The olfactory critical period is determined by activity-dependent Sema7A/PlxnC1 signaling within glomeruli
Abstract
In mice, early exposure to environmental odors affects social behaviors later in life. A signaling molecule, Semaphorin 7A (Sema7A), is induced in the odor-responding olfactory sensory neurons. Plexin C1 (PlxnC1), a receptor for Sema7A, is expressed in mitral/tufted cells, whose dendrite-localization is restricted to the first week after birth. Sema7A/PlxnC1 signaling promotes post-synaptic events and dendrite selection in mitral/tufted cells, resulting in glomerular enlargement that causes an increase in sensitivity to the experienced odor. Neonatal odor experience also induces positive responses to the imprinted odor. Knockout and rescue experiments indicate that oxytocin in neonates is responsible for imposing positive quality on imprinted memory. In the oxytocin knockout mice, the sensitivity to the imprinted odor increases, but positive responses cannot be promoted, indicating that Sema7A/PlxnC1 signaling and oxytocin separately function. These results give new insights into our understanding of olfactory imprinting during the neonatal critical period.
Introduction
Mammalian sensory systems are generated by a combination of activity-dependent and -independent processes. The basic architecture of sensory systems is built before birth based on a genetic program (Hong and Luo, 2014; Luo et al., 2008; Yogev and Shen, 2014). However, the sensory maps and neural circuits are further refined in an activity-dependent manner (Espinosa and Stryker, 2012; Hensch, 2005; Hooks and Chen, 2007; Kirkby et al., 2013; Okawa et al., 2014). In the mouse olfactory system, a coarse map is generated independently from neuronal activity (Mori and Sakano, 2011). Axon targeting along the dorsal-ventral (D-V) axis is regulated by positional information of olfactory sensory neurons (OSNs) (Takeuchi et al., 2010). In contrast, targeting along the anterior-posterior (A-P) axis is instructed by odorant receptor (OR) molecules using cAMP as a second messenger (Imai et al., 2006). Each OR species generates a specific level of cAMP by intrinsic, non-neuronal OR-activity (Nakashima et al., 2013) that determines expression levels of A-P targeting molecules including Semaphorin (Sema) 3A, Neuropilin (Nrp) 1, and Plexin (Plxn) A1 (Imai et al., 2009). The map is then refined by axon-sorting molecules, for example, Kirrel 2/3 and Ephrin A/eph A receptors (Serizawa et al., 2006) whose expression is regulated by neuronal activity using cAMP generated by olfactory G protein (Golf) and cyclic-nucleotide-gated (CNG) channels (Sakano, 2010). It has been reported that this activity for the map refinement is intrinsic and not odor-evoked (Nakashima et al., 2019). Intrinsic OSN activity is also needed for synapse formation within glomeruli. Inoue et al., 2018 found that odor-evoked OSN activity further promotes synapse formation and dendrite selection in mitral/tufted (M/T) cells using Sema7A/PlxnC1 signaling.
In neonates, there is a narrow time frame, referred to as the critical period, that allows proper development in response to environmental inputs. This activity-dependent process is initially plastic but soon becomes irreversible. If the circuit is left unstimulated, the brain function served by that circuit is impaired. Thus, sensory inputs during the critical period are important to make the system functional. Unlike other sensory systems, the olfactory system constantly regenerates OSNs throughout the animal’s life span (Costanzo, 1991; Graziadei and Monti Graziadei, 1985; Leung et al., 2007). Although OSNs are replaced and form new connections with second-order neurons, proper circuits cannot be regenerated once the existing OSNs are completely ablated after the early neonatal period. This time frame of olfactory circuit formation has been referred to as the olfactory critical period (Ma et al., 2014; Tsai and Barnea, 2014). However, the olfactory critical period has not been defined at the molecular level, and the key process that makes this time-window critical has yet to be clarified.
In mammals, neonatal exposure to environmental odorants can affect odor perception and behavior later in life (Logan et al., 2012; Mennella et al., 2001; Sullivan et al., 2000; Wilson and Sullivan, 1994). Salmon and trout return to their home river based on olfactory memory (Nevitt et al., 1994). In the visual system, ducklings follow the first moving object upon hatching recognizing it as the parent bird (Horn, 1998; Nakamori et al., 2013). Although such phenomena are widely known, little is known about how imprinting is established and how the imprinted memory modulates innate behavioral decisions.
In an effort to address these unanswered questions about the olfactory critical period, we studied olfactory imprinting in mice by analyzing a pair of signaling molecules, Sema7A and PlxnC1, which induces post-synaptic events within glomeruli. We found that Sema7A/PlxnC1 signaling enlarges glomeruli resulting in increased sensitivity to experienced odors. Neonatal odor experience also induces positive responses to the imprinted odor memory. Separately from Sema7A/PlxnC1 signaling, oxytocin in neonates is responsible for imposing the positive quality on imprinted memory.
Results
Naris occlusion/reopen experiments
In order to precisely determine the critical period that causes plastic changes in synapse formation and odor detection, we performed naris occlusion/reopen experiments (Figure 1A and Figure 1—figure supplement 1). Unilateral naris occlusion at postnatal day 0 (P0) was continued through various time points, and resultant synapse markers within glomeruli were examined at P21. We found that the levels of a pre-synapse marker vGlut2 (Honma et al., 2004) and a post-synapse marker GluR1 (Montague and Greer, 1999), were normal when the occluded naris was reopened at P6 or before. However, both markers decreased and remained low when the occluded naris was reopened after P8.
We then studied the effect of naris occlusion on odor detection in the habituation/dishabituation test (Figure 1B). The ability to sense odors using the occluded naris was tested by plugging the non-occluded naris at the 6th week of life (6w). When the occluded naris was reopened at P6, mice demonstrated normal abilities to detect odor information. In contrast, when the occlusion was continued to P10, the mice demonstrated reduced responsiveness to odorants and poor ability to discriminate enantiomers, (+) and (–) carvones (CARs). Taken together, the critical period in the mouse olfactory system appears to be restricted to the early neonatal period, approximately during the first week after birth.
Odor perception is specifically affected by the odorants exposed as neonates
In order to study whether neonatal odor experience can affect odor perception, we exposed pups to a particular odorant and analyzed their responses as adults. In the habituation/dishabituation test at 6w, the mice conditioned to vanillin (VNL) at P2~4 demonstrated increased responsiveness/sensitivity to VNL (Figure 2). Furthermore, unlike the unconditioned control, the VNL-conditioned mice showed lasting interests for VNL and were not fully habituated when VNL was presented a second time. Such conditioning effects were neither found in mice conditioned to VNL at P9~11, nor observed toward other unconditioned odorants, for example, eugenol (EUG).
Odor exposure promotes dendrite selection resulting in glomerular enlargement
As neonatal odor experience affects odor perception in adults, we examined whether any changes could be found in the glomeruli in response to odor exposure. We focused on an OR, MOR29A (also known as Olfr1510), whose ligand is known as VNL (Tsuboi et al., 2011). This is because the onset of its expression is relatively early during olfactory development and its intrinsic level of Sema7A is low compared with other OR species. Exposure to VNL at P2~4 increased Sema7A expression (Figure 3A and Figure 3—figure supplement 1A), which promotes post-synaptic events and dendrite selection in mitral/tufted (M/T) cells (Inoue et al., 2018). VNL stimulation also increased the levels of synaptic markers, vGlut2 and GluR1 (Figure 3B left and Figure 3—figure supplement 1B). We then examined dendrite maturation by injecting Lucifer yellow into the glomeruli to visualize a single connecting M/T cell (Figure 3—figure supplement 2). The selection of primary dendrites was promoted within the MOR29A glomeruli (Figure 3B right). In this experiment, M/T cells with a selected primary-dendrite were classified as mature, whereas those with multiple unselected dendrites in the glomerular layer were classified as immature.
We found that sizes of the MOR29A glomeruli increased with a ligand odor as previously reported for another OR, M72 (also known as Olfr160) (Liu et al., 2016; Todrank et al., 2011). When the pups were exposed to VNL at P2~4, the numbers of VNL-responsive cells surrounding the MOR29A glomeruli increased at P21 (Figure 3C left and Figure 3—figure supplement 1C), resulting in the enlargement of MOR29A glomeruli (Figure 3D). Interestingly, the number of MOR29A-expressing OSNs remained the same (Figure 3C right). These changes were not found when VNL was exposed at P7~9 or P9~11.
It is notable that in the olfactory bulb (OB) of naris-occluded mice, the levels of synaptic markers in the MOR29A glomeruli were lowered (Figure 3—figure supplement 3 left) and the glomerular sizes for MOR29A was reduced (Figure 3—figure supplement 3 middle). Furthermore, VNL-responding tufted cells and periglomerular cells connecting to the MOR29A glomeruli were reduced (Figure 3—figure supplement 3 right). These changes were observed when occlusion was performed at P0~4, but not at P7~11 (Figure 3—figure supplement 3 right). The time frame that allows for activity-dependent changes appears to be restricted to the early neonatal period, roughly during the first week after birth. It should be noted that the critical period as determined by naris occlusion, represents the maximum time frame covering for all OR species. In contrast, in the odor exposure experiment, the critical period is determined for a specific OR species and can be different among various ORs depending upon the onset of their expression.
Sema7A/PlxnC1 signaling defines the olfactory critical period
We next proceeded to study the key process that causes the neonatal time frame to become critical. Our previous study (Inoue et al., 2018) indicated that post-synaptic events and dendrite selection in M/T cells are regulated by a pair of signaling molecules, Sema7A (Pasterkamp et al., 2003) and its receptor PlxnC1 (Tamagnone et al., 1999) expressed in OSN axons and M/T-cell dendrites, respectively. We analyzed the cell-type-specific conditional knockout (cKO) of PlxnC1 for synapse formation. The M/T-cell-specific cKO was generated using the Pcdh21 promoter for the Cre driver. We also analyzed the Sema7A total knockout (KO) in parallel. It was found that post-synaptic density (PSD) is rarely formed at P5, however, targeting of OSN axons is not affected in these KOs (Inoue et al., 2018).
Expression of the Sema7a gene appears to be regulated in an activity-dependent manner, as Sema7A is diminished in the KO of CNG channels (Brunet et al., 1996) that generate OR-specific OSN activity (Figure 4A left and Figure 4—figure supplement 1A left). Supporting this observation, the mutant OR defective in G-protein coupling (Imai et al., 2006), also diminished Sema7a expression (Figure 4A right and Figure 4—figure supplement 1A right). Individual glomeruli possess unique but different levels of Sema7A expression, determined by the intrinsic activity of ORs, thereby forming the glomerular rank of Sema7A. Sema7A expression increased with a ligand odor and the glomerular rank of MOR29A became higher for Sema7A by VNL exposure (Figure 4B). In contrast to Sema7A, PlxnCl expression or its localization to the M/T-cell dendrites was not affected in the KO of Sema7A or CNG-A2 (Figure 4C and Figure 4—figure supplement 1B). Interestingly, however, its localization to the tuft structure of M/T cells was restricted to the first week after birth (Inoue et al., 2018; Figure 4D). Thus, Sema7A expression in OSNs supports the activity dependency, and PlxnC1 localization in M/T-cell dendrites limits the time frame of plastic changes within glomeruli during the olfactory critical period.
Sema7A is sufficient to induce post-synaptic events in M/T cells
We wanted to determine whether there are any additional components for synapse formation, whose expression is downstream of OR-specific OSN activity. To address this question, we performed rescue experiments in the CNG-A2 KO. In the KO mice, synapse formation is significantly affected (Figure 5—figure supplement 1). Since the Cnga2 gene is located on chromosome X, duplicated glomeruli are formed in the hemizygous KO (Zhao and Reed, 2001) due to stochastic X-chromosome inactivation. In the CNG-negative glomeruli of rI7 (also known as Lofr226), both pre- and post-synaptic markers decreased and primary-dendrite selection was delayed in contrast to the CNG-positive glomeruli (Figure 5A). We then introduced the transgenic (Tg) Sema7a gene with the activity-independent promotor of MOR23 (also known as Olfr16) into the Cnga2+/- female mice with the Sema7A KO background. When the rI7 glomeruli were analyzed, constitutive expression of the Tg Sema7a alone restored synapse formation and dendrite selection in the CNG-negative glomeruli (Figure 5B). Compared with the endogenous Sema7A in the WT mice, expression levels of Tg-Sema7A were approximately 10% within the rI7 glomeruli in the Cnga2+/- mice with the Sema7A KO background (Figure 5—figure supplement 2A). The mutant Sema7A (Y213S) (Liu et al., 2010), which is incapable of interacting with PlxnC1, did not rescue the KO phenotype of the CNG channels (Figure 5C and Figure 5—figure supplement 2B). We, therefore, conclude that Sema7A-PlxnC1 interaction is sufficient to induce the activity-dependent post-synaptic events in M/T cells.
Blockage of Sema7A/PlxnC1 signaling affects social responses later in life
We then studied how mice are affected when Sema7A/PlxnC1 signaling is blocked. Since the Sema7A KO is a total KO and would not be appropriate for behavioral analyses (Carcea et al., 2014), we chose the M/T-cell-specific cKO of PlxnC1. Absence of Plxnc1 expression was confirmed by in situ hybridization and immunostaining in the PlxnC1 cKO (Inoue et al., 2018). Similar to naris-occluded mice (Figure 1B), odor detection was lowered (Figure 6A), but not entirely abolished because the cKO mice remained attracted to food smells (Figure 6B). It appears that the basic olfactory circuitry is established by intrinsic OSN activity separately from odor-evoked activity (Gire et al., 2012).
In the PlxnC1 cKO, social responses in adults were perturbed. While male mice normally demonstrate strong curiosity for unfamiliar mouse scents of both genders, the cKO showed avoidance behavior toward them (Figure 6B). A similar impairment in social interactions was observed in the three-chamber test, a standard method to detect autism spectrum disorders (ASD) in rodents (Moy et al., 2004). In the three-chambered box, the mouse being tested was placed in a center chamber, an unfamiliar mouse in a plastic cage was to the left, and an empty cage was to the right (Figure 6C). Time duration spent in each room was measured during a 15-min test period. Although the WT mouse spent most of its time in the left chamber inspecting the stranger, the PlxnC1 cKO spent longer times in the right chamber, thereby avoiding social interactions with the unfamiliar mouse. These results indicate that Sema7A/PlxnC1 signaling is involved in establishing positive imprinted memory in neonates, whose defect causes the impairment of social responses in adulthood. While the cKO mice fail to demonstrate positive interest toward the odor of unfamiliar mice, they are still able to detect food smells.
Mice demonstrate stress responses in an unfamiliar environment. When they are transferred to a new cage, the rectal temperature rises and remains elevated for about 40 min (Spooren et al., 2002). However, in the mice conditioned to VNL at P2~4, stress was eased by VNL at 6w (Figure 6D). We tested propionic acid (PPA) as a positive control, whose quality is innately attractive (Figure 6D). PPA lowers the stress, but its effect is more moderate compared with the imprinted VNL whose quality is innately neutral. It is notable that such a conditioning effect was not observed in the mice exposed to VNL after the critical period at P9~11. In the PlxnC1 cKO, stress was not eased by the conditioned odor VNL in the hyperthermia test (Figure 6D).
Oxytocin in neonates is needed for smooth social interactions in adults
Increased sensitivity is a form of imprinted memory that can be established at the level of the glomeruli, and its blockage in the PlxnC1 cKO can be explained by the failure of odor-evoked synapse formation. However, lasting interest (Figure 2) or easing of stress (Figure 6D) is another example of conditioned memory that needs to be further formed in the central brain; a neuronal level not affected by the PlxnC1 cKO. How is it then that the perceptive change takes place for the exposed odors? What is responsible for establishing smooth social interactions by imposing the positive quality on imprinted odor memory? We therefore, turned our attention to various neuronal hormones, including norepinephrine, dopamine, and oxytocin; all of which are known to induce positive mental status and mediate attractive social behaviors (Andersson, 2000; de Wied et al., 1993). Among them, oxytocin appeared to be promising to us because it is highly expressed in the neonatal brain (Sannino et al., 2017) and promotes attractive social interactions (Bosch and Young, 2018; Insel, 2010). Furthermore, it has been reported that the KO of oxytocin (Nishimori et al., 1996) or its receptor (Takayanagi et al., 2005) affects social interactions and causes ASD-like responses. We, therefore, studied whether oxytocin is needed in neonates by the rescue experiment with the oxytocin KO (Smith et al., 2019).
To examine a possible effect of neonatal oxytocin on smooth social interactions later in life, we analyzed the oxytocin-administrated KO mice for social memory. In the oxytocin KO, increase in the sensitivity to the imprinted odor can still be seen (Figure 7A) and Sema7A expression is not affected by the KO (Figure 7—figure supplement 1). However, the positive quality is not imposed on the imprinted odor, as observed by loss of lasting interest (Figure 7A) and stress-reducing effect in the hyperthermia test (Figure 7B). In the social memory test, male mice demonstrate a characteristic decline in the time spent investigating an ovariectomized female during repeated pairings (Ferguson et al., 2000). In the male KO of oxytocin, time duration of sniffing did not decrease in the subsequent presentations of the same female mouse.
We then injected oxytocin into the KO pups by intraperitoneal administration (Mizuno et al., 2015) and tested their social memory after 10w. In the KO male treated with oxytocin at P0~6, sniffing times progressively decreased for the subsequent presentations (trials 1–5) of the same female mouse, but increased for a newly introduced female (trial 6) (Figure 8A). This rescue effect was not observed when the oxytocin was administrated after the critical period, at P8~14 (Figure 8B). Taken together, oxytocin in early neonates appears to be needed for smooth social interactions as adults by imposing positive quality on imprinted odor memory. Thus, imprinting (increase in the sensitivity) and positive memory formation are separately regulated by Sema7A/PlxnC1 signaling and oxytocin, respectively, during the neonatal critical period.
Discussion
During development, an olfactory map is generated by a combination of A-P and D-V targeting using distinct sets of axon guidance molecules (Nishizumi and Sakano, 2020). These processes are independent from the neuronal activity of OSNs. The map is then refined by glomerular segregation molecules (Serizawa et al., 2006) whose expression is regulated by the intrinsic neuronal activity reported by Reisert, 2010 (Nakashima et al., 2019). It appears that the intrinsic activity also mediates synapse formation within glomeruli. The olfactory map is further modified by the odor-evoked activity in neonates by enlarging the responsive glomeruli. This process is plastic and mediated by Sema7A/PlxnC1 signaling during the critical period. Activity-dependent Sema7A expression in OSNs triggers post-synaptic events in M/T cells via PlxnC1 (Inoue et al., 2018). Our present study demonstrates that Sema7A/PlxnC1 signaling is key to imprinting the neonatal odor experience during the critical period in mice. Rescue experiments with the Tg Sema7a gene in the CNG KO indicate that Sema7A is sufficient to promote activity-dependent synapse formation and dendrite selection within glomeruli. In M/T cells, PlxnC1 is localized to the dendrites only during the first week after birth. Thus, Sema7A supports the activity-dependency of plastic changes within glomeruli and PlxnC1 limits the olfactory critical period to the early neonatal stage. It will be interesting if we can see shifts in the time window for the olfactory critical period by changing the time frame of PlxnC1 localization in the M/T-cell dendrites.
Sema7A expression increases in the responding glomeruli when newborns are exposed to a particular odorant. As a result, post-synaptic events are enhanced in the connecting M/T cells and dendrite selection is promoted. Unstimulated dendrites that connect other glomeruli are likely to be removed by synaptic competition. Thus, the number of primary dendrites increases in the stimulated glomerulus causing it to become larger. A specific set of enlarged glomeruli may be recognized as an imprinted odor code in adults. Sema7A/PlxnC1 signaling appears to have a prime role in adapting pups to environmental odorants and generating imprinted memory with lasting effects later in life. How does this imprinting occur in the context of circuit formation for adaptive odor responses? It has been reported that postnatal odor exposure heightens odor-evoked mitral-cell responses (Liu and Urban, 2017). It is likely that the enlarged glomeruli generate stronger signals of exposed odorants, which is then transmitted to the olfactory cortex, increasing the possibility that imprinted memory is established in neonates.
KO studies indicate that oxytocin is involved in imposing the positive quality on imprinted odor memory and is necessary for smooth social interactions. In order to determine when oxytocin is needed to achieve normal social skills, we attempted the rescue experiment for the oxytocin KO by intraperitoneal injection of oxytocin in neonates. Defective phenotypes of the KO mice were partly restored by this treatment when oxytocin was administrated during the critical period. The establishment of smooth social interactions by imprinted memory may be associated with a pleasant mental state in the nursed neonates. It is notable that in the oxytocin KO, Sema7A expression is not affected and the sensitivity to the imprinted odor still increases. Thus, imprinting and positive memory formation are independently regulated by Sema7A/PlxnC1 signaling and oxytocin, respectively, during the olfactory critical period. There are three major molecules involved in olfactory imprinting during the critical period. They are Sema7A which supports activity-dependency of the imprinting process, PlxnC1 which restricts the time frame of the critical period to the first week after birth, and oxytocin which is responsible for imposing the positive quality on imprinted memory.
It has been reported that early exposure to environmental odors affects social behavior later in life (Logan et al., 2012; Mennella et al., 2001; Sullivan et al., 2000; Wilson and Sullivan, 1994). Our present study suggests that odor-induced Sema7A/PlxnC1 signaling in neonates is essential for establishing odor imprinting, whose impairment cannot be recovered in adults. Male mice normally demonstrate strong curiosity toward unfamiliar mice of both genders. If Sema7A/PlxnC1 signaling is blocked during the critical period, the mice do not respond in their usual manner but demonstrate avoidance response to the stranger’s scent. Correlating to this observation, there is a report showing that micro-deletion in the human chromosome 15q24 covering the Sema7a locus is associated with ASD (McInnes et al., 2010). It is well known that social stress induces the release of adrenocorticotropic hormone (Backström and Winberg, 2013). We assume that mice innately avoid stressful interactions with unfamiliar mice. However, imprinting mediated by Sema7A/PlxnC1 signaling may be necessary for the pups to adapt to their community. It will be interesting to study whether similar neonatal imprinting as that described in the mouse olfactory system can be found in humans, not only in the olfactory system, but also in other sensory systems. It is also important to determine when the critical period starts and how long it lasts in humans.
Our present study demonstrates that the imprinted odor induces positive responses even when its quality is innately aversive. In such a situation, where the hard-wired decision is modified by imprinting to elicit the final response, how does the amygdala arbitrate the conflicting decisions, one which is innate and the other which is modified by imprinted memory? In order to reconcile the two decisions, there may be a cross talk between the positive and negative valence cells in the amygdala (Mori and Sakano, 2021). These studies will give new insights into our understanding of decision making in humans and will shed light on the neurodevelopmental disorders, such as ASD and attachment disorders, that may be caused by improper sensory inputs during the critical period.
Materials and methods
Contact for reagent and resource sharing
Request a detailed protocolFurther information and requests for resources and reagents should be directed to and will be fulfilled by the lead contact, Hitoshi Sakano (sakano.hts@gmail.com).
Mutant mice
Request a detailed protocolThe Cnga2 KO (No. 002905) (Lin et al., 2000) and Sema7a KO (No. 005128) (Pasterkamp et al., 2003) were purchased from the Jackson Laboratory. The KO mice for oxytocin (Nishimori et al., 1996) was obtained from Tohoku University. BAC Tg mice containing the MOR29A (also known as Olfr1510) and MOR29B (also known as Olfr1511) genes (Tsuboi et al., 2011), Tg mice expressing the rI7 (also known as Lofr226) gene with the MOR23 (also known as Olfr16) promoter (Imai et al., 2006), and rI7 (RDY)-ires-gap-YFP mice (Imai et al., 2006) were generated in our group. For the cell-type-specific cKOs of Plxnc1, we cloned the exon 5 of Plxnc1 into the double-floxed, inverted open-reading frame plasmid, DT-A/Conditional FW (http://www2.clst.riken.jp/arg/cassettes.html). The cKOs mice were generated according to the published protocol (http://www2.clst.riken.jp/arg/gene_targeting.html). To generate the Tg mice carrying the rI7-ires-Sema7a-ires-YFP or the rI7-ires-Sema7a (Y213S)-ires-YFP, an EagI cassette containing the transgene was inserted into the EagI site in the pMOR23-rI7-ires-gap-YFP construct. All five transgenic lines obtained for each, successfully expressed Tg-Sema7A in the rI7-positive OSNs, although expression levels of Tg-Sema7A were ~10% compared with the endogenous Sema7A. The Pcdh21-Cre mouse (Nagai et al., 2005) was obtained from the RIKEN Bioresource Research Center (RBRC02189) and crossed with the Plxnc1flox/flox mice.
All animals were housed under a standard 12 hr light/dark cycle (light on from 8 am to 8 pm), constant temperature (22 ± 2°C), and humidity (50 ± 10%). Food and water were provided ad libitum. All animal experiments were approved by the Animal Care Committees at University of Fukui and Azabu University.
Naris occlusion
Request a detailed protocolUnilateral naris occlusion was performed at P0 on the right nostril with an electrical cautery, SURE SX-10C (Ishizaki, Japan). The occluded pups were examined daily to ensure blockade of the occluded nostril by checking the scar formation. The right-side naris was reopened at various time points during the postnatal period using a 31G needle. In some experiments, occlusion was initiated after P0 for different time lengths during the neonatal period.
Antibodies
Antibodies used in this study are as follows: Sema7A (goat, 1:3000, #AF-1835, R and D Systems); PlxnC1 (goat, 1:3000, discontinued, Abcam); CNG-A2 (rabbit, 1:200, #APC-045, Alomone Labs); vGlut2 (guinea pig, 1:1000, #AB2251-l, Millipore); GluR1 (rabbit, 1:1000, #ab51092, Abcam); GFP (rabbit, 1:1000, #A-10260, Thermo Fisher Scientific); Lucifer yellow (rabbit, 1:2000, #A-5750, Thermo Fisher Scientific); and EGR1 (rabbit, 1:1000, #ab6054, Abcam).
Immunostaining
Request a detailed protocolFor immunohistochemistry, OB sections were fixed with 4% paraformaldehyde in PBS and treated with the primary and secondary antibodies. The sections were then photographed with a fluorescence microscope, Model IX70 (Olympus), coupled to a cooled CCD camera, C4742-95-12ERG (Hamamatsu Photonics).
Intensity measurement
Request a detailed protocolFor fluorescent signals of immunostaining, digital images were taken with a digital CCD camera, C4742-95-12ERG (Hamamatsu Photonics), or with two-photon microscopy (Olympus FV1000). Tone was reversed and a monochrome image was used for the measurement. For staining of the OB sections, digital images were taken with a digital CCD camera, Model DP70 (Olympus). To quantify the staining level of each glomerulus, the mean pixel intensity within the region surrounded by the periglomerular-cell nuclei was measured using Scion Image (Scion Corp.).
Neonatal odor conditioning
View detailed protocolBefore conditioning, mother mice were pre-exposed to the odor for 10 min, three times a day for 3 consecutive days. A filter paper (2×2 cm) spotted with a test odorant was placed near the mice. For conditioning, pups were exposed to the odor for 10 min with the foster mother, three times a day for 3 consecutive days. Odor concentration was 0.5 μl of 20 mM for vanillin (VNL) or 100 mM for 4-methyl-thiazole (4MT) (Wako).
Habituation/dishabituation test
Request a detailed protocolFor the test, adult littermates (6w-old male) were kept individually in a new cage (26×40×18 cm) containing a plain filter paper for 5 min prior to the experiment. This treatment was to habituate the mice to a filter paper and to evaluate the net curiosity to a novel odor in the following filter presentation. During the treatment, about 8% of total mice did not demonstrate any interest in the plain filter paper. We precluded such mice for further experiments. In the test, a filter paper spotted with 0.5 μl of distilled water was intermittently presented for 3 min. This operation was repeated three times with 1 min intervals. Next, a filter paper spotted with the 1st test odorant was presented three times as in the presentation of water-spotted filter paper (habituation). Then, the 2nd test odorant was presented three times in the same way (dishabituation). Investigation times (s) observed during each odorant presentation were measured. In this test, the mice were used only once not to confound the data due to the previous learning. The test was blinded to avoid unconscious bias and to insure the objectivity. The mouse behavior was recorded with a digital video camera, DCR-SR100 (SONY). In this assay, investigation was defined as a nasal contact with filter papers within 1 mm of distance. Odorants used were (+)-carvone (CAR) (Sigma-Aldrich), (–)-CAR (Sigma-Aldrich), eugenol (EUG) (Tokyo-Kasei), and vanillin (VNL) (Wako).
Stress-induced hyperthermia test
Request a detailed protocolFor the stress-induced hyperthermia test (Spooren et al., 2002), mice (6w-old) were individually habituated to the cage and their body temperature was checked to be normal. The mice were then transferred to a new cage and exposed to a testing odorant. The rectal temperature was measured with a digital thermistor, BAT-12 (Physitemp Instruments Inc) by inserting the probe to a length of 10–20 mm until stable reading was obtained. Differences of rectal temperatures before (T) and after (Tx) the transfer were individually measured every 20 min. Odor concentrations were 0.5 μl of 20 mM for vanillin (VNL), 100 mM for propionic acid (PPA) (Tokyo-Kasei), and 100 mM for 4-methyl-thiazole (4MT) (Wako).
In situ hybridization
Request a detailed protocolTo prepare cRNA probes, DNA fragments of 500–3,000 bp were amplified by PCR from the OB cDNA of C57BL/6 mice. Only unique sequences were amplified for each gene. PCR products were subcloned into the pGEM-T vector (Promega) and used as templates to make cRNA probes. Hybridization was performed according to the standard method (Inoue et al., 2018).
Electron microscopy
Request a detailed protocolMice were transcardially perfused with 2% paraformaldehyde (PFA) and 2.5% glutaraldehyde (GA) in 0.1 M PB. The mice were then immersed in the same buffer at 4°C for 1–2 hr and rinsed in 0.1 M PB prior to being sectioned transversely into 50 μm slices with a vibratome, LinearSlicer PRO7 (D.S.K.). The sections for electron microscopy were incubated in 2% osmium tetroxide for 60 min, dehydrated through an ascending ethanol series, and embedded in EPON. Thin sections (70–100 nm) were then sliced with EM UC7 (Leica), mounted on Cu grids, and examined in a transmission electron microscope VE-9800 (KEYENCE). Images were photographed at primary magnification.
Odor preference test
Request a detailed protocolFor the social response test, 6w-old adult littermates (male mice) were individually habituated in a new cage (26×40×18 cm). Then, food pellet (Japan SLC, Inc) or beds with unfamiliar mouse odor were carefully placed not to disturb the mice in a corner of the test cage. To avoid direct contacts of the mouse nose, the food pellet or bed samples were presented in a small plastic cup. The cup positions were alternated to avoid side preference. This apparatus evaluates a simple social interest in the volatile smell of unfamiliar mice, excluding visual, auditory, and vomeronasal inputs. Times (s) spent in the room with or without a test odorant were measured during the 5-min test period. The mice were used only once to avoid possible bias attribute to learning. The test was blinded to avoid unconscious bias to insure the objectivity. The mouse behavior was recorded with a digital video camera, DCR-SR100 (SONY).
Three-chamber social interaction test
Request a detailed protocolLittermate mice (6w-old male) were used in the experiment. The three-chamber test was conducted as described by Moy et al., 2004. Before the test, a mouse was allowed to explore all three chambers divided with plastic walls with a loophole in the apparatus for 10 min. Only those mice moving actively in the cage were used for the test. Then, the test mouse was placed in the center chamber, an empty plastic cage was to the right, and an unfamiliar mouse (male C57BL/6J) in a cage was to the left. The mouse being tested was allowed to move freely and explore all three chambers for 15 min. The mouse behavior was recorded with a digital video camera, DCR-SR100 (SONY). Time duration spent in each room was measured during the 15-min test period. The positions of the empty cage and unfamiliar mouse were alternated to avoid left/right side preference.
Social memory test
Request a detailed protocolAll pups in each litter received either an intra-peritoneal injection of oxytocin (2 μg/20 μl for P0~6 and 5 μg/20 μl for P8~14) or saline (SAL, 20 μl) on postnatal days 0, 2, 4, and 6, or 8, 10, 12, and 14. The same cohort of male mice that were examined for alloparental responsiveness, was tested by the habituation/dishabituation paradigm (Ferguson et al., 2000 and Ferguson et al., 2001). In this paradigm, mice exposed to the same stimulus animal should decrease their investigation times with each exposure (habituation) and increase their investigation time when a novel animal is introduced (dishabituation). One male subject was transferred from its group to an individual cage of the same size as the breeding cage for 7–10 days before testing for the establishment of a home-cage territory. Testing began with a stimulus mouse, an ovariectomized female unfamiliar to the subject, was introduced into the home cage of each subject for a 1 min confrontation. At the end of the 1 min trial, the stimulus was removed and returned to its home cage. This trial was repeated for five times with 10 min intertrial intervals, and each stimulus was introduced to the same male subject in all five trials. In the 6th dishabituation trial, an ovariectomized female mouse different from the previous one was introduced into the home cage of the subject mouse. Each trial was recorded with a CCD camera connected to a video recorder. The duration of sniffing the stimulus’ anogenital area was measured by a well-trained observer who was blinded to both the mice genotypes and treatments.
Statistical analyses
Request a detailed protocolAll statistical analyses were performed using Excel 2016 (Microsoft) with the Statcel2 add-on (OMS).
Data availability
All data generated or analyzed during this study are included in the manuscript and supporting files.
References
-
Neurotransmitters: central and peripheral mechanismsInternational Journal of Impotence Research 12 Suppl 4:S26–S33.https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.ijir.3900574
-
Central corticotropin releasing factor and social stressFrontiers in Neuroscience 7:117.https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2013.00117
-
Oxytocin and social relationships: from attachment to bond disruptionCurrent Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences 35:97–117.https://doi.org/10.1007/7854_2017_10
-
ConferenceRegeneration of olfactory receptor cellsCiba Foundation Symposium. pp. 233–248.https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470514122.ch12
-
Central nervous system effects of the neurohypophyseal hormones and related peptidesFrontiers in Neuroendocrinology 14:251–302.https://doi.org/10.1006/frne.1993.1009
-
Social amnesia in mice lacking the oxytocin geneNature Genetics 25:284–288.https://doi.org/10.1038/77040
-
Oxytocin in the medial amygdala is essential for social recognition in the mouseThe Journal of Neuroscience 21:8278–8285.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-20-08278.2001
-
Mitral cells in the olfactory bulb are mainly excited through a multistep signaling pathJournal of Neuroscience 32:2964–2975.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5580-11.2012
-
Neurogenesis and plasticity of the olfactory sensory neuronsAnnals of the New York Academy of Sciences 457:127–142.https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.1985.tb20802.x
-
Critical period plasticity in local cortical circuitsNature Reviews Neuroscience 6:877–888.https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1787
-
Visual imprinting and the neural mechanisms of recognition memoryTrends in Neurosciences 21:300–305.https://doi.org/10.1016/S0166-2236(97)01219-8
-
Contribution of olfactory neural stem cells to tissue maintenance and regenerationNature Neuroscience 10:720–726.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1882
-
Early odorant exposure increases the number of mitral and tufted cells associated with a single glomerulusThe Journal of Neuroscience 36:11646–11653.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0654-16.2016
-
Differential distribution of ionotropic glutamate receptor subunits in the rat olfactory bulbThe Journal of Comparative Neurology 405:233–246.https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19990308)405:2<233::AID-CNE7>3.0.CO;2-A
-
How is the olfactory map formed and interpreted in the mammalian brain?Annual Review of Neuroscience 34:467–499.https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-112210-112917
-
Olfactory circuitry and behavioral decisionsAnnual Review of Physiology 83:231–256.https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-physiol-031820-092824
-
Neural basis of imprinting behavior in chicksDevelopment, Growth & Differentiation 55:198–206.https://doi.org/10.1111/dgd.12028
-
Perisomatic-targeting granule cells in the mouse olfactory bulbThe Journal of Comparative Neurology 515:409–426.https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.22063
-
Circuit formation and synaptic plasticity in the mouse olfactory systemThe Senses: A Comprehensive Reference, 2nd Edition 3:624–639.https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-809324-5.23829-7
-
Origin of basal activity in mammalian olfactory receptor neuronsJournal of General Physiology 136:529–540.https://doi.org/10.1085/jgp.201010528
-
Lifespan oxytocin signaling: maturation, flexibility, and stability in newborn, adolescent, and aged brainDevelopmental Neurobiology 77:158–168.https://doi.org/10.1002/dneu.22450
-
Effects of in utero odorant exposure on neuroanatomical development of the olfactory bulb and odour preferencesProceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 278:1949–1955.https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2010.2314
-
Neurobiology of associative learning in the neonate: early olfactory learningBehavioral and Neural Biology 61:1–18.https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-1047(05)80039-1
-
Cellular and molecular mechanisms of synaptic specificityAnnual Review of Cell and Developmental Biology 30:417–437.https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-cellbio-100913-012953
Article and author information
Author details
Funding
JSPS (KAKENHI 24000014&17H06160)
- Hitoshi Sakano
JSPS (KAKENHI 17K19386 & 20K06909)
- Hirofumi Nishizumi
Japan Foundation for Applied Enzymology (J180000322)
- Nobuko Inoue
The Naito Foundation
- Hirofumi Nishizumi
MEXT (KAKENHI 17H05943 & 19H04745)
- Hirofumi Nishizumi
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Acknowledgements
We are grateful to H Naritsuka for her advice in single-cell labeling and H Sagara for his help in EM analysis. We thank members of our laboratory for valuable suggestions and discussion. This research was supported by the JSPS KAKENHI (Grant Numbers 24000014 and 17H06160 to HS; 17K19386 and 20K06909 to HN) and the MEXT KAKENHI (Grant Numbers 17H05943 and 19H04745 to HN). This work was also supported by the Japan Foundation for Applied Enzymology (J180000322 to NI) and the Naito Foundation (HN).
Ethics
Animal experimentation: the Animal Care Committees at University of Fukui and Azabu University.
Copyright
© 2021, Inoue et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 2,626
- views
-
- 332
- downloads
-
- 21
- citations
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Developmental Biology
Mutations in the well-known Myostatin (MSTN) produce a ‘double-muscle’ phenotype, which makes it commercially invaluable for improving livestock meat production and providing high-quality protein for humans. However, mutations at different loci of the MSTN often produce a variety of different phenotypes. In the current study, we increased the delivery ratio of Cas9 mRNA to sgRNA from the traditional 1:2 to 1:10, which improves the efficiency of the homozygous mutation of biallelic gene. Here, a MSTNDel73C mutation with FGF5 knockout sheep, in which the MSTN and FGF5 dual-gene biallelic homozygous mutations were produced via the deletion of 3-base pairs of AGC in the third exon of MSTN, resulting in cysteine-depleted at amino acid position 73, and the FGF5 double allele mutation led to inactivation of FGF5 gene. The MSTNDel73C mutation with FGF5 knockout sheep highlights a dominant ‘double-muscle’ phenotype, which can be stably inherited. Both F0 and F1 generation mutants highlight the excellent trait of high-yield meat with a smaller cross-sectional area and higher number of muscle fibers per unit area. Mechanistically, the MSTNDel73C mutation with FGF5 knockout mediated the activation of FOSL1 via the MEK-ERK-FOSL1 axis. The activated FOSL1 promotes skeletal muscle satellite cell proliferation and inhibits myogenic differentiation by inhibiting the expression of MyoD1, and resulting in smaller myotubes. In addition, activated ERK1/2 may inhibit the secondary fusion of myotubes by Ca2+-dependent CaMKII activation pathway, leading to myoblasts fusion to form smaller myotubes.
-
- Computational and Systems Biology
- Developmental Biology
The initially homogeneous epithelium of the early Drosophila embryo differentiates into regional subpopulations with different behaviours and physical properties that are needed for morphogenesis. The factors at top of the genetic hierarchy that control these behaviours are known, but many of their targets are not. To understand how proteins work together to mediate differential cellular activities, we studied in an unbiased manner the proteomes and phosphoproteomes of the three main cell populations along the dorso-ventral axis during gastrulation using mutant embryos that represent the different populations. We detected 6111 protein groups and 6259 phosphosites of which 3398 and 3433 were differentially regulated, respectively. The changes in phosphosite abundance did not correlate with changes in host protein abundance, showing phosphorylation to be a regulatory step during gastrulation. Hierarchical clustering of protein groups and phosphosites identified clusters that contain known fate determinants such as Doc1, Sog, Snail, and Twist. The recovery of the appropriate known marker proteins in each of the different mutants we used validated the approach, but also revealed that two mutations that both interfere with the dorsal fate pathway, Toll10B and serpin27aex do this in very different manners. Diffused network analyses within each cluster point to microtubule components as one of the main groups of regulated proteins. Functional studies on the role of microtubules provide the proof of principle that microtubules have different functions in different domains along the DV axis of the embryo.