B). Stimulation was delivered through a 100 μm diameter microwire that had been targeted to the left or right entorhinal area (two subjects received stimulation on each side (in separate sessions); Figure 1—source data 1, Figure 2). In post hoc analysis, all stimulation microwires were determined to have been located in regions known to include afferent inputs to the hippocampus, including entorhinal white matter (angular bundle), entorhinal gray matter, or subiculum (Figure 2, Figure 2—figure supplements 1–2, Figure 1—source data 1) (Yassa et al., 2010; Zeineh et al., 2017). Because the subiculum contains—in fact, is perforated by—fibers of the perforant path, a major output of the entorhinal cortex, it was included in our sample. After the encoding phase, subjects performed a 30 s distractor task and then were presented with a series of images and asked whether each was 'old’ (presented during the encoding phase) or ‘new.’ For each image presented during the encoding phase, two images were included in the test phase; one, the ‘target’ was the exact same image; the other, the ‘lure,’ was a portrait of a different person who looked similar to the person in the target portrait (Figure 1A,C). These images were presented one at a time, in pseudo-random order, during the test phase. Whenever possible, subjects performed the task more than once, using a new set of images each time, such that a total of 40 experimental sessions were conducted (Figure 1—source data 1; Materials and methods).
-
Figure 1—source data 1
- https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.29515.003
To determine the behavioral effects of microstimulation, we defined several performance metrics. For each image presented during encoding, we noted whether it was recognized as ‘old’ during the test phase, as well as whether its corresponding lure image was correctly identified as ‘new.’ For each session, we computed the target acceptance rate as the proportion of target images that were correctly identified as old, and the lure rejection rate as the proportion of lure images that were correctly identified as new. Independently, these measures are not fully informative; for example, if a subject has a bias toward answering ‘old’, that would inflate the target acceptance rate while decreasing the lure rejection rate. Thus, a comparison of responses to targets and lures is required. On the whole-session level, we can compute the discrimination index (DI), which is the probability of answering ‘old’ on target images minus the probability of answering ‘old’ on lure images. DI works to subtract out the bias toward one answer or another, but does not explicitly compare performance for specific targets and their corresponding lures. Thus, for each image we assigned a behavioral label of ‘remembered’ or ‘missed’. ‘Remembered’ images were those that were accepted as previously viewed, along with rejection of the corresponding lure. This required not only recognizing the target image but also remembering the details sufficiently to distinguish it from the lure image. We computed the remembered rate as the proportion of target images that met these criteria. All other images were categorized as ‘missed.’
To evaluate how microstimulation affected these behavioral metrics, we computed each metric separately for the subset of stimulated trials and the subset of non-stimulated trials within each session and used generalized estimating equations (GEEs) to model the effects of stimulation condition on these measures. GEEs are a class of generalized linear models that were developed for analyzing repeated measures data (in our case, multiple experiments for an individual subject) and, contrary to the conventional repeated measures ANOVA tests, can handle different numbers of observations per subject and do not assume equal correlations between within-subject observations (Gardiner et al., 2009; Gueorguieva and Krystal, 2004; Hubbard et al., 2010; Subramanian and O'Malley, 2010), thus making this a rigorous statistical approach for our study (see Materials and methods for details)
We hypothesized that stimulation would improve memory specificity, but the effects of stimulation might vary with the precise location of the stimulating electrode. In particular, because previous studies in both human and non-human primates have indicated that facial processing may be lateralized within the hippocampus (Fried et al., 1997; Haxby et al., 1996), we predicted that stimulation within the left or right entorhinal area may be differentially effective. Thus, using GEEs, we modeled the difference in proportion of remembered pictures between stimulated and non-stimulated trials as a function of stimulation hemisphere. We found that there was a significant effect of stimulation hemisphere on the degree to which stimulation changed the proportion of remembered portraits (p=6.17 × 10−4, Wald = 11.72). Specifically, stimulation of the right entorhinal area significantly improved performance (Estimated Mean (EM) = 0.12, 95% CI [0.05, 0.18]), while stimulation of the left entorhinal area had no effect (EM = −0.017, 95% CI [−0.060, 0.025]) (Figure 3A, Figure 3—source data 1, Source code 1). We confirmed that these results were robust to the precise statistical method chosen; even a basic t-test on the difference between mean performance on stimulated and nonstimulated trials per participant revealed an effect of stimulation hemisphere (t(13) = −2.98, p=0.011), and post hoc t-tests indicated that right entorhinal stimulation caused significant improvement (mean change = 0.12 ± 0.04, t(8) = 3.53, p=0.008; effect size: Cohen’s dz = 1.18, CL = 0.88), while left entorhinal stimulation did not (mean change = −0.02 ± 0.03, t(5) = −0.77, p=0.48) (Figure 3B). Further, out of 9 subjects who received microstimulation in the right entorhinal area, eight had a higher remembered rate for stimulated trials compared to nonstimulated trials, which is more than expected by chance (p=0.020, binomial test). Finally, because our subjects were patients with epilepsy, we wanted to ensure that the effect of stimulation was not driven by epileptic activity. To test this, we applied the GEE model described above to the subset of data that included only sessions in which the stimulating electrode was located outside of the seizure onset zone. Our results remained qualitatively the same, with a strong effect of stimulation hemisphere on behavior, and significant improvement induced by right-sided stimulation (p=9.49 × 10−3, Wald = 6.73, right EM = 0.11; 95% CI [.029. 184]).
-
Figure 3—source data 1
- https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.29515.008
-
Figure 3—source data 2
- https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.29515.009
-
Figure 3—source data 3
- https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.29515.010
Although our experimental design included selecting at random which trials in each session received stimulation, we wanted to ensure that our results were independent of any potential trial order effects. We thus modeled the behavioral data on a trial by trial basis while accounting for the trial order as well as whether the target image was presented before the lure image during the test phase (1207 trials). Here, the model included main effects of normalized trial number, whether the target was presented first, stimulation condition (trial received stimulation or not), stimulation hemisphere (left or right), and an interaction term between the last two. We found that the interaction term between the stimulation condition and hemisphere had a strong significant effect on behavior (p=2.19 × 10−3, Wald = 9.38), while neither of the order variables were a significant predictor (normalized trial number: p=0.19, presentation of target before lure: p=0.053). Consistent with the previous model, stimulation of the right entorhinal area increased the probability of remembering a portrait to a greater degree than stimulation of the left entorhinal area (Figure 3C; Figure 3—source data 2–3, Source code 2).
Because the order of presentation of target and lure trended strongly toward a behavioral effect—memory was generally better when the target was presented before the lure—we wanted to ensure that the interaction between stimulation condition and hemisphere was independent of whether the target or lure had been presented first. We thus divided our dataset into trials in which the target was presented first (618 trials) and trials in which the lure was presented first (589 trials), and modeled behavioral outcome with normalized trial number, stimulation condition, stimulation hemisphere and an interaction for each dataset. Consistent with the full dataset, we found in each case that normalized trial number was not a significant predictor (target first: p=0.33; lure first: p=0.55), while the interaction between stimulation site and stimulation condition was (target first: p=0.018; lure first: p=0.011).
Finally, we tested whether precise electrode targeting had an effect on the degree to which stimulation improved memory. Our electrodes were localized to three distinct regions containing fibers of the perforant path: the angular bundle (entorhinal white matter), gray matter of the entorhinal cortex (where perforant path fibers originate), and the nearby subiculum (which is perforated by the perforant path prior to its arrival in the hippocampus [Zeineh et al., 2017]). We hypothesized that, because stimulation to the perforant path can induce LTP, stimulating in the region where perforant path fibers are most densely concentrated might be the most effective. Specifically, we compared stimulation in the angular bundle, where perforant path fibers are most densely concentrated (Yassa et al., 2010; Zeineh et al., 2017), to stimulation in either the entorhinal or subicular gray matter, where these fibers are more spread out and fewer axons would be recruited by stimulation. Accordingly, we modeled the proportion of the remembered portraits as a function of two factors: stimulation region (whether the stimulating electrode was in the angular bundle or gray matter) and stimulation hemisphere (left versus right). We found significant main effects of each stimulation hemisphere (p=1.49 × 10−4, Wald =14.39) and stimulation region (p=0.011, Wald =6.53). The interaction between these factors was not significant (p=0.121, Wald =2.41), indicating that the effects of stimulation on the right side and in the angular bundle each contributed to improved behavioral performance. Specifically, stimulation was most effective for improving memory specificity when the electrodes were positioned in the right angular bundle (EM = 0.16, 95% CI [0.09, 0.22]) (Figure 4A, Figure 3—source data 1; Figure 4—source data 1, Source code 3).
-
Figure 4—source data 1
- https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.29515.013
We next applied this expanded model to test the effect of stimulation on the other behavioral metrics defined above. As expected, DI—also a measure of memory specificity—was improved by right-sided stimulation (p=2.79 × 10−3, Wald =8.94) and stimulation in the angular bundle (p=2.53 × 10−3, Wald =9.12; interaction term: p=0.15, Wald =2.07; right angular bundle EM = 0.15, 95% CI [0.08, 0.23]) (Figure 4B, Figure 3—source data 1; Figure 4—source data 1, Source code 3). A similar pattern was present in target acceptance, with stimulation in each the right side (p=0.034, Wald =4.47) and the angular bundle (p=2.52 × 10−5, Wald =17.75) significantly increasing target acceptance rates (right angular bundle EM = 0.074, 95% CI [0.0069. 0.1414]) (Figure 4C, Figure 3—source data 1; Figure 4—source data 1, Source code 3). A different pattern emerged when considering lure rejection rates. Here the interaction between stimulation region and hemisphere was significant (p=0.018, Wald =5.62; right angular bundle EM = 0.080, 95% CI [0.025, 0.134]). The difference appears to be driven by stimulation in the right gray matter showing a trend toward impairing lure rejection (Figure 4D, Figure 3—source data 1; Figure 4—source data 1, Source code 3, Source code 4).
When considering lure rejection and target acceptance together, we found that stimulation on the right side increased the probability of correctly accepting previously viewed target images, regardless of the stimulation region. When right-sided stimulation was applied in the angular bundle, this also led to an increase in appropriately rejecting lure images. When applied in gray matter, however, stimulation led to an increase in incorrectly accepting lure images. Thus, it appears that right angular bundle stimulation improved memory specificity by simultaneously increasing the probability of accepting target images and rejecting lure images. Stimulation of the right gray matter, on the other hand—by increasing acceptance of both targets and lures—introduced a bias towards reporting positive memory, rather than increasing memory per se. Conversely, stimulation in the left entorhinal-adjacent gray matter decreased target acceptance rates and showed a moderate trend toward decreasing lure acceptance rates, introducing a bias toward negative memory. Together these results suggest that in entorhinal/subicular gray matter, stimulation in the left and right hemispheres may introduce opposite biases.
Finally, it should be noted that, although we report uncorrected p-values throughout the results section, all reported statistically significant effects survive correction for multiple comparisons (see Materials and methods), further demonstrating the statistical robustness of our results. We also provide additional analysis in the supplement showing the main effects we report in Figures 3 and 4 when data from the subiculum is excluded (Figure 4—figure supplement 1). The overall results are similar, but further studies will be necessary to examine potential differences between microstimulation of subicular and entorhinal cortices.
Discussion
We found that microstimulation to the entorhinal area, targeted to maximally affect the entorhinal projections into the hippocampus, resulted in enhancement of memory specificity for images of novel people when the stimulating electrode was in the right hemisphere. This suggests a possible lateralization effect for memory of people, consistent with previous results: the right hippocampus shows increased metabolic activity during the learning of faces (Haxby et al., 1996), and single-unit activity in the right hippocampus encodes different facial expressions (Fried et al., 1997). While direct recordings of neural activity have shown evidence for lateralization, imaging studies have indicated that the encoding of pictorial material involves bilateral activation (Kim, 2011), and lesion studies noted a lack of asymmetry for encoding of non-verbal material (Lee et al., 2002). This discrepancy may indicate a difference between encoding of faces and visual encoding in general, as the latter studies were not specific to images of faces or people. On the other hand, our results indicate that that the left hippocampus is not uninvolved during facial encoding, as stimulation of the left entorhinal area had a behavioral effect, even if it was not to increase memory specificity. Previous findings of bilateral activation, therefore, may indicate only that the two hemispheres are both involved, not that they are performing the same function. Thus, these results warrant further studies into potential lateralization effects, both of function and of the effect of stimulation. It is important to note that our study was conducted in people with epilepsy, a condition which may disproportionately affect one side of the brain and could result in lateralized effects, such as impairment of cognitive skills lateralized to the affected hemisphere. Thus conclusions with respect to lateralization of function should be viewed with caution. However, we have repeated our analysis with data restricted to the sessions in which the stimulating electrode was positioned outside the clinically-determined seizure onset zone, and found qualitatively similar results.
Previous studies in animals have noted the importance of the frequency and mode of stimulation on behavioral outcomes. Specifically, theta-burst stimulation has been used to elicit LTP in a wide range of animal models from hippocampal slices (Nguyen and Kandel, 1997; Staubli and Lynch, 1987) to whole animals (Larson et al., 1986). In the current study, we present evidence supporting the efficacy of theta-burst microstimulation in protocols aimed at human memory improvement. Further research is warranted to determine whether theta-burst stimulation is more effective than other protocols, such as continuous stimulation. We also made no effort in this study to synchronize our stimulation protocols to endogenous theta rhythms in the hippocampus; we hypothesize that the memory effects we observed may be enhanced further by developing a closed-loop stimulation system, allowing the theta-burst pattern to be initiated on specific phases of theta.
Beyond the temporal dynamics of stimulation, we found that the spatial targeting of the stimulating electrode was also a critical factor in determining the effects of stimulation on behavior. Controlling this factor is more feasible with microstimulation than macrostimulation, as it is challenging to confine the spatial extent of delivered current when using macroelectrodes, due to their large contact surface area, wide inter-contact distance for bipolar stimulation, and high magnitude of stimulation current (Figure 2—figure supplement 1). In addition, high frequency stimulation delivered via macroelectrodes has been shown to inhibit nearby neuronal somata, while also providing excitation to axonal projections, indicating that small changes in electrode location could lead to substantially different results (Herrington et al., 2016). For example, stimulating in gray matter could disrupt neuronal computations, whereas electrodes that are confined to white matter may enhance excitatory drive to downstream regions (Afraz et al., 2006; Arcot Desai et al., 2014; Fetsch et al., 2014; Histed et al., 2009). Future work examining the physiological effects of microstimulation in downstream regions, such as the hippocampus, would further enlighten the mechanisms of the differential effects when stimulation is applied in gray matter or the angular bundle.
Recently, studies on the effects of DBS—delivered via macroelectrodes in the entorhinal area—on memory performance have resulted in contradicting outcomes (Jacobs et al., 2016; Suthana et al., 2012). Stimulation delivered in these studies likely affected different neuronal assemblies, which—together with other methodological differences between them—may account for the variability in the reported results. Microstimulation is delivered via a single, small electrode, which eliminates the heterogeneity introduced in macrostimulation studies by variable inter-contact distances and implantation trajectories, and may make the final localization of the electrode easier to compare between research groups; this may increase the ease of replicability of studies using microstimulation, even in the face of other methodological differences.
Additional evidence for the importance of stimulation specificity arises from optogenetic studies in animals. In particular, a transgenic mouse model of early Alzheimer’s disease shows that the more focally targeted an intervention is, the more specific of an effect can be elicited. This study demonstrated that direct optogenetic activation of specific hippocampal memory engram cells resulted in highly specific memory retrieval (Roy et al., 2016). Other optogenetic studies highlight the advantage of afferent stimulation to target downstream neuronal assemblies compared to direct stimulation of gray matter (Gradinaru et al., 2009; Rajasethupathy et al., 2016).
While optogenetics is the state-of-the-art method for targeting neural circuits with precise spatial and temporal resolution (Rajasethupathy et al., 2016), it is not likely to be feasible in humans in the near future. The microstimulation approach may, thus, be the method available in humans that most closely serves a similar purpose. Compared to macroelectrodes, microelectrodes have much smaller electrode contacts and affect a more localized region (Arcot Desai et al., 2014). The lower current levels typically delivered through microelectrodes have also been shown to excite, rather than inhibit, nearby neurons (Afraz et al., 2006; Arcot Desai et al., 2014; Fetsch et al., 2014), as well as those downstream of the nearby axonal projections (Histed et al., 2009). Moreover, high frequency microstimulation of the perforant path that induces LTP in hippocampus has been shown to cause widespread reorganization in hippocampal networks, as well as in cortical areas, such as medial frontal cortex and nucleus accumbens (Canals et al., 2009). These observations are compatible with the efficacy of microstimulation of the perforant path to cause overt behavioral changes in memory performance.
It has been suggested that stimulation in the entorhinal white matter, but not the adjacent gray matter, might lead to beneficial effects on memory (Suthana et al., 2012). Indeed, the results of the current study are consistent with this idea, such that applying microstimulation in the right angular bundle had a significant positive effect on memory for portraits, while applying microstimulation in the right entorhinal gray matter or subiculum did not improve memory, despite the presence of hippocampal afferents in these regions. This may be due to the fact that axons of the perforant path—a common locus for induction of LTP—are most densely concentrated in the angular bundle (Yassa et al., 2010; Zeineh et al., 2017) and hence a larger number would be recruited via stimulation. In parallel, stimulation in the gray matter may more directly impact or interfere with neuronal computations, which could drive the biases observed in our results due to gray matter stimulation. Although we hypothesize that LTP induced via the perforant path is likely responsible for the positive effects of right angular bundle stimulation, it must be borne in mind that the entorhinal region includes several additional fiber tracts—including the direct projections to CA1 via the alvear path, and hippocampal efferents—any of which may be affected by microstimulation, and current imaging techniques do not allow us to separate these various tracts.
Further, it is worth noting that the present study does not address the precise mechanisms by which microstimulation wields its behavioral influence. The distinct patterns observed as a result of stimulation in different hemispheres or different regions offer hints of a complex effect. Future studies investigating how neural signals change in response to microstimulation (and how these changes vary with the precise targeting of the stimulating electrode) will be critical for increasing our understanding, not only of the physiological signatures of microstimulation, but also the microcircuit dynamics underlying memory.
This study presents the first evidence that microstimulation has the potential to improve hippocampal-dependent memory in humans. In addition to the interesting implications for our understanding of microcircuit dynamics of memory, this paves the way to developing new avenues that could one day be used for treating patients with chronic memory impairment. Chronic neural implants that use macrostimulation to treat conditions such as Parkinson’s Disease and Epilepsy have become increasingly common (Fisher and Velasco, 2014; Schuepbach et al., 2013). As the use of neural implants moves toward treating cognitive disorders, one advantage of including microstimulation is the precise spatial targeting it affords, allowing for highly-controlled manipulation of neural circuits. Additionally, microstimulation requires lower levels of current to be delivered, thus allowing the implant to consume considerably less power. Future studies will be required to fully understand the stimulation protocols that provide the best cognitive enhancement for a wide variety of tasks. Large-scale datasets that evaluate the efficacy of macro- vs microstimulation, stimulation region and hemisphere, precise timing of stimulation relative to endogenous brain states, and more, will be required to establish the clinical relevance of stimulation for cognitive enhancement.
At present, there is only one relatively large-scale dataset published to date that attempts to report the effects of (macro) stimulation on memory (Jacobs et al., 2016). Even that study includes only five patients with entorhinal stimulation for one task (spatial memory) and seven for the other (verbal memory). Although that study reported an overall negative effect of stimulation on memory, it is important to note the many differences between it and the present study. In addition to the critical differences between micro- and macrostimulation described above, the tasks in the two studies were quite different. Here we investigated memory specificity in a person recognition task, while in the Jacobs et al., 2016 the tasks required spatial navigation or memorization of a list of words, which may be differentially affected by stimulation. We also accounted for possible confounds of the effectiveness of stimulation, such as the precise location of the stimulating electrode and the hemisphere to receive stimulation. Without accounting for those factors, the positive effects of stimulation could be washed out.
In conclusion, our findings suggest that microstimulation, with its anatomical precision, physiologic-level currents, and action via axonal projections, holds promise for modification of memory circuits and thus for the treatment of memory impairments in people suffering from neurological disorders. More generally, this method may provide a tool for highly specific modulation of neuronal activity and human behavior (Young and Deisseroth, 2017).
Materials and methods
Study participants
Request a detailed protocolThe study subjects were thirteen patients (N = 13)* with pharmacoresistant epilepsy (Figure 1—source data 1) who met clinical criteria for depth-electrode placement in the entorhinal area for seizure localization and possible surgical cure by resection of the identified seizure focus. Electrodes fashioned with macro contacts along the shaft for intracranial EEG (Adtech Medical Instrument Corp., Racine, WI) and with micro wires at the distal end (California Fine Wire, Grover Beach, CA) were implanted stereotactically with the aid of CT angiography (CTA) and magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) (Fried et al., 1993; 1999). Among the 13 subjects, 40 experimental sessions were conducted (n = 40), in which stimulation was applied in the right entorhinal area (N = 9, n = 19) or the left entorhinal area (N = 6, n = 21) (Two subjects performed sessions with right and sessions with left.)**. Language dominance testing was carried out when deemed clinically necessary. Language dominance was determined either through a standard Wada protocol or through fMRI language testing (Połczyńska et al., 2015). All research was carried out at the UCLA Medical Center and the UCLA Institutional Review Board approved the study protocol. All subjects provided written consent to participate in the study.
* Fourteen subjects participated in the study, however one subject was excluded due to severe psychological issues that arose during hospitalization and interfered with the patient’s ability to complete any task.
** In one session (participant 13, left-sided stimulation), a small fraction of trials (17/52) received mistimed stimulation. These trials were excluded from analysis, but the session was included in the statistics reported in the main text. Additionally, we re-ran all statistics excluding this session entirely and the results remain nearly identical.
Behavioral paradigm
Request a detailed protocolEach experiment consisted of three phases: encoding, distraction, and retrieval. During the encoding phase, participants completed a behavioral task in which they viewed 31 ± 12 novel portraits of people (taken from Versluis and Uyttenbroek, 2002) on a computer screen (Figure 1A). People with epilepsy exhibit a wide spectrum of cognitive ability, due most likely to the effects of disease etiology as well as anti-epileptic drugs (AEDs). As such, we titrated the number of photographs shown for each subject with the guidance of their previous neuropsychological testing, as well as a short 4-portrait pre-test, using distinct stimuli, before the first experiment to ensure that the participants understood the task and could follow instructions. For our titrations, we targeted a 60–70% target acceptance rate to avoid ceiling or floor effects. During each encoding phase, for half of the images stimulation was applied for one second during the fixation period preceding image onset (Figure 1A–B); the subset of images to receive stimulation was selected randomly for each participant to avoid potential confounds based on stimulus complexity or order effects. After viewing the novel photographs, subjects performed a distractor task for 30 s in which single digits were presented serially, one per second, and subjects identified them as odd or even. This task is a non-mnemonic distractor task that has been shown to increase the demand for hippocampal involvement during the subsequent test phase (Stark and Squire, 2001). Finally, during the test phase, the same images from the encoding phase (‘targets’) were intermixed with an equal number of images of similar-looking portraits of different people (‘lures’) and presented one at a time (Figure 1A,C). In the test phase, participants were asked to identify whether each image was ‘new’ or ‘old’ by pressing a button. Participants also reported subjective confidence scores for each image, but these data are not presented. Target images were classified as ‘remembered’ if the subject correctly accepted the target as seen before and correctly rejected the corresponding lure. Otherwise, images were classified as ‘missed’.
Behavioral performance outcomes were studied in four measurements: the proportion of pictures remembered, the proportion of targets accepted, the proportion of lures rejected, and discrimination index (DI), which is the proportion of correctly accepted targets minus the proportion of incorrectly accepted lure images (Glosser et al., 1998; Suthana et al., 2015).
Microstimulation protocol
Request a detailed protocolMonopolar constant-current microstimulation was applied using a BlackRock R96 Micro-Stimulator (BlackRock Microsystems, Salt Lake City, UT). The stimulation electrodes consisted of 100 µm diameter Formvar-insulated Platinum-Iridium (Pt/Ir) micro wires with the insulation removed from 1 mm around the tip, implanted into the right or left entorhinal area (Fried et al., 1999). Stimulation electrode impedance was measured immediately before testing to ensure it remained below 60 kΩ. Impedance values averaged 25.2 ± 13.9 kΩ. 150 µA cathodic-first, biphasic microstimulation was applied with a pulse width of 200 µs and an inter-pulse interval of 100 µs. Theta burst microstimulation pulse trains consisted of 4 such pulses at 100 Hz every 200 ms (Figure 1B), as this protocol has previously been demonstrated to be optimal for eliciting LTP in hippocampal slices from rat (Larson et al., 1986). This stimulation protocol resulted in a charge delivery of 30 nC per phase and a charge density of 9.32 µC/cm2, well below the generally accepted upper safety limit of 100–150 µC/cm2 for stimulation of neural tissue through Pt/Ir electrodes (Merrill et al., 2005; Rose and Robblee, 1990). Stimulation was applied for 1 s (5 pulse-trains composed of 4 pulses each) beginning at a time selected randomly from a uniform distribution between 2.2 and 2.7 s before picture presentation. All stimulation parameters were kept constant across patients, with the exception of precise electrode localization. Although two patients received stimulation on each side, the stimulating electrode was held constant throughout each experimental session. Electrode placements were confirmed post-surgically with co-registration of CT scans with preoperative MRI (See below; Figure 2 and Figure 2—figure supplement 1). Sites of stimulation for each participant are summarized in Figure 1—source data 1.
Brain imaging parameters
Request a detailed protocolMRI data were acquired on a Siemens Magnetom Prisma 3 Tesla system housed in the Department of Radiology at UCLA. The whole brain MRI images were collected over 176 axial slices using a T1-weighted gradient echo sequence (TR 11 ms; TE 2.81 ms; flip angle 20 degrees; matrix size 256 × 256 mm; FOV 256 mm; in-plane resolution 1 × 1 mm; slice thickness 1 mm; voxel size 1 mm isotropic). A high-resolution T2 weighted structural scan was also acquired for each subject (TR: 5300 ms; TE: 70 ms; flip angle: 178 degrees; matrix size: 500 × 500 mm; FOV: 200 mm; in-plane resolution: 0.4 × 0.4 mm; slice thickness: 3 mm, voxel size: 0.4 × 0.4 × 3 mm, 19 slices).
Spiral computed CT scans were performed on a 64-row multi-detector CT scanner. All scans had a pre-contrast series and single phase, post-contrast acquisition, synchronized using bolus tracking technique for arterial phase. Omnipaque 350 contrast media volume was set as 100 cc with an infusion rate of 3.0 cc/s.
Electrode localization
Request a detailed protocolA high-resolution post-operative computed tomography (CT) scan was co-registered to a pre-operative whole brain magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and high-resolution MRI (Figure 2; Figure 2—Figure supplement 1) using BrainLab stereotactic localization software (www.brainlab.com; [Gumprecht et al., 1999; Schlaier et al., 2004]) and FSL FLIRT (FMRIB’s Linear Registration Tool [Jenkinson et al., 2002; Jenkinson and Smith, 2001]). Medial temporal lobe regions (entorhinal, perirhinal, and parahippocampal cortices, and hippocampal subfields CA23DG [CA2, CA3, dentate gyrus], CA1, and subiculum) were delineated using the Automatic Segmentation of Hippocampal Subfields (ASHS [Pluta et al., 2012; Yushkevich et al., 2010]) software using boundaries determined from MRI visible landmarks that correlate with underlying cellular histology (Amaral and Insausti, 1990; Duvernoy and Bourgouin, 1998). In some cases, pixels that were clearly misplaced (e.g. single pixels from one region falling well within another or pixels that were entirely outside the MtL) were modified for aesthetics. In no case did this modification affect the outcome of the electrode localization procedure. In a single subject, whose electrode was farther anterior than ASHS can accommodate, subfields were delineated manually, with reference to brain atlases (Amaral and Insausti, 1990; Duvernoy and Bourgouin, 1998). White matter and cerebral spinal fluid areas were outlined using FSL FAST software (Zhang et al., 2001). Together, similar methods have been used previously to localize microelectrodes and investigate structural and functional dissociations within human medial temporal lobe subregions (Ekstrom et al., 2008; Suthana et al., 2009; Zeineh et al., 2017). A single 100 µm microelectrode at the distal tip, 3 mm from the most distal macro- contact, was used for microstimulation. Macro- and micro-electrode contacts were identified and outlined on the post-operative CT scan. To confirm white matter location of microelectrodes, the high-resolution MRI, with ASHS and FAST segmentation results, was overlaid with the co-registered electrode (Figure 2, Figure 2—figure supplement 1). For group-level electrode placements, individual localizations were mapped onto a standard MNI brain and visualized using the BrainNet Matlab toolbox (Figure 2—figure supplement 2) (Xia et al., 2013).
A neurologist reviewed clinical recordings in real-time while stimulation was applied to ensure absence of after-discharges. Participants did not report noticing any effects of stimulation, nor did they exhibit any seizures during pre-testing or task execution.
Statistical analysis methods
Request a detailed protocolData were analyzed using SPSS (IBM Corporation, Armonk, NY) and custom scripts developed in Matlab (Mathworks, Natick, MA). All statistical models were implemented in SPSS using Generalized Estimating Equations (GEEs), which are a class of regression marginal models for investigating the relationships between clustered response data and outcome measures in a multivariate manner and a within-subject repeated measures design (Data and SPSS model definitions are in Figure 3—source data 1–2, Source code 1–3; Matlab code is in Source code 4). In our dataset, each subject performed the task a variable number of times (range: 1–7), due to circumstances that were beyond our control and unrelated to the experiment itself (most notably, the length of a patient stay in the hospital is determined by clinical criteria and can vary from several days to several weeks). Due to this variability, we sought a class of models that could accept different numbers of data points from each subject—without sacrificing the statistical power that could be derived from having multiple data points from most subjects—and GEEs were well-suited to the task (Gardiner et al., 2009; Gueorguieva and Krystal, 2004; Hardin, 2005; Hubbard et al., 2010; Subramanian and O'Malley, 2010).
Four session-level behavioral metrics were examined: the proportion of pictures remembered, DI, proportion of targets accepted, and proportion of lures rejected. For each of these, we computed the effect of stimulation on performance for each session by computing the metric for the subset of non-stimulated trials and subtracting that from the metric computed for the subset of stimulated trials. We implemented the models with participant identity as a within-subject variable, independent correlation matrices with robust covariance estimates, and identifying the metrics as linear scale responses, due to their approximately normal distributions. We included stimulation hemisphere—and later whether or not the stimulation electrode was in the angular bundle—as factors. We included model terms for these main effects, and an interaction term when applicable. Model statistics are reported in Figure 4—source data 1. Error bars in the figures (3A, 4A-D) denote Wald 95% confidence intervals for the estimated means.
For the trial-by-trial model, we selected a binomial logit model, as we had a two-level response variable (remembered vs missed), and used exchangeable correlation matrices. In addition to stimulation hemisphere, stimulation condition, and the interaction between these, we included whether the target was presented before the lure as a factor, and normalized trial number (trial number divided by the number of trials in the set) as a covariate. Model statistics are reported in Figure 3—source data 3. Error bars in Figure 3C denote Wald 95% confidence intervals for the estimated means.
Correction for multiple comparisons was performed using the Holm-Bonferroni method. To be as conservative as possible, we included all statistical tests that evaluated how stimulation’s effect on memory depended on hemispheric and/or regional effects in a single correction test. This included the a priori tests from each figure panel in Figures 3 and 4. All tests that were originally reported as statistically significant at an alpha level of 0.05 were found to remain significant after this correction. Original, rather than corrected, p-values are reported in the text.
References
-
BookThe hippocampal formationIn: Paxinos G, editors. The Human Nervous System. San Diego: Academic Press. pp. 711–755.
-
Deep brain stimulation macroelectrodes compared to multiple microelectrodes in rat hippocampusFrontiers in Neuroengineering 7:16.https://doi.org/10.3389/fneng.2014.00016
-
BookThe Human Hippocampus: Functional Anatomy, Vascularization, and Serial Sections with MRIBerlin: Springer.
-
The hippocampus and memory for "what," "where," and "when"Learning & Memory 11:397–405.https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.73304
-
Electrical brain stimulation for epilepsyNature Reviews Neurology 10:261–270.https://doi.org/10.1038/nrneurol.2014.59
-
Stereotactic Surgery and Radiosurgery149–158, Implantation of depth electrodes for EEG recording, Stereotactic Surgery and Radiosurgery, Madison, WI, Medical Physics Publishing.
-
Fixed effects, random effects and GEE: what are the differences?Statistics in Medicine 28:221–239.https://doi.org/10.1002/sim.3478
-
Differential lateralization of memory discrimination and response bias in temporal lobe epilepsy patientsJournal of the International Neuropsychological Society 4:502–511.https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617798455097
-
Move over anovaArchives of General Psychiatry 61:310–317.https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.61.3.310
-
Memory enhancement induced by hypothalamic/fornix deep brain stimulationAnnals of Neurology 63:119–123.https://doi.org/10.1002/ana.21295
-
Mechanisms of deep brain stimulationJournal of Neurophysiology 115:19–38.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00281.2015
-
Insights into cortical mechanisms of behavior from microstimulation experimentsProgress in Neurobiology 103:115–130.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pneurobio.2012.01.006
-
A global optimisation method for robust affine registration of brain imagesMedical Image Analysis 5:143–156.https://doi.org/10.1016/S1361-8415(01)00036-6
-
The effects of electrical microstimulation on cortical signal propagationNature Neuroscience 13:1283–1291.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2631
-
Electrical stimulation of excitable tissue: design of efficacious and safe protocolsJournal of Neuroscience Methods 141:171–198.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2004.10.020
-
In vivo analysis of hippocampal subfield atrophy in mild cognitive impairment via semi-automatic segmentation of T2-weighted MRIJournal of Alzheimer's disease : JAD 31:85–99.https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-2012-111931
-
Microstimulation of the human substantia nigra alters reinforcement learningJournal of Neuroscience 34:6887–6895.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5445-13.2014
-
Electrical stimulation with Pt electrodes. VIII. Electrochemically safe charge injection limits with 0.2 ms pulsesIEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering 37:1118–1120.https://doi.org/10.1109/10.61038
-
Neurostimulation for Parkinson's disease with early motor complicationsNew England Journal of Medicine 368:610–622.https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMoa1205158
-
The medial temporal lobe memory systemScience 253:1380–1386.https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1896849
-
Simple and associative recognition memory in the hippocampal regionLearning & Memory 8:190–197.https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.40701
-
Human hippocampal CA1 involvement during allocentric encoding of spatial informationJournal of Neuroscience 29:10512–10519.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0621-09.2009
-
Memory enhancement and deep-brain stimulation of the entorhinal areaNew England Journal of Medicine 366:502–510.https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMoa1107212
-
Episodic memory: from mind to brainAnnual Review of Psychology 53:1–25.https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135114
-
Pattern separation in the hippocampusTrends in Neurosciences 34:515–525.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2011.06.006
-
Segmentation of brain MR images through a hidden Markov random field model and the expectation-maximization algorithmIEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging 20:45–57.https://doi.org/10.1109/42.906424
Article and author information
Author details
Funding
Schweizerischer Nationalfonds zur Förderung der Wissenschaftlichen Forschung (PBSKP3-124730)
- Michael R H Hill
National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NS084017)
- Itzhak Fried
National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NS058280)
- Emily A Mankin
- Zahra M Aghajan
A.P. Giannini Foundation (Postdoctoral Fellowship)
- Emily A Mankin
Defense Advanced Research Projects Agency (N66001-14-2-4029)
- Itzhak Fried
G. Harold and Leila Y. Mathers Charitable Foundation (09212007)
- Itzhak Fried
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Acknowledgements
We thank Tony Fields, Kirk Shattuck, Michael Jenkins, and Antonio Campos for technical assistance; Deena Pourshaban, Marianna Holliday, Samantha Briones, Nancy Guerrero, Güldamla Kalender, and Brooke Salaz for general assistance; the IDRE statistical consulting group at UCLA for providing insightful discussions regarding statistical analysis methods, and the participants for volunteering for this study.
Ethics
Human subjects: All research was carried out at the UCLA Medical Center and the UCLA Institutional Review Board approved the study protocol (IRB#10-000973). All subjects provided written consent to participate in the study.
Copyright
© 2017, Titiz et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 4,931
- views
-
- 680
- downloads
-
- 95
- citations
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Neuroscience
Since its inception, the concept of neurodiversity has been defined in a number of different ways, which can cause confusion among those hoping to educate themselves about the topic. Learning about neurodiversity can also be challenging because there is a lack of well-curated, appropriately contextualized information on the topic. To address such barriers, we present an annotated reading list that was developed collaboratively by a neurodiverse group of researchers. The nine themes covered in the reading list are: the history of neurodiversity; ways of thinking about neurodiversity; the importance of lived experience; a neurodiversity paradigm for autism science; beyond deficit views of ADHD; expanding the scope of neurodiversity; anti-ableism; the need for robust theory and methods; and integration with open and participatory work. We hope this resource can support readers in understanding some of the key ideas and topics within neurodiversity, and that it can further orient researchers towards more rigorous, destigmatizing, accessible, and inclusive scientific practices.
-
- Neuroscience
As the early step of food ingestion, the swallow is under rigorous sensorimotor control. Nevertheless, the mechanisms underlying swallow control at a molecular and circuitry level remain largely unknown. Here, we find that mutation of the mechanotransduction channel genes nompC, Tmc, or piezo impairs the regular pumping rhythm of the cibarium during feeding of the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster. A group of multi-dendritic mechanosensory neurons, which co-express the three channels, wrap the cibarium and are crucial for coordinating the filling and emptying of the cibarium. Inhibition of them causes difficulty in food emptying in the cibarium, while their activation leads to difficulty in cibarium filling. Synaptic and functional connections are detected between the pharyngeal mechanosensory neurons and the motor circuit that controls swallow. This study elucidates the role of mechanosensation in swallow, and provides insights for a better understanding of the neural basis of food swallow.