Vestigial auriculomotor activity indicates the direction of auditory attention in humans

  1. Daniel J Strauss  Is a corresponding author
  2. Farah I Corona-Strauss
  3. Andreas Schroeer
  4. Philipp Flotho
  5. Ronny Hannemann
  6. Steven A Hackley
  1. Systems Neuroscience and Neurotechnology Unit, Faculty of Medicine, Saarland University & School of Engineering, htw saar, Germany
  2. Audiological Research Unit, Sivantos GmbH, Germany
  3. Clinical and Cognitive Neuroscience Laboratory, Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri, United States

Abstract

Unlike dogs and cats, people do not point their ears as they focus attention on novel, salient, or task-relevant stimuli. Our species may nevertheless have retained a vestigial pinna-orienting system that has persisted as a 'neural fossil’ within in the brain for about 25 million years. Consistent with this hypothesis, we demonstrate that the direction of auditory attention is reflected in sustained electrical activity of muscles within the vestigial auriculomotor system. Surface electromyograms (EMGs) were taken from muscles that either move the pinna or alter its shape. To assess reflexive, stimulus-driven attention we presented novel sounds from speakers at four different lateral locations while the participants silently read a boring text in front of them. To test voluntary, goal-directed attention we instructed participants to listen to a short story coming from one of these speakers, while ignoring a competing story from the corresponding speaker on the opposite side. In both experiments, EMG recordings showed larger activity at the ear on the side of the attended stimulus, but with slightly different patterns. Upward movement (perking) differed according to the lateral focus of attention only during voluntary orienting; rearward folding of the pinna’s upper-lateral edge exhibited such differences only during reflexive orienting. The existence of a pinna-orienting system in humans, one that is experimentally accessible, offers opportunities for basic as well as applied science.

eLife digest

Dogs, cats, monkeys and other animals perk their ears in the direction of sounds they are interested in. Humans and their closest ape relatives, however, appear to have lost this ability. Some humans are able to wiggle their ears, suggesting that some of the brain circuits and muscles that allow automatic ear movements towards sounds are still present. This may be a ‘vestigial feature’, an ability that is maintained even though it no longer serves its original purpose.

Now, Strauss et al. show that vestigial movements of muscles around the ear indicate the direction of sounds a person is paying attention to. In the experiments, human volunteers tried to read a boring text while surprising sounds like a traffic jam, a baby crying, or footsteps played. During this exercise, Strauss et al. recorded the electrical activity in the muscles of their ears to see if they moved in response to the direction the sound came from. In a second set of experiments, the same electrical recordings were made as participants listened to a podcast while a second podcast was playing from a different direction. The individuals’ ears were also recorded using high resolution video.

Both sets of experiments revealed tiny involuntary movements in muscles surrounding the ear closest to the direction of a sound the person is listening to. When the participants tried to listen to one podcast and tune out another, they also made ear ‘perking’ movements in the direction of their preferred podcast.

The results suggest that movements of the vestigial muscles in the human ear indicate the direction of sounds a person is paying attention to. These tiny movements could be used to develop better hearing aids that sense the electrical activity in the ear muscles and amplify sounds the person is trying to focus on, while minimizing other sounds.

Introduction

Watching the ears allows an equestrian to gauge their mount’s shifting attention. Ear movements are not a useful cue in humans or apes because higher primates have lost the ability to orient by adjusting pinna shape and focal direction. Instead we judge a person’s attention by their gaze direction. In thousands of research reports each year, though, casual observation of ocular orienting is replaced by sophisticated recording techniques. We show in the present paper that similar electrical and optical techniques allow us to extract muscular correlates of pinna-orienting in our species and even render subtle pinna-orienting movements visible. Activation of the ear muscles is directionally specific and it occurs during voluntary as well as reflexive attention.

A review of research in Hackley, 2015 on pinna-orienting in humans identified three relevant findings scattered across the preceding 100-or-so years. The first was Wilson’s oculo-auricular phenomenon (Wilson, 1908), in which shifting the gaze hard to one side elicits a 1 to 4 mm deflection of the lateral rim of both ears. The relevance to spatial attention is uncertain, though, with diverging results across studies, for example see Gerstle and Wilkinson, 1929; Urban et al., 1993; O'Beirne and Patuzzi, 1999. Additional evidence comes from a 1987 study (Hackley et al., 1987) of the bilateral postauricular muscle (PAM) reflex (onset latency = 10 ms) to acoustic onset transients. Increased amplitudes were observed when subjects directed their attention to a stream of tones on the same side as the recorded muscle while ignoring a competing, contralateral stream. Comparisons across left/right stimulus, attention, and PAM combinations localized modulation to the motor limb of the reflex arc. This pattern could indicate that the muscle behind an ear is primed when attention is directed toward that side. Finally, an experiment in Stekelenburg and van Boxtel, 2002 found that the automatic capture of attention by unexpected sounds coming from a speaker hidden to the left of the participant elicited greater activity in the left than right PAM.

Apart from the research just described, functional studies of the human auriculomotor system have been mainly limited to the PAM reflex, in the context of audiometry or affective psychophysiology. The auriculomotor system lies essentially untouched in the literature. Here we present evidence that our brains retain vestigial circuitry for orienting the pinnae during both exogenous, stimulus-driven attention to brief, novel sounds and endogenous, goal-directed attention to sustained speech. We also demonstrate a complex interplay of different auricular muscles which may be causally linked to subtle movements of the pinnae.

Results

Experiment 1 - Exogenous attention

To examine automatic, stimulus-driven attention we used novel sounds similar to those in the Stekelenburg and van Boxtel, 2002 study, for example traffic jam, baby crying, footsteps. However, we presented them randomly from four different speakers (at ± 30°, ± 120°; Figure 1) rather than just one, while the subject read a boring essay. As we were interested in the interactive role of distinct muscles in attempting to shape and point the pinnae, we recorded EMG from posterior, anterior, superior, and transverse auricular muscles (PAM, AAM, SAM, and TAM). Visual evidence had previously been limited to still photos of Wilson’s oculo-auricular phenomenon (Wilson, 1908), so we supplemented our EMG data with videos from four high-definition cameras, see Methods and Video 1, Video 2, and Video 3. To confirm that our findings would generalize to different age groups, older (62.7 ± 5.9 y) as well as younger (24.1 ± 3.1 y) adults were tested.

Experimental setup.

(A) Four loudspeakers presented novel sounds (Exp. 1) or stories (Exp. 2) at 30° to the left or right of fixation or behind the interaural axis. Instructions, text, or fixation cross was displayed on a 55 in flat screen. (B) Surface EMGs were recorded bilaterally from four auricular muscles as well as from left zygomaticus major, frontalis, and sternocleidomastoideus, using a bandpass of 10 – 1000 Hz and a sampling rate of 9600 Hz. Separation of paired auricular electrodes was 1 cm.

Video 1
Experiment 1 –Ear movement example from a trial with a novel sound at the right posterior speaker in Experiment 1.

The right half of the display portrays evoked movements of the ipsilateral pinna in three ways. The large video clip of the pinna uses digital magnification to render the overall pattern of movement apparent. The color overlay in these videos indicates the motion magnitude. Just below the video and to the right, an unrectified EMG recording of the postauricular muscle is shown in co-registration with the video. The global head motion was reduced by a 2-dimensional rigid pre-registration with respect to a set of manually specified reference points on the head (see also Figure 2—figure supplement 1). The 3-dimensional graph medial to the 2-D graph includes a vector that indicates moment-by-moment changes in EMG activity of the superior auricular muscle (SAM, the vertical axis), transverse auricular muscle (TAM, a horizontal axis), and the difference between activity in the posterior and anterior auricular muscles (PAM-AAM, the other horizontal axis). The left half of this video gives corresponding information for the contralateral ear which, consistent with evidence presented in the main text, was not as active as the ipsilateral one.

Video 2
Experiment 1 –The right ear example from the previous video, but with four different videos in sequence.

The first video of the sequence shows the raw recording (without digital magnification). The second video shows the digitally magnified motion, the third video shows the magnified motion with color overlay as in the previous video supplement, and the fourth video shows the three dimensional motion from a different angle. This video sequence shows the impact of the digital motion magnification and the depth information about ear motion that can be derived from a stereo computer vision setup such as the one used here.

Video 3
Experiment 2 –Ear movement example from a participant who exhibited exceptionally large, long-lasting involuntary auricular muscle activations and ear motion during the endogenous attention task in Experiment 2.

The attention of the participant was directed to the story played from the posterior right speaker. The organization of the plots and co-registration is as in Video 1. However, this time the raw videos without digital magnification are shown. The raw videos are played faster, time-locked to the time axis given in minutes in the one dimensional plots of the rectified postauricular muscle activity. Note that time-axis reflects the entire timeline including the instructions and the introduction to the stories before the directional listening task. The listening task started at approximately 2 min. The video also documents the end of the listening task (around 7 min) accompanied with a time–locked offset of the muscle activation and pinna displacement. A causal relation of the rectified postauricular muscle activity and the motion magnitude in the videos is clearly noticeable, especially for the ipsilateral ear.

The signal-averaged EMG waveforms of Figure 2 show well-defined responses with an onset latency of about 70 ms, responses that vary in amplitude, duration, and morphology according to the relative direction of the sound source. The inter- and intra-subject variability for the analyzed auricular muscles is shown in Figure 3 and Figure 4, respectively. These plots portray the consistency of the PAM, AAM, and TAM responses across stimuli and subjects, especially for stimulation from the back. For the statistical analysis, mean amplitudes were subjected to a mixed, repeated-measures analysis of variance, with factors of age group, stimulus-muscle correspondence (ipsi-/contralateral), and anterior/posterior stimulus direction. EMG amplitudes were larger for stimulus sources on the same side as the recorded ear for PAM, AAM, and TAM [F(1,26) = 47.44, 17.01, and 47.53, respectively; p-values < 0.001; ηp2 = 0.65, 0.40, and 0.65] but not SAM.

Figure 2 with 1 supplement see all
Experiment 1.

Grand average (N = 28) of the baseline corrected and normalized event-related electromyograms at the four auricular muscles for the recordings ipsilateral (left panel) and contralateral (right panel) to stimulation; top: front speakers (30°), bottom: back speakers (120°). The contralateral-ipsilateral organization of our data set is justified by a preliminary analysis that obtained null effects for left–versus–right using a more complete factorial structure (left/right stimulus direction × left/right recording site). The following figure supplement is available for Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Analysis of video recordings from one participant who exhibited submillimeter pinna displacements in response to stimulation from the back speakers. This figure supplement is complemented by Video 1 and Video 2.

Figure 3 with 3 supplements see all
Experiment 1 – Responses of the PAM to stimuli from the back speakers, showing intersubject variability: Top panels: Every row corresponds to the averaged response of one participant.

Amplitude is encoded in color. The top rows (1-16) represent younger adult participants; the bottom rows (17-28), older adults. Bottom panels: Mean and standard deviation based on the above plots. The following figure supplements are available for Figure 3—figure supplement 1. The described intersubject variability analysis for the AAM, Figure 3—figure supplement 2. SAM, and Figure 3—figure supplement 3. TAM.

Figure 4 with 3 supplements see all
Experiment 1 – Intrasubject variability of the PAM: Mean and standard deviations of the phasic responses 50 - 300 ms) of every participant.

Top panels: responses to the front speakers. Bottom panels: responses to the back speakers. Left panels: Responses of the younger adults. Right panels: responses of the older adults. Blue represents ipsilateral responses, red represents contralateral responses. The following figure supplements are available for Figure 4—figure supplement 1. The described intrasubject variability analysis for the AAM, Figure 4—figure supplement 2. SAM, and Figure 4—figure supplement 3. TAM.

Responses were also larger to sounds emanating from the back than the front speakers for PAM, AAM, and TAM [F(1,26) = 32.1, 12.0, and 19.9, respectively; p-values < 0.003, ηp2 = 0.55, 0.32, and 0.43]. Posterior, ipsilateral stimulation elicited the most vigorous responses from these three muscles. In particular, the interaction between the factors ipsi/contralateral and anterior/posterior for PAM, AAM, and TAM yields F(1,26) = 40.4, 14.9, and 23.6, respectively; p-values < 0.002; ηp2 = 0.61, 0.36, and 0.48. There were no interactions involving age group, but a main effect indicated that older participants had smaller AAM responses [F(1,26) = 6.0, p < 0.03, ηp2 = 0.19].

These results support the hypothesis that the human brain retains circuits that attempt to point the ears in the direction of unexpected, potentially relevant sounds. The corresponding vestigial auriculomotor drive appears to be causally linked to very small ear displacements, see Figure 2—figure supplement 1, Video 1, and Video 2. Having documented the existence of directionally-appropriate responses of the ear muscles to brief novel sounds, we turn now to a qualitatively distinct type of attention.

Experiment 2 – Endogenous Attention

To examine voluntary, goal-directed attention we used the classic, dichotic-listening paradigm, see Hillyard et al., 1973; Cherry, 1953. Two competing short stories were played either over the two front speakers or the two back speakers. To increase motivation participants were allowed to choose, after a brief introduction, which of the two stories (podcasts) they would like to listen to. They were then told which speaker that story would be presented from. Our subjects were instructed to listen carefully while looking at a fixation cross and, as in the immediately preceding study, holding their head still on a chin rest. Upon completion, a new story was picked and the listening direction was switched to one of the other speakers. Recording methods were identical to those of the exogenous experiment. Muscle activity was quantified as the mean of the absolute EMG energy over the entire course of each 5 min listening trial in Figure 5 and for consecutive segments of 10 s duration in a temporal analysis shown in Figure 6.

Figure 5 with 2 supplements see all
Experiment 2.

Grand average of the PAM, AAM, and SAM activity when stories were played from the front (top) and back speakers (bottom). Shown is the normalized (total) energy of the left/right recording channels during attention to the left or right story (bars represent the standard error). The following figure supplements are available for Figure 5—figure supplement 1. Subband analysis of the described ipsi- vs. contralateral effect for PAM, AAM, and SAM; Figure 5—figure supplement 2. Reported results for a selected narrow frequency band.

Experiment 2.

Time–resolved activity (each sampling point represents the energy induced in consecutive 10 s segments) after pooling the ipsi– and contralateral signals with a segment–wise normalization for the front (top) and back speakers (bottom).

As in the exogenous study, EMG energy at PAM and AAM was largest on the side to which attention was focused [analysis corresponding to Figure 5: F(1, 19) = 15.2 and 4.6, respectively; p= 0.001 and 0.04; ηp2 = 0.44 and 0.20]; an effect that is particularly strong in narrow–band middle frequency components of the signal, see Figure 5—figure supplements 1 and 2 and the tables in Supplementary file 1.

A different pattern emerged for the other two muscles. Whereas TAM but not SAM activity had reflected lateralization of transient, exogenous attention, the reverse was true for sustained, goal-directed attention. That is to say, mean EMG energy at SAM was larger at the ipsi– than contralateral ear [F(1, 19) = 16.3; p=0.001ηp2 = 0.46] in Experiment 2, but there was no such difference for TAM.

Another main effect indicated that activation of all four muscles was generally enhanced when participants listened to one of the two speakers that were slightly behind as opposed to in front of them [PAM, AAM, TAM, SAM: F(1, 19) = 5.7, 3.1, 8.1, and 12.0, respectively; p= 0.03, 0.09, 0.01, and 0.003; ηp2 = 0.23, 0.14, 0.30, and 0.39]. These effects did not interact with each other or with age. Although PAM activity declines over time, EMG energy of all three muscles is clearly sustained across the 5-min sessions, see Figure 6. A corresponding sustained deflection of the pinna is also noticeable in the co-registered Video 3.

Potential motor confounds

An alternative to the account we have been developing is that participants in Experiments 1 and 2 may have shifted their gaze toward the attended source. This would have then triggered Wilson, 1908 phenomenon, that is auriculomotor activity secondary to large gaze shifts. To test this hypothesis, we segmented the horizontal electrooculogram (EOG) in the same way as the auricular EMG. Voltages were converted to degrees of arc separately for each participant, based on findings from a cursor tracking protocol (± 35°). Figure 7 and Figure 8 document a complete absence of eye movements that were systematically related to attention direction. A limitation of these findings is that electro-oculographic recordings have a resolution of only 1 − 2°. However, gaze shifts less than 30° are rarely accompanied by auriculomotor activity, see Urban et al., 1993. Representative examples of macrosaccades during reading in Experiment one with co-registered auricular muscle activity can be found in Figure 7—figure supplement 1. There is no obvious linkage of saccades and PAM responses. Note that the mean visual angle range observed in this example generalizes across subjects, see Figure 7—figure supplement 2. Also when considering all the macrosaccades from all the subjects in Experiment 1, our data do not exhibit a regularity between auditory stimuli and macrosaccades, see Figure 7—figure supplement 3.

Figure 7 with 9 supplements see all
Experiment 1: Averaged horizontal EOG activity at around the time of stimulation from the back speakers, showing an apparent absence of systematic shifts in gaze direction: Top panels: Every row corresponds to the averaged response of one participant.

Gaze angle is encoded in color, such that positive values (yellow) indicate rightward eye movements/positive angles. The top rows (1-16) represent younger adult participants; the bottom rows (17-28), older adults. Bottom panels: Mean and standard deviation based on the above plots.The following figure supplements are available for Figure 7—figure supplement 1. Macrosaccades during reading for one subject as example; Figure 7—figure supplement 2. Boxplots of the EOG for all subjects; Figure 7—figure supplement 3. Density of all detected macrosaccades during Experiment 1; Figure 7—figure supplement 4. Responses of the M. sternocleidomastoideus to stimuli from the front speakers; Figure 7—figure supplement 5. Responses of the M. sternocleidomastoideus to stimuli from the back speakers; Figure 7—figure supplement 6. Responses of the M. frontalis to stimuli from the front speakers; Figure 7—figure supplement 7. Responses of the M. frontalis to stimuli from the back speakers; Figure 7—figure supplement 8. Responses of the M. zygomaticus to stimuli from the front speakers; Figure 7—figure supplement 9. Responses of the M. zygomaticus to stimuli from the back speakers.

Figure 8 with 2 supplements see all
Experiment 2: Intrasubject variability of the horizontal EOG: Mean and standard deviations of the EOG during the complete trial of every participant.

Top panels: attending the front speakers. Bottom panels: attending the back speakers. Left panels: younger adults. Right panels: older adults. Blue represents the EOG when attending the left, red when attending the right speaker. A positive EOG indicates that the gaze is directed toward the side of the attended speaker. Note that the deviation of the mean from 0 is well within one standard deviation and therefore indicates that participants did not systematically divert their gaze to the attended speaker. The following figure supplements are available for Figure 8—figure supplement 1. Time-resolved EOG analysis in Experiment 2; Figure 8—figure supplement 2. Activity of the frontalis and zygomaticus muscle in Experiment 2.

Another line of evidence that the auricular responses observed in our study were not secondary to eye movements, concerns their pattern of lateralization. Activation of TAM during Wilson’s oculo-auricular phenomenon is more vigorous on the side opposite the direction of gaze, see Gerstle and Wilkinson, 1929; Urban et al., 1993. By contrast, we found in Experiment one that TAM activation was relatively enhanced at the ear on the same side as the attention-engaging sounds. The PAM component of Wilson's phenomenon does exhibit enhanced activity on the ipsilateral side, but this effect appears to be reliable only for gaze shifts greater than about 40 degrees (Patuzzi and O'Beirne, 1999, Figure 4).

Another alternative interpretation is that participants oriented not with their ears or eyes, but by lifting their chin from the chin rest and rotating their head toward the attended sound. If humans have a vestibulo-auricular response as do cats (Tollin et al., 2009), such head rotations could have indirectly triggered activity in the ear muscles. However, recent research has shown that azimuthal head rotations have little effect on auricular activity in humans, see Cook and Patuzzi, 2014. Moreover, analysis of sternocleidomastoid EMG in our data suggest that movements of the neck were rare, small, and unsystematic, see Figure 7—figure supplements 4 and 5. An additional statistical analysis in Supplementary file 1 also rejects an influence of the sternocleidomastoid EMG and horizontal EOG. Finally, head rotations would have been too slow to generate the rapid responses of around 70 ms onset latency observed in the ear muscles in Experiment 1. We note with interest, though, the possibility that subtle, covert activation of head turning muscles (Corneil et al., 2008) might be correlated with ocular and auricular orienting. Note that there was also no corresponding co–activation of the other measured (non–auricular) facial muscles, the zygomaticus and frontalis muscle, see Figure 7—figure supplements 69 for Experiment one and Figure 8—figure supplement 2 for Experiment 2.

Discussion

These data provide compelling evidence that our brains retain, in vestigial form, circuitry for orienting the pinnae during both exogenous and endogenous modes of attention. The neural drive to our ear muscles is so weak that the actual movements (see co-registered video data in the Supplementary Information) are at least one to two orders of magnitude smaller compared to those generated during biting, smiling, grimacing, or voluntary ear-wiggling. To understand what remains of the vestigial pinna-orienting system so as to exploit it for practical or scientific purposes it is helpful to take a comparative, phylogenetic approach (Hackley, 2015, Hackley et al., 2017).

Vestigial Pinna-Orienting

The ability to swivel and point the pinnae seems to have been lost during the transition from the primarily nocturnal lifestyles of prosimians to the diurnal ones of New World monkeys, and then, Old World monkeys (Coleman and Ross, 2004). Mobility continued to decline as the ears became shorter and more rigid, see Waller et al., 2008; Coleman and Ross, 2004. The musculature degenerated. For example, an inferior auricular muscle to oppose SAM still exists in lesser apes such as gibbons and siamangs (Burrows et al., 2011), but not in chimpanzees (Burrows et al., 2011) or humans (Cattaneo and Pavesi, 2014). Given that head rotation has little effect on PAM activity (Cook and Patuzzi, 2014), it seems likely that the vestibulo–auricular reflex as documented in cats (Tollin et al., 2009) has not been conserved in our species. Also presumably lost is the ability to use proprioceptive information to adjust auditory processing in accordance with pinna position, orientation, and shape as documented in cats, see Kanold and Young, 2001. Although the ear muscles of Old World monkeys have spindles (Lovell et al., 1977), those of humans do not (Cattaneo and Pavesi, 2014).

When pinna-orienting movements became too small to modify acoustic input substantially, possibly 25 million years ago when lesser apes branched off from Old World monkeys, see Gibbs et al., 2007 as discussed in Hackley, 2015, selective environmental pressure ceased. The neural system became more-or-less 'frozen’ in a form optimized for controlling taller, more flexible ears, mounted on a smaller, more spherical head. This evolutionary perspective helps us to understand the surprising finding that AAM, which pulls the base of the pinna forward, was activated in Experiment one by novel sounds coming from the rear. Co-activation of opposing muscles AAM and PAM in our remote ancestors would have reduced occlusion of the ear canal by the tragus. Note that this occlusion occurs in monkeys when contraction of the PAM homolog is unopposed, see Waller et al., 2008, supplementary video clip 17. In addition, PAM-AAM co-activation would have stabilized the base of the pinna and reduced myotendinous elasticity, thereby allowing quick changes in position or orientation. This perspective also illuminates our unexpected finding of ipsilateral SAM suppression in Experiment 1. A study of pinna orienting in cats, whose tall ears resemble those of prosimians, showed that they tend to tilt the ear downward slightly when orienting to a lateral target, see Populin and Yin, 1998.

Potential neural mechanisms

Neurobiologists have distinguished two types of pinna-orienting movements in cats, based on onset latency, see Siegmund and Santibáñez, 1982. The short-latency response is specific to auditory stimuli and is chronometrically uncorrelated with saccades toward the target. By contrast, the long-latency response can be elicited by visual as well as auditory stimuli and it is roughly synchronous with ocular orienting, see Populin and Yin, 1998. Using a 4-speaker set-up similar to that of the present Experiment 1, Siegmund and Santibáñez, 1982 found cats’ unconditioned pinna responses to have an EMG onset latency that averaged 78 ms, similar to our value of about 70 ms. The animals were then trained to make gaze shifts toward the sound sources. Onset latency of the auriculomotor responses dropped to a remarkable 29 ms and the responses were resistant to extinction over the course of 125 trials. Both findings were replicated by Populin and Yin, 1998 (mean = 26 ms; failure to extinguish across 10,000 unreinforced trials). The latter authors obtained an even more rapid response (mean = 21 ms) when the sound was preceded by a visual stimulus that served as warning signal and indicated that the cat should maintain gaze at a fixation point. They argued that the short-latency pinna response is too rapid to be mediated by the brain region most centrally involved in orienting, the superior colliculus (SC). This is because an earlier study, Populin and Yin, 1997, had found the average first-spike latency in the relevant portion of this structure to be 19 ms.

Comparisons with these cat studies suggest that our participants' auriculomotor responses may have been primarily also of the short-latency variety that is not mediated by the SC. Two of the conditions tested by Populin and Yin, 1998 involved brief, lateralized auditory stimuli that, as in the present Experiment 1, were not task-relevant. The stimuli elicited short-latency ipsilateral pinna movements that were temporally uncorrelated with gaze shifts (see their Figures 6 and 8). During the delayed-saccade condition of their study, laterally presented sounds were task-relevant and forward fixation was required, as in our Experiment 2. Ipsilateral pinna movements triggered by onset of these sounds were of the short-latency variety (21 ms, as noted above). Subsequent, smaller movements were then observed in synchrony with ocular orienting, roughly 400 ms after the fixation point was extinguished (Figure 7). It is long-latency pinna movements of this sort that Populin and Yin, 1998 suggested might be mediated by the SC.

Given the major role of the SC in controlling eye fixation (Krauzlis et al., 2017), this structure may also be responsible for sustained maintenance of pinna orientation, such as in Experiment 2. Pinna movements can be triggered by electrical stimulation of the deep and intermediate layers of the SC in accordance with a topographical pattern that is in register with that of eye movements (Stein and Clamann, 1981). Lesions of this structure reduce the likelihood of pinna orienting as well as its accuracy, see Czihak et al., 1983. Although monosynaptic connections from SC to auriculomotor neurons in the facial nucleus do exist (Vidal et al., 1988), pinna control is dominated by disynaptic pathways from the SC that include the paralemniscal, oculomotor, or pontine reticular zones, see Henkel and Edwards, 1978; Takeuchi et al., 1979; Vidal et al., 1988. Among these, the paralemniscal zone appears to be the most important, and its auditory input originates in the nearby nucleus sagulum, see Henkel, 1981.

Portions of neocortex also play a role in controlling pinna movements. Lesions of auditory cortex reduce the kinematic complexity of pinna orienting and slow its habituation, see Alvarado and Santibañez, 1971. Stimulation and recording studies in the macaque have identified a premotor ear-eye field (area 8B), which is connected with both auditory cortical areas and the SC, see Lanzilotto et al., 2013. These animal neuroanatomy and physiology studies, coupled with Wilson, 1908 seminal report, make it clear that the eyes and pinnae work together during endogenously cued attentional orienting.

Future work

A recent study in humans by Gruters et al., 2018, showed a close relationship between movements of the left and right eardrums and multiple parameters of task-related left- and right-directed eye movements. It will be important in future research to test whether muscular responses of the middle and outer ears are linked in a coordinated manner to ocular orienting. Furthermore, exploration of the relationship between human auriculomotor activity and subtle markers of covert attention (see the recent review given in van Ede et al., 2019) corticofugal modulation of ascending auditory pathways (Perrot et al., 2006) in endogenous attention, and the neural mechanisms of orienting discussed in the preceding section has scarcely begun.

Our results have implications for applied science, as well. They suggest that patterns of auricular muscle activity might serve as an easily accessible correlate of top-down processing in endogenous modes of attention. As such, the described effects might complement electroencephalographic indices of attentional focus (de Cheveigné et al., 2018; Schäfer et al., 2018) in that their sensitivity is exclusively spatial, rather than reflecting a context-specific mixture of modality, feature, location, and object representations. Registration of pinna-orienting might better support near real-time decoding of the attentional focus and, as compared to EEG-based stimulus reconstruction approaches, does not require the exogenous sound source, for example see the discussion in Schäfer et al., 2018. Thus, auricular muscle monitoring might support the decoding of auditory attention in technical applications such as attentionally controlled hearing aids that preferentially amplify sounds the user is attempting to listen to. We wish to underscore, though, that the development of such applications would benefit crucially from a better understanding of how the auditory and visual attention systems interact. We hope that the results presented here will stimulate research in this direction.

Materials and methods

Participants

Both older (N = 12, mean age = 62.7 ± 5.9 y, 8 F, all right-handed) and younger adult (N = 16, mean age = 24.1 ± 3.1 y, 8 F, 15 right-handed, one left-handed) volunteers in Experiment one had age-typical, pure tone audiometric thresholds (1, 2, 4, and 8 kHz; young < 20 dB; old < 40 dB). All served in both studies, but after the 8th participant, Experiment two was altered in several ways (e.g., four stimulus directions rather than two). Only data from the final 21 subjects were retained for Experiment 2. The two groups in this experiment comprised 11 older adults (mean age = 62.6 ± 6.2 y, 8 F, all right-handed) and 10 younger adult (mean age = 24.1 ± 3.6 y, 5 F, nine right-handed, one left-handed). After a detailed explanation of the procedure, all subjects signed a consent form. The study was approved by the responsible ethics committee (ethics commission at the Ärztekammer des Saarlandes, Saarbrücken, Germany; Identification Number: 79/16).

Stimuli and tasks

Request a detailed protocol

The four active loudspeakers (KH120A, Neumann, Germany) were positioned at head level, 115 cm. Sounds in Experiment 1 and 2 were reproduced with a soundcard (Scarlett 18i20, Focusrite, UK). The experimental paradigms were programmed using software for scientific computing (Matlab, Mathworks, USA) and Psychtoolbox 3. In Experiment 1, sounds lasted 1.7 – 10.0 s, were delivered every 15 – 40 s, and had an average intensity of 70 dBC, except for foot steps (65 dBC). Each of the nine stimuli (lemur howling, dog barking, helicopter flying, cell phone vibrating, birds singing, baby crying, mosquito buzzing, footsteps, and traffic jam) was repeated four times (i.e., once per speaker). In Experiment 2, the stories were 5 min long, with an average intensity of 50 dBA for younger and 60 dBA for older participants. Participants answered content questions at the conclusion of each condition in this experiment.

Electrophysiological recordings and signal processing

Surface EMGs were recorded with non-recessed, Ag/AgCl electrodes (BME4, BioMed Electrodes, USA), which were 4 mm in diameter for TAM and 6 mm (BME6) in all other cases, see Figure 1. The signals were AD-converted at 9600 Hz and 24 bit resolution per channel (4 × USBamp, g.tec GmbH, Austria). Skin temperature, skin resistance, electrocardiograms, and EOGs were also recorded. All signal processing algorithms were implemented using the scientific computing software Matlab (Mathworks, USA, Version: 2018a). Because surface electrodes had not previously been used to record from intrinsic ear muscles, we conducted preliminary tests with a participant who exhibited a large, reliable Wilson’s phenomenon and who could voluntarily contract her SAM and PAM. Isolation of the corresponding responses indicated that EMG from TAM electrodes was not an artifact of volume conduction from PAM or SAM. In other words, the TAM activity was not correlated with forced SAM/PAM innervation. Sternocleidomastoid EMG signals were zero-phase bandpass filtered from 60 to 1000 Hz (FIR, 2000th order), the auricular EMG signals from 10 to 1000 Hz (FIR, 2000th order) with a notch filter at 50 Hz (IIR, 2nd order). Horizontal EOG signals were zero-phase filtered from 0.01 to 20 Hz (IIR, 2nd order). All filter operations were performed using Matlab’s filtfilt-function for zero-phase filtering. The filtered signals were then downsampled to 2400 Hz for further processing. The statistical analysis was performed using repeated measures ANOVA (with IBM SPSS Statistics 26). Within-subjects factors were stimulus-muscle correspondence (ipsi- vs. contralateral responses) and anteriority (front vs. back speakers). The only between-subjects factor our statistical model accounted for was age and, in association with that, also stimulus level in the endogenous experiment. Other factors like head-size, audiogram shape or small electrode placement differences were not included in the model. All main and interaction effects were tested.

Exogeneous (transient) data

Request a detailed protocol

Root-mean-square (RMS) envelopes of the filtered and downsampled EMG signals were calculated with a sliding window (step size = 1 sample, window length = 150 samples/62.5 ms.) The data were then segmented into epochs extending from 3 s prior to stimulus onset until 3 s following termination of the auditory stimulus, which was of variable duration. Epochs were baseline corrected with respect to the mean RMS envelope amplitude of the pre-stimulus interval, that is this mean was subtracted from the epoch. For every participant, normalization was performed within every monitored auricular muscle (e.g., left PAM, right SAM) and for a specific stimulus type (e.g., traffic jam). Since every stimulus type was repeated four times, the largest voltage in the corresponding four epochs was used for normalization. The reference value in each case was the largest voltage at any time point within the four relevant epochs (e.g., the four directions/trials with a traffic jam stimulus) in the EMG recordings of that particular muscle. Each participant’s data were pooled according to whether the side of the stimulus and recorded muscle did or did not match, and then were averaged into contralateral and ipsilateral waveforms. Eighteen trials contributed to each of these per subject contralateral and ipsilateral waveforms, nine from the left speaker and nine from the right. Mean amplitudes were computed across a measurement window extending from 100 to 1500 ms following stimulus onset and were then subjected to statistical analysis.

Endogenous (sustained) data

Request a detailed protocol

Artifacts of the filtered and downsampled EMG data were reduced by averaging the signal energy of 1 s, non-overlapping segments and rejecting segments that deviated by more than two standard deviations from the mean. For each participant, the mean energy of a given channel during the four listening conditions (left/right × front/back) was calculated and then normalized to the largest value across the 5-min run. These normalized data were then averaged into ipsilateral/contralateral categories and subjected to statistical analysis.

EMG time-frequency decomposition (for Figure 5—figure supplements 1 and 2)

Request a detailed protocol

As the rather sustained muscle activity during endogenous attention might be reflected in low frequency components according to convolution models of the EMG (Farina et al., 2014), filtered and downsampled EMG signals in Experiment two were decomposed into eight frequency bands by a nonsubsampled octave-band filter bank (5th order Daubechies filter). Each frequency band was then further processed in the same fashion as the broadband signals reported in the main text.

Computer vision setup and motion analysis (for Videos 1, 2 and 3)

Request a detailed protocol

Videos were acquired using four Ximea MQ022CG–CM color sensors with a resolution of 1936 × 1216 at 120 frames per second and an exposure of 2 ms. Two cameras were positioned on each side of the head and focused on the ears to record pairwise stereo videos. We used hardware triggering for all four cameras and recorded each camera onto a separate m.2 solid-state-drive to reduce frame loss. We used a KOWA 35 mm macro lens with an aperture of F0.4 which gave us a close-up view of the ear with acceptable depth of field to allow slight movements towards the camera and enough distance such that the cameras did not cast shadows on to the scene. We illuminated the face uniformly with flicker-free LED studio illumination. The cameras were calibrated with the stereo camera calibrator app from the Mathworks Matlab Computer Vision System Toolbox. Calibration was performed whenever camera adjustment required re-alignment of relative stereo camera positions or a change of focus of one of the cameras.

For 3D reconstruction and motion visualization/quantification, we used functions from the Mathworks Matlab Computer Vision System Toolbox and custom written code. Our analysis system was able to reduce redundancies in optic flow and stereo depth estimation by exploiting the unilateral scene composition and limited degrees of freedom for ear and head movements. For 3D reconstructions, we initialized a sequence with one initial estimation of disparity and subsequently tracked points independently for the left and right image sequence.

We tracked points with respect to the first frame of the sequence as reference frame with dense optical flow initialized with a rigid motion estimation. Motion was visualized with a Lagrangian motion magnification approach that had a constant magnification factor with respect to the reference frame and prior removal of affine motion with respect to manually selected stable points. The results of the motion analysis with and without magnification can be seen in the videos.

Data availability

The code with and data are available from GitHub https://github.com/a-schroeer/ExogEndogEarProject (copy archived at https://github.com/elifesciences-publications/ExogEndogEarProject) and the Dryad Digital Repository https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.d4md86r, respectively.

The following data sets were generated
    1. Strauss DJ
    2. Corona-Strauss FI
    3. Schroeer A
    4. Flotho P
    5. Hannemann R
    6. Hackley SA
    (2020) Dryad Digital Repository
    Data from: Vestigial auriculomotor activity indicates the direction of auditory attention in humans.
    https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.d4md86r

References

    1. Alvarado M
    2. Santibañez G
    (1971)
    Targeting reflex: some features and inhibition targeting reflex: some features and inhibition
    Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis 31:33–45.
    1. Czihak E
    2. Santibañez M
    3. Klimann M
    4. Santibañez G
    (1983)
    Audio-visual reaction after unilateral lesions of the superior colliculus in cats
    Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis 43:15–25.
    1. Gibbs RA
    2. Rogers J
    3. Katze MG
    4. Bumgarner R
    5. Weinstock GM
    6. Mardis ER
    7. Remington KA
    8. Strausberg RL
    9. Venter JC
    10. Wilson RK
    11. Batzer MA
    12. Bustamante CD
    13. Eichler EE
    14. Hahn MW
    15. Hardison RC
    16. Makova KD
    17. Miller W
    18. Milosavljevic A
    19. Palermo RE
    20. Siepel A
    21. Sikela JM
    22. Attaway T
    23. Bell S
    24. Bernard KE
    25. Buhay CJ
    26. Chandrabose MN
    27. Dao M
    28. Davis C
    29. Delehaunty KD
    30. Ding Y
    31. Dinh HH
    32. Dugan-Rocha S
    33. Fulton LA
    34. Gabisi RA
    35. Garner TT
    36. Godfrey J
    37. Hawes AC
    38. Hernandez J
    39. Hines S
    40. Holder M
    41. Hume J
    42. Jhangiani SN
    43. Joshi V
    44. Khan ZM
    45. Kirkness EF
    46. Cree A
    47. Fowler RG
    48. Lee S
    49. Lewis LR
    50. Li Z
    51. Liu YS
    52. Moore SM
    53. Muzny D
    54. Nazareth LV
    55. Ngo DN
    56. Okwuonu GO
    57. Pai G
    58. Parker D
    59. Paul HA
    60. Pfannkoch C
    61. Pohl CS
    62. Rogers YH
    63. Ruiz SJ
    64. Sabo A
    65. Santibanez J
    66. Schneider BW
    67. Smith SM
    68. Sodergren E
    69. Svatek AF
    70. Utterback TR
    71. Vattathil S
    72. Warren W
    73. White CS
    74. Chinwalla AT
    75. Feng Y
    76. Halpern AL
    77. Hillier LW
    78. Huang X
    79. Minx P
    80. Nelson JO
    81. Pepin KH
    82. Qin X
    83. Sutton GG
    84. Venter E
    85. Walenz BP
    86. Wallis JW
    87. Worley KC
    88. Yang SP
    89. Jones SM
    90. Marra MA
    91. Rocchi M
    92. Schein JE
    93. Baertsch R
    94. Clarke L
    95. Csürös M
    96. Glasscock J
    97. Harris RA
    98. Havlak P
    99. Jackson AR
    100. Jiang H
    101. Liu Y
    102. Messina DN
    103. Shen Y
    104. Song HX
    105. Wylie T
    106. Zhang L
    107. Birney E
    108. Han K
    109. Konkel MK
    110. Lee J
    111. Smit AF
    112. Ullmer B
    113. Wang H
    114. Xing J
    115. Burhans R
    116. Cheng Z
    117. Karro JE
    118. Ma J
    119. Raney B
    120. She X
    121. Cox MJ
    122. Demuth JP
    123. Dumas LJ
    124. Han SG
    125. Hopkins J
    126. Karimpour-Fard A
    127. Kim YH
    128. Pollack JR
    129. Vinar T
    130. Addo-Quaye C
    131. Degenhardt J
    132. Denby A
    133. Hubisz MJ
    134. Indap A
    135. Kosiol C
    136. Lahn BT
    137. Lawson HA
    138. Marklein A
    139. Nielsen R
    140. Vallender EJ
    141. Clark AG
    142. Ferguson B
    143. Hernandez RD
    144. Hirani K
    145. Kehrer-Sawatzki H
    146. Kolb J
    147. Patil S
    148. Pu LL
    149. Ren Y
    150. Smith DG
    151. Wheeler DA
    152. Schenck I
    153. Ball EV
    154. Chen R
    155. Cooper DN
    156. Giardine B
    157. Hsu F
    158. Kent WJ
    159. Lesk A
    160. Nelson DL
    161. O'brien WE
    162. Prüfer K
    163. Stenson PD
    164. Wallace JC
    165. Ke H
    166. Liu XM
    167. Wang P
    168. Xiang AP
    169. Yang F
    170. Barber GP
    171. Haussler D
    172. Karolchik D
    173. Kern AD
    174. Kuhn RM
    175. Smith KE
    176. Zwieg AS
    177. Rhesus Macaque Genome Sequencing and Analysis Consortium
    (2007) Evolutionary and biomedical insights from the rhesus macaque genome
    Science 316:222–234.
    https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1139247
    1. Krauzlis RJ
    2. Goffart L
    3. Hafed ZM
    (2017) Neuronal control of fixation and fixational eye movements
    Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 372:20160205.
    https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2016.0205
  1. Book
    1. Populin LC
    2. Yin TCT
    (1997)
    Sensitivity of auditory cells in the superior colliculus to eye position in the behaving cat
    In: Palmer A. R, Rees A, Summerfield A, Meddis R, editors. Psychophysical and Physiological Advances in Hearing. London: Whurr. pp. 441–448.
    1. Siegmund H
    2. Santibáñez G
    (1982)
    Effector pattern of the audio-visual targeting reflex in cats
    Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis 42:311–326.
    1. Wilson SAK
    (1908)
    A note on an associated movement of the eyes and ears in man
    Review of Neurology and Psychiatry 6:331–336.

Article and author information

Author details

  1. Daniel J Strauss

    Systems Neuroscience and Neurotechnology Unit, Faculty of Medicine, Saarland University & School of Engineering, htw saar, Homburg/Saar, Germany
    Contribution
    Conceptualization, Resources, Formal analysis, Supervision, Validation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing - original draft, Project administration, Writing - review and editing
    For correspondence
    daniel.strauss@uni-saarland.de
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0001-8481-499X
  2. Farah I Corona-Strauss

    Systems Neuroscience and Neurotechnology Unit, Faculty of Medicine, Saarland University & School of Engineering, htw saar, Homburg/Saar, Germany
    Contribution
    Conceptualization, Software, Formal analysis, Supervision, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
  3. Andreas Schroeer

    Systems Neuroscience and Neurotechnology Unit, Faculty of Medicine, Saarland University & School of Engineering, htw saar, Homburg/Saar, Germany
    Contribution
    Software, Formal analysis, Validation, Investigation, Visualization
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0002-7904-3622
  4. Philipp Flotho

    Systems Neuroscience and Neurotechnology Unit, Faculty of Medicine, Saarland University & School of Engineering, htw saar, Homburg/Saar, Germany
    Contribution
    Data curation, Software, Formal analysis, Validation, Investigation, Visualization
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
  5. Ronny Hannemann

    Audiological Research Unit, Sivantos GmbH, Erlangen, Germany
    Contribution
    Conceptualization, Methodology
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
  6. Steven A Hackley

    Clinical and Cognitive Neuroscience Laboratory, Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri, Columbia, United States
    Contribution
    Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing - original draft, Writing - review and editing
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared

Funding

Bundesministerium für Bildung und Forschung (BMBF-FZ No. 03FH004IX5)

  • Daniel J Strauss

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Acknowledgements

This study was partially supported by the German Federal Ministry of Education and Research, Grant No. BMBF-FZ 03FH004IX5 (PI: DJS). We thank Larissa Arand for assistance with data collection and Becca Sullinger for the artwork in Figure 1. We acknowledge support by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, German Research Foundation) and Saarland University within the funding programme Open Access Publishing.

Ethics

Human subjects: The study was approved by the responsible ethics committee (ethics commission at the Ärztekammer des Saarlandes, Saarbrücken, Germany; app. number 79/16) After a detailed explanation of the procedure, all subjects signed a consent form.

Version history

  1. Received: January 24, 2020
  2. Accepted: May 28, 2020
  3. Version of Record published: July 3, 2020 (version 1)

Copyright

© 2020, Strauss et al.

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.

Metrics

  • 20,686
    Page views
  • 278
    Downloads
  • 14
    Citations

Article citation count generated by polling the highest count across the following sources: Crossref, PubMed Central, Scopus.

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Daniel J Strauss
  2. Farah I Corona-Strauss
  3. Andreas Schroeer
  4. Philipp Flotho
  5. Ronny Hannemann
  6. Steven A Hackley
(2020)
Vestigial auriculomotor activity indicates the direction of auditory attention in humans
eLife 9:e54536.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.54536

Share this article

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.54536

Further reading

    1. Neuroscience
    Lei Yang, Jingtao Zhang ... Chen Zhang
    Tools and Resources Updated

    Synapse is the fundamental structure for neurons to transmit information between cells. The proper synapse formation is crucial for developing neural circuits and cognitive functions of the brain. The aberrant synapse formation has been proved to cause many neurological disorders, including autism spectrum disorders and intellectual disability. Synaptic cell adhesion molecules (CAMs) are thought to play a major role in achieving mechanistic cell-cell recognition and initiating synapse formation via trans-synaptic interactions. Due to the diversity of synapses in different brain areas, circuits and neurons, although many synaptic CAMs, such as Neurexins (NRXNs), Neuroligins (NLGNs), Synaptic cell adhesion molecules (SynCAMs), Leucine-rich-repeat transmembrane neuronal proteins (LRRTMs), and SLIT and NTRK-like protein (SLITRKs) have been identified as synaptogenic molecules, how these molecules determine specific synapse formation and whether other molecules driving synapse formation remain undiscovered are unclear. Here, to provide a tool for synapse labeling and synaptic CAMs screening by artificial synapse formation (ASF) assay, we generated synaptotagmin-1-tdTomato (Syt1-tdTomato) transgenic mice by inserting the tdTomato-fused synaptotagmin-1 coding sequence into the genome of C57BL/6J mice. In the brain of Syt1-tdTomato transgenic mice, the tdTomato-fused synaptotagmin-1 (SYT1-tdTomato) signals were widely observed in different areas and overlapped with synapsin-1, a widely-used synaptic marker. In the olfactory bulb, the SYT1-tdTomato signals are highly enriched in the glomerulus. In the cultured hippocampal neurons, the SYT1-tdTomato signals showed colocalization with several synaptic markers. Compared to the wild-type (WT) mouse neurons, cultured hippocampal neurons from Syt1-tdTomato transgenic mice presented normal synaptic neurotransmission. In ASF assays, neurons from Syt1-tdTomato transgenic mice could form synaptic connections with HEK293T cells expressing NLGN2, LRRTM2, and SLITRK2 without immunostaining. Therefore, our work suggested that the Syt1-tdTomato transgenic mice with the ability to label synapses by tdTomato, and it will be a convenient tool for screening synaptogenic molecules.

    1. Neuroscience
    Daniel R Schonhaut, Aditya M Rao ... Michael J Kahana
    Research Article Updated

    Memory formation depends on neural activity across a network of regions, including the hippocampus and broader medial temporal lobe (MTL). Interactions between these regions have been studied indirectly using functional MRI, but the bases for interregional communication at a cellular level remain poorly understood. Here, we evaluate the hypothesis that oscillatory currents in the hippocampus synchronize the firing of neurons both within and outside the hippocampus. We recorded extracellular spikes from 1854 single- and multi-units simultaneously with hippocampal local field potentials (LFPs) in 28 neurosurgical patients who completed virtual navigation experiments. A majority of hippocampal neurons phase-locked to oscillations in the slow (2–4 Hz) or fast (6–10 Hz) theta bands, with a significant subset exhibiting nested slow theta × beta frequency (13–20 Hz) phase-locking. Outside of the hippocampus, phase-locking to hippocampal oscillations occurred only at theta frequencies and primarily among neurons in the entorhinal cortex and amygdala. Moreover, extrahippocampal neurons phase-locked to hippocampal theta even when theta did not appear locally. These results indicate that spike-time synchronization with hippocampal theta is a defining feature of neuronal activity in the hippocampus and structurally connected MTL regions. Theta phase-locking could mediate flexible communication with the hippocampus to influence the content and quality of memories.