A sleep state in Drosophila larvae required for neural stem cell proliferation
Abstract
Sleep during development is involved in refining brain circuitry, but a role for sleep in the earliest periods of nervous system elaboration, when neurons are first being born, has not been explored. Here we identify a sleep state in Drosophila larvae that coincides with a major wave of neurogenesis. Mechanisms controlling larval sleep are partially distinct from adult sleep: octopamine, the Drosophila analog of mammalian norepinephrine, is the major arousal neuromodulator in larvae, but dopamine is not required. Using real-time behavioral monitoring in a closed-loop sleep deprivation system, we find that sleep loss in larvae impairs cell division of neural progenitors. This work establishes a system uniquely suited for studying sleep during nascent periods, and demonstrates that sleep in early life regulates neural stem cell proliferation.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.33220.001eLife digest
Nearly all animals sleep more while they are still developing, suggesting that sleep is important in early life. Previous studies have shown that sleep may be required for building connections in the brain. However, it has been difficult to study the effects of sleep in earlier stages of brain development, when stem cells divide to create brain cells in a process known as “neurogenesis”. This is partly because, in mammals, most neurogenesis occurs in the womb.
Scientists have successfully studied sleep using the common fruit fly. But these studies have so far focused on adult flies, in which neurogenesis is mostly complete. Fly larvae, on the other hand, are widely used to study brain development and neurogenesis. Compared to mammals in the womb, fruit fly larvae are very easy to access and manipulate. However, unlike adult flies, no one had previously looked to see if larvae even display a behaviour that would fit the definition of sleep.
To see if fruit fly larvae do sleep, Szuperak et al. created the “LarvaLodge”, an apparatus in which individual larvae can be housed while having their activity monitored over time. In these lodges, a bright light was used to test how hard it is to arouse inactive fruit fly larvae, and further experiments asked what happens when larvae are prevented from resting. Then, to look at neurogenesis in the larvae, Szuperak et al. used a stain that labels dividing stem cells within the nervous system. Those cells could then be seen and counted when a larva was dissected and examined under a microscope.
The results from the LarvaLodge showed that fruit fly larvae do indeed sleep: they have extended periods of rest during which they react less to outside disturbances and adopt a particular posture (they retract their heads towards their bodies). Also when larvae were deprived of sleep, by shining a light or shaking, they compensated by sleeping more afterwards. Importantly, depriving the larvae of sleep also led to lower levels of neurogenesis. These findings establish the fruit fly larva as a new and useful system for studying the role of sleep in early development, and may help shed light on the role sleep plays in disorders affecting brain development.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.33220.002Introduction
Nearly all animals exhibit increased sleep throughout development, suggesting a crucial role for sleep in early life (Kayser and Biron, 2016; Roffwarg et al., 1966). Many studies have focused on the role of sleep in synaptic formation, homeostasis, and plasticity (Abel et al., 2013; Frank, 2011; Kayser et al., 2014; Li et al., 2017; Tononi and Cirelli, 2014), underscoring that a core function of sleep during development is likely to involve modification of neural connectivity. However, examination of a function for sleep in earlier phases of brain development has been limited by the lack of a tractable experimental system. Indeed, there is no established platform to investigate the role of sleep in developmental neurogenesis, which in mammals predominately occurs in utero. The adult fruit fly is a widely studied model organism for sleep (Hendricks et al., 2000; Shaw et al., 2000), and while significant brain plasticity is ongoing in young adult flies (Barth et al., 1997), neurogenesis is largely complete; adult flies are therefore not suitable for investigating the function of sleep during initial periods of neural development. In contrast, the timing of these events in Drosophila larvae is ideal. The first wave of neurogenesis occurs during embryonic stages but contributes only 10% of neurons in the adult brain; 90% of neurons in the adult brain are born during the second wave of neurogenesis that is timed to larval stages (Homem and Knoblich, 2012; Kohwi and Doe, 2013). Fly larvae are widely used to study the neural basis of complex behaviors, and are readily accessible for experimental manipulation (Heckscher et al., 2012; Honjo et al., 2012; Luo et al., 2010; Vogelstein et al., 2014). Yet, it has remained unknown whether Drosophila larvae sleep. Here, we identify a sleep state in Drosophila larvae, define the principal cellular controls of larval sleep, and provide evidence that sleep during early development is required for normal proliferation of neural stem cells. Our results suggest that sleep is a critical factor in the initial steps of nervous system elaboration.
Results
Prolonged monitoring of larval rest-activity behaviors
Most prior research on Drosophila larval behavior (Heckscher et al., 2012; Vogelstein et al., 2014; Widmann et al., 2016) employed short-term assays not sufficient for analysis of rest/activity. Building on work in Caenorhabditis elegans (Churgin et al., 2017; Raizen et al., 2008), we constructed a multi-well imaging device, the LarvaLodge, for automated, prolonged monitoring of larval activity and quiescence (Figure 1A–C). We simultaneously imaged up to 20 larvae at 35 µm resolution (a second instar larva is ~1500 µm in length and ~350 µm in width), acquiring images every 6 s under dark field infrared illumination (Video 1). We focused on second instar larvae because this period coincides with the second wave of neurogenesis (Homem and Knoblich, 2012). With this system, rest/activity during the entire second instar phase (~22 hr) was recorded, with tightly aligned developmental staging across larvae as indicated by synchronized onset of the second molt (Figure 1C). We observed that larvae exhibit dynamic transitions between quiescence and activity throughout the imaging period (Figure 1C; Video 2). Our recordings of activity were sensitive to both locomotor and feeding behaviors (Figure 1—figure supplement 1A; Video 3), demonstrating our ability to distinguish behavioral quiescence from eating. Total quiescence was higher early in the second instar period compared to later, resulting from more frequent and longer sleep bouts in immature larvae (Figure 1D–F; Figure 1—figure supplement 1B,C). These results demonstrate our ability to monitor larval activity over extended periods, and reveal behavioral quiescence that is developmentally timed.
Larval behavioral quiescence is a sleep state
All animals appear to sleep, defined behaviorally as a state of quiescence that is rapidly reversible, associated with a postural change, and during which arousal threshold is increased; sleep loss is followed by homeostatic sleep rebound (Allada and Siegel, 2008; Campbell and Tobler, 1984). We tested whether behavioral quiescence in larvae represents a sleep state by examining these behavioral criteria. First, we asked if larval quiescence is associated with a postural change. Using frame-by-frame postural analysis of individual sleep bouts, we observed that during periods of rest, larvae frequently retract their head towards the body, resulting in a quantitative change in body length and width (Figure 2A–E; Video 4). Head retraction occurred in 76.6% of prolonged quiescence bouts (>36 s; Figure 2D) and less commonly (47.1%) in shorter bouts (18 s; Figure 2C), suggesting that postural change is related to duration of the sleep bout. The postural change most often occurred as part of a stereotyped sequence of behaviors: we found in 72.0% of sleep-associated postural changes that the animal transitioned from activity to behavioral quiescence prior to postural change; quiescence then persisted following postural change in 64.6% of episodes, prior to resumption of activity (Video 4). Less often (28.0%) a larva transitioned directly from activity to coincident postural change with quiescence, or directly from head retraction to an active phase (35.4%), consistent with the observation that posture is more likely to change in association with prolonged rest. Our data thus show that Drosophila larval quiescence is a patterned behavior with a previously unrecognized temporal sequence, and that like animals ranging from nematodes (Iwanir et al., 2013) to humans, larvae demonstrate postural changes during sleep.
To determine whether larval rest is a rapidly reversible behavioral state, we randomly presented a high intensity blue light stimulus (irradiance 39.8 µW/mm2) for 4 s to second instar larvae and retrospectively analyzed behavioral data for larvae that were inactive at the time of stimulus presentation. We found that nearly 100% of inactive larvae were aroused (as determined by altered position in next captured frame) by this intense stimulus (Figure 2F; Video 5), indicating that behavioral quiescence is rapidly reversible. Using a low intensity blue light stimulus (irradiance 3.98 µW/mm2) to perturb larvae, we found that only ~60% of inactive larvae became active (Figure 2F), demonstrating an altered responsiveness to sensory stimuli during larval rest. Importantly, only ~20% of quiescent larvae spontaneously became active without a stimulus (Figure 2F), confirming that the low intensity stimulus arouses resting larvae more than chance. We next increased the temporal resolution of our system to monitor behavior every 2 s, with the goal of examining whether the behavioral quiescence described above is qualitatively distinct from brief (2 s) periods of inactivity. Focusing first on probability of spontaneous arousal, we found that following 2 s of inactivity, 54% of larvae became active in the subsequent frame (Figure 2G). In contrast, following 6 s of inactivity, only 19% of larvae became active spontaneously; longer bouts of quiescence were not associated with a further reduction in probability of subsequent spontaneous activity (Figure 2G). These results suggest that ≥6 s of quiescence represents an altered behavioral state in comparison to brief (2 s) periods of inactivity. To test directly whether arousal threshold is altered with ≥6 s of behavioral quiescence, we presented a 1 s high intensity blue light stimulus and assessed arousal in larvae that were inactive at the time of stimulus presentation. While 82.4% of larvae were aroused after 2 s of inactivity, only 46.5% were aroused by the same stimulus if quiescent for ≥6 s prior to light presentation (n = 108 quiescent episodes for 2 s and 357 quiescent episodes for ≥6 s; p<0.0001, Fisher’s exact test). Together, these findings indicate that larval quiescence is a reversible state during which arousal threshold is increased from baseline, and that behavioral quiescence lasting at least 6 s can be defined as sleep.
Does larval quiescence exhibit homeostatic properties of sleep, with rebound following enforced sleep loss? We perturbed sleep using blue light pulses over a 3 hr window, which resulted in 40% reduction in sleep during that time (Figure 3A–C). Over the subsequent 3 hr, sleep deprived larvae exhibited increased quiescence compared to non-deprived controls (Figure 3C), resulting primarily from increased quiescence bout length (Figure 3D,E). Use of a mechanical stimulus to disturb quiescence likewise resulted in subsequent quiescence rebound (Figure 3—figure supplement 1), showing that this rebound is not specific to the modality of quiescence loss. During rebound, sleep-deprived larvae also exhibit deeper sleep, as evidenced by an increased arousal threshold compared to non-deprived larvae (Figure 3—figure supplement 2). Importantly, disrupting quiescence did not cause long-lasting deficits to larval rest/activity or disruption of normal developmental progression (Figure 3—figure supplement 1), demonstrating that increased quiescence immediately following deprivation does not reflect a nonspecific impairment or injury related to deprivation.
In most animals, including adult fruit flies, sleep timing is controlled by a circadian clock (Borbély, 1982). We examined the role of circadian factors in Drosophila larval sleep. Anatomical analysis has shown that clock cells are present in Drosophila larvae (Liu et al., 2015), and light exposure in first instars results in synchronized adult behavioral rhythms, indicating that photic cues for circadian entrainment can be perceived at this stage (Sehgal et al., 1992). However, we found that rearing animals in constant light, which disrupts the circadian clock, did not change features of larval sleep (Figure 4A; Figure 4—figure supplement 1A,B), including the propensity for increased sleep in the early second instar stage. Behavioral analysis of the circadian clock mutants clockjrk and cyc01 likewise revealed no alteration of larval sleep (Figure 4B; Figure 4—figure supplement 1C,D), suggesting that circadian factors do not play a prominent role in developmentally timed sleep in Drosophila larvae.
We sought to use Drosophila larvae to understand mechanisms of sleep control during development, and first examined whether previously characterized adult short-sleeping mutants exhibit sleep deficits as larvae. Surprisingly, mutants for the gene sleepless (Koh et al., 2008), which regulates potassium channel activity, and for the gene fumin (Kume et al., 2005), which encodes a dopamine transporter, both showed normal larval sleep levels during the second instar period (Figure 4C; Figure 4—figure supplement 2), despite exhibiting severe short-sleeping phenotypes in adulthood. We next asked whether sleep as larvae is predictive of sleep in adulthood. Sleep amount was assessed in second instar larvae, which were subsequently collected and housed individually into adulthood, at which time sleep was again measured, revealing that sleep of individual larvae was not correlated with their sleep as adults (Figure 4D). Finally, in contrast to adulthood when female flies sleep less than males (Andretic and Shaw, 2005), we did not observe sexual dimorphisms to larval sleep (Figure 4D). Taken together, these data indicate that genetic controls of developmental and adult sleep are at least partially distinct.
Octopamine modulates sleep-wake in larvae
To gain insight into the cellular control of larval sleep, we next examined whether the neurotransmitter systems dopamine (DA) and octopamine (OA), which are known to be wake-promoting in adult flies (Andretic et al., 2005; Crocker et al., 2010; Kume et al., 2005), function analogously in larvae. Both DA and OA neurons are present in second instar larvae (Figure 5A,B). We experimentally activated larval DA or OA neurons by expressing a bacterial sodium channel (UAS-NaChBac [Nitabach et al., 2006]) under control of TH-GAL4 (DA) or Tdc2-GAL4 (OA), respectively. Excitation of OA neurons in larvae resulted in >50% reduction in sleep time (Figure 5C), due primarily to a reduction in sleep bout length (Figure 5—figure supplement 1). In contrast, excitation of DA neurons had no significant effect on larval sleep (Figure 5C; Figure 5—figure supplement 1), consistent with the absence of a sleep phenotype in fumin larvae (Figure 4C), which harbor a mutation in the dopamine transporter (Kume et al., 2005).
To ask whether the relevant wake-promoting neurotransmitter used by OA neurons is indeed octopamine, we conducted sleep analysis in a series of OA synthesis mutants. Mutation of tyramine β-hydroxylase (Tβh), which converts tyramine to octopamine, resulted in increased sleep (Figure 5D; Figure 5—figure supplement 2A–C); this phenotype could result from reduced octopamine or from increased tyramine, which builds up in the absence of TβH. To distinguish between these possibilities, we examined a tyrosine decarboxylase (Tdc2) mutant that fails to convert tyrosine to tyramine, and therefore has low levels of both tyramine and octopamine. This mutant likewise exhibited increased sleep (Figure 5E; Figure 5—figure supplement 2D–F), indicating that reduced octopamine levels are causative. While both Tβh and Tdc2 mutants show increased sleep compared to controls, only the Tβh mutation resulted in reduced activity, demonstrating that quiescence and activity levels are dissociable (Figure 5—figure supplement 2C,F). Next, to determine whether OA is necessary specifically in the larval nervous system for regulation of arousal, we expressed Tβh RNAi under control of the pan-neuronal driver Elav-GAL4. Knockdown of TβH resulted in increased sleep (Figure 5F; Figure 5—figure supplement 3), indicating that octopamine functions in larval neurons to control arousal. Lastly, the OA receptor subtype OAMB has been implicated in octopamine-dependent wake in adult flies (Crocker et al., 2010). We examined larval sleep in an Oamb null mutant, and found that these larvae exhibit increased sleep (Figure 5G; Figure 5—figure supplement 4). In contrast, mutation of a distinct OA receptor subtype Octβ2R did not affect sleep amount (Figure 5G). Together, these results identify OA as a key arousal-promoting signal during larval development.
Sleep deprivation attenuates proliferation of neural progenitor cells
The ability to manipulate sleep in larvae opens new avenues to study the function of sleep in development. Type I neuroblasts (NBs) make up the majority of stem cells in the larval nervous system; these NBs undergo asymmetric divisions to self-renew and form a ganglion mother cell (GMC), which then produces two differentiated cells (neurons/glia) (Homem and Knoblich, 2012; Kohwi and Doe, 2013). The first wave of neurogenesis in embryos produces only 10% of neurons in the adult brain; onset of the second wave of neurogenesis, during which 90% of neurons in the adult brain are born, largely coincides with the second instar period (Homem and Knoblich, 2012). We tested whether sleep in second instar larvae regulates nervous system stem cell proliferation. Approximately 2 hr following the molt to second instars, larvae were sleep deprived for 3 hr using a mechanical stimulus. Immediately after sleep deprivation, we assessed the number of dividing NBs in the ventral nerve cord using the mitotic marker phospho-histone H3 (PH3) in conjunction with the neural stem cell marker Miranda. Strikingly, we observed a ~15% reduction in the number of dividing NBs following mechanical sleep deprivation in comparison to non-deprived controls (Figure 6A–C). We detected no difference in total NB number (dividing and non-dividing) between deprived (156.1 ± 1.6 NBs) or control groups (155.7 ± 2.3 NBs) (Figure 6—figure supplement 1), confirming that mechanical stimulation selectively impairs dividing cells without affecting the total reservoir of NBs. To determine whether this effect on proliferation is reversible, we examined cell division in the nervous system of larvae that were sleep deprived and then permitted to sleep rebound for the subsequent hour. Following recovery sleep, the number of dividing cells returned to baseline (Figure 6C), demonstrating that suppression of neuronal proliferation with sleep deprivation is not a permanent impairment.
Is sleep loss the key component to reduced rate of cell division? To answer this question, we developed a closed-loop sleep deprivation system to test for a specific effect of sleep loss on neural stem cell proliferation. In this system, an individual larva was tracked in real time and illuminated with a 4 s pulse of blue light whenever a 6 s period of quiescence was detected (Figure 6D). As a control, the system was reversed to deliver the same total amount of light stimulus, but only during periods of activity (Figure 6E). Sleep/activity for all larvae was monitored throughout the experiment. This approach yielded a dramatic reduction in sleep of target larvae compared to controls (Figure 6F), despite all larvae receiving the same total stimulus (53.0 ± 0.99 light pulses, control; 50.6 ± 5.19 light pulses, deprived), demonstrating the ability to use this system to rule out a non-specific stimulus-dependent effect on cell division rate. Consistent with results following mechanical sleep loss, sleep deprivation using this closed-loop light-based system caused a reduction in the number of dividing NBs (Figure 6G). In contrast, larvae receiving the same total stimulus during active periods and without sleep loss showed no change in cell division (Figure 6G). Collectively, our data demonstrate that sleep loss during development is associated with reduced proliferation of neural progenitor cells.
Discussion
A critical function for sleep in the developing brain has been hypothesized for more than 50 years (Roffwarg et al., 1966), supported by evidence from numerous animal models. Most of these studies have focused on early postnatal life (in mammals) or equivalent periods of elevated brain plasticity in other systems (such as young adult fruit flies) (Blumberg et al., 2013; Cirelli and Tononi, 2015; Frank, 2011; Kayser et al., 2014). However, investigation of a role for sleep during even earlier developmental time points has been limited by experimental inaccessibility of mammals to in utero sleep monitoring and deprivation. Here, we develop a platform for long-term monitoring of behavior in Drosophila larvae. We demonstrate that Drosophila larvae exhibit periods of reversible behavioral quiescence characterized by a change in posture and increased arousal threshold, with homeostatic properties such that quiescence deprivation is followed by subsequent increased quiescence amount and depth. These features meet the behavioral criteria for sleep shared across phylogeny, and establish Drosophila larvae as a new system to study questions at the intersection of sleep and development.
Little is known regarding the cellular and molecular control of sleep during early life. Surprisingly, we find that the most severe short sleeping mutations identified in adult flies exhibit no sleep phenotype in larvae, and that sleep time in larvae is not correlated with that of adults. Moreover, direct activation of dopamine neurons in second instar larvae fails to promote wakefulness as it does in adult flies. Together these findings suggest that genetic and neural sleep regulatory mechanisms might be partially distinct at different developmental time points. What purpose might be served by this mechanistic divergence? One possibility is that the drives facing a developing animal (e.g., high levels of feeding to meet metabolic demands related to tissue growth) are different than those in adulthood (e.g., reproductive drive). Sleep must be suppressed to fulfill these demands (Beckwith et al., 2017; Chen et al., 2017; Keene et al., 2010; Machado et al., 2017), and perhaps sleep regulatory mechanisms are dynamically coupled to the most relevant environmental cues for drives that change throughout maturation. Notably, sleep bouts in Drosophila larvae are relatively brief, with an average length of 15–20 s. We identify 6 s as the minimum period of behavioral quiescence considered sleep. Though brief, sleep bouts of this length are similar to that described in other organisms, including adult zebrafish in which inactivity for 6 s is also the minimum period defined as sleep (Yokogawa et al., 2007). Moreover, the fragmented nature of sleep we observe in larvae is consistent with the idea that rapid sleep-wake cycling is a shared feature of early life sleep across phylogeny: human infants exhibit fragmented sleep (Kleitman and Engelmann, 1953), and early postnatal rats show sleep bout durations averaging <15 s that begin to consolidate in the first postnatal weeks (Blumberg et al., 2005a). Fragmented sleep in Drosophila larvae (and perhaps other young animals) might reflect high metabolic needs of a rapidly developing organism, raising the possibility that sleep regulatory mechanisms during development show divergence from adulthood in order to facilitate brief rest bouts interspersed among frequent feeding required to meet the developmental challenge.
Not all sleep-wake systems are distinct between Drosophila larvae and adults, as octopamine is a key arousal-promoting signal in both. Activation of OA neurons in larvae promotes wake while OA synthesis or receptor mutants exhibit more sleep. Given the conserved role of OA/NE in mammalian sleep-wake (Aston-Jones and Bloom, 1981), this neurotransmitter may also be a critical regulator of early developmental sleep in other species including humans. Our results are consistent with the idea that sleep control from early life to maturity is characterized by sequential elaboration of regulatory mechanisms (Blumberg et al., 2005b; Cirelli and Tononi, 2015; Karlsson et al., 2005), perhaps starting with noradrenergic signaling. In the adult fly, specific clusters of OA-releasing neurons mediate arousal/wake (Crocker et al., 2010). Whether such circuit-specific features exist in larval sleep is not known. While OA serves an arousal function, we have yet to identify a sleep-promoting brain region in larvae. Sleep/arousal centers are organized with common neural logic across phylogeny, where activity of one region dampens the other to switch between states (Artiushin and Sehgal, 2017; Saper et al., 2005). Specifically, in the adult fly brain, wake-promoting neurons project to and inhibit activity of a sleep-promoting area (Liu et al., 2012; Ueno et al., 2012). A circuit-level understanding of sleep/wake control during development is necessary for studying sleep ontogeny across species. Given the detailed understanding of sleep circuitry in adult flies (Artiushin and Sehgal, 2017), the relatively simple larval nervous system presents a unique opportunity to examine how sleep-arousal circuitry in early development compares to adulthood.
Most physiological functions, including cell cycle progression and DNA replication, are gated within optimal time windows (Matsuo et al., 2003; Tu et al., 2005). Work in adult rodents suggests that sleep restriction/fragmentation reduces hippocampal neurogenesis (Guzmán-Marín et al., 2003; Lucassen et al., 2010), but it has been difficult to untangle the effect of sleep loss per se from the well-known impairment of neurogenesis with stress of sensory stimuli used to keep animals awake (Lucassen et al., 2010; Warner-Schmidt and Duman, 2006); sleep deprivation paradigms have, for the most part, not controlled for this issue (Lucassen et al., 2010). Moreover, the magnitude of neurogenesis (and therefore consequences of its disruption) is dramatically higher in early development compared to adulthood. The timing of a major wave of neurogenesis to larval life, a period accessible for experimental manipulation, makes Drosophila larvae ideal for examining sleep function and control during early development. Using a closed-loop system in which control animals received the same degree of sensory stimulation as sleep-deprived larva, we uncovered a previously unknown role for sleep in developmental neurogenesis, with broad implications for both brain patterning and the ramifications of sleep loss in early life.
A number of areas for investigation emerge from this work. First, while sleep quantity is greater in early second instar larva compared to later (Figure 1D), the rate of neuronal proliferation does not decrease over this same period (Ito and Hotta, 1992). Together, these findings suggest sleep might act as a permissive, but not instructive, cue for neurogenesis. Alternatively, sleep could play an important role in initiating the wave of neurogenesis, though not be essential to sustain this process. Second, we provide evidence that sleep can influence NB proliferation, but have not established whether this relationship is bidirectional. While critical to control for more general effects on larval health and function, it will be interesting to dissect how accelerating/slowing cell division affects sleep. Finally, it remains unknown whether the effect of sleep loss on neuronal proliferation is general, or if specific NB subtypes are particularly sensitive to sleep deprivation, which could lead to predictable deficits following sleep loss. Our work describes an ~15% reduction in rate of cell division with 2–3 hr of sleep loss. We aim to develop approaches that achieve long-lasting sleep deprivation, with the goal of understanding how impaired neurogenesis during early development affects brain and behavioral development. More broadly, future work will leverage the tractable larval system to gain novel insights into the genetic/molecular regulation of sleep during development and examine how sleep and neurogenesis are mechanistically coupled.
Materials and methods
Drosophila stocks
Request a detailed protocolThe following lines have been maintained as lab stocks or were obtained from Dr. Amita Sehgal: iso31, clockjrk (Allada et al., 1998), cyc01 (Rutila et al., 1998), sssP1 (Koh et al., 2008), fmn (Kume et al., 2005), UAS-NachBac (Nitabach et al., 2006), TH-Gal4 (Friggi-Grelin et al., 2003), Oamb286 (Lee et al., 2003), Tdc2R054 (Cole et al., 2005), TbhnM18 (Monastirioti et al., 1996) and CS control. Tdc2-Gal4, Elav-Gal4, UAS-CD8::GFP, and Octβ2Rf05679 are from the Bloomington Stock Center. Tbh-RNAi was obtained from the Vienna Drosophila Resource Center (VDRC#: 107070).
Larval rearing and sleep assays
Request a detailed protocolAdult flies were maintained on standard molasses-based diet at 25°C on a 12:12 light:dark (LD) cycle. In order to collect synchronized second instar larvae, adult flies were placed in an embryo collection cage (Genesee Scientific, cat#: 59–100) and eggs were laid on a petri dish containing 3% agar, 2% sucrose, and 2.5% apple juice with yeast paste on top. Animals developed on this media for two days. On the day of the sleep assay, molting first instar larvae were collected and moved to a separate dish with yeast to complete the molt to second instar. Freshly molted second instars were carefully placed into individual wells of the LarvaLodge containing 100 µl of 3% agar and 2% sucrose media covered with a thin layer of yeast paste. The LarvaLodge was covered with a transparent acrylic sheet and placed into a DigiTherm (Tritech Research) incubator at 25°C for imaging. Unless otherwise indicated, experiments were performed in constant dark.
LarvaLodge design, fabrication, and preparation
Request a detailed protocolLarvaLodge design was adapted from the WorMotel, a custom microplate consisting of individual wells developed for monitoring of C. elegans (Churgin et al., 2017). We designed a chip containing a rectangular array of rounded wells with 11 mm diameter and 3 mm depth. Designs of the LarvaLodge masters were created using MATLAB. We printed a master corresponding to the negative of this shape with an Objet30 photopolymer 3D printer using the material VeroBlack. To mold the devices, we mixed Dow Corning Sylgard 184 PDMS according to the manufacturer’s instructions and poured 20 g into the master. Devices were cured overnight at room temperature and then removed from molds using a spatula.
Image acquisition
Request a detailed protocolImages were captured every 6 s with an Imaging Source DMK 23GP031 camera (2592 × 1944 pixels, The Imaging Source, USA) equipped with a Fujinon lens (HF12.5SA-1, 1:1.4/12.5 mm, Fujifilm Corp., Japan) with a Hoya 49mm R72 Infrared Filter. We used IC Capture (The Imaging Source) to acquire time-lapse images through a gigabit Ethernet connection. All experiments were carried out under dark-field illumination using infrared LED strips (Ledlightsworld LTD, 850 nm wavelength) positioned below the LarvaLodge.
Image processing
Request a detailed protocolImages were analyzed using custom-written MATLAB software. Temporally adjacent images were subtracted to generate maps of pixel value intensity change (Churgin et al., 2017; Raizen et al., 2008). A binary threshold was applied such that individual pixel intensity changes that fell below 50 gray-scale units within each region of interest (individual lodge) were set equal to zero (‘not changed’) to eliminate noise, whereas pixel changes greater than or equal to 50 gray-scale units were set equal to one (‘changed’). We then calculated the activity, defined as the sum of all pixels changed between images (i.e., the total number of pixels where a change in intensity occurred above the noise threshold). Quiescence was defined as an activity value of zero between frames. Total quiescence (sleep) was summed in hourly bins (Figures 1 and 4A,B) or over 6 hr beginning 2 hr after the molt to second instar (Figures 4C and 5). Timing of sleep deprivation experiments (Figures 3 and 6) is provided below.
Feeding behavior analysis
Request a detailed protocolFreshly molted wild type second instar larvae were starved on 3% agar plates for 2 hr and placed into LarvaLodges with agar/sucrose covered by yeast paste containing red food coloring. Activity was captured for 5 min (6 s per frame). Larvae were removed and inspected for red color accumulated in their guts to verify feeding during the monitoring period.
Postural change analysis
Request a detailed protocol107 long sleep bouts (36 s or more) and 104 shorter sleep bouts (18 s) from six animals were selected for further analysis. Each sleep bout was manually analyzed, including three frames (18 s) before and after the period of inactivity. We noticed that postural changes most commonly bridged two otherwise continuous period of inactivity, and less often occurred at the initiation or termination of quiescence. Head retraction and body proportion measurement (NIH ImageJ) were made on all long sleep bouts containing a postural change (n = 82), measuring the animals from the tip of the head to the end of the tail, and across the body half way along the anterior-posterior axis. Measurements were made before and after the postural change.
Arousal and sleep deprivation with blue light stimulation
Request a detailed protocolTo supply the blue light illumination, we used two high power LEDs (Luminus Phatlight PT-121, 460 nm peak wavelength, Sunnyvale, CA) secured to an aluminum heat sink. We used a relay (Schneider Electric, France) controlled by MATLAB through a LabJack (LabJack Corp., Lakewood, CO) to drive the LEDs at a current of either 0.1 A (low intensity, 3.98 µW/mm2) or 1 A (high intensity, 39.8 µW/mm2) for 4 s through a power supply. We measured blue light irradiance using a power meter (LaserMate-Q, Coherent, Santa Clara, CA). To maximize the blue light uniformity delivered to the microplate, we constructed a mirror box consisting of four mirrored-acrylic panels with mirrored sides facing inwards (Churgin et al., 2017) and placed it around the LarvaLodge. For light-based sleep deprivation, we used a high intensity stimulus for 30 s every 2 min for 3 hr beginning 2 hr after the molt to second instar. Undisturbed control animals were placed in a separate incubator during deprivation, and both lodges were moved to the same incubator during the rebound.
Mechanical sleep deprivation
Request a detailed protocolFor mechanical sleep deprivation, LarvaLodges were placed on a Trikinetics vortexer mounting plate, with shaking for 2 s randomly within every 10 s window for 3 hr beginning 2 hr after the molt to second instar. Non-deprived larvae were housed in a separate incubator during this period. After 3 hr, half of the larvae were removed from the lodges and their brains were dissected while the other half remained in the LarvaLodges and placed into the same incubators to allow for rebound. Remaining animals were then dissected after 1 hr and processed for immunostaining.
Closed-loop sleep deprivation system
Request a detailed protocolFor closed-loop sleep deprivation with blue light stimulation, the activity of individual second instar larvae was tracked in real time over 2 hr beginning 2 hr after the molt to second instar. Individual larvae were stimulated with a high intensity (39.8 µW/mm2) light pulse for 4 s whenever a > 6 s period of quiescence was detected. For controls, individual animals in a separate experiment were tracked in real time and stimulated with the same light pulse for 4 s whenever a > 18 s period of continuous activity was detected. After stimulation, a refractory period of two minutes was enforced during which the control larvae would not be stimulated again. This refractory period enabled us to ensure that control (stimulated only during periods of activity) and experimental (stimulated only during periods of quiescence) animals experienced approximately the same total number of blue light stimulation pulses. Individual larva were immediately dissected at termination of the experiment and processed for immunostaining.
Immunohistochemistry and imaging
Request a detailed protocolBrains were dissected in PBS, fixed in 4% PFA for 20 min at room temperature. Following 3 × 10 min washes in PBST, brains were incubated with primary antibody at 4° overnight. Following 3 × 10 min washes in PBST, brains were incubated with secondary antibody for 2 hr at room temperature. Following 3 × 10 min washes in PBST, brains and ventral nerve cords were mounted in 90% Glycerol. Primary antibodies included: Rat anti-Miranda (1:50, ab197788, Abcam), Rabbit anti-Ph3 (1:1000, PA5-17869, Invitrogen), and Rabbit anti-GFP (1:1000, A-11122, ThermoFisher Scientific). Secondary antibodies included: FITC donkey anti-rat (1:200, Jackson), Cy5 donkey anti-rabbit (1:200, Jackson). Brains were visualized and imaged with a TCS SP5 or SP8 confocal microscope. An observer who was blinded to the experimental condition manually counted total NBs (Miranda positive) and dividing NBs (Miranda and PH3 positive) in the ventral nerve cord from 1.5 µm step confocal stacks using NIH ImageJ.
Statistical analysis and data reproducibility
Request a detailed protocolAnalysis was done using Prism (GraphPad Software). ANOVA with Tukey’s test was used in Figure 1; Figure 2F; Figure 3C–E; Figure 4C; Figure 5C,F,G; Figure 6C; Figure 3—figure supplement 1; Figure 4—figure supplement 2; Figure 5—figure supplements 1 and 3. Student’s t-test was used in Figure 2E; Figure 5D,E; Figure 6F,G; Figure 3—figure supplement 2; Figure 5—figure supplements 2 and 4; Figure 6—figure supplement 1. Fisher’s exact test was used in Figure 2G. For significance: *p≤0.05; **p<0.01; ***p<0.001. Each experiment was generated from a minimum of 3 independent biological replicates. Samples were allocated based on genotype or experimental manipulation and statistics performed on aggregated data. Outliers were never excluded.
References
-
Sleep, plasticity and memory from molecules to whole-brain networksCurrent Biology 23:R774–R788.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.07.025
-
Unearthing the phylogenetic roots of sleepCurrent Biology 18:R670–R679.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2008.06.033
-
Essentials of sleep recordings in Drosophila: moving beyond sleep timeMethods in Enzymology 393:759–772.https://doi.org/10.1016/S0076-6879(05)93040-1
-
Dopaminergic modulation of arousal in DrosophilaCurrent Biology 15:1165–1175.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2005.05.025
-
The Drosophila circuitry of sleep-wake regulationCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology 44:243–250.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2017.03.004
-
Activity of norepinephrine-containing locus coeruleus neurons in behaving rats anticipates fluctuations in the sleep-waking cycleJournal of Neuroscience 1:876–886.
-
Experience-dependent developmental plasticity in the optic lobe of Drosophila melanogasterJournal of Neuroscience 17:1493–1504.
-
The ontogeny of mammalian sleep: a response to Frank and Heller (2003)Journal of Sleep Research 14:91–98.https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2869.2004.00430_1.x
-
Twitching in sensorimotor development from sleeping rats to robotsCurrent Biology 23:R532–R537.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.04.075
-
A two process model of sleep regulationHuman Neurobiology 1:195–204.https://doi.org/10.1111/jsr.12371
-
Animal sleep: a review of sleep duration across phylogenyNeuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 8:269–300.https://doi.org/10.1016/0149-7634(84)90054-X
-
Cortical development, electroencephalogram rhythms, and the sleep/wake cycleBiological Psychiatry 77:1071–1078.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.12.017
-
Two functional but noncomplementing Drosophila tyrosine decarboxylase genes: distinct roles for neural tyramine and octopamine in female fertilityThe Journal of Biological Chemistry 280:14948–14955.https://doi.org/10.1074/jbc.M414197200
-
Sleep and developmental plasticity not just for kidsProgress in Brain Research 193:221–232.https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-444-53839-0.00014-4
-
Sleep deprivation reduces proliferation of cells in the dentate gyrus of the hippocampus in ratsThe Journal of Physiology 549:563–571.https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.2003.041665
-
Drosophila neuroblasts: a model for stem cell biologyDevelopment 139:4297–4310.https://doi.org/10.1242/dev.080515
-
Clock and cycle limit starvation-induced sleep loss in DrosophilaCurrent Biology 20:1209–1215.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2010.05.029
-
Sleep characteristics of infantsJournal of Applied Physiology 6:269–282.https://doi.org/10.1152/jappl.1953.6.5.269
-
Temporal fate specification and neural progenitor competence during developmentNature Reviews Neuroscience 14:823–838.https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3618
-
Dopamine is a regulator of arousal in the fruit flyJournal of Neuroscience 25:7377–7384.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2048-05.2005
-
Octopamine receptor OAMB is required for ovulation in Drosophila melanogasterDevelopmental Biology 264:179–190.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ydbio.2003.07.018
-
REM sleep selectively prunes and maintains new synapses in development and learningNature Neuroscience 20:427–437.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4479
-
Navigational decision making in Drosophila thermotaxisJournal of Neuroscience 30:4261–4272.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4090-09.2010
-
Characterization of Drosophila tyramine beta-hydroxylase gene and isolation of mutant flies lacking octopamineJournal of Neuroscience 16:3900-11.
-
Identification of a dopamine pathway that regulates sleep and arousal in DrosophilaNature Neuroscience 15:1516–1523.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3238
Article and author information
Author details
Funding
National Institutes of Health (K08NS090461)
- Matthew S Kayser
Burroughs Wellcome Fund
- Matthew S Kayser
Alfred P. Sloan Foundation
- Matthew S Kayser
March of Dimes Foundation
- Matthew S Kayser
National Institutes of Health (R01NS088432)
- David M Raizen
National Institutes of Health (R01NS084835)
- Christopher Fang-Yen
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Acknowledgements
We thank members of the Kayser Lab and Dr. Amita Sehgal for helpful discussions. This work was supported by NIH grants K08NS090461 (MSK), R01NS088432 (DMR and CF-Y), and R01NS084835 (CF-Y), and a Burroughs Wellcome Career Award for Medical Scientists, March of Dimes Basil O’Connor Scholar Award, and Sloan Research Fellowship (MSK). MS designed and performed experiments, analyzed data, and contributed to writing the manuscript. MAC developed the LarvaLodge, designed experiments, wrote closed-loop deprivation and analysis software, and provided technical assistance. AJB designed and performed experiments, and analyzed data. DMR designed experiments and interpreted data. CF-Y developed the LarvaLodge, provided technical assistance, and interpreted data. MSK conceived and analyzed experiments, interpreted the data, and wrote the manuscript with input from other authors.
Copyright
© 2018, Szuperak et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 6,257
- views
-
- 751
- downloads
-
- 39
- citations
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Neuroscience
Granule cells of the cerebellum make up to 175,000 excitatory synapses on a single Purkinje cell, encoding the wide variety of information from the mossy fibre inputs into the cerebellar cortex. The granule cell axon is made of an ascending portion and a long parallel fibre extending at right angles, an architecture suggesting that synapses formed by the two segments of the axon could encode different information. There are controversial indications that ascending axon (AA) and parallel fibre (PF) synapse properties and modalities of plasticity are different. We tested the hypothesis that AA and PF synapses encode different information, and that the association of these distinct inputs to Purkinje cells might be relevant to the circuit and trigger plasticity, similar to the coincident activation of PF and climbing fibre inputs. Here, by recording synaptic currents in Purkinje cells from either proximal or distal granule cells (mostly AA and PF synapses, respectively), we describe a new form of associative plasticity between these two distinct granule cell inputs. We show for the first time that synchronous AA and PF repetitive train stimulation, with inhibition intact, triggers long-term potentiation (LTP) at AA synapses specifically. Furthermore, the timing of the presentation of the two inputs controls the outcome of plasticity and induction requires NMDAR and mGluR1 activation. The long length of the PFs allows us to preferentially activate the two inputs independently, and despite a lack of morphological reconstruction of the connections, these observations reinforce the suggestion that AA and PF synapses have different coding capabilities and plasticity that is associative, enabling effective association of information transmitted via granule cells.
-
- Neuroscience
Time estimation is an essential prerequisite underlying various cognitive functions. Previous studies identified ‘sequential firing’ and ‘activity ramps’ as the primary neuron activity patterns in the medial frontal cortex (mPFC) that could convey information regarding time. However, the relationship between these patterns and the timing behavior has not been fully understood. In this study, we utilized in vivo calcium imaging of mPFC in rats performing a timing task. We observed cells that showed selective activation at trial start, end, or during the timing interval. By aligning long-term time-lapse datasets, we discovered that sequential patterns of time coding were stable over weeks, while cells coding for trial start or end showed constant dynamism. Furthermore, with a novel behavior design that allowed the animal to determine individual trial interval, we were able to demonstrate that real-time adjustment in the sequence procession speed closely tracked the trial-to-trial interval variations. And errors in the rats’ timing behavior can be primarily attributed to the premature ending of the time sequence. Together, our data suggest that sequential activity maybe a stable neural substrate that represents time under physiological conditions. Furthermore, our results imply the existence of a unique cell type in the mPFC that participates in the time-related sequences. Future characterization of this cell type could provide important insights in the neural mechanism of timing and related cognitive functions.