Response to short-term deprivation of the human adult visual cortex measured with 7T BOLD
Abstract
Sensory deprivation during the post-natal ‘critical period’ leads to structural reorganization of the developing visual cortex. In adulthood, the visual cortex retains some flexibility and adapts to sensory deprivation. Here we show that short-term (2 hr) monocular deprivation in adult humans boosts the BOLD response to the deprived eye, changing ocular dominance of V1 vertices, consistent with homeostatic plasticity. The boost is strongest in V1, present in V2, V3 and V4 but absent in V3a and hMT+. Assessment of spatial frequency tuning in V1 by a population Receptive-Field technique shows that deprivation primarily boosts high spatial frequencies, consistent with a primary involvement of the parvocellular pathway. Crucially, the V1 deprivation effect correlates across participants with the perceptual increase of the deprived eye dominance assessed with binocular rivalry, suggesting a common origin. Our results demonstrate that visual cortex, particularly the ventral pathway, retains a high potential for homeostatic plasticity in the human adult.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.40014.001eLife digest
The world around us changes all the time, and the brain must adapt to these changes. This process, known as neuroplasticity, peaks during development. Abnormal sensory input early in life can therefore cause lasting changes to the structure of the brain. One example of this is amblyopia or ‘lazy eye’. Infants who receive insufficient input to one eye – for example, because of cataracts – can lose their sight in that eye, even if the cataracts are later removed. This is because the brain reorganizes itself to ignore messages from the affected eye.
Does the adult visual system also show neuroplasticity? To explore this question, Binda, Kurzawski et al. asked healthy adult volunteers to lie inside a high-resolution brain scanner with a patch covering one eye. At the start of the experiment, roughly half of the brain’s primary visual cortex responded to sensory input from each eye. But when the volunteers removed the patch two hours later, this was no longer the case.
Some areas of the visual cortex that had previously responded to stimuli presented to the non-patched eye now responded to stimuli presented to the patched eye instead. The patched eye had also become more sensitive to visual stimuli. Indeed, these changes in visual sensitivity correlated with changes in brain activity in a pathway called the ventral visual stream. This pathway processes the fine details of images. Groups of neurons within this pathway that responded to stimuli presented to the patched eye were more sensitive to fine details after patching than before.
Visual regions of the adult brain thus retain a high degree of neuroplasticity. They adapt rapidly to changes in the environment, in this case by increasing their activity to compensate for a lack of input. Notably, these changes are in the opposite direction to those that occur as a result of visual deprivation during development. This has important implications because lazy eye syndrome is currently considered untreatable in adulthood.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.40014.002Introduction
To interact efficiently with the world, our brain needs to fine-tune its structure and function, adapting to a continuously changing external environment. This key property of the brain, called neuroplasticity, is most pronounced early in life, within the so called critical period, when abnormal experience can produce structural changes at the level of the primary sensory cortex (Berardi et al., 2000; Hubel and Wiesel, 1970; Hubel et al., 1977; Wiesel and Hubel, 1963). During development, occluding one eye for a few days induces a dramatic and permanent reorganization of ocular dominance columns (the V1 territory representing each eye) in favor of the open eye (Berardi et al., 2000; Gordon and Stryker, 1996; Hubel and Wiesel, 1970; Hubel et al., 1977; Wiesel and Hubel, 1963), while the deprived eye becomes functionally blind or very weak. These forms of structural plasticity have been documented in animal models, including non-human primates (Gordon and Stryker, 1996; Kiorpes et al., 1998; Levi and Carkeet, 1993; Wiesel and Hubel, 1963). A corresponding perceptual phenomenon known as amblyopia is observed in humans, and may result from exposing infants to monocular deprivation during the critical period, for example due to cataracts (Braddick and Atkinson, 2011; Maurer et al., 2007). In infants, even a partial deprivation produced by optical defects like astigmatism and myopia leads to a permanent acuity loss that cannot be compensated in adulthood, even after correction the optical aberrations (Freeman and Thibos, 1975) through Adaptive Optics (Rossi et al., 2007). Hebbian plasticity, endorsed by Long-Term synaptic Potentiation and Depression (LTP/LTD) of early stage of cortical processing, underlies these changes in animal models and probably also in humans.
After the closure of the critical period, structural changes of V1 resulting from Hebbian plasticity are not typically observed (Mitchell and Sengpiel, 2009; Sato and Stryker, 2008). However, there is evidence that Hebbian plasticity can be restored in adult animal models under special conditions, associated with manipulation of the excitability of the visual cortex (Fong et al., 2016; Frégnac et al., 1988; He et al., 2006; Maya Vetencourt et al., 2008).
Besides Hebbian plasticity, other mechanisms can reshape primary visual cortex processing both within and outside the critical period. At the cellular level, there is evidence for homeostatic plasticity, which increases the gain of cortical responses following sensory deprivation; for example, after a brief monocular deprivation, the response gain of the deprived eye increases (Maffei et al., 2004). This is interpreted as an homeostatic response to preserve cortical excitability in spite of the synaptic depression produced by Hebbian plasticity, suggesting a close link between these two types of plasticity (Maffei and Turrigiano, 2008; Turrigiano, 2012) (Mrsic-Flogel et al., 2007; Turrigiano and Nelson, 2004).
In adult animal models and humans, there is clear evidence for both functional plasticity and for stability of the early sensory cortex (Baseler et al., 2002; Baseler et al., 2011; Wandell and Smirnakis, 2009). Functional changes have been observed with perceptual learning (Dosher and Lu, 2017; Fahle and Poggio, 2002; Fiorentini and Berardi, 1980; Karni and Sagi, 1991; Karni and Sagi, 1993; Watanabe and Sasaki, 2015), adaptation that, in some cases, may be very long-lasting, (McCollough, 1965), and short-term visual deprivation (Binda and Lunghi, 2017; Kwon et al., 2009; Lunghi et al., 2015a; Lunghi et al., 2011; Lunghi et al., 2013; Mon-Williams et al., 1998; Zhang et al., 2009; Zhou et al., 2013; Zhou et al., 2014). The effect of short-term deprivation in adults is paradoxical, boosting the perception of the deprived stimulus – opposite to the long-term deprivation effects during development. One of the first examples of short-term deprivation in adults is by Mon-Williams et al., 1998, who found that thirty minutes of simulated myopia (optical blur achieved by wearing a +1D lens) was followed by a transient improvement of visual acuity – opposite to the long-lasting acuity deficit produced by early onset myopia (Rossi et al., 2007). Contrast attenuation for 4 hr leads to improved contrast discrimination thresholds and enhanced BOLD response in V1/V2 (Kwon et al., 2009). A few hours deprivation of one cardinal orientation leads to enhanced sensitivity to the deprived orientation (Zhang et al., 2009) – opposite to the reduced sensitivity to orientations deprived during development, for example due to astigmatism. Similarly, two hours of monocular contrast deprivation is followed by a transient boost of the deprived eye (Binda and Lunghi, 2017; Lunghi et al., 2015a; Lunghi et al., 2011; Lunghi et al., 2013; Lunghi et al., 2015b; Zhou et al., 2013; Zhou et al., 2014) and an enlargement of the deprived-eye representation at the level of V1 in non-human primates (Begum and Tso, 2016; Tso et al., 2017) – opposite to the amblyopia induced by monocular deprivation during the critical period. The mechanism supporting the perceptual boost of the deprived information could be either a form of homeostatic plasticity (like that observed in animal models), and/or a release of contrast adaptation for the deprived stimulus (Blakemore and Campbell, 1969; Boynton et al., 1999; Gardner et al., 2005; Maffei et al., 1973; Movshon and Lennie, 1979). Irrespective of the interpretation, the data clearly indicate that effects can be long-lasting or even permanent. For example, in patients with keratoconus (adult-onset corneal dystrophia, often monocular), best corrected visual acuity is worse than in emmetropic eyes, but it is better than predicted by the corneal dystrophy (Sabesan and Yoon, 2009; Sabesan and Yoon, 2010): when corneal aberrations of the keratoconic (KC) eyes are simulated in the emmetropic eyes, visual acuity is worse than in the KC eyes, demonstrating a permanent perceptual boost of the deprived information. Moreover, in adult amblyopes (Lunghi et al., 2018), short-term monocular deprivation (of the amblyopic eye) may lead to permanent partial recovery of acuity (of the amblyopic eye). This observation resonates with the idea – introduced in the context of work at the cellular level – that homeostatic plasticity and Hebbian plasticity may be fundamentally linked (Maffei and Turrigiano, 2008) and may open important new pathways for the therapy of amblyopia and, in general, for the rehabilitation of early-onset visual dysfunctions (Legge and Chung, 2016).
This possibility highlights the importance of understanding the neural substrates of short-term deprivation in adult humans. So far, monocular deprivation effects have been indirectly studied with MR spectroscopy (showing a GABA concentration change in the occipital cortex, Lunghi et al., 2015b) and Visual Evoked Potentials (showing a modulation of the early visual response components, Lunghi et al., 2015a). Indirect evidence also indicates that deprivation effects are not generalized but preferentially involve the parvocellular pathway – given that effects are more prominent and longer-lasting for chromatic equiluminant stimuli in humans (Lunghi et al., 2013), and strongest in macaques when deprivation mainly affects the parvocellular activity (Begum and Tso, 2016). Here we directly measure the changes in early visual cortical areas using 7T fMRI in adult humans, before and after two hours of monocular deprivation. Assessing the BOLD change and its selectivity to spatial frequency with a newly developed approach (conceptually similar to the population Receptive Field method, Dumoulin and Wandell, 2008), we demonstrate a change of ocular drive of BOLD signals in primary visual cortex, selective for the higher spatial frequencies and strongest along the ventral pathway, consistent with a stronger plasticity potential of the parvocellular pathway in adulthood.
Results
Monocular deprivation boosts V1 responses to the deprived eye and shifts BOLD ocular dominance
To investigate the visual modulation of BOLD signal by short term deprivation, we performed ultra-high field (UHF, 7T) fMRI during the presentation of high contrast dynamic visual stimuli, delivered separately to the two eyes, before and after 2 hr of monocular contrast deprivation (see schematic diagram in Figure 1A).
The reliability and high signal-to-noise ratio of our system allow us to obtain significant activations with only two blocks of stimulation (Figure 1C shows the profile of V1 BOLD response), thereby targeting the first 10 min after deprivation, when the perceptual effects are strongest (Lunghi et al., 2011; Lunghi et al., 2013). As shown in Figure 1B, the stimulation was sufficient to reliably activate most early visual areas (dashed lines outline ROIs limited by stimulus eccentricity, as detailed in the Materials and method).
We measured the plasticity effect by comparing activity before/after deprivation in response to stimulation in the two eyes with low- and high-spatial frequency bandpass stimuli that differentially stimulate the magno- and parvocellular pathways (see Figure 1—figure supplement 1 panels C-D for maps of responses to stimuli in both eyes, before and after deprivation). Consistent with prior evidence suggesting higher susceptibility to plasticity of the parvocellular pathway (Lunghi et al., 2015a; Lunghi et al., 2011; Lunghi et al., 2015b; Lunghi and Sale, 2015), we observe a strong effect of Monocular Deprivation on BOLD responses to stimuli of high spatial frequency (peak 2.7 cycles per degree, high-frequency cut-off at half-height 7.5 cpd). Figure 1D shows that the V1 response to the high spatial frequency stimuli presented in the left and right eye is nearly equal before deprivation (‘PRE’) (see Figure 1—figure supplement 1, panels C-D and Figure 1—figure supplement 2, panel A, mapping the difference between responses to the two eyes). However, after deprivation (‘POST’), the response in the two eyes changes in opposite directions, with a boost of the BOLD response (measured as GLM Beta values, expressed in units of % signal change) of the deprived eye and a suppression of the non-deprived eye (see also Figure 1—figure supplement 2, panel B). This was formally tested with a two-way repeated measure ANOVA, entered with the mean BOLD responses across all vertices in the left and right V1 region, for the four conditions and each participant (Figure 1D show averages of this values across participants). The result reveals a significant interaction between the factors time (PRE, POST deprivation) and eye (deprived, non-deprived; interaction term F(1,18) = 13.80703, p = 0.00158; the result survives a split-half reliability test: see Figure 1—figure supplement 3).
Fig. 1E confirms these findings with an analysis of the aggregate subject data, obtained by pooling all V1 vertices across all subjects. For each vertex, we defined an index of Ocular Dominance computed as the difference of BOLD response to the deprived and non-deprived eye. This index is not to be confused with the anatomical arrangement of vertices with different eye preference that define the ocular dominance columns (Cheng et al., 2001; Yacoub et al., 2007), that cannot be directly imaged with voxel size of 1.5 mm. However, at this low resolution, each voxel is expected to average signals from a biased sample of ocular dominance columns leading to an eye preference of that particular voxel (the Ocular Dominance index in Figure 1E).
Before deprivation, the Ocular Dominance index is symmetrically distributed around zero, indicating a balanced representation of the two eyes before deprivation (yellow distribution in Figure 1E). After deprivation (black distribution in Figure 1E), the Ocular Dominance distribution shifts to the right of 0, indicating a preference for the deprived eye (non-parametric Wilcoxon sign-rank test comparing the PRE and POST Ocular Dominance medians, z = 115.39, p < 0.001).
In principle, the boost of responses to the deprived eye seen in Figure 1D could be produced by enhancing the response of vertices that originally preferred the deprived eye (without shifting ocular dominance) or by changing Ocular Dominance of vertices that originally preferred the non-deprived eye, driving them to prefer the deprived eye. The shift of the Ocular Dominance histogram in Figure 1E is more compatible with the latter case, implying a recruitment of cortical resources for the representation of the deprived eye. To investigate this further, we monitored the final POST-deprivation Ocular Dominance of individual vertices that, PRE-deprivation, preferred the deprived eye (yellow half distribution in Figure 2B). The majority of vertices continue to prefer the same eye before and after deprivation. The median Ocular Dominance is significantly larger than 0 both PRE and POST (Wilcoxon sign-rank test, z > 101.54, p < 0.0001 in both cases) and the correlation between Ocular Dominance indices before and after deprivation is strong and positive (Pearson’s R(32236) = 0.22 [0.21–0.23], p < 0.0001). Note that a completely random reassignment of Ocular Dominance after deprivation would have produced a histogram centered at 0 and no correlation between Ocular Dominance indices PRE- and POST deprivation. This is not consistent with the results of Figure 2B, which thereby provide evidence that our estimates of Ocular Dominance before and after deprivation are congruent, even though they were collected in different fMRI sessions separated by 2 hr. In addition, the distribution of Ocular Dominance after deprivation is well predicted by adding only a small amount of noise to the original half distribution (Gaussian noise with 0.12 standard deviation, black line), suggesting that these vertices were largely unaffected by monocular deprivation. This is also supported by the repeated measure ANOVA of individual subject data (Figure 2A), revealing a strong main effect of eye (F(1,18) = 48.28901, p < 10−5): the response to the deprived eye is stronger than the non-deprived eye, both before deprivation (due the selection, t(18) = −8.616, p < 10−5), and after deprivation (t(18) = −4.281, p < 10−5), with no effect of time and no time × eye interaction (all F(1,18) = 0.20429, p > 0.5).
A completely different pattern is observed for the vertices originally preferring the non-deprived (yellow half-distribution in Figure 2D). Here the distribution of Ocular Dominance clearly shifts after deprivation; the median moves from significantly negative before deprivation (Wilcoxon sign-rank test, z = −175.97, p < 0.0001) to significantly positive after deprivation (Wilcoxon sign-rank test, z = 64.46, p < 0.0001), implying a shift of dominance in favor of the deprived eye. Again, this is not consistent with a random reassignment of Ocular Dominance after deprivation, which predicts a distribution centered at 0. Contrary to Figure 2B, the POST- Ocular Dominance distribution cannot be predicted by injecting Gaussian noise to the PRE- Ocular Dominance distribution (black line, 0.12 standard deviation like for Figure 2B): for these vertices, there is a shift of Ocular Dominance with short term monocular deprivation. This is confirmed with the repeated measure ANOVA (Figure 2C), where the time × eye interaction is significant (F(1,18) = 44.82812, p < 10−5), implying a different modulation PRE and POST deprivation. In addition and crucially, POST-deprivation BOLD responses to the deprived eye are significantly larger than POST-deprivation responses to the non-deprived eye (t(18) = −2.775 p = 0.012; whereas, by selection, the opposite is true before deprivation: t(18) = 12.034, p < 10−5).
In summary, Ocular Dominance before deprivation defines two similarly sized sub-regions of V1 vertices (44.58 ± 5.38% and 55.42 ± 5.38% of analyzed V1 vertices; 44.84 ± 5.12% and 55.16 ± 5.12% of all V1 vertices) with radically different behaviors that are not consistent with an artifact induced by vertex selection. The sub-region that originally represents the deprived eye does not change with deprivation; the sub-region that originally represents the non-deprived eye is rearranged with deprivation, as a large portion of vertices turn to prefer the deprived eye.
If plasticity were not eye-specific and/or we failed to match our V1 vertices before/after deprivation, we would expect that splitting the distribution of V1 ocular dominance generates opposite effects in the two subpopulations: vertices preferring the deprived eye before deprivation should swap to prefer the other eye, mirroring the effect seen in the vertices preferring non-deprived eye. This is not seen, implying that we did successfully match vertices across the 2 hr of deprivation and that the selective Ocular Dominance shift, observed for about half of our vertices, is not an artifact.
We also measured the perceptual effects of short-term monocular deprivation effects using Binocular Rivalry, just before the PRE- and POST-deprivation fMRI sessions. In line with previous studies (Binda and Lunghi, 2017; Lunghi et al., 2015a; Lunghi et al., 2011; Lunghi et al., 2015b; Lunghi and Sale, 2015), short-term monocular contrast deprivation induced a 30% increase of phase duration for the deprived eye (POST to PRE-deprivation ratio: 1.31 ± 0.30) and a 15% decrease of phase duration for the non-deprived eye (ratio: 0.86 ± 0.30), producing a significant time × eye interaction (Figure 3A, repeated measure ANOVA on the mean phase durations for each participant, interaction: F(1,18) = 23.56957, p = 0.00013). This effect size is similar to that measured in recent experiments using the same paradigm, but letting subjects continue normal activity during the 2 hr of monocular deprivation (Lunghi et al., 2011; Lunghi et al., 2015b; Lunghi and Sale, 2015). This indicates that the prolonged high contrast stimulation delivered for retinotopic mapping to the non-deprived eye during the first ~30 min of deprivation did not modulate the deprivation effects.
We defined a psychophysical index of the deprivation effect (DIpsycho) by using Equation. 6 in Materials and methods section, where the POST to PRE-deprivation ratio of phase durations for the deprived eye, is divided by the same ratio for the non-deprived eye. Values larger than one imply a relative increase of the deprived eye phase duration, that is the expected effect; a value less than 1 indicates the opposite effect and a value of 1 indicates no change of mean phase duration across eyes. All but two subjects have values larger than 1, indicating a strong effect of deprivation. However, the scatter is large with values ranging from 0.7 to 3, suggesting that susceptibility to visual plasticity varies largely in our pool of participants. Capitalizing on this variability, we tested whether the size of the psychophysical effect correlates with the BOLD effect across participants. Using the same Equation 6 to compute the deprivation effect on BOLD responses (DIBOLD), we observed a strong correlation between the effect of monocular deprivation on psychophysics and BOLD (shown in Figure 3B). Subjects who showed a strong deprivation effect at psychophysics (DIpsycho >2) also showed a strong deprivation effect in BOLD responses (DIBOLD = 1.85 ± 0.42). Given that the psychophysics was measured only for central vision and at two cpd stationary grating, whereas BOLD responses were pooled across a large portion on V1 and were elicited using broadband dynamic stimuli, the correlation suggests that the psychophysical effect may be used as a reliable proxy of a general change of cortical excitability, which can be measured by fMRI.
Monocular deprivation shifts BOLD spatial frequency tuning for the deprived eye
The BOLD measure we use here gives us the chance to measure the effect of Monocular Deprivation across spatial frequencies and as function of eccentricity. We used five band-pass noise (1.25 octaves half-width at half maximum) stimuli with peak spatial frequency selected to have a complete coverage of spatial frequencies from 0.03 to 12.5 cpd (see Figure 4—figure supplement 1). In contrast with the strong and reliable plasticity of responses to the high spatial frequency stimulus (peaking at 2.7 cpd, Figures 1–3), we find that the plasticity effect is absent at low spatial frequencies (interaction index for the highest spatial frequency stimulus: 0.70 ± 0.19; for the lowest spatial frequency stimulus: 0.16 ± 0.15; paired t-test t(18) = −3.441, p = 0.003).
Thus, monocular deprivation produces a change of the spatial frequency selectivity of the V1 BOLD response. Before deprivation, the BOLD response shows a broad band-pass selectivity for our stimuli, with a preference for the stimulus peaking at intermediate spatial frequencies, between 0.4 and 1.1 cpd, and a slight attenuation at higher spatial frequencies, similar for the two eyes (Figure 4A). After deprivation (Figure 4B), the non-deprived eye shows similar selectivity and an overall decrease of responses. For the deprived eye, the shape of the curve changes: from band-pass to high-pass, implying that the enhancement affects primarily the higher spatial frequencies.
To model this effect, we assume that each vertex on the cortical surface subtends a multitude of neuronal channels, each with narrow tuning for spatial frequency and collectively spanning a large range of spatial frequencies – an approach conceptually similar to the population Receptive Field model for retinotopic mapping (Dumoulin and Wandell, 2008). Independently of the exact bandwidth and peak preference of the neuronal population contributing to the final BOLD selectivity, we find that the shape of all these curves is captured with a simple one-parameter model: what we term the population tuning for Spatial Frequency. This is given by a Difference-of-Gaussians (DoG) function with one free parameter, the spatial constant (while the excitation/inhibition spatial constant ratio is fixed; see Equation 4 in the Materials and method and curves in Figure 5—figure supplement 1). The free parameter sets the high spatial frequency cut-off at half-height of the filter. The continuous lines in Figure 4 show how the model fits the grand-average of V1 responses, with best fit cut-off around five cpd similar for all conditions except for the POST-deprivation deprived eye, where the cut-off is 6.2 cpd (single vertex examples are given in Figure 5—figure supplement 1 panels C-I). The DoG equation has been successfully used in previous studies to model CSF and neural responses at variable stimulus parameters for example illumination levels (Enroth-Cugell and Robson, 1966; Hawken et al., 1988), validating this equation for modeling the overall selectivity of large neuronal ensembles.
Using this model to analyze single vertex responses, we evaluated the best-fit spatial frequency cut-off of the neural population contributing to the vertex BOLD response (see details in the Materials and method and Figure 5—figure supplement 1 panels A-C; briefly, we used the DoG model to predict the response elicited by our five band-pass noise stimuli in populations with different spatial frequency selectivity, that is filters with different cut-off; we then found the cut-off value that maximizes the correlation between the predicted responses and the observed BOLD responses). We used this procedure to fit BOLD responses in each of our four conditions, estimating spatial frequency selectivity in individual vertices in each condition: separately for the two eyes, PRE/POST deprivation. Before deprivation, the spatial frequency cut-off decays with eccentricity as expected. Figure 5A maps both eccentricity (pRF eccentricity estimates from a separate retinotopic mapping scan) and spatial frequency cut-off values, obtained by fitting responses to the deprived eye, before deprivation (averaged across hemispheres and subjects). The cut-off is around 16 in the para-fovea (eccentricity around 1.5 deg) and down to four in the periphery (eccentricity around 8 deg). This relationship between eccentricity and spatial frequency preference is consistent with previous fMRI results (D’Souza et al., 2016; Henriksson et al., 2008) and with psychophysics (Rovamo et al., 1978). The model captures well the selectivity of an example V1 vertex (Figure 5B, goodness of fit better than 0.9), sampled from the mid-periphery (3.4 deg) for the deprived eye, both before and after deprivation. The spatial frequency cut-off after deprivation shifts to higher values, increasing (in this example) by about a factor of three. Figure 5C–D shows that this behavior is systematically observed across V1 vertices, but only for the deprived eye. Here the average cut-off is plotted as function of eccentricity, and the roll-off is consistent with the map in Figure 5A. For the non-deprived eye, there is no effect of deprivation on spatial frequency selectivity (Figure 5C). In contrast, for the deprived eye (Figure 5D), there is a shift towards preferring higher spatial frequencies, at all eccentricities, which is captured by an increased value of the cut-off frequency parameter leading to an increased acuity of the BOLD response to the deprived eye.
Note that the change of spatial frequency selectivity for the deprived eye is most evident at eccentricities of 4 deg and higher (see Figure 5D), where vertices have peak sensitivity at mid-to-low spatial frequencies before deprivation. In the fovea, where many vertices already prefer the highest spatial frequency stimulus before deprivation, our fitting procedure is likely to underestimate the change of spatial frequency selectivity. Importantly, the spatial frequency selectivity for the non-deprived eye is unchanged at all eccentricities, corroborating the eye and stimulus specificity of the short-term monocular deprivation effect. These findings are consistent with maps in Figure 1—figure supplement 1 panels C-D showing that deprivation effects are largely homogenous across all V1 eccentricities, with no obvious clustering of effects in the fovea or in the periphery.
To test the significance of these effects, we pooled the best fit cut-off values from all selected V1 vertices across eccentricities and averaged them across participants (Figure 6A). The repeated measure ANOVA (performed on the log-transformed values, which are distributed normally as assessed by the Jarque-Bera test) shows no significant time × eye interaction (F(1,18) = 3.67607, p 0.07121) and non significant main effect of time (F(1,18) = 2.62546, p = 0.12255) but a significant main effect of eye (F(1,18) = 13.58079, p = 0.00169). This is clarified by post-hoc t-tests revealing that the increase of spatial frequency cut-off for the deprived eye is significant (t(18) = −2.263, p = 0.036) whereas there is no significant change for the non-deprived eye (t(18) = 0.440, p = 0.665). Given that the time × eye interaction in the full V1 region is not significant, and to minimize noise contamination, we evaluated the effect of deprivation on spatial frequency cut-off at the individual level by a ‘Deprived Eye Change (DepCcutoff)’ index (Equation 7 in the Materials and method), that is taking the POST vs. PRE-deprivation ratio of the spatial frequency cut-off for the deprived eye alone. As this ratio varies widely across participants, over more than three octaves, we asked whether this variability correlates with our psychophysical probe of plasticity: binocular rivalry. We used the same Equation 7 to index the psychophysical change of the deprived eye (DepCpsycho), the POST to PRE- ratio of mean phase duration for the deprived eye, and found a strong positive correlation (Figure 6B). POST-deprivation, the deprived eye shows an increase of mean phase duration (in binocular rivalry) and an increase of the spatial frequency cut-off (best fit of the BOLD responses): participants showing a stronger increase of phase duration, also showed a larger shift of selectivity towards higher spatial frequency. The correlation is consistent with the result of Figure 3 showing that the enhancement of BOLD responses is correlated with the change of binocular rivalry and selective for the highest spatial frequency stimulus.
Monocular deprivation affects BOLD responses in the ventral stream areas beyond V1
We measured the effect over the main extra-striate visual cortical areas. The selective boost of the deprived eye response to the high spatial frequency is as strong in V2 as in V1 (Figure 1—figure supplement 1 and Figure 7E). The boost is present also in V3 and V4. In V4 the boost appears to be present also for lower spatial frequencies, but again only for the deprived eye (Figure 7A–B), possibly reflecting the larger spatial frequency bandwidth of V4 neurons compared to V1.
The results are very different for dorsal area V3a (Figure 7C–D) and hMT+ (Figure 7E–F), which do not show any significant change of responses in either eye at high spatial frequencies. Although the preferred response moves to lower spatial frequencies, consistent with a stronger input of the magnocellular pathway to the dorsal visual stream (Henriksson et al., 2008; Singh et al., 2000), the response to the highest spatial frequency stimulus is still strong and reliable in both V3a and hMT+. Note that the reliable BOLD estimates of Figure 7 are computed after pooling vertices within the ROI and then averaging across subjects. However, the response of hMT+ evaluated at the individual vertex do not show significant activation (Figure 1B), probably reflecting more variable organization of activity within this ROI across subjects (Smith et al., 2006).
Fig 7G quantifies the effect of short-term monocular deprivation (using the ANOVA time x eye interaction term, which measures the eye-selective modulation of BOLD response after deprivation for the highest spatial frequency) across the main visual areas. The plasticity effect is strongest in V1, V2 and V3; it is still strong and significant in ventral area V4 (t(18) = 2.41 p = 0.0270), but it is absent in V3a and hMT+, where the time x eye interaction is not significantly different from 0 (t(18) = 0.52 p = 0.6115 and t(18) = −0.19 p = 0.8513 respectively). The plasticity effect in ventral area V4 is significantly stronger than in dorsal areas V3a and hMT+ (t(18) = 2.39, p = 0.0278 and t(18) = 2.36, p = 0.0299 for V4-V3a and V4-hMT+ respectively).
This result suggests a preferential involvement of the parvocellular vs. magnocellular pathway, leading to the differential plasticity effect in extra-striate visual areas of the ventral and dorsal pathway. Interestingly, the plasticity effect is robust in areas where the majority of cells are binocular (like V3 and V4), indicating that the effect does not require segregated representations of the two eyes (e.g. Ocular Dominance columns).
Discussion
We demonstrate that two hours of abnormal visual experience has a profound impact on the neural sensitivity and selectivity of V1. BOLD activity across the V1 cortical region paradoxically increases for the eye that was deprived of contrast vision, and decreases for the eye exposed to normal visual experience.
The enhanced response to the deprived eye fits well with the concept of homeostatic plasticity, first observed in rodent visual cortex, both juvenile and adult (Maffei et al., 2004; Mrsic-Flogel et al., 2007; Turrigiano and Nelson, 2004), which is the tendency of neural circuits to keep the average firing rates constant in spite of anomalous stimulation (Maffei and Turrigiano, 2008; Turrigiano, 2012) (Mrsic-Flogel et al., 2007; Turrigiano and Nelson, 2004). More recently, similar observations have been made in the adult macaque V1 after two hours of monocular deprivation during anesthesia (Begum and Tso, 2016; Tso et al., 2017). The post-deprivation gain boost observed in the monkey is consistent with our observations of an increased BOLD response to the deprived eye. We also observe an antagonistic suppression of the non-deprived eye BOLD response; together, the two effects lead to a shift of ocular preference of individual vertices in favor of the deprived eye. However, this effect is only observed in those V1 vertices that responded preferentially to the non-deprived eye before deprivation. No change of ocular preference is seen in vertices that already prefer the deprived eye before deprivation, which maintain their eye-preference after deprivation. This pattern of results cannot be explained by an overall gain increase; rather, it is consistent with the idea that the representation of the deprived eye recruits cortical resources (which may or may not correspond to cortical territory), normally dedicated to the other eye.
A similar antagonist effect on the two eyes (boosting the deprived eye and suppressing the non-deprived eye) was also observed in the VEP responses after short-term monocular deprivation (Lunghi et al., 2015a), and could be implemented through a modulation of the excitatory/inhibitory circuitry. Regulation of the excitation/inhibition balance through GABAergic signaling is considered to be a key factor for cortical plasticity, including homeostatic plasticity (Maffei and Turrigiano, 2008). Interestingly, the involvement of GABAergic signaling in the effect of short-term monocular deprivation is directly supported by MR Spectroscopy data in adult humans, showing that resting GABA in a large region of the occipital cortex is specifically reduced after short-term monocular deprivation (Lunghi et al., 2015b).
The functional relevance of the BOLD changes we observe is demonstrated by their correlation with our behavioral assay of plasticity, obtained through binocular rivalry. This correlates both with the BOLD ocular dominance change (relative boost/suppression of the deprived/non-deprived eye), and with the BOLD acuity change for the deprived eye (change of spatial frequency tuning, assessed with our pRF-like modeling approach). The correlation holds despite binocular rivalry being restricted to foveal vision, whereas the assessment of BOLD plasticity is pooled across V1 (including the mid-periphery). This implies that the change of binocular rivalry dynamics is a proxy for the more general plasticity effects that involves the whole primary visual cortex. This finding has long reaching implications, as it could validate the use of binocular rivalry as a biomarker of adult cortical plasticity, based on the neural mechanisms revealed by the present 7T fMRI results. Interestingly, the binocular rivalry phenomenon originates in the primary visual cortex – probably at the earliest stages – and is an expression of the dynamics of excitatory transmission and inhibitory feedback (Tong et al., 2006); as such it is a measure that could reflect the overall excitation-inhibition ratio (van Loon et al., 2013), and its modulation in plasticity (Lunghi et al., 2015b; Maffei and Turrigiano, 2008).
Our data support the notion that V1 circuitry may be optimized by perceptual experience (Fiorentini and Berardi, 1980). They are also consistent with a large perceptual learning literature suggesting that associative cortical areas retain a high degree of flexibility (Dosher and Lu, 2017; Dosher and Lu, 1999; Fuchs and Flügge, 2014; Harris et al., 2012; Kahnt et al., 2011; Karni et al., 1995; Lewis et al., 2009; Shibata et al., 2012; Watanabe and Sasaki, 2015). Although the monocular deprivations effects observed here are more robust in V1, they are reliable in V2 and V3 as well. However, a clear difference emerges between extra-striate visual areas in the ventral and dorsal stream. While ventral area V4 shows a strong deprivation effect, area V3a, located at a similar tier in the dorsal stream, shows no BOLD change after short-term monocular deprivation. V4 is a primary target of the parvocellular system, which is best stimulated by our highest spatial frequency stimulus; V3a and hMT+ are preferential targets of the magnocellular system, which respond more strongly to our lower spatial frequency stimuli (see Figure 7). The different plasticity response of the ventral and dorsal stream, together with the selectivity for the high spatial frequencies of the V1 plasticity, suggests that the parvocellular pathway is most strongly affected by short-term plasticity. This is consistent with the finding in non-human primates that deprivation of the stimuli that optimally drive the parvocellular system is sufficient to produce a reliable plasticity effect (Begum and Tso, 2016). It is also consistent with the finding that the effect of short-term monocular deprivation is strongest and more long-lasting for chromatic equiluminant stimuli (Lunghi et al., 2013).
Other evidence shows that short-term deprivation may affect other properties of vision. In particular, selective deprivation of orientation (Zhang et al., 2009) or spatial frequency (Zhou et al., 2014) or color (Zhou et al., 2017) or even simply phase scrambling of the image in one eye (Bai et al., 2017) may lead to a boost of the deprived signal. These effects have been interpreted as a form of release of inhibition from the adapted signal (Zhang et al., 2009) – a concept that is not distant from homeostatic plasticity, where the network aims to keep overall activity constant. The conceptual border between adaptation and plasticity is fuzzy, given that some mechanisms are shared and both effects have the same outcomes. Be it adaptation or plasticity, the monocular deprivation mechanisms are probably cortical and affect mainly the deprived eye. There is evidence that the boost of the deprived eye is also observed when the two eyes receive equally strong stimulation, but perception of one eye stimulus is suppressed experimentally (by the continuous flash suppression technique, Kim et al., 2017); this result dismisses the retinal or thalamic contribution to the deprivation effect. Only in rare occasions does adaptation induce effects that last over days (McCollough, 1965), yet our recent work shows that deprivation effects of short-term monocular deprivation is retained across 6 hr sleep (Menicucci et al., 2018), consistent with plasticity reinforcement during sleep (Raven et al., 2018; Timofeev and Chauvette, 2017). Most importantly, in adult amblyopic patients, short-term monocular deprivation is able to induce improvement of visual acuity and stereovision (Lunghi et al., 2018) for up to one year. All this evidence supports the concept that homeostatic plasticity in the human adult cortex may be linked with or may promote more stable forms of Hebbian-like plasticity. This may endorse stable changes even in the adult brain, well after the closure of the critical period. Functional changes in associative cortex in adults have been demonstrated by short-term paired TMS studies (Chao et al., 2015). Interestingly, the decay of this functional connectivity change has a similar time-course as the monocular deprivation effect, about one hour. Also, Hebbian changes at the single cell level can be observed in V1 of adult anaesthetized cat, following activity pairing over a similar time-scale (from minutes to a few hours) (Frégnac et al., 1988). All these results demonstrate that V1 retains potential for Hebbian plasticity outside the critical period – although it may need particular conditions to exploit such potential.
Understanding homeostatic plasticity and its relation to Hebbian plasticity may be fundamental to open the way to new approaches to treat brain dysfunction. Particularly important is Ocular Dominance plasticity in amblyopia (Webber and Wood, 2005), a cortical deficit still without cure in adults, although recent advancements in training procedures are opening new hopes (Levi and Li, 2009; Sengpiel, 2014). Endorsing plasticity may increase the effectiveness of these treatments and preliminary data from our laboratory suggest that monocular deprivation of the amblyopic eye may indeed boost sensitivity of the deprived eye and improve its acuity (Lunghi et al., 2018) – just like an acuity change is revealed by the present BOLD measurements in normally sighted participants. Our data demonstrate that two hours of abnormally unbalanced visual experience is sufficient to induce a functional reorganization of cortical circuits, particularly of the parvocellular pathway, leading to an alteration of basic visual perceptual abilities.
Materials and methods
Experimental model and subject details
Human subjects
Request a detailed protocolExperimental procedures are in line with the declaration of Helsinki and were approved by the regional ethics committee [Comitato Etico Pediatrico Regionale—Azienda Ospedaliero-Universitaria Meyer—Firenze (FI)] and by the Italian Ministry of Health, under the protocol ‘Plasticità e multimodalità delle prime aree visive: studio in risonanza magnetica a campo ultra alto (7T)’ version #1 dated 11/11/2015. Written informed consent was obtained from each participant, which included consent to process and preserve the data and publish them in anonymous form.
Twenty healthy volunteers with normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity were examined (8 females and 12 males, mean age = 27 years).
Method details
Experimental design
Request a detailed protocolEach participant underwent two scanning sessions separated by two hours, during which they were subject to the short-term monocular deprivation procedure described below. Just before each scanning section, their binocular rivalry was measured psychophysically. One (male) participant was excluded because of strong eye dominance tested with binocular rivalry before the deprivation. This left 19 participants (8 females and 11 males) whose complete datasets were entered all analyses. Sample size was set to enable testing for correlations between neuroimaging and psychophysical data. Previous work (Lunghi et al., 2015b) reveals a correlation between MR spectroscopy data and binocular rivalry measures r = 0.62 (or higher), which implies a minimum of 17 participants to detect a significant correlation at 0.05 significance level, with test power of 80% (Lachin, 1981).
Short-term monocular deprivation
Request a detailed protocolMonocular deprivation was achieved by patching the dominant eye for 2 hr. The operational definition of dominant eye applied to the eye showing the longer phase durations during the baseline binocular rivalry measurements. Like in previous studies (Binda and Lunghi, 2017; Lunghi et al., 2011; Lunghi et al., 2013), we used a translucent eye-patch made of plastic material allowing light to reach the retina (attenuation 0.07 logUnits, at least 3 times smaller than the threshold for discriminating a full-field luminance decrement (Knau, 2000) and more than ten times smaller than the minimum photopic luminance decrement required for shifting the spatial (Van Nes and Bouman, 1967) or temporal contrast sensitivity function (Kelly, 1961). The patch prevents pattern vision, as assessed by the Fourier transform of a natural world image seen through the eye-patch. During the 2 hr of monocular deprivation, observers were either engaged in the retinotopic mapping experiment (about 30’, described below) or they were free to read and use a computer.
Binocular rivalry
Request a detailed protocolBinocular rivalry was measured in two short sessions (each comprising two runs of 3 min each), immediately before the Pre- and Post-deprivation MR sessions, in a quiet space adjacent to the MR control room. Visual stimuli were created in MATLAB running on a laptop (Dell) using PsychToolbox (Brainard, 1997), and displayed on a 15- inch monitor (BenQ). Like in (Lunghi et al., 2015b), observers viewed the visual stimuli presented on the monitor at a distance of 57 cm through anaglyph red-blue goggles (right lens blue, left lens red). Responses were recorded with the computer keyboard by continuous alternate keypresses. Visual stimuli were two oblique orthogonal red and blue gratings (orientation:±45°, size: 3°, spatial frequency: 2 cpd, contrast 50%), surrounded by a white smoothed circle, presented on a black uniform background in central vision. Peak luminance of the red grating was reduced to match the peak luminance of the blue one using photometric measures. All included participants had typical binocular rivalry dynamics, with low percentage of mixed percepts (reported for 8.5 ± 2.04% of time on average). Only one participant experienced of mixed percepts for more than 20% of time (exactly for 31.2%) and his data are in line with the others’.
Stimuli for fMRI
Request a detailed protocolVisual stimuli were projected with an MR-compatible goggle set (VisuaStimDigital, Resonance Technologies, Los Angeles, USA), connected to a computer placed in the control room. The goggles covered a visual field of approximately 32 × 24 deg, with a resolution of 800 × 600 pixels, mean luminance 25 cd/m2; the images in the two eyes were controlled independently.
During all functional MRI scans participants were instructed to maintain central fixation on a red point (0.5 degrees) that was constantly visible at the center of the screen. Bandpass noise stimuli were white noise images filtered to match the spatial frequency tuning of neurons in the visual cortex (Morrone and Burr, 1988). We generated a large white noise matrix (8000 × 6000) and filtered it with a two-dimensional circular bandpass filter Bp defined by Equation 1:
where P is the peak spatial frequency, q is the filter half-width at half maximum in octaves. We generated five band-pass noise stimuli, by setting q = 1.25 octaves and p = 0.1 cpd, 0.2 cpd, 0.4 cpd, 1.1 cpd, 2.7 cpd. Each stimulus was presented for a block of 3 TRs, during which the image was refreshed at 8 Hz (randomly resampling a 800 × 600 window from the original matrix). Stimuli were scaled to exploit the luminance range of the display, and this yielded very similar RMS contrast values (shown in Figure 4—figure supplement 1). Stimulus blocks were separated by 4 TRs blanks, consisting of a mid-level gray screen. The five band-pass noise stimuli blocks were presented in pseudo-random order, twice per run, for a total of 70 TRs. In each run, stimuli were only presented to one eye, while the other was shown a mid-level gray screen. Each eye was tested once, before and after deprivation.
Immediately upon application of the monocular patch, we performed two additional scans to perform retinotopic mapping of visual areas. Meridian and ring stimuli were presented monocularly (to the non-patched eye) and were defined as apertures of a mid-level gray mask that uncovered a checkerboard pattern, 1 deg at 1 deg eccentricity to 2.5 deg at 9 deg eccentricity, rotating and contracting at a rate of one check per second. Meridians were defined by two 45° wedges centered around 0° or around 90°. The horizontal and vertical meridian were presented interchangeably for 5 TRs each (without blanks) and the sequence was repeated six times for a total of 60 TRs. Rings partitioned screen space into six contiguous eccentricity bands (0–0.9 deg, 0.9–1.8 deg, 1.8–3.3 deg, 3.3–4.7 deg, 4.7–6.48 deg, 6.48–9 deg). Odd and even rings were presented in two separate runs. In each run, the three selected rings and one blank were presented in random order for 5 TRs each, and the sequence was repeated (with different order) 6 times for a total of 120 TRs.
MR system and sequences
Request a detailed protocolScanning was performed on a Discovery MR950 7 T whole body MRI system (GE Healthcare, Milwaukee, WI, USA) equipped with a 2-channel transmit driven in quadrature mode, a 32-channel receive coil (Nova Medical, Wilmington, MA, USA) and a high-performance gradient system (50 mT/m maximum amplitude and 200 mT/m/ms slew rate).
Anatomical images were acquired at 1 mm isotropic resolution using a T1-weighted magnetization-prepared fast Fast Spoiled Gradient Echo (FSPGR) with the following parameters: TR = 6 ms, TE = 2.2 ms. FA = 12 deg, rBW = 50 kHz, TI = 450 ms, ASSET = 2.
Functional images were acquired with spatial resolution 1.5 mm and slice thickness 1.4 mm with slice spacing = 0.1 mm, TR = 3 s, TE = 23 ms, rBW = 250 kHz, ASSET = 2, phase encoding direction AP-PA. No resampling was performed during the reconstruction. For each EPI sequence, we acquired two additional volumes with the reversed phase encoding direction.
Quantification and statistical analysis
ROI definition
Request a detailed protocolAreas V1, V2 and V3 were manually outlined for all participants using retinotopic data projected on surface models of white matter. The V1/V2 boundary was traced from the vertical/horizontal meridian flip superior/inferior to the calcarine sulcus, and the V2/V3 border and V3 border from the subsequent opposite flips. Areas V4, V3a and hMT+ (merging MT and MST) were defined based on the cortical parcellation atlas by Glasser et al. (Glasser et al., 2016). V1, V2, V3, V4 and V3a ROIs were further restricted to select the representation of our screen space. Specifically, the anterior boundaries were defined based on activation from most peripheral (6.48°−9°) ring stimuli of the retinotopic mapping scans; in addition, vertices were only included in the analysis if their preferred eccentricity (estimated through Population Receptive Field modelling, see below) was larger than 1, since no reliable mapping could be obtained for the central-most part of the visual field.
Pre-processing of imaging data
Request a detailed protocolAnalyses were performed mainly with Freesurfer v6.0.0, with some contributions of the SPM12 and BrainVoyager 20.6 and FSL version 5.0.10 (Jenkinson et al., 2012) packages.
Anatomical images were corrected for intensity bias using SPM12 (Friston, 2007) and processed by a standard procedure for segmentation implemented in Freesurfer (recon-all: Fischl et al., 2002). In addition, each hemisphere was aligned to a left/right symmetric template hemisphere (fsaverage_sym: Greve et al., 2013).
Functional images were corrected for subject movements (Goebel et al., 2006) and undistorted using EPI images with reversed phase encoding direction (Brain Voyager COPE plug-in Jezzard and Balaban, 1995). We then exported the preprocessed images from BrainVoyager to NiFTi format. These were aligned to each participant’s anatomical image using a boundary based registration algorithm (Freesurfer bbergister function) and projected to the cortical surface of each hemisphere. All analyses were conducted on data in the individual subject space. In addition, for visualization purposes, we also aligned the results of timecourse analyses (GLM and subsequent pRF and spatial frequency tuning estimates) to the left/right symmetric template hemisphere. Averaged results across the 18 × 2 hemispheres are shown in the maps of Figure 1B, Figure 5A and Figure 1—figure supplement 1.
GLM analysis of fMRI data
Request a detailed protocolGeneral Linear Model analysis was performed with in-house MATLAB software (D’Souza et al., 2016). We assumed that fMRI timecourses result from the linear combination of N predictors: boxcar functions representing stimulus presence/absence (one per stimulus type) convolved by a standard hemodynamic response function (see Equation 2), plus two nuisance variables (a linear trend and a constant). We modeled the hemodynamic response function as a gamma function h(t):
with parameters n = 3, t = 1.5 s, and d = 2.25 s (Boynton et al., 1996). Beta weights of the stimuli predictors were taken as estimates of the BOLD response amplitude and normalized by the predictor amplitude to obtain a measure that directly corresponds to % signal change; beta weights were also scaled by an error measure to obtain t-values, following the same procedure as in (Friston et al., 1994). Computing BOLD responses for each individual vertex of the cortical surface leads to up-sampling the functional data (each 1.5 × 1.5 × 1.5 mm functional voxel projecting on an average of 3 vertices). We ensured that this does not affect our statistical analyses by first averaging data from all vertices within a region of interest (e.g. V1), thereby entering all ANOVAs with a single value per subject and region of interest.
Population receptive field mapping
Request a detailed protocolThe pRFs of the selected voxels were estimated with custom software in MATLAB, implementing a method related to that described by Dumoulin and Wandell (Dumoulin & Wandell, 2008) and provided as supplementary material. We modeled the pRF with a 1D Gaussian function defined over eccentricity, with parameters and as mean and standard deviation respectively, and representing the aggregate receptive field of all neurons imaged within the vertex area. We defined the stimulus as a binary matrix S representing the presence of visual stimulation over space (here, eccentricity between 0 and 10 deg with 40 steps per deg) for each of 6 ring stimuli. We used the results of our GLM analysis to estimate the vertex response to each of our 6 rings (as t-values; using beta values yields very similar results). We assumed that each vertex response is the linear sum over space (eccentricity) of the overlap between the pRF of the voxel and the input stimulus, which is mathematically equivalent to the matrix multiplication between the stimulus and the pRF.
where is the index to ring number and varies between 1 and 6.
We used this equation to predict the response to our six rings for a large set of initial pRF parameters; for each vertex, we measured the correlation (our goodness-of-fit index) between the predicted response and the observed t-values. If the highest correlation was .7 the vertex was discarded; otherwise, the parameters yielding the highest correlation were used to initialize a nonlinear search procedure (MATLAB simplex algorithm), which manipulated and to maximize goodness-of-fit, with the constraint that could not exceed 20 deg or be smaller than 1 deg, and could not be smaller than. 1 deg. Successful fits were obtained for 72.00 ± 1.86% of V1 vertices, for which the initial coarse grid search gave a correlation > 0.7 and the nonlinear search settled within the constraints. All analyses (on average and distribution of responses and tuning parameters) considered the sub-region of V1 for which a successful fit was obtained. We used to estimate the preferred eccentricity of each vertex.
The main modifications of our procedure relative to that described by Dumoulin and Wandell (Dumoulin and Wandell, 2008) are the following: (a) fMRI data were acquired in a block design with only six stimulus types (six eccentricity bands) rather than varying stimulus position at each TR; this allowed us to use a standard GLM approach to estimate each vertex response to the six stimuli (assuming a standard hemodynamic response function) and then use the pRF model to predict these six time-points – much faster than predicting the full fMRI series of 120 × 2 TRs; (b) our stimuli and consequently our pRFs were defined in one dimension (eccentricity) – whereas the standard pRF is defined in 2D, eccentricity and polar angle (or Cartesian x and y); (c) we maximized the correlation between the predicted and observed fMRI response time-courses rather than minimizing the root mean square error; this eliminates the need to estimate a scale factor to account for the unknown units of the BOLD signal.
Population tuning for spatial frequency
Request a detailed protocolUsing a similar logic, we also estimated the population tuning for Spatial Frequency, which represents the aggregate Spatial Frequency tuning of the population of neurons imaged within each vertex area. We modeled the population tuning using a family of functions that includes the psychophysical Contrast Sensitivity Function (CSF) and can be specified by the following one-parameter equation (Difference-of-Gaussians):
Like we did for the pRF mapping, we defined a stimulus matrix S representing the Fourier spectra of our five bandpass noise stimuli, that is the energy of visual stimulation in the frequency domain (here, between 0.03 cpd and 12.5 cpd) for each stimulus. We used the results of our GLM analysis to estimate the vertex response to each of our five bandpass noise stimuli (as t-values; using beta values yields very similar results). We assumed that each vertex response is the linear sum over frequency of the overlap between the pSFT of the voxel and the input stimulus, which is mathematically equivalent to the matrix multiplication between the stimulus and the pSFT.
Like for pRFs, we estimated the best-fit parameter of each vertex pSFT with a two-step procedure: a coarse-grid search followed by the simplex search. We used the matrix multiplication of the pSFT and the stimulus to predict the response to our five bandpass noise stimuli for a large set of initial values (between 1 and 1,000 in 100 logarithmic steps); for each vertex, we measured the correlation (our goodness-of-fit index) between the predicted response and the observed t-values. If the highest correlation was <.5, the voxel was discarded, otherwise the parameter yielding the highest correlation were used to initialize a nonlinear search procedure (MATLAB simplex algorithm), which manipulated to maximize goodness-of-fit, with the constraint that could not be smaller than. 3 and larger than 10,000. Successful fits were obtained for 88.84 ± 1.28% of V1 vertices for which we obtained a successful eccentricity fit (86.77 ± 1.25% of all V1 vertices).
We express the parameter in terms of the high-spatial frequency cutoff of the filter (highest spatial frequency at half maximum), for each vertex:
Indices defining the effect of deprivation
Request a detailed protocolWe computed the effects of short-term monocular deprivation on both the dynamics of binocular rivalry and our fMRI results, estimating the degree to which the two measures are correlated. In all cases, the same equation was applied to psychophysical and fMRI data.
The first index, called ‘Deprivation Index’ or DIpsycho and DIBOLD is given by Equation 6
For psychophysics, y = mean duration of Binocular Rivalry phases of the Dep or Ndep eye, during the PRE- or POST deprivation sessions; for fMRI, y = mean BOLD response across V1 vertices to stimuli in the Dep or Ndep eye, during the PRE- or POST-deprivation sessions.
The second index, called ‘Deprived-eye change’ or DepCpsycho and DepCcutoff is given by Equation 7
For psychophysics, y = mean duration of Binocular Rivalry phases of the Dep eye, during the PRE- or POST deprivation sessions. For fMRI, y = mean spatial frequency cut-off across V1 vertices estimated for stimuli in the Dep eye, during the PRE- or POST-deprivation sessions.
Statistics
Data from individual participants (mean binocular rivalry phase durations or mean BOLD responses/pRF/pST across V1 or V2 vertices) were analyzed with a repeated measure ANOVA approach, after checking that distributions do not systematically deviate from normality by means of the Jarque-Bera test for composite normality (MATLAB jbtest function, p-values given in the relevant figures). F statistics are reported with associated degrees of freedom and p-values in the Results section, in the form: F(df,dferr) = value; p = value. Post-hoc paired t-tests comparing conditions follow the ANOVA results, in the form: t(df)= value, p = value. Associations between variables are assessed with Pearson product-moment correlation coefficient, reported in the form: r(n) = value, p = value. Aggregate subject data (i.e. vertices pooled across participants and hemispheres) were typically non-normally distributed and thereby were analysed with non-parametric tests. The Wilcoxon sign-rank test was used for comparing medians, and results are reported in the form: z = value, p = value.
Data availability
BOLD responses and pRF fits as shown in all figures (main and supplementary) have been deposited on Dryad, through a link provided with the current submission (doi:10.5061/dryad.tp24j18). Custom Matlab code, used for pRF fitting, is included as Source code file 1.
-
Dryad Digital RepositoryShort-term plasticity of the human adult visual cortex measured with 7T BOLD.https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.tp24j18
References
-
Reorganization of human cortical maps caused by inherited photoreceptor abnormalitiesNature Neuroscience 5:364–370.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn817
-
Critical periods during sensory developmentCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology 10:138–145.https://doi.org/10.1016/S0959-4388(99)00047-1
-
Short-Term monocular deprivation enhances physiological pupillary oscillationsNeural Plasticity 2017:e6724631.https://doi.org/10.1155/2017/6724631
-
Linear systems analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging in human V1The Journal of Neuroscience 16:4207–4221.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-13-04207.1996
-
Neuronal basis of contrast discriminationVision Research 39:257–269.https://doi.org/10.1016/S0042-6989(98)00113-8
-
Development of human visual functionVision Research 51:1588–1609.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2011.02.018
-
Mechanisms of perceptual learningVision Research 39:3197–3221.https://doi.org/10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00059-0
-
Visual Perceptual Learning and ModelsAnnual Review of Vision Science 3:343–363.https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-vision-102016-061249
-
Dependence of chromatic responses in V1 on visual field eccentricity and spatial frequency: an fMRI studyJournal of the Optical Society of America A 33:A53.https://doi.org/10.1364/JOSAA.33.000A53
-
The contrast sensitivity of retinal ganglion cells of the catThe Journal of Physiology 187:517–552.https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1966.sp008107
-
Whole brain segmentation: automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brainNeuron 33:341–355.
-
Contrast sensitivity in humans with abnormal visual experienceThe Journal of Physiology 247:687–710.https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1975.sp010952
-
Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: A general linear approachHuman Brain Mapping 2:189–210.https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.460020402
-
BookStatistical Parametric Mapping: The Analysis of Funtional Brain Images (First Edition)Amsterdam; Boston: Elsevier/Academic Press.
-
Adult Neuroplasticity: More Than 40 Years of ResearchNeural Plasticity 2014:1–10.https://doi.org/10.1155/2014/541870
-
Experience-dependent plasticity of binocular responses in the primary visual cortex of the mouseThe Journal of Neuroscience 16:3274–3286.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-10-03274.1996
-
A surface-based analysis of language lateralization and cortical asymmetryJournal of Cognitive Neuroscience 25:1477–1492.https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00405
-
Generalized perceptual learning in the absence of sensory adaptationCurrent Biology 22:1813–1817.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.07.059
-
Visual deprivation reactivates rapid ocular dominance plasticity in adult visual cortexThe Journal of Neuroscience : The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience 26:2951–2955.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5554-05.2006
-
Spatial frequency tuning in human retinotopic visual areasJournal of Vision 8:5–13.https://doi.org/10.1167/8.10.5
-
Plasticity of Ocular Dominance Columns in Monkey Striate CortexPhilosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 278:377–409.https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.1977.0050
-
The period of susceptibility to the physiological effects of unilateral eye closure in kittensThe Journal of Physiology 206:419–436.https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1970.sp009022
-
Correction for geometric distortion in echo planar images from B0 field variationsMagnetic Resonance in Medicine 34:65–73.https://doi.org/10.1002/mrm.1910340111
-
Visual response to time-dependent stimuli. I. Amplitude sensitivity measurementsJournal of the Optical Society of America 51:422–429.https://doi.org/10.1364/JOSA.51.000422
-
Thresholds for detecting slowly changing Ganzfeld luminancesJournal of the Optical Society of America A 17:1382–1387.https://doi.org/10.1364/JOSAA.17.001382
-
Introduction to sample size determination and power analysis for clinical trialsControlled Clinical Trials 2:93–113.https://doi.org/10.1016/0197-2456(81)90001-5
-
Low Vision and Plasticity: Implications for RehabilitationAnnual Review of Vision Science 2:321–343.https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-vision-111815-114344
-
BookAmblyopia: a consequence of abnormal visual developmentIn: Simons K, editors. Early Visual Development, Normal and Abnormal. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. pp. 391–408.
-
Improving the performance of the amblyopic visual systemPhilosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 364:399–407.https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0203
-
Short-term monocular deprivation alters early components of visual evoked potentialsThe Journal of Physiology 593:4361–4372.https://doi.org/10.1113/JP270950
-
A cycling lane for brain rewiringCurrent Biology 25:R1122–R1123.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.10.026
-
Selective reconfiguration of layer 4 visual cortical circuitry by visual deprivationNature Neuroscience 7:1353–1359.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1351
-
Multiple modes of network homeostasis in visual cortical layer 2/3Journal of Neuroscience 28:4377–4384.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5298-07.2008
-
Effects of early visual deprivation on perceptual and cognitive developmentFrom Action to Cognition 164:87–104.
-
ConferenceHomeostatic Plasticity in Primary Visual Cortex Affects Local Sleep Expression: A High-Density EEG Study11th FENS - Forum of Neuroscience.
-
Neural mechanisms of recovery following early visual deprivationPhilosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 364:383–398.https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0192
-
Improving vision: neural compensation for optical defocusProceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 265:71–77.https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.1998.0266
-
Feature Detection in Human Vision: A Phase-Dependent Energy ModelProceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 235:221–245.https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.1988.0073
-
Neural compensation for long-term asymmetric optical blur to improve visual performance in keratoconic eyesInvestigative Opthalmology & Visual Science 51:3835–3839.https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.09-4558
-
Distinctive features of adult ocular dominance plasticityJournal of Neuroscience 28:10278–10286.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2451-08.2008
-
Plasticity of the visual cortex and treatment of amblyopiaCurrent Biology 24:R936–R940.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2014.05.063
-
Sensitivity to optic flow in human cortical areas MT and MSTEuropean Journal of Neuroscience 23:561–569.https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04526.x
-
Sleep slow oscillation and plasticityCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology 44:116–126.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2017.03.019
-
Neural bases of binocular rivalryTrends in Cognitive Sciences 10:502–511.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.09.003
-
Homeostatic synaptic plasticity: local and global mechanisms for stabilizing neuronal functionCold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Biology 4:a005736.https://doi.org/10.1101/cshperspect.a005736
-
Homeostatic plasticity in the developing nervous systemNature Reviews Neuroscience 5:97–107.https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1327
-
GABA shapes the dynamics of bistable perceptionCurrent Biology 23:823–827.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.03.067
-
Spatial Modulation Transfer in the Human EyeJournal of the Optical Society of America 57:401–406.https://doi.org/10.1364/JOSA.57.000401
-
Plasticity and stability of visual field maps in adult primary visual cortexNature Reviews Neuroscience 10:873–884.https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2741
-
Perceptual learning: toward a comprehensive theoryAnnual Review of Psychology 66:197–221.https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015214
-
Amblyopia: prevalence, natural history, functional effects and treatmentClinical and Experimental Optometry 88:365–375.https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1444-0938.2005.tb05102.x
-
Effects of visual deprivation on morphology and physiology of cells in the cats lateral geniculate bodyJournal of Neurophysiology 26:978–993.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1963.26.6.978
-
Real-time modulation of perceptual eye dominance in humansProceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 281:20141717.https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2014.1717
-
Chromatic and achromatic monocular deprivation produce separable changes of eye dominance in adultsProceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 284:20171669.https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2017.1669
Article and author information
Author details
Funding
European Research Council (ERC ECSPLAIN 338866)
- Paola Binda
- Jan W Kurzawski
- Maria Concetta Morrone
H2020 European Institute of Innovation and Technology (NextGenVis 641805)
- Jan W Kurzawski
Ministero dell’Istruzione, dell’Università e della Ricerca (PRIN2015)
- Claudia Lunghi
- Maria Concetta Morrone
European Research Council (ERA-NET Neuro-DREAM)
- Claudia Lunghi
- Maria Concetta Morrone
Fondazione Roma
- Maria Concetta Morrone
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Acknowledgements
This research was supported by the European Research Council under the European Union’s Seventh Framework Programme (FPT/2007–2013) under grant agreement number 338866 (PB, CL, and MCM) and under ERA-NET project ‘Neuro-DREAM’ (CL, and MCM) and by the European Union’s Horizon 2020 Research and Innovation Programme under the Marie Sklodowska-Curie grant agreement number 641805 (JWK) and by the Italian Ministry of University and Research under the project PRIN2015 (MCM) and by Fondazione Roma under the Grants for Biomedical Research: Retinitis Pigmentosa (RP)-Call for proposals 2013- ‘Cortical Plasticity in Retinitis Pigmentosa: an Integrated Study from Animal Models to Humans’. The authors would like to thank Mauro Costagli for help with the data acquisition, and David C Burr for his comments on the manuscript.
Ethics
Human subjects: Experimental procedures are in line with the declaration of Helsinki and were approved by the regional ethics committee [Comitato Etico Pediatrico Regionale-Azienda Ospedaliero-Universitaria Meyer-Firenze (FI)] and by the Italian Ministry of Health, under the protocol 'Plasticità e multimodalità delle prime aree visive: studio in risonanza magnetica a campo ultra alto (7T)'.
Copyright
© 2018, Binda et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 2,483
- views
-
- 380
- downloads
-
- 66
- citations
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Neuroscience
Spinal cord interneurons play critical roles shaping motor output, but their precise identity and connectivity remain unclear. Focusing on the V1 interneuron cardinal class we defined four major V1 subsets in the mouse according to neurogenesis, genetic lineage-tracing, synaptic output to motoneurons, and synaptic inputs from muscle afferents. Sequential neurogenesis delineates different V1 subsets: two early born (Renshaw and Pou6f2) and two late born (Foxp2 and Sp8). Early born Renshaw cells and late born Foxp2-V1 interneurons are tightly coupled to motoneurons, while early born Pou6f2-V1 and late born Sp8-V1 interneurons are not, indicating that timing of neurogenesis does not correlate with motoneuron targeting. V1 clades also differ in cell numbers and diversity. Lineage labeling shows that the Foxp2-V1 clade contains over half of all V1 interneurons, provides the largest inhibitory input to motoneuron cell bodies, and includes subgroups that differ in birthdate, location, and proprioceptive input. Notably, one Foxp2-V1 subgroup, defined by postnatal Otp expression, is positioned near the LMC and receives substantial input from proprioceptors, consistent with an involvement in reciprocal inhibitory pathways. Combined tracing of ankle flexor sensory afferents and interneurons monosynaptically connected to ankle extensors confirmed placement of Foxp2-V1 interneurons in reciprocal inhibitory pathways. Our results validate previously proposed V1 clades as unique functional subtypes that differ in circuit placement, with Foxp2-V1 cells forming the most heterogeneous subgroup. We discuss how V1 organizational diversity enables understanding of their roles in motor control, with implications for their diverse ontogenetic and phylogenetic origins.
-
- Neuroscience
Monitoring neuronal activity at single-cell resolution in freely moving Drosophila engaged in social behaviors is challenging because of their small size and lack of transparency. Extant methods, such as Flyception, are highly invasive. Whole-brain calcium imaging in head-fixed, walking flies is feasible but the animals cannot perform the consummatory phases of social behaviors like aggression or mating under these conditions. This has left open the fundamental question of whether neurons identified as functionally important for such behaviors using loss- or gain-of-function screens are actually active during the natural performance of such behaviors, and if so during which phase(s). Here, we perform brain-wide mapping of active cells expressing the Immediate Early Gene hr38 using a high-sensitivity/low background fluorescence in situ hybridization (FISH) amplification method called HCR-3.0. Using double-labeling for hr38 mRNA and for GFP, we describe the activity of several classes of aggression-promoting neurons during courtship and aggression, including P1a cells, an intensively studied population of male-specific interneurons. Using HI-FISH in combination with optogenetic activation of aggression-promoting neurons (opto-HI-FISH), we identify candidate downstream functional targets of these cells in a brain-wide, unbiased manner. Finally, we compare the activity of P1a neurons during sequential performance of courtship and aggression, using intronic vs. exonic hr38 probes to differentiate newly synthesized nuclear transcripts from cytoplasmic transcripts synthesized at an earlier time. These data provide evidence suggesting that different subsets of P1a neurons may be active during courtship vs. aggression. HI-FISH and associated methods may help to fill an important lacuna in the armamentarium of tools for neural circuit analysis in Drosophila.