Finger somatotopy is preserved after tetraplegia but deteriorates over time
Abstract
Previous studies showed reorganised and/or altered activity in the primary sensorimotor cortex after a spinal cord injury (SCI), suggested to reflect abnormal processing. However, little is known about whether somatotopically specific representations can be activated despite reduced or absent afferent hand inputs. In this observational study, we used functional MRI and a (attempted) finger movement task in tetraplegic patients to characterise the somatotopic hand layout in primary somatosensory cortex. We further used structural MRI to assess spared spinal tissue bridges. We found that somatotopic hand representations can be activated through attempted finger movements in the absence of sensory and motor hand functioning, and no spared spinal tissue bridges. Such preserved hand somatotopy could be exploited by rehabilitation approaches that aim to establish new hand-brain functional connections after SCI (e.g. neuroprosthetics). However, over years since SCI the hand representation somatotopy deteriorated, suggesting that somatotopic hand representations are more easily targeted within the first years after SCI.
Introduction
A spinal cord injury (SCI) refers to damage of the spinal cord caused by a trauma, disease, or degeneration of vertebral discs. SCI mostly results in a partial or complete loss of motor control and sensation. Following a tetraplegia (or cervical SCI), individuals mostly experience a loss of muscle function and sensation in their limbs and torso (Curt et al., 1998; Kalsi-Ryan et al., 2014; Petersen et al., 2012). Accordingly, the primary somatosensory cortex (S1) mostly receives weakened, or is fully deprived of, afferent inputs and is exposed to altered motor behaviour (Ozdemir and Perez, 2018).
Research in non-human primate models of chronic and complete cervical SCI has shown that the S1 hand area becomes largely unresponsive to tactile hand stimulation after the injury (Jain et al., 2008; Kambi et al., 2014; Liao et al., 2021). The surviving finger-related activity became disorganised such that a few somatotopically appropriate sites but also other somatotopically non-matched sites were activated (Liao et al., 2021). Seminal non-human primate research has further demonstrated that SCI leads to extensive cortical reorganisation in S1, such that tactile stimulation of cortically adjacent body parts (e.g. of the face) activated the deprived brain territory (e.g. of the hand; Halder et al., 2018; Jain et al., 2008; Kambi et al., 2014). Although the physiological hand representation appears to largely be altered following a chronic cervical SCI in non-human primates, the anatomical isomorphs of individual fingers are unchanged (Jain et al., 1998). This suggests that while a hand representation can no longer be activated through tactile stimulation after the disruption of afferent spinal pathways, a latent and somatotopic hand representation may be preserved regardless of large-scale physiological reorganisation.
A similar pattern of results has been reported for human SCI patients. Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) studies induced current in localised areas of SCI patients’ primary motor cortex (M1) to induce a peripheral muscle response. They found that representations of more impaired muscles retract or are absent while representations of less impaired muscles shift and expand (Fassett et al., 2018; Freund et al., 2011a; Levy et al., 1990; Streletz et al., 1995; Topka et al., 1991; Urbin et al., 2019). Similarly, human fMRI studies have shown that cortically neighbouring body part representations can shift towards, though do not invade, the deprived M1 and S1 cortex (Freund et al., 2011b; Henderson et al., 2011; Jutzeler et al., 2015; Wrigley et al., 2018; Wrigley et al., 2009). Other human fMRI studies hint at the possibility of latent somatotopic hand representations following SCI by showing that attempted movements with the paralysed and sensory-deprived body part can still evoke signals in the sensorimotor system (Cramer et al., 2005; Freund et al., 2011b; Kokotilo et al., 2009; Solstrand Dahlberg et al., 2018). This attempted ‘net’ movement activity was, however, shown to substantially differ from healthy controls: activity levels have been shown to be increased (Freund et al., 2011b; Kokotilo et al., 2009; Solstrand Dahlberg et al., 2018) or decreased (Hotz-Boendermaker et al., 2008), volumes of activation have been shown to be reduced (Cramer et al., 2005; Hotz-Boendermaker et al., 2008), activation was found in somatotopically non-matched cortical sites (Freund et al., 2011b), and activation was poorly modulated when patients switched from attempted to imagined movements (Cramer et al., 2005). These observations have therefore mostly been attributed to abnormal and/or disorganised processing induced by the SCI. It remains possible though that, despite certain aspects of sensorimotor activity being altered after SCI, somatotopically typical representations of the paralysed and sensory deprived body parts can be preserved (e.g. finger somatotopy of the affected hand(s)). Such preserved representations have the potential to be exploited in a functionally meaningful manner (e.g. via neuroprosthetics).
Case studies using intracortical stimulation in the S1 hand area to elicit finger sensations in tetraplegic patients hint at such preserved somatotopic representations (Fifer et al., 2020; Flesher et al., 2016), with one exception (Armenta Salas et al., 2018). Negative results were suggested to be due to a loss of hand somatotopy and/or reorganisation in S1 of the implanted tetraplegic patient, or due to potential misplacement of the implant (Armenta Salas et al., 2018). Whether fine-grained somatotopy is generally preserved in the tetraplegic patient population remains unknown. It is also unclear what clinical, behavioural, and structural spinal cord determinants may influence such representations to be maintained.
Here we used functional MRI (fMRI) and a visually cued (attempted) finger movement task in tetraplegic patients to examine whether hand somatotopy is preserved following a disconnect between the brain and the periphery. We instructed patients to perform the fMRI tasks with their most impaired upper limb and matched control participants’ tested hands to patients’ tested hands. If a patient was unable to make overt finger movements due to their injury, then we carefully instructed them to make attempted (i.e. not imagined) finger movements. To see whether patient’s maps exhibited characteristics of somatotopy, we visualised finger selectivity in S1 using a travelling wave approach. To investigate whether fine-grained hand somatotopy was preserved and could be activated in S1 following tetraplegia, we assessed inter-finger representational distance patterns using representational similarity analysis (RSA). These inter-finger distance patterns are thought to be shaped by daily life experience such that fingers used more frequently together in daily life have lower representational distances (Ejaz et al., 2015). RSA-based inter-finger distance patterns have been shown to depict the invariant representational structure of fingers in S1 and M1 better than the size, shape, and exact location of the areas activated by finger movements (Ejaz et al., 2015). Over the past years RSA has therefore regularly been used to investigate somatotopy of finger representations both in healthy (e.g. Akselrod et al., 2017; Ariani et al., 2020; Ejaz et al., 2015; Gooijers et al., 2021; Kieliba et al., 2021; Kolasinski et al., 2016b; Liu et al., 2021; Sanders et al., 2019) and patient populations (e.g. Dempsey-Jones et al., 2019; Ejaz et al., 2016; Kikkert et al., 2016; Wesselink et al., 2019). We closely followed procedures that have previously been used to map preserved and typical somatotopic finger selectivity and inter-finger representational distance patterns of amputees’ missing hands in S1 using volitional phantom finger movements (Kikkert et al., 2016; Wesselink et al., 2019). However, in amputees, these movements generally recruit the residual arm muscles that used to control the missing limb via intact connections between the brain and spinal cord. Whether similar preserved somatotopic mapping can be observed in SCI patients with diminished or no connections between the brain and the periphery is unclear. If finger somatotopy is preserved in tetraplegic patients, then we should find typical inter-finger representational distance patterns in the S1 hand area of these patients. By measuring a group of 14 chronic tetraplegic patients with varying amounts of spared spinal cord tissue at the lesion level (quantified by means of midsagittal tissue bridges based on sagittal T2w scans), we uniquely assessed whether preserved connections between the brain and periphery are necessary to preserve fine somatotopic mapping in S1 (Huber et al., 2017; Pfyffer et al., 2019). If spared connections between the periphery and the brain are not necessary for preserving hand somatotopy, then we should find typical inter-finger representational distance patterns even in patients without spared spinal tissue bridges. We also investigated what clinical and behavioural determinants may contribute to preserving S1 hand somatotopy after chronic tetraplegia. If spared sensorimotor hand function is not necessary for preserving hand somatotopy, then we should find typical inter-finger representational distance patterns even in patients who suffer from full sensory loss and paralysis of the hand(s).
Results
Patient impairments
We tested 14 chronic tetraplegic patients that were heterogenous in terms of completeness of the SCI (ranging from AIS-A to AIS-D), neurological level of the injury (ranging from C2 to C7), years since injury (ranging from 6 months to 33 years since SCI), and sensorimotor upper limb impairments (ranging from a Graded Redefined Assessment of Strength, Sensibility and Prehension test (GRASSP) score for both limbs of 21 to 220; healthy GRASSP score = 232). We further tested 18 age-, sex-, and handedness-matched healthy control participants.
Finger selectivity is preserved following tetraplegia
We used 3T fMRI and a travelling wave paradigm to investigate the somatotopic layout of finger selectivity on the S1 cortical surface (Besle et al., 2013; Kolasinski et al., 2016a). To visualise whether there was a consistent and somatotopic layout of finger selectivity across participants in the control and SCI patient groups, we created probability maps of finger selectivity. A characteristic hand map shows a gradient of finger preference, progressing from the thumb (laterally) to the little finger (medially). We found a characteristic progression of finger selective clusters in both the control and tetraplegic patient groups (Figure 1). Qualitative inspection suggests that inter-participant consistency was lowest for the little finger representation in both groups. It further appears that overall inter-participant consistency was reduced in the patient group compared to the control group.

Inter-participant somatotopic finger-specific probability maps of the control and tetraplegic patient groups.
Colours indicate the number of participants (ranging from 1 [red] till 18 and 13 [blue] participants for the control and SCI patient group, respectively) who demonstrated finger selectivity for a given vertex. Characteristic finger selectivity is characterised by a progression of finger selectivity from the thumb (laterally) to the little finger (medially). These characteristic finger progressions can be observed in both the control (top) and the tetraplegic patient (bottom) group’s probability maps. Qualitative inspection suggests that inter-participant consistency was lowest for the little finger representation in both groups. It further appears that overall inter-participant consistency was reduced in the patient group compared to the control group. White arrows indicate the central sulcus. A: anterior; P: posterior.
Given the known the relatively high inter-participant variability in finger selectivity (Kolasinski et al., 2016a) and the clinical heterogeneity of our SCI patient group, we further visualised each individual participant’s finger selectivity map (Figure 2A and B). Overall, we found aspects of somatotopic finger selectivity in the maps of SCI patients’ hands, in which neighbouring clusters showed selectivity for neighbouring fingers in contralateral S1, similar to those observed in 18 age-, sex-, and handedness-matched healthy controls. Notably, a characteristic hand map was even found in a patient who suffered complete paralysis and sensory deprivation of the hands (Figure 2B, patient map 1; patient S01). Despite most maps (Figure 2, except patient map 3; patient S04) displaying aspects of characteristic finger selectivity, some finger representations were not visible in the thresholded patient and control maps.

Finger selectivity is preserved in tetraplegic patients.
Colours indicate selectivity for the thumb (finger 1, red), index finger (finger 2, yellow), middle finger (finger 3, green), ring finger (finger 4, blue), and little finger (finger 5, purple). Maps of participants for whom the left hand was tested are horizontally mirrored for visualisation purposes. Typical finger selectivity is characterised by a gradient of finger preference, progressing from the thumb (laterally) to the little finger (medially). These characteristic gradients of finger selectivity can be observed in both the able-bodied controls (A) and the tetraplegic patients (B). Despite most maps (except patient map 3) displaying aspects of characteristic finger maps, some finger representations were not visible in the thresholded patient and control maps. Patients’ hand maps are sorted according to their overall upper-limb impairments (assessed using the Graded Redefined Assessment of Strength, Sensibility and Prehension test [GRASSP]): from most to least impaired – as indicated by the white numbers. Black numbers indicate the years since spinal cord injury (SCI). Multiple comparisons were adjusted using a false discovery rate (FDR) with q < 0.05. Other figure annotations are as in Figure 1. (C) To ensure that the observed clusters were not representing noise, but rather true finger selectivity, we calculated split-half consistency between two halves of the minimally thresholded (Z > 2) travelling wave dataset (see Figure 2—figure supplement 1 for the travelling wave maps used to calculate split-half consistency). Both controls and patients showed higher split-half consistency (assessed using the Dice overlap coefficient) for comparison of the same fingers between two halves of the travelling wave dataset (light blue), compared to neighbouring (blue), and non-neighbouring fingers (dark blue). Moreover, neighbouring fingers showed greater overlap across the split-halves of the dataset then non-neighbouring fingers for both patients and controls. The same results were obtained when calculating split-half consistency on maps thresholded using FDR q < 0.05 (as was used for the maps in A, B; see Figure 2—figure supplement 2). Error bars show the standard error of the mean. *** = corrected p≤0.001, ns: non-significant.
To ensure that the observed finger selective clusters were not representing noise, but rather true finger selectivity, we calculated split-half consistency between two halves of the travelling wave dataset using the Dice overlap coefficient (DOC; Dice, 1945). Minimally thresholded finger maps were compared across the split-halves of the data within an S1 mask. Overall, split-half consistency was not significantly different between patients and controls, as tested using a robust mixed ANOVA (see Figure 2C; F(1,20.32) = 0.51, p=0.48). There was a significant difference in split-half consistency between pairs of same, neighbouring, and non-neighbouring fingers (F(2,18.59) = 159.69, p<0.001). This neighbourhood relationship was not significantly different between the control and patient groups (i.e. there was no significant interaction; F(2,18.59) = 2.44, p=0.11).
The DOC was highest for comparison of the same fingers between two halves of the dataset compared to neighbouring (controls: W = 171, p<0.001, BF10 = 2761.24; patients: W = 91, p<0.001, BF10 = 742.45) and non-neighbouring fingers (controls: W = 171, p<0.001, BF10 = 248.14; patients: W = 91, p<0.001, BF10 = 133.05). Moreover, neighbouring fingers showed greater overlap across the split-halves of the dataset than non-neighbouring fingers (controls: W = 171, p<0.001, BF10 = 835.29; patients: W = 91, p=0.001, BF10 = 65.32). This demonstrates that there was a somatotopic gradient in split-half consistency that was similar between the control and patient groups, demonstrating that the finger maps represented true finger selectivity.
Typical hand somatotopy is preserved following tetraplegia
Next, we assessed 3T univariate task-related activity during individual finger movements performed in a blocked design fashion. Task-related activity was quantified by extracting the percent signal change for finger movement (across all fingers) versus baseline within the contralateral S1 hand area (see Figure 3A). Overall, all patients were able to engage their S1 hand area by moving individual fingers (t(13) = 7.46, p<0.001; BF10 = 4.28 e + 3), as did controls (t(17) = 9.92, p<0.001; BF10 = 7.40 e + 5). Furthermore, patients’ task-related activity was not significantly different from controls (t(30) = –0.82, p=0.42; BF10 = 0.44), with the Bayes factor (BF) showing anecdotal evidence in favour of the null hypothesis. Similar results were found when exploring univariate task-related activity in the contralateral M1 hand ROI (see Figure 3—figure supplement 1).

Typical multivariate hand somatotopy is preserved following tetraplegia.
(A) Percent signal change in the S1 hand area during finger movement for able-bodied controls (grey) and tetraplegic patients (orange). Similar results were found in the M1 hand ROI (see Figure 3—figure supplement 1). (B, C) Two-dimensional projection of the representational structure of inter-finger distances in the control (B) and tetraplegic patient groups (C). Inter-finger distance is reflected by the distance in the two dimensions. Individual fingers are represented by different colours: thumb, red; index finger, yellow; middle finger, green; ring finger, blue; little finger, purple. Ellipses represent the between-participants’ standard error after Procrustes alignment. Inter-finger distances across finger pairs were significantly different across finger pairs (as would be expected based on somatotopic mapping), but not between controls and tetraplegic patients (see Figure 3—figure supplement 2). Individual participant inter-finger distance patterns are visualised in Figure 3—figure supplement 3 and Figure 3—figure supplement 4 for the controls and patients, respectively. (D) Separability, measured as mean inter-finger distance, of the representational structure in the S1 hand area of controls and patients. Patients are presented on a colour scale representing the sensory and motor functioning of their tested upper limb, measured using the Graded Redefined Assessment of Strength, Sensibility and Prehension test (GRASSP) (0 = no upper limb function, 116 = normal upper limb function). (E) Typicality of the representational structure in controls, patients, and congenital one-handers (Cong. in the figure). *** p<0.001; ns: non-significant; Dim: dimension; a.u.: arbitrary unit; Cong: congenital one-handers.
While the travelling wave maps demonstrate finger selectivity, they provide little information about the overlap between finger representations. We therefore examined the intricate relationship between finger representations in the S1 hand area for all patients and controls using RSA (see Figure 3B and C). The resulting inter-finger distances were averaged across finger pairs within each participant to obtain an estimate for average inter-finger separability (see Figure 3D). We found that inter-finger separability in the S1 hand area was greater than 0 for patients (t(13) = 9.83, p<0.001; BF10 = 6.77 e + 4) and controls (t(17) = 11.70, p<0.001; BF10 = 6.92 e + 6), indicating that the S1 hand area in both groups contained information about individuated finger representations. Furthermore, for both controls (W = 171, p<0.001; BF10 = 4059) and patients (W = 105, p<0.001; BF10 = 279) there was significant greater separability (or representation strength) in the S1 hand area than in a control cerebral spinal fluid (CSF) ROI that would not be expected to contain information about individuated finger representations. We did not find a significant group difference in inter-finger separability of the S1 hand area (t(30) = 1.52, p=0.14; BF10 = 0.81), with the BF showing anecdotal evidence in favour of the null hypothesis.
We then tested whether the inter-finger distances were different across finger pairs between controls and tetraplegic patients using a robust mixed ANOVA with a within-participants factor for finger pair (10 levels) and a between-participants factor for group (two levels: controls and tetraplegic patients; Figure 3—figure supplement 2). We did not find a significant difference in inter-finger distances between patients and controls (F(1,21.66) = 1.50, p=0.23). The inter-finger distances were significantly different across finger pairs, as would be expected based on somatotopic mapping (F(9,15.38) = 27.22, p<0.001). This pattern of inter-finger distances was not significantly different between groups (i.e. no significant finger pair by group interaction; F(9,15.38) = 1.05, p=0.45). When testing for group differences per finger pair, the BF only revealed inconclusive evidence (BF >0.37 and < 1.11; note that we could not run a Bayesian ANOVA due to normality violations).
Although inter-finger separability was not significantly different between patients and controls, it is possible that the pattern of inter-finger distances was atypical in the patients. We therefore examined whether the inter-finger distance pattern was normal (or typical) in tetraplegic patients (see Figure 3E) by correlating each participant’s inter-finger distance pattern with a canonical inter-finger distance pattern. Tetraplegic patients’ typicality scores were compared to those of the controls and of a group congenital one-handers (data taken from an independent study; Wesselink et al., 2019). Congenital one-handers are born without a hand and therefore do not have a cortical representation of the missing hand (unlike amputees who develop the representation before losing the hand and hence have a ‘missing hand representation’; Wesselink et al., 2019). This group was therefore included as a control for absence of hand representation. We found a significant difference in typicality between tetraplegic patients, healthy controls, and congenital one-handers (H(2) = 26.64, p<0.001). As expected, post hoc tests revealed significantly higher typicality in controls compared to congenital one-handers (U = 0, p<0.001; BF10 = 113.60). Importantly, inter-finger distances typicality of the SCI patients was significantly higher than the typicality scores of the congenital one-handers (U = 4, p<0.001; BF10 = 90.33), but not significantly different from the typicality scores of the controls (U = 103, p=0.40; BF10 = 0.55). The BF for the comparison between SCI patients and controls showed anecdotal evidence for equivalence between both groups.
Typical hand somatotopy deteriorates over years after tetraplegia
Next, we aimed to understand which clinical, behavioural, and structural spinal cord determinants may allow hand representations in S1 to be maintained. We first explored correlations with S1 hand representation typicality. We found that the number of years since SCI significantly correlated with hand representation typicality (see Figure 4A; rs = –0.59, p=0.028), suggesting that S1 hand representation typicality may deteriorate over time after SCI. We further found that patients with more retained GRASSP motor function of the tested upper limb had more typical hand representations in S1 (see Figure 4B; rs = 0.60, p=0.02). We did not find a significant correlation between S1 hand representation typicality and GRASSP sensory function of the tested upper limb, spared midsagittal spinal tissue bridges at the lesion level, or cross-sectional spinal cord area (see Figure 4C–E; rs = 0.40, p=0.16, rs = 0.46, p=0.14, and rs = 0.33, p=0.25, respectively).

Years since spinal cord injury and retained motor function correlate with hand representation typicality in the primary somatosensory cortex (S1).
We examined clinical, behavioural, and spinal cord structural correlates for hand representation typicality. Increasing marker sizes represent increasing years since spinal cord injury (SCI) in graphs A–E. (A) There was a negative correlation between years since SCI and hand representation typicality. (B) We found a positive correlation between motor function of the tested upper limb (measured using the Graded Redefined Assessment of Strength, Sensibility and Prehension test [GRASSP]) and hand representation typicality. There was no significant correlation between hand representation typicality and sensory function of the tested upper limb (C; measured using the GRASSP), spared midsagittal spinal tissue bridges (D), and cross-sectional spinal cord area (E). (F) Bootstrapped distribution of controls’ and congenital one-handers’ mean S1 hand representation typicality. Dark grey bars indicate the distribution of congenital one-handers (data taken from an independent study; Wesselink et al., 2019), and light grey bars indicate the distribution of the able-bodied controls (tested for this study). The typicality scores of the SCI patients are plotted as orange lines. Increasing line thickness represent increasing years since SCI. Grey shaded areas indicate the 95% confidence intervals of the mean for congenital one-handers and able-bodied controls.
We further explored the hand representation typicality of patients S01 and S03 who did not have any spared midsagittal spinal tissue bridges at the lesion level, a complete (S01) or near complete (S03) hand paralysis, and a complete (S01) or near complete loss (S03) of hand sensory function (as assessed using the GRASSP test). Interestingly, both patients had a highly typical hand representation in S1 that was significantly different from congenital one-handers (i.e. who are born without a hand and do not have a missing hand representation; S01: t(12) = 3.20, p=0.008; S03: t(12) = 2.97, P = 0.01), but not controls (S01: t(17) = 0.95, p=0.36; S03: t(17) = 0.04, p=0.97). This suggests that retained connections between the periphery and the brain, retained motor functioning, and retained sensory functioning may not be necessary to maintain typical hand representations in S1.
We then ran an exploratory stepwise linear regression to investigate which of these clinical, behavioural, and structural spinal cord characteristics were predictive of hand representation typicality in S1. Years since SCI significantly predicted hand representation typicality in S1 with R2 = 0.40 (F(1,10) = 6.73, p=0.027). Motor function of the tested upper limb, sensory function of the tested hand, spared midsagittal spinal tissue bridges at the lesion level, and spinal cord area did not significantly add to the prediction (t = 1.43, p=0.19, t = 1.44, p=0.18, t = 1.19, p=0.26, and t = 0.41, p=0.69, respectively). This analysis suggests that while hand representations are preserved following tetraplegia, they may deteriorate over time.
To inspect this further, we bootstrapped the mean typicality of the able-bodied controls and congenital one-handers 10,000 times to infer their population means. While most tetraplegic patients’ typicality scores fell in, or very close to, the able-bodied controls’ distribution, we found that some SCI patients’ typicality scores fell in-between the able-bodied controls’ and congenital one-handers’ distributions (Figure 4F). This suggests that these patients may not be similar to congenital one-handers or to able-bodied controls. Interestingly, this included those patients for whom most years had passed since their SCI. This suggests that S1 hand representations might deteriorate after an SCI, but some weak hand information may be maintained in S1 even >30 years after an SCI.
Discussion
In this study, we investigated whether hand somatotopy is preserved and can be activated through attempted movements following tetraplegia. We tested a heterogenous group of SCI patients to examine what clinical, behavioural, and structural spinal cord determinants contribute to preserving S1 somatotopy. Our results revealed that detailed hand somatotopy can be preserved following tetraplegia, even in the absence of sensory and motor hand function and a lack of spared spinal tissue bridges. However, over time since SCI these finger maps deteriorated such that the hand somatotopy became less typical.
Spared spinal cord tissue bridges can be found in most patients with a clinically incomplete injury, their width being predictive of electrophysiological information flow, recovery of sensorimotor function, and neuropathic pain (Huber et al., 2017; Pfyffer et al., 2021; Pfyffer et al., 2019; Vallotton et al., 2019). However, in this study, spared midsagittal spinal tissue bridges at the lesion level and sensorimotor hand function did not seem necessary to maintain and activate a somatotopic hand representation in S1. We found a highly typical hand representation in two patients (S01 and S03) who did not have any spared spinal tissue bridges at the lesion level, a complete (S01) or near complete (S03) hand paralysis, and a complete (S01) or near complete loss (S03) of hand sensory function. Our predictive modelling results were in line with this notion and showed that these behavioural and structural spinal cord determinants were not predictive of hand representation typicality. Note however that our sample size was limited, and it is challenging to draw definite conclusions from non-significant predictive modelling results.
Time since injury was predictive of a deteriorated, or less typical, somatotopic S1 hand representation. In fact, both patients with a typical S1 hand representations but absent spinal tissue bridges suffered their SCI only 4 years ago (the group was on average 12 years since SCI). The hand representation typicality of patients who suffered their SCI further in the past were not similar to congenital one-handers’ nor to controls’ hand representation. Thus, S1 hand representations may deteriorate over time after an SCI, but some weak hand information appears to be maintained in S1 even >30 years after an SCI. This finding complements previous studies in amputees showing that missing hand somatotopy is preserved even decades after arm amputation and that years since injury was not related to missing hand representation typicality (Kikkert et al., 2016; Wesselink et al., 2019). While both amputees and SCI patients suffer from major sensory input loss and changed motor behaviour, their injuries are inherently different. The injured axons within amputees’ residual limb and the remaining part of the peripheral nerves mostly generate some spontaneous (ectopic) activity that is propagated to the brain and could contribute to maintaining S1 representations (Kikkert et al., 2019; Kikkert et al., 2018; Kikkert et al., 2016; Makin et al., 2013; Nyström and Hagbarth, 1981; Vaso et al., 2014). Furthermore, amputees mostly remain able to move and receive afferents from the residual limb muscles that used to control the missing hand as most of their motor system remains intact. Although their hand is missing, amputees’ residual arm muscles are often still used (either to move the residual limb and/or to control a prosthetic arm). Lastly, amputees’ vividness of kinaesthetic sensations during phantom finger movements was found to be predictive of the typicality of the S1 missing hand representation (Wesselink et al., 2019). A continued, though altered, experience relating to the missing hand in amputees may contribute to maintaining the somatotopic missing hand representation in S1. Contrarily, tetraplegic patients mostly have reduced or a complete loss of communication between the brain and periphery. They therefore have problems activating the adequate muscles and will lose orderly afferents from their muscles and the skin. It is possible that this continued disuse causes somatotopic S1 representations to deteriorate after SCI.
How may these representations be preserved over time and activated through attempted movements in the absence of peripheral information? S1 is reciprocally connected with various brain areas, for example,, M1, lateral parietal cortex, posterior parietal area 5, secondary somatosensory cortex, and supplementary motor cortex (Delhaye et al., 2018). After a loss of sensory inputs and paralysis through SCI, S1 representations may be activated and preserved through its interconnections with these areas. Firstly, it is possible that cortico-cortical efference copies may keep a representation ‘alive’ through occasional corollary discharge (London and Miller, 2013). While motor and sensory signals no longer pass through the spinal cord in the absence of spinal tissue bridges, S1 and M1 remain intact. When a motor command is initiated (e.g. in the form of an attempted hand movement), an efference copy is thought to be sent to S1 in the form of corollary discharge. This corollary discharge resembles the expected somatosensory feedback activity pattern and may drive somatotopic S1 activity even in the absence of ascending afferent signals from the hand (Adams et al., 2013; London and Miller, 2013). It is possible that our patients occasionally performed attempted movements which would result in corollary discharge in S1. Second, it is likely that attempting individual finger movements poses high attentional demands on tetraplegic patients. Accordingly, attentional processes might have contributed to eliciting somatotopic S1 activity. Evidence for this account comes from studies showing that it is possible to activate somatotopic S1 hand representations through attending to individual fingers (Puckett et al., 2017) or through touch observation (Kuehn et al., 2018). Attending to fingers during our attempted finger movement task may have been sufficient to elicit somatotopic S1 activity through top-down processes in the tetraplegic patients who lacked motor and sensory hand function. Furthermore, one might speculate that observing others’ or one’s own fingers being touched or directing attention to others’ hand movements or one’s own fingers may help preserve somatotopic representations. Third, it is possible that these somatotopic maps are relatively hardwired, and while they deteriorate over time, they never fully disappear. Indeed, somatotopic mapping of a sensory-deprived body part has been shown to be resilient after dystonia (Ejaz et al., 2016; though see Burman et al., 2008 and Elbert et al., 1998) and arm amputation (Bruurmijn et al., 2017; Kikkert et al., 2016; Wesselink et al., 2019). Fourth, it is possible that even though a patient is clinically assessed to be complete and is unable to perceive sensory stimuli on the deprived body part, there is still some ascending information flow that contributes to preserving somatotopy (Wrigley et al., 2018). A recent study found that although complete paraplegic SCI patients were unable to perceive a brushing stimulus on their toe, 48% of patients activated the location-appropriate S1 area (Wrigley et al., 2018). However, the authors of this study defined the completeness of patients’ injuries via behavioural testing, while we additionally assessed the retained connections passing through the SCI directly via quantification of spared spinal tissue bridges through structural MRI. It is unlikely that spinal tissue carrying somatotopically organised information would be missed by our assessment (Huber et al., 2017; Pfyffer et al., 2019). Our experiment did not allow us to tease apart these potential processes, and it is likely that various processes simultaneously influence the preservation of S1 somatotopy and elicited the observed somatotopic S1 activity.
Our finding of preserved S1 somatotopy may appear inconsistent with the wealth of evidence showing cortical reorganisation in S1 following SCI (Halder et al., 2018; Jain et al., 2008; Kambi et al., 2014). In these studies, experimenters indirectly probed the deprived S1 hand cortex via stimulation of cortically adjacent body parts. Human fMRI studies similarly probed the intact and cortically neighbouring body parts and suggested that their representations shift towards the deprived S1 cortex (Freund et al., 2011b; Freund et al., 2011a; Henderson et al., 2011; Jutzeler et al., 2015; Wrigley et al., 2018; Wrigley et al., 2009). TMS studies similarly induce current in localised areas of M1 to induce a peripheral muscle response of cortically neighbouring body parts. These studies demonstrated that the representations of less impaired muscles shift and expand following a complete or incomplete SCI (Fassett et al., 2018; Freund et al., 2011a; Levy et al., 1990; Streletz et al., 1995; Topka et al., 1991; Urbin et al., 2019). Our fMRI results showed that tetraplegic patients had a preserved somatotopic hand representation in S1, though this deteriorated over time. We did not probe body parts other than the hand and could therefore not investigate whether any remapping of other (neighbouring and/or intact) body part representations towards or into the deprived S1 hand cortex may have taken place. Whether reorganisation and preservation of the original function can simultaneously take place within the same cortical area therefore remains a topic for further investigation. It is possible that reorganisation and preservation of the original function could co-occur within cortical areas. Indeed, non-human primate studies demonstrated that remapping observed in S1 actually reflects reorganisation in subcortical areas of the somatosensory pathway, principally the brainstem (Chand and Jain, 2015; Kambi et al., 2014). As such, the deprived S1 area receives reorganised somatosensory inputs upon tactile stimulation of neighbouring intact body parts. This would simultaneously allow the original S1 representation of the deprived body part to be preserved, as observed in our results when we directly probed the deprived S1 hand area through attempted finger movements.
Together, our findings indicate that in the first years after a tetraplegia the somatotopic S1 hand representation is preserved and can be activated through attempted movements even in the absence of retained sensory hand function, motor hand function, and spared spinal tissue bridges. These preserved S1 finger maps could be exploited in a functionally meaningful manner by rehabilitation approaches that aim to establish new functional connections between the brain and the hand after a tetraplegia, for example, through neuroprosthetic limbs or advanced exoskeletons that are directly controlled by the brain (Ajiboye et al., 2017; Armenta Salas et al., 2018; Bouton et al., 2016; Lebedev and Nicolelis, 2017).
Materials and methods
Participants
15 chronic (i.e. > 6 months post injury) tetraplegic patients were recruited and 14 patients completed the measurements (mean age ± s.e.m. = 55 ± 3.6 years ; one female; six dominant left-handers; see Table 1 for demographic and clinical details). Patient inclusion criteria were as follows: aged 18–75 years, no MRI contraindications, at least 6 months post SCI, no neurological impairment or body function impairments not induced by SCI, and able to provide informed consent. 18 age-, sex-, and handedness-matched able-bodied control participants (age = 56 ± 3.6 years; one female; five dominant left-handers) also participated in this study. Control participant inclusion criteria were as follows: aged 18–75 years, no MRI contraindications, no impairment of body function induced by SCI, no neurological illness, no hand impairments, and able to provide informed consent.
Demographic and clinical details.
Tetraplegic patients are ordered according to their retained upper-limb sensory and motor function (assessed using the Graded Redefined Assessment of Strength, Sensibility and Prehension test [GRASSP]). Sex: F, female; M, male; Age, age in years; AIS grade, American Spinal Injury Association (ASIA) Impairment Scale grade defined based on the International Standards for Neurological Classification of Spinal Cord Injury (ISNCSCI); A, complete; B, sensory incomplete; C, motor incomplete; D, motor incomplete; E, normal; Neurological level of injury, defined based on the ISNCSCI; dominant hand, defined using the Edinburgh handedness inventory: L, left; R, right; GRASSP, Graded Redefined Assessment of Strength, Sensibility and Prehension (maximum score: 232 points); tested side, side with the lowest score on the GRASSP measurement; GRASSP motor/sensory score of the tested upper limb (maximum scores: 50/24; see Table 2 for further details).
Sex | Age | Years since injury | AIS grade | Cause of injury | Neurological level of injury | Dominant hand | GRASSP score | Hand tested | GRASSP tested side motor/sensory | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
S01 | M | 32 | 4 | A | Trauma | C4 | L | 21 | L | 9/0 |
S02 | M | 52 | 32 | A | Trauma | C5 | R | 78 | L | 16/5 |
S03 | M | 35 | 4 | A | Trauma | C4 | R | 90 | L | 16/17 |
S04 | M | 41 | 19 | A | Trauma | C6 | L | 105 | L | 19/11 |
S05 | M | 52 | 10 | A | Trauma | C2 | L | 118 | R | 23/0 |
S06 | M | 67 | 26 | A | Trauma | C4 | R | 119 | L | 29/2 |
S07 | M | 57 | 33 | C | Trauma | C5 | L | 145 | R | 25/19 |
S08 | F | 67 | 4 | D | Trauma | C5 | L | 173 | L | 41/9 |
S09 | M | 59 | 12 | D | Trauma | C2 | R | 187 | R | 43/9 |
S10 | M | 42 | 2 | D | Trauma | C4 | R | 187 | R | 41/13 |
S11 | M | 58 | 0.5 | D | Ischaemic | C4 | R | 194 | R | 37/17 |
S12 | M | 71 | 16 | D | Trauma | C7 | R | 196 | R | 32/24 |
S13 | M | 65 | 1 | D | Trauma | C2 | R | 218 | R | 42/24 |
S14 | M | 74 | 6 | D | Surgery | C4 | L | 220 | R | 47/24 |
Participants’ informed consent was obtained according to the Declaration of Helsinki prior to study onset. Ethical approval was granted by the Kantonale Ethikkommission Zürich (KEK-2018-00937). This study is registered on clinicaltrials.gov under NCT03772548. Two patients and one control participant were scanned twice due to excessive head motion during fMRI acquisition or suboptimal slice placement. One patient withdrew from the study prior to study completion. Data of one control participant were distorted and not usable for analysis. All data relating to these participants were discarded from all data analysis. Patient S02 did not complete the travelling wave measurements due to time constraints.
Clinical characterisation
Request a detailed protocolBehavioural testing was conducted in a separate session. We used the International Standards for Neurological Classification of Spinal Cord Injury (ISNCSCI) to neurologically classify patients’ completeness of injury and impairment level. We used the GRASSP assessment to define sensory and motor integrity of the upper limbs (Kalsi-Ryan et al., 2012). Each upper limb’s maximum score is 116 and refers to healthy conditions. We determined each patient’s most impaired upper limb according to the GRASSP. Note that GRASSP motor scores reflect overall upper-limb motor function (i.e. including arm and shoulder functioning; see Table 2 for muscle-specific GRASSP scores). GRASSP sensory scores are hand specific.
GRASSP motor sub-scores.
Each muscle was tested with resistance through its full range of motion and given a muscle grade between 0 and 5: 0, flaccid motion; 1, flicker motion; 2, full range of motion with gravity eliminated; 3, full range of motion against gravity; 4, full range of motion with moderate resistance; 5, full range of motion with maximal resistance (Kalsi-Ryan et al., 2012).
Elbow flexion | Shoulder (deltoideus) | Wrist extension | Elbow extension | Fingers 2–5 extension | Thumb opposition | Thumb flexion | Middle finger flexion | Little finger abduction | Index finger abduction | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
S01 | 5 | 4 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 |
S02 | 5 | 5 | 4 | 2 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 |
S03 | 4 | 4 | 4 | 3 | 1 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 |
S04 | 5 | 5 | 5 | 4 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 |
S05 | 4 | 1 | 3 | 4 | 3 | 3 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 2 |
S06 | 5 | 5 | 5 | 5 | 1 | 1 | 4 | 3 | 0 | 0 |
S07 | 5 | 5 | 5 | 5 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 1 |
S08 | 5 | 4 | 5 | 4 | 4 | 5 | 5 | 4 | 4 | 1 |
S09 | 5 | 4 | 4 | 4 | 4 | 4 | 5 | 5 | 4 | 4 |
S10 | 5 | 5 | 4 | 4 | 3 | 4 | 4 | 4 | 4 | 4 |
S11 | 5 | 5 | 4 | 2 | 5 | 4 | 5 | 5 | 1 | 1 |
S12 | 5 | 5 | 5 | 5 | 4 | 1 | 4 | 1 | 1 | 1 |
S13 | 5 | 4 | 4 | 4 | 5 | 4 | 5 | 3 | 4 | 4 |
S14 | 5 | 5 | 5 | 5 | 5 | 5 | 5 | 4 | 4 | 4 |
fMRI tasks
Request a detailed protocolWe employed two separate paradigms to uncover fine-grained somatotopic hand representations using fMRI: first, we used a travelling wave paradigm to investigate the somatotopic hand layout on the S1 cortical surface (Besle et al., 2013; Kolasinski et al., 2016a). Second, we employed a blocked design and RSA that takes into account the entire fine-grained activity pattern of each finger (i.e. including the representational inter-finger relationships; Ejaz et al., 2015; Kriegeskorte et al., 2008).
Participants were visually cued to perform individual finger movements while their palm was positioned up. Patients were instructed to perform the fMRI tasks with their most impaired upper limb (identified using the GRASSP, see previous section). Controls’ tested hands were matched to the patients. Due to their injury, not all patients were able to make overt finger movements. In these cases, patients were carefully instructed by the experimenter to make attempted (i.e. not imagined) finger movements. The experimenter explained that although it is not possible for the patient to perform an overt movement, attempting to perform the movement may still produce a motor command in the brain. Importantly, complete paraplegic patients are able to distinguish between attempted and imagined movements with their paralysed body part (Cramer et al., 2005; Hotz-Boendermaker et al., 2008; Sabbah et al., 2002). Furthermore, attempted foot movements activated SCI patients’ motor network similarly to controls performing overt foot movements (Cramer et al., 2005; Hotz-Boendermaker et al., 2008; Sabbah et al., 2002).
Participants saw five horizontally aligned white circles, corresponding to the five fingers, via a visual display viewed through a mirror mounted on the head coil. For participants moving their left hand, the leftmost and rightmost circles corresponded to the thumb and little finger, respectively. For participants moving their right hand, the leftmost circle corresponded to the little finger and the rightmost circle to the thumb. To cue a finger movement, the circle corresponding to this finger turned red. Participants performed self-paced flexion/extension movements with the cued finger for the duration of the colour change. Instructions were delivered using Psychtoolbox (v3) implemented in MATLAB (v2014). Head motion was minimised using over-ear MRI-safe headphones or padded cushions.
The travelling wave paradigm involved individuated finger movements in a set sequence. Each 10 s finger movement block was immediately followed by a movement block of a neighbouring finger. The forward sequence cycled through the fingers: thumb-index-middle-ring-little. To account for order-related biases due to the set movement cycle and sluggish haemodynamic response, we also collected data using a backward sequence: the backward sequence cycled through the movements in a reverse of the forward sequence: little-ring-middle-index-thumb fingers. The forward and backward sequences were employed in separate runs. A run lasted 6 min and 4 s, during which a sequence was repeated seven times. The forward and backward runs were repeated twice, with a total duration of 24 min and 16 s.
The blocked design consisted of six conditions: movement conditions for each of the five fingers and a rest condition. Finger movement instructions were as described above, and the word ‘Rest’ indicated the rest condition. A movement block lasted 8 s, and each condition was repeated five times per run in a counterbalanced order. Each run comprised a different block order and had a duration of 4 min and 14 s. We acquired four runs, with a total duration of 16 min and 56 s.
MRI acquisition
Request a detailed protocolMRI data were acquired using a Philips 3 Tesla Ingenia system (Best, The Netherlands) with a 17-channel HeadNeckSpine or, in case of participant discomfort due to the coil’s narrowness, a 15-channel HeadSpine coil. Anatomical T1-weighted images covering the brain and cervical spinal cord were acquired using the following acquisition parameters: 0.8 mm3 resolution, repetition time (TR) = 9.3 ms, echo time (TE) = 4.4 ms, and flip angle 8°. Anatomical T2-weighted images of the cervical spinal cord were acquired sagittally using the following acquisition parameters: 1 × 1 × 3 mm resolution, TR = 4500 ms, TE = 85 ms, flip angle = 90°, and slice gap = 0.3 mm, 15 slices. Task-fMRI data were acquired using an echo-planar-imaging (EPI) sequence with partial brain coverage: 22 sagittal slices were centred on the anatomical location of the hand knob with coverage over the thalamus and brainstem. We used the following acquisition parameters: 2 mm3 resolution, TR = 2000 ms, TE = 30 ms, flip angle = 82°, and SENSE factor = 2.2. We acquired 182 and 127 volumes for each of the travelling wave and blocked design runs, respectively.
fMRI analysis
Request a detailed protocolfMRI analysis was implemented using FSL v6.0 (https://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki), Advanced Normalization Tools (ANTs) v2.3.1 (http://stnava.github.io/ANTs), the RSA toolbox (Nili et al., 2014; Wesselink and Maimon-Mor, 2017), and MATLAB (R2018a). Cortical surface visualisations were realised using FreeSurfer (https://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/; Dale et al., 1999; Fischl et al., 2001) and Connectome Workbench (https://www.humanconnectome.org/software/connectome-workbench).
fMRI preprocessing
Request a detailed protocolCommon preprocessing steps were applied using FSL’s Expert Analysis Tool (FEAT). The following preprocessing steps were included: motion correction using MCFLIRT (Jenkinson et al., 2002), brain extraction using automated brain extraction tool BET (Smith, 2002), spatial smoothing using a 2 mm full-width-at-half-maximum (FWHM) Gaussian kernel, and high-pass temporal filtering with a 100 s (blocked design runs) or 90 s (travelling wave runs) cut-off.
Image registration
Request a detailed protocolImage co-registration was done in separate, visually inspected, steps. For each participant, a midspace was calculated between the four blocked design runs, that is, an average space in which images are minimally reoriented. We then transformed all fMRI data to this midspace using purely rigid probability mapping in ANTs. Next, we registered each participant’s midspace to the T1-weighted image, initially using 6 degrees of freedom and the mutual information cost function, and then optimised using boundary-based registration (BBR; Greve and Fischl, 2009) Each co-registration step was visually inspected and, if needed, manually optimised using blink comparison in Freeview.
Travelling wave analysis
Request a detailed protocolThe travelling wave approach is characterised by set finger movement cycles that are expected to result in neighbouring cortical activations. It is designed to capture voxels that show preferential activity to one condition, above and beyond all other conditions (i.e. winner-takes-all principle; testing for finger selectivity). The travelling wave approach is especially powerful to reveal the smooth progression of neighbouring representations that are specific for topographic maps. This technique is therefore frequently used to uncover retinotopic (DeYoe et al., 1996; Engel et al., 1997; Sereno et al., 1995), somatotopic (Besle et al., 2013; Kikkert et al., 2016; Kolasinski et al., 2016a; Mancini et al., 2012; Zeharia et al., 2015), and tonotopic representations (Da Costa et al., 2015; Talavage et al., 2004). Importantly, S1 finger movement somatotopy assessed using the travelling wave approach is highly consistent across scanning sessions (Kolasinski et al., 2016a).
Travelling wave analysis was conducted separately for each participant, closely following procedures previously described in Kikkert et al., 2016. A reference model was created using a gamma haemodynamic response function (HRF) convolved boxcar, using a 10 s ‘on’ (a single finger movement duration) and 40 s ‘off’ period (movement duration of all other fingers). This reference model was then shifted in time to model activity throughout the full movement cycle. Since we had a 2 s TR and a 50 s movement cycle, the reference model was shifted 25 times.
Within each individual run, each voxel’s preprocessed BOLD signal time course was cross-correlated with each of the 25 reference models. This resulted in 25 r-values per voxel per run that were normalised using a Fisher r-to-z transformation. To create finger-specific (i.e. hard-edged) maps, we assigned individual lags to specific fingers and averaged the z-values across these lags. This resulted in five z-values, one for each finger per voxel per run. These z-values were averaged across runs per voxel and per finger assignment. As a result, each individual voxel now contained five averaged z-values, one for each finger. Next, a winner-take-all approach was used to assign each voxel to one finger exclusively based on the maximum z-value, providing us with finger specificity.
To visualise the smooth gradient of progression across fingers, we produced lag-specific maps. The backward runs’ standardised cross-correlation z-values were lag-reversed and averaged with the forward runs per voxel and per lag. This resulted in 25 z-values per voxel (one per lag). Next, we used a winner-take-all principle to find the maximum z-value across lags for each voxel, providing us with lag specificity.
Cortical surface projections were constructed from participant’s T1-weighted images. The winner-take-all finger-specific and lag-specific gradient maps were projected onto the cortical surface using cortical-ribbon mapping. Thresholding was applied to the winner-take-all finger-specific maps and lag-specific maps on the cortical surface using a false discovery criterion q < 0.05 based on the native (3D) values. The false discovery rate (FDR) thresholded finger-specific maps were combined to form a hand map. Within this hand map, the lag-specific map was used to visualise the smooth gradient of progression across fingers. We were unable to find a characteristic hand map in one patient who anecdotally reported post hoc that he was unable to performed attempted finger movements.
Inter-participant probability of finger selectivity
Request a detailed protocolTo visualise inter-participant consistency of somatotopic finger-selective representations, we calculated cortical activation probability maps. To ensure that the tested hemisphere was consistently aligned for all participants, we first flipped the acquired T1-weighted images and the travelling wave winner-take-all finger-specific maps along the x-axis for the left-hand tested participants. For these left-hand tested participants, we created new cortical surface projections using their flipped T1-weighted images. Each participant’s cortical surface was then inflated into a sphere and aligned to the FreeSurfer 2D average atlas using sulcal depth and curvature information. The travelling wave winner-take-all finger-specific maps were resampled to the FreeSurfer 2D average atlas and thresholded using a false discovery criterion q < 0.05 based on the native (3D) values. We then calculated finger-specific probability maps for the control and SCI patient groups, separately.
Spatial correspondence of finger maps over time: DOC analysis
Request a detailed protocolTo confirm that the travelling wave finger-specific maps did not represent random noise, we quantified spatial consistency of finger preference between two halves of the data using the Dice overlap coefficient (DOC; Dice, 1945). The DOC calculates the spatial overlap between two representations relative to the total area of these representations. The DOC ranges from 0 (no spatial overlap) to 1 (perfect spatial overlap). If A and B represent the areas of two representations, then the DOC is expressed as
We followed previously described procedures for calculating the DOC between two halves of the travelling wave data (Kikkert et al., 2016; Kolasinski et al., 2016a; Sanders et al., 2019). The averaged finger-specific maps of the first forward and backward runs formed the first data half. The averaged finger-specific maps of the second forward and backward runs formed the second data half. The finger-specific clusters were minimally thresholded (Z > 2) on the cortical surface and masked using an S1 ROI that was created based on Brodmann area parcellation using FreeSurfer. We used minimally thresholded finger-specific clusters for DOC analysis to ensure that we were sensitive to overlaps that would be missed when using high thresholds (see Figure 2—figure supplement 1 for a visualisation of the minimally thresholded split-half hard-edged finger maps used to calculate the DOC). Note that the same results were found when thresholding the finger-specific clusters using an FDR q < 0.05 criterion (see Figure 2—figure supplement 2). The DOC was calculated between same, neighbouring, and non-neighbouring fingers across the two data halves (see Figure 2C).
If the finger maps would be spatially consistent and represent true finger selectivity, then one would expect a higher DOC between pairs of ‘same’ fingers across the two data halves compared to neighbouring and non-neighbouring finger pairs. One would further expect to find a somatotopic relationship in the DOCs: that is, a higher DOC between neighbouring compared to non-neighbouring finger pairs. We tested whether the somatotopic relationship in the DOCs was different in controls and patients using a robust mixed ANOVA with a within-participants factor for finger pair (three levels: same, neighbouring and non-neighbouring finger pairs) and a between-participants factor for group (two levels: controls and SCI patients).
Univariate analysis
Request a detailed protocolTo assess univariate task-related activity of the blocked design data, time-series statistical analysis was carried out per run using FMRIB’s Improved Linear Model (FILM) with local autocorrelation correction, as implemented in FEAT. We obtained activity estimates using a general linear modelling (GLM) based on the double-gamma HRF and its temporal derivative. Each finger movement condition was contrasted with rest. A further contrast was defined for overall task-related activity by contrasting all movement conditions with rest. A fixed effects higher-level analysis was ran for each participant to average across runs.
We defined an S1 hand ROI by converting the S1 ROI used to calculate split-half consistency to volumetric space. Any holes were filled and non-zero voxels were mean dilated. Next, the axial slices spanning 2 cm medial/lateral to the hand knob (Yousry et al., 1997) were identified on the 2 mm MNI standard brain (min-max MNI z-coordinates = 40–62). This mask was non-linearly transformed to each participant’s native structural space. Finally, we used this mask to restrict the S1 ROI and extracted an S1 hand area ROI. The percent signal change for overall task-related activity was then extracted for voxels underlying this S1 hand ROI per participant. A similar analysis was used to investigate overall task-related activity in an M1 hand ROI (see Figure 3—figure supplement 1). We further compared activity levels in finger-specific ROIs in S1 between groups and conducted a geodesic distance analysis to assess whether the finger representations of SCI patients were aligned differently and/or shifted compared to the control group (see Appendix 1).
Representational similarity analysis
Request a detailed protocolWhile the traditional travelling wave approach is powerful to uncover the somatotopic finger arrangement, a fuller description of hand representation can be obtained by taking into account the entire fine-grained activity pattern of all fingers. RSA-based inter-finger overlap patterns have been shown to depict the invariant representational structure of fingers better than the size, shape, and exact location of the areas activated by finger movements (Ejaz et al., 2015). RSA-based measures are furthermore not prone to some of the problems of measurements of finger selectivity (e.g. dependence on map thresholds). We estimated inter-finger overlap using RSA. Note that it is also possible to estimate somatotopic overlap from travelling wave data using an iterated Multigrid Priors (iMGP) method and population-receptive field modelling (Da Rocha Amaral et al., 2020; Puckett et al., 2020). We computed the distance between the activity patterns measured for each finger pair within the S1 hand ROI using the cross-validated squared Mahalanobis distance (or crossnobis distance; Nili et al., 2014). We extracted the blocked design voxel-wise parameter estimates (betas) for each finger movement condition versus rest (identified in the univariate analysis) and the model fit residuals under the S1 hand ROI. We prewhitened the extracted betas using the model fit residuals. We then calculated the cross-validated squared Mahalanobis distances between each possible finger pair, using our four runs as independent cross-validation folds, and averaged the resulting distances across the folds. If it is impossible to statistically differentiate between conditions (i.e. when this parameter is not represented in the ROI), the expected value of the distance estimate would be 0. If it is possible to distinguish between activity patterns, this value will be larger than 0.
The distance values for all finger pairs were assembled in a representational dissimilarity matrix (RDM), with a width and height corresponding to the five finger movement conditions. Since the RDM is mirrored across the diagonal with meaningless zeros on the diagonal, all statistical analyses were conducted on the 10 unique off-diagonal values of the RDM. We first estimated the strength of the finger representation or ‘finger separability’ by averaging the 10 unique off-diagonal values of the RDM. If there is no information in the ROI that can statistically distinguish between the finger conditions, then due to cross-validation the expected separability would be 0. If there is differentiation between the finger conditions, the separability would be larger than 0 (Nili et al., 2014). Note that this does not directly indicate that this region contains topographic information, but rather that this ROI contains information that can distinguish between the finger conditions. To further ensure that our S1 hand ROI was activated distinctly for different fingers, we created a CSF ROI that would not contain finger-specific information. We repeated our RSA analysis in this ROI and statistically compared the separability of the CSF and S1 hand area ROIs. Second, we tested whether the inter-finger distances were different between controls and patients using a robust mixed ANOVA with a within-participants factor for finger pair (10 levels) and a between-participants factor for group (two levels: controls and SCI patients). Third, we estimated the somatotopic typicality (or normality) of each participant’s RDM by calculating a Spearman correlation with a canonical RDM. We followed previously described procedures for calculating the typicality score (Dempsey-Jones et al., 2019; Ejaz et al., 2015; Kieliba et al., 2021; Wesselink et al., 2019). The canonical RDM was based on 7T finger movement fMRI data in an independently acquired cohort of healthy controls (n = 8). The S1 hand ROI used to calculate this canonical RDM was defined similarly as in the current study (see Wesselink et al., 2021 for details). Note that results were unchanged when calculating typicality scores using a canonical RDM based on the averaged RDM of the age-, sex-, and handedness-matched control group tested in this study (see Appendix 1). The typicality scores were Fisher r-to-z transformed prior to statistical analysis (the rs typicality scores are used solely for visualisation). Controls’ and SCI patients’ typicality scores were compared to each other and to those of a group of individuals with congenital hand loss (n = 13), hereafter one-handers, obtained in another study (data publicly available on https://osf.io/gmvua/; Wesselink et al., 2019). Congenital one-handers are born without a hand and do not have an S1 hand representation contralateral to the missing hand.
Finally, we performed multidimensional scaling (MDS) to visualise the distance structure of the RDM in an intuitive manner. MDS projects the higher-dimensional RDM into a lower-dimensional space, while preserving the inter-finger distance values as well as possible (Borg and Groenen, 2005). MDS was performed for each individual participant and then averaged per group after Procrustes alignment to remove arbitrary rotation induced by MDS.
Structural MRI analysis
Midsagittal tissue bridges analysis
Request a detailed protocolWe used sagittal T2w structural images of the cervical spinal cord at the lesion level to quantify spared tissue bridges. It has been shown that already in the sub-acute stage after SCI oedema and haemorrhage have largely resolved and hyperintense signal changes reliably reflect intramedullary neural damage (Huber et al., 2017; Pfyffer et al., 2019; Vallotton et al., 2019). We closely followed previously described procedures (Huber et al., 2017; Pfyffer et al., 2019; Vallotton et al., 2019). We used Jim 7.0 software (Xinapse Systems, Aldwincle, UK) for manual lesion segmentation at the lesion level, for which high intra- and interobserver reliability has previously been reported (Huber et al., 2017; Pfyffer et al., 2019). The experimenter conducting the manual segmentation was blinded to patient identity. We only included patients’ T2w scans if the lesion (i.e. hyperintense, CSF-filled cystic cavity) was clearly visible on the midsagittal slice. We excluded images of two patients with metal artefacts or insufficient data quality which would not allow a reliable quantification of lesion measures. Tissue bridges were defined as the relatively hypointense intramedullary region between the hyperintense CSF on one side and the cystic cavity on the other side. We assessed the width of ventral and dorsal tissue bridges on the midsagittal slice and summed these to get the total width of tissue bridges.
Cervical cross-sectional spinal cord area analysis
Request a detailed protocolWe used Jim 7.0 software (Xinapse Systems) to extract the cross-sectional spinal cord area at cervical level C2/3 of the spinal cord from the sagittal T2w scans. We used multi-planar reconstruction of sagittal images, resulting in 10 contiguous axial slices at C2/3 with a thickness of 2 mm (Losseff et al., 1996). Using the active-surface model from Horsfield et al., 2010, the cross-sectional spinal cord area was calculated semi-automatically for every slice and averaged over all 10 slices.
Statistical analysis
Request a detailed protocolStatistical analysis was carried out using SPSS (v25). Standard approaches were used for statistical analysis, as mentioned in the Results section. If normality was violated (assessed using the Shapiro–Wilk test), non-parametric statistical testing or robust ANOVAs (in RStudio v1.4; WRS2 package; Mair and Wilcox, 2020) were used. We used a Crawford–Howell t-test to compare single patients to the congenital and control groups (Corballis, 2009). All testing was two-tailed, and corrected p-values were calculated using the Benjamini–Hochberg procedure to control the FDR with q < 0.05. The correlational analysis was considered exploratory and we did not correct for multiple comparisons in this analysis.
Bayesian analysis was carried out using JASP (v0.12.2) for the main comparisons to investigate support for the null hypothesis with the Cauchy prior width set at 0.707 (i.e. JASP’s default). Following the conventional cut-offs, a BF smaller than 1/3 is considered substantial evidence in favour of the null hypothesis. A BF greater than 3 is considered substantial evidence, and a BF greater than 10 is considered strong evidence in favour of the alternative hypothesis. A BF between 1/3 and 3 is considered weak or anecdotal evidence (Dienes, 2014; Kass and Raftery, 1995).
Appendix 1
Percent signal change in finger-specific clusters
To assess whether finger movement activity levels were different between patients and controls, we created finger-specific ROIs and extracted the activity level of the corresponding finger movement for each participant. To create the finger-specific ROIs, we thresholded the probability finger surface maps that were created from the travelling wave data of the control group (see main text) at 25% (i.e. meaning that at least 5 out of 18 control participants needed to significantly activate a vertex for this vertex to be included in the ROI) and binarised. We then used the separately acquired blocked design data to extract the finger movement activity levels underlying these finger-specific ROIs. We first flipped the contrast images resulting from the fixed effects analysis (i.e. that was ran to average across the four blocked design runs) along the x-axis for the left-hand tested participants. Each participant’s contrast maps were then resampled to the FreeSurfer 2D average atlas, and the averaged z-standardised activity level was extracted for each finger movement vs. rest contrast underlying the finger-specific ROIs.

Finger-specific activity levels in finger-specific regions of interest (ROIs).
(A) Finger-specific ROIs were based on the control group’s binarised 25% probability travelling wave finger selectivity maps. White arrows indicate the central sulcus. A: anterior; P: posterior. (B) Finger movement activity levels in the corresponding finger-specific ROIs. There were no significant differences in activity levels between thetetraplegic patient and control groups. Controls are projected in grey; patients are projected in orange. Error bars show the standard error of the mean.
We compared the activity levels for each finger movement in the corresponding finger ROI (i.e. thumb movement activity in the thumb ROI, index finger movement activity in the index finger ROI, etc.) between groups. After correction for multiple comparisons, there was no significant difference between groups for the thumb (U = 93, p=0.37), index (t(30) = –0.003, p=0.99), middle (t(30) = 1.11, p=0.35), ring (t(30) = 2.02, p=0.13), or little finger (t(30) = 2.14, p=0.20).
Geodesic distance analysis
To assess whether the finger representations of the SCI patients were aligned differently and/or shifted compared to the control participants, the cortical distance was calculated between each participant’s peak finger activation and a reference anchor in the S1 foot cortex. We used the blocked design data for this analysis as the travelling wave paradigm does not lend itself to accurately extract peak finger activation locations due to the consecutive movement task.

Geodesic distance between each finger’s peak activated vertex and a cortical anchor.
Cortical geodesic distances were calculated between a reference anchor in the S1 foot cortex (MNI coordinates: −16.93, –32.06, 73.80) and the peak activated vertex per finger movement for each participant. Controls are projected in grey; spinal cord injury (SCI) patients are projected in orange. Error bars show the standard error of the mean.
Each participant’s fixed effect contrast maps that were already resampled to the FreeSurfer 2D average atlas were masked using the S1 hand ROI. The geodesic distance was then calculated between the peak activated vertex in the S1 hand cortex and a reference anchor in the S1 foot cortex (MNI coordinates: −16.93, –32.06, 73.80). Note that geodesic distance measures calculated onto average cortical surfaces are not confounded by inter-subject variability in gyrification, follow the anatomical constraints of the brain (i.e. do not cross white matter boundaries), and allow comparison between different subjects. This is a key advantage of this approach over using Euclidean distance measures (Mancini et al., 2019). A robust mixed ANOVA with a within-participants factor for finger (five levels: thumb, index, middle, ring, and little finger) and a between-participants factor for group (two levels: controls and patients) showed that, as expected, the geodesic distances were significantly different across fingers (i.e. a main effect for fingers; F(1,18.81) = 4.29, p=0.05). While the peak finger vertexes were located more lateral for the patient compared to the control group (i.e. a main effect for group; F(4,13.93) = 85.66, p<0.001), the geodesic distance pattern across the fingers was not significantly different across groups (i.e. no significant finger by group interaction; F(4,13.93) = 0.91, p=0.48). When testing for group differences per finger, the BF only revealed inconclusive evidence (BF >0.34 and < 1.88; note that we could not run a Bayesian ANOVA due to normality violations).
Typicality analysis using a canonical RDM based on the controls’ average RDM
To ensure that our typicality results did not change when using a canonical inter-finger RDM based on the age-, sex-, and handedness-matched subjects tested in this study, we recalculated the typicality scores of all participants using the averaged inter-finger RDM of our control sample as the canonical RDM. We found a strong and highly significant correlation between the typicality scores calculated using the canonical inter-finger RDM from the independent dataset (reported in the main text) and the typicality scores calculated using our controls’ average RDM. This was true for both the SCI patient (rs = 0.92, p<0.001) and control groups (rs = 0.78, p<0.001).
We then repeated all typicality analysis reported in the main text. As expected, we found the same results using the typicality scores calculated using our controls’ average RDM as when using the canonical inter-finger RDM from the independent dataset: There was a significant difference in typicality between tetraplegic patients, healthy controls, and congenital one-handers (H(2) = 27.61, p<0.001). We further found significantly higher typicality in controls compared to congenital one-handers (U = 0, p<0.001; BF10 = 76.11). Importantly, the typicality scores of the SCI patients were significantly higher than the congenital one-handers (U = 2, p<0.001; BF10 = 50.98), but not significantly different from the controls (U = 94, p=0.24; BF10 = 0.55). Number of years since SCI significantly correlated with hand representation typicality (rs = −0.54, p=0.05) and patients with more retained GRASSP motor function of the tested upper limb had more typical hand representations in S1 (rs = 0.58, p=0.03). There was no significant correlation between S1 hand representation typicality and GRASSP sensory function of the tested upper limb, spared midsagittal spinal tissue bridges at the lesion level, or cross-sectional spinal cord area (rs = 0.40, p=0.15, rs = 0.50, p=0.10, and rs = 0.48, p=0.08, respectively). An exploratory stepwise linear regression analysis revealed that years since SCI significantly predicted hand representation typicality in S1 with R2 = 0.33 (F(1,10) = 4.98, p=0.05). Motor function, sensory function, spared midsagittal spinal tissue bridges at the lesion level, and spinal cord area did not significantly add to the prediction (t = 1.31, p=0.22, t = 1.62, p=0.14, t = 1.70, p=0.12, and t = 1.09, p=0.30, respectively).
Data availability
Full details of the experimental protocol are available on clinicaltrials.gov under the number NCT03772548. Data is shared on https://osf.io/e8u95/.
References
-
Predictions not commands: Active inference in the motor systemBrain Structure & Function 218:611–643.https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-012-0475-5
-
Brain motor system function after chronic, complete spinal cord injuryBrain: A Journal of Neurology 128:2941–2950.https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awh648
-
Functional outcome following spinal cord injury: significance of motor-evoked potentials and ASIA scoresArchives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation 79:81–86.https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-9993(98)90213-1
-
Neural basis of touch and proprioception in primate cortexComprehensive Physiology 8:1575–1602.https://doi.org/10.1002/cphy.c170033
-
Organized Toe Maps in Extreme Foot UsersCell Reports 28:2748–2756.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2019.08.027
-
Using Bayes to get the most out of non-significant resultsFrontiers in Psychology 5:1–17.https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00781
-
Hand use predicts the structure of representations in sensorimotor cortexNature Neuroscience 18:1034–1040.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4038
-
BookFinger Representations in Sensorimotor Cortex Are Not Disrupted in Musicians’ DystoniaSociety for Neuroscience Annual MeetingSan Diego, USA: Society for Neuroscience Annual Meeting.
-
Automated manifold surgery: Constructing geometrically accurate and topologically correct models of the human cerebral cortexIEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging 20:70–80.https://doi.org/10.1109/42.906426
-
Intracortical microstimulation of human somatosensory cortexScience Translational Medicine 8:1–11.https://doi.org/10.1126/scitranslmed.aaf8083
-
Corticomotor representation to a human forearm muscle changes following cervical spinal cord injuryEuropean Journal of Neuroscience 34:1839–1846.https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07895.x
-
Disability, atrophy and cortical reorganization following spinal cord injuryBrain: A Journal of Neurology 134:1610–1622.https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awr093
-
Are midsagittal tissue bridges predictive of outcome after cervical spinal cord injury?Annals of Neurology 81:740–748.https://doi.org/10.1002/ana.24932
-
Large-scale reorganization in the somatosensory cortex and thalamus after sensory loss in macaque monkeysThe Journal of Neuroscience 28:11042–11060.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2334-08.2008
-
Outcome of the upper limb in cervical spinal cord injury: Profiles of recovery and insights for clinical studiesThe Journal of Spinal Cord Medicine 37:503–510.https://doi.org/10.1179/2045772314Y.0000000252
-
Bayes FactorsJournal of the American Statistical Association 90:773–795.https://doi.org/10.1080/01621459.1995.10476572
-
Reaffirming the link between chronic phantom limb pain and maintained missing hand representationCortex; a Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior 106:174–184.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2018.05.013
-
Neural basis of induced phantom limb pain reliefAnnals of Neurology 85:59–73.https://doi.org/10.1002/ana.25371
-
Investigating the stability of fine-grain digit somatotopy in individual human participantsThe Journal of Neuroscience 36:1113–1127.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1742-15.2016
-
Representational similarity analysis - connecting the branches of systems neuroscienceFrontiers in Systems Neuroscience 2:1–28.https://doi.org/10.3389/neuro.06.004.2008
-
Visually-driven maps in area 3bThe Journal of Neuroscience 38:1295–1310.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0491-17.2017
-
Corticocuneate projections are altered after spinal cord dorsal column lesions in New World monkeysThe Journal of Comparative Neurology 529:1669–1702.https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.25050
-
Responses of somatosensory area 2 neurons to actively and passively generated limb movementsJournal of Neurophysiology 109:1505–1513.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00372.2012
-
Robust statistical methods in R using the WRS2 packageBehavior Research Methods 52:464–488.https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-019-01246-w
-
Fine-grained nociceptive maps in primary somatosensory cortexThe Journal of Neuroscience 32:17155–17162.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3059-12.2012
-
Fine-Grained Mapping of Cortical Somatotopies in Chronic Complex Regional Pain SyndromeThe Journal of Neuroscience 39:9185–9196.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2005-18.2019
-
A Toolbox for Representational Similarity AnalysisPLOS Computational Biology 10:e1003553.https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003553
-
Afferent Input and Sensory Function after Human Spinal Cord InjuryJournal of Neurophysiology 119:134–144.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00354.2017
-
Chronic Cervical Spinal Cord Injury: DTI Correlates with Clinical and Electrophysiological MeasuresJournal of Neurotrauma 29:1556–1566.https://doi.org/10.1089/neu.2011.2027
-
Predictive Value of Midsagittal Tissue Bridges on Functional Recovery After Spinal Cord InjuryNeurorehabilitation and Neural Repair 35:33–43.https://doi.org/10.1177/1545968320971787
-
Fast robust automated brain extractionHuman Brain Mapping 17:143–155.https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.10062
-
Brain changes after spinal cord injury, a quantitative meta-analysis and reviewNeuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 90:272–293.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.04.018
-
Tonotopic Organization in Human Auditory Cortex Revealed by Progressions of Frequency SensitivityJournal of Neurophysiology 91:1282–1296.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.01125.2002
-
Localization of the motor hand area to a knob on the precentral gyrus A new landmarkBrain: A Journal of Neurology 120:141–157.https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/120.1.141
Decision letter
-
J Andrew PruszynskiReviewing Editor; Western University, Canada
-
Richard B IvrySenior Editor; University of California, Berkeley, United States
-
Sliman J BensmaiaReviewer; University of Chicago, United States
Our editorial process produces two outputs: (i) public reviews designed to be posted alongside the preprint for the benefit of readers; (ii) feedback on the manuscript for the authors, including requests for revisions, shown below. We also include an acceptance summary that explains what the editors found interesting or important about the work.
Acceptance summary:
This paper demonstrates that the spatial organization of the somatosensory cortex deteriorates very slowly following spinal cord injury that results in tetraplegia. The findings contribute to a developing story on how sensory representations are formed, changed and maintained and has implications for the development of brain-machine interfaces.
Decision letter after peer review:
Thank you for submitting your article "Finger somatotopy is preserved after tetraplegia but deteriorates over time" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by 3 peer reviewers, and the evaluation has been overseen by Andrew Pruszynski as Reviewing Editor and Richard Ivry as the Senior Editor. The following individuals involved in review of your submission have agreed to reveal their identity: Sliman J Bensmaia (Reviewer #3).
The reviewers have discussed their reviews with one another, and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this letter to help you prepare a revised submission.
Essential revisions:
1) The authors need to provide a clearer hypothesis about the mechanistic changes of the sensorimotor maps in the patients. This requires a thorough description of prior results and how these prior results motivated the present study and the analyses conducted. The reviewers emphasize that this will also require additional analyses that bring the present study into alignment with previous work. In this respect, Reviewer 1 and 2 indicate specific figures and analyses that need to be done and presented in a revised manuscript.
2) There is substantial concern that the key "typicality" analysis is not valid. As detailed by Reviewer 2 there are previous studies that have approached this issue but the present manuscript largely ignores this previous literature and uses their own ad hoc approach. The issue of defining typicality needs to be meaningfully (analytically) addressed in reference to previous established approaches.
3) There are several analytical points that require attention, either because the choices are unclear or that critical details are not reported.
4) Provide a more thorough Discussion, especially in the context of alternative explanations for the present findings. The reviewers all raise important concerns that the present account is very motor-effector dominated and dismisses other potential explanations. In particular, pay close attention to the comments raised by Reviewer #1 in terms of attentional mechanisms and how they may confound the present empirical results. This limitation needs to be addressed either experimentally, or given that possibility is unlikely, explicitly in the Discussion.
Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):
Abstract: It is unclear what evidence serves the foundation for the authors' claim that rehabilitation approaches would be most promising when applied within the first years after injury. I can see why this might be speculated, but is there any previous work showing that those with in-tact hand representations are more conducive to rehabilitation than those with deteriorated representations?
Lines 106-116: This section somewhat gives the impression that complete digit maps were found in everyone. Consider tempering this a bit by pointing out that, despite all maps displaying characteristics of typical digit maps, some digit representations are missing. That said, I appreciate the authors including each participant's map in the main paper – very nice to see all the examples!
Line 180: Upon first read, I misinterpreted the statement that "congenital one-handers do not have a missing hand representation". Consider hashing this out a bit more by explicitly stating that they do not develop a cortical representation for the missing body part and hence do not have a hand representation unlike amputees that develop the representation before losing the hand – and hence have a "missing hand representation". This notion is made a bit clearer on lines 266-267; maybe bring forward.
Line 527-529: Given that the map wasn't identified in the patient that reported performing imagined movement as well as the authors' emphasis that the movement was attempted and not imagined (lines 85-86, plus methods), consider adding a bit in the discussion regarding the difference between attempting and imagining a movement – and why they might lead to different activity patterns.
Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):
– I would suggest that the authors perform all analyses including the factor of finger, and that they report finger-specific statistics and tables on all reported analyses. Indeed, looking at the individual maps, it seems that in the patients, fingers are missing. It is also evident that the thumb seems more distant in controls compared to patients. This has to be tested for.
– I would suggest that the authors report % signal change both for the averaged map, for each single finger representation, and for the peak voxels of each finger.
– Prior results on SCI patients have to be described in more detail, also with respect to the used methods (TMS, fMRI).
– To target no 4 in the public review: Remove analyses.
– Conduct analyses on representation order and alignment, map size, and similarity and argue accordingly. Also, the conducted ANOVA is not mentioned in the Methods section, where I indeed wondered why a repeated measure ANOVA was conducted and not a mixed ANOVA with the factor "group". How do the authors want to detect group differences when "group" is not a factor in the ANOVA?
– Methods. It is not clear to me how the mean r was calculated. From what I understand, the authors use the higher r-value as a winner-takes-it-all approach to say which finger wins over the other fingers. When I understand correctly, no statistical threshold is applied here. Therefore, am I correct in assuming that a non-thresholded r map was then transferred to FDR correction?
[Editors' note: further revisions were suggested prior to acceptance, as described below.]
Thank you for resubmitting your work entitled "Finger somatotopy is preserved after tetraplegia but deteriorates over time" for further consideration by eLife. Your revised article has been reviewed by 3 reviewers and the evaluation has been overseen by Andrew Pruszynski as the Reviewing Editor and Richard Ivry as the Senior Editor.
The manuscript has been substantially improved and there remain only some small issues that need to be addressed as described below by Reviewers #1 and #2. Please play particular attention to (1) ensuring the abstract is clear and (2) that the finger specific RSA analysis is included in the supplement. We will not send this out to external review again.
Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):
I appreciate the thorough response from the authors and believe that the revised manuscript presents a much more complete and balanced view. My only remaining comment is that although I find the RSA analysis to be appropriate here, I disagree with the authors' claims that it has become the "most common" / "gold standard" approach to investigating inter-finger overlap / somatotopy. And I'm not confident that you'd find consensus for this position among those in the field. As such, consider walking this claim back a bit.
Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):
Overall
I thank the authors for performing additional analyses that improved the quality of manuscript and significantly strengthened the claims they made.
Concept
I thank the authors for adding more background information on nonhuman primates, behavioral and fMRI human studies to argue what (or what not) to expect in their study results. Nevertheless, I still think that the major question posed in the abstract is still not accurate. They write in the abstract:
It is not clear "whether somatotopic representations can be preserved despite alterations in net activity". This is really confusing because in the manuscript, the authors do not show alterations in net activity. In addition, in response to both reviewer 2 and 3, the authors removed the formulation "preserved" from the discussion. Please also remove this from the abstract (at present it is still used 2 times). In addition, as argued by reviewer 1, if attention is one of the mechanisms that triggers the activity, it is not clear whether attention-related net activity should be higher or lower. I would therefore suggest to be precise also in the abstract and write that they investigate "whether somatotopic representations can be activated topographically despite reduced or absent afferent input" (or similar).
Analyses/Results
I thank the authors for conducting an ANOVA including the factors group and finger pair for the RSA, and for conducting finger-specific analyses using percent signal change. It is interesting to see that there is no interaction between group and finger pair, and that individual fingers do not differ in signal change. The authors then say that they conducted individual finger analyses where the BF revealed inconclusive evidence for the RSA analyses. Later in the response letter, they say that those analyses have been included as figure supplement into the manuscript: however, within the material downloaded, I could not find these figures. If the finger pair-specific RSA values are therefore not yet included in the supplemental material, I would suggest adding those because 1. Prior studies hint towards finger-specific differences and researchers would like to see what this study revealed in this respect, 2. The amplitude analyses provide a hint towards differences for D4 that would be interesting to inspect for the RSA results too.
It is good to see that the typicality results did not change when using data of the control group as a basis. This makes this analysis much more convincing. Given the authors argue that it would be good to always use the same data set to compare patients against, in my view, it would be of great benefit for the community if the data and results could be made publicly available (in particular the data taken from the Wesselink study) so that potentially other researchers can compare their own results against those.
Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):
I am satisfied with the revisions.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.67713.sa1Author response
Essential revisions:
1) The authors need to provide a clearer hypothesis about the mechanistic changes of the sensorimotor maps in the patients. This requires a thorough description of prior results and how these prior results motivated the present study and the analyses conducted. The reviewers emphasize that this will also require additional analyses that bring the present study into alignment with previous work. In this respect, Reviewer 1 and 2 indicate specific figures and analyses that need to be done and presented in a revised manuscript.
We have revised the Introduction accordingly and conducted the analysis suggested by the reviewers. For more details, please see our responses to the specific comments of the reviewers.
2) There is substantial concern that the key "typicality" analysis is not valid. As detailed by Reviewer 2 there are previous studies that have approached this issue but the present manuscript largely ignores this previous literature and uses their own ad hoc approach. The issue of defining typicality needs to be meaningfully (analytically) addressed in reference to previous established approaches.
We respectfully disagree that the typicality measure is not standard, invalid, or inaccurate. The representational structure of the fingers (assessed using RSA) has been shown to depict the invariant organization of S1 better than the exact spatial distribution of finger selective clusters (Ejaz et al., 2015). RSA-based measures are furthermore not prone to some of the problems of the measurements of finger selectivity (e.g., dependence on map thresholds). Indeed, over the past years RSA has become the gold standard to investigate somatotopy of finger representations, both in healthy (e.g. Akselrod et al., 2017; Ejaz et al., 2015; Kieliba et al., 2021; Kolasinski et al., 2016; Sanders et al., 2019) and patient populations (e.g. Dempsey-Jones et al., 2019; Ejaz et al., 2016; Kikkert et al., 2016; Wesselink et al., 2019). Moreover, various papers have been published in eLife and elsewhere that used the same RSA-based typicality criteria to assess plasticity in finger representations (Dempsey-Jones et al., 2019; Ejaz et al., 2015; Kieliba et al., 2021; Wesselink et al., 2019). We have added clarification regarding this to Introduction and Methods of the manuscript.
3) There are several analytical points that require attention, either because the choices are unclear or that critical details are not reported .
We thank the reviewers for pointing out this lack of clarity. We have adjusted our text to clarify these choices and added the suggested figures and analysis. Please see our response to the reviewers’ specific comments for further details.
4) Provide a more thorough Discussion, especially in the context of alternative explanations for the present findings. The reviewers all raise important concerns that the present account is very motor-effector dominated and dismisses other potential explanations. In particular, pay close attention to the comments raised by Reviewer #1 in terms of attentional mechanisms and how they may confound the present empirical results. This limitation needs to be addressed either experimentally, or given that possibility is unlikely, explicitly in the Discussion.
We agree that a more balanced speculation of the potential mechanisms underlying the phenomenon of preserved finger mapping would be valuable and have therefore adjusted the Discussion accordingly.
Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):
Abstract: It is unclear what evidence serves the foundation for the authors' claim that rehabilitation approaches would be most promising when applied within the first years after injury. I can see why this might be speculated, but is there any previous work showing that those with in-tact hand representations are more conducive to rehabilitation than those with deteriorated representations?
This is a speculation and we have removed this statement from the abstract in the revised manuscript. Instead, we now state the following:
Revised text Abstract:
“However, over years since SCI the hand representation somatotopy deteriorated, suggesting that somatotopic hand representations are more easily targeted within the first years after SCI.”
Lines 106-116: This section somewhat gives the impression that complete digit maps were found in everyone. Consider tempering this a bit by pointing out that, despite all maps displaying characteristics of typical digit maps, some digit representations are missing. That said, I appreciate the authors including each participant's map in the main paper – very nice to see all the examples!
We agree with the reviewer and we therefore removed the sentence stating that the patient and control maps were qualitatively similar between patients and controls. We instead state that despite all maps displaying aspects of characteristic finger maps, some finger representations were not visible in the thresholded patient and control maps.
Revised text Results:
“Overall, we found aspects of somatotopic finger selectivity in the maps of SCI patients’ hands, in which neighbouring clusters showed selectivity for neighbouring fingers in contralateral S1, similar to those observed in eighteen age-, sex-, and handedness-matched healthy controls. Notably, a characteristic hand map was even found in a patient who suffered complete paralysis and sensory deprivation of the hands (Figure 2, patient map 1; patient S01). Despite most maps (Figure 2, except patient map 3) displaying aspects of characteristic finger selectivity, some finger representations were not visible in the thresholded patient and control maps.”
Line 180: Upon first read, I misinterpreted the statement that "congenital one-handers do not have a missing hand representation". Consider hashing this out a bit more by explicitly stating that they do not develop a cortical representation for the missing body part and hence do not have a hand representation unlike amputees that develop the representation before losing the hand – and hence have a "missing hand representation". This notion is made a bit clearer on lines 266-267; maybe bring forward.
We thank the reviewer for highlighting this to us. We have added the suggested text to these lines in our revised manuscript.
Revised text Results:
“Congenital one-handers are born without a hand and therefore do not have a cortical representation of the missing hand (unlike amputees who develop the representation before losing the hand and hence have a ‘missing hand representation’; Wesselink et al., 2019).”
Line 527-529: Given that the map wasn't identified in the patient that reported performing imagined movement as well as the authors' emphasis that the movement was attempted and not imagined (lines 85-86, plus methods), consider adding a bit in the discussion regarding the difference between attempting and imagining a movement – and why they might lead to different activity patterns.
We thank the reviewer for their suggestion. We are however hesitant to make any strong claims about the putative difference between imagined and attempted movements based on the anecdotal report of this patient. We have revised the manuscript to stress the fact that after the measurement, this patient anecdotally mentioned that he had trouble performing the attempted movements. We are hesitant to speculate about what he might have done instead since we did not include a systematic debriefing procedure to document the patients’ strategies in sufficient detail (e.g., was this patient performing kinaesthetic or visual imagery, or was rather something different). We are aware this is a weakness and have therefore added this debriefing procedure to our follow-up studies.
Revised text Methods:
“We were unable to find a characteristic hand map in one patient who anecdotally reported post hoc that he was unable to perform attempted finger movements.”
Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):
– I would suggest that the authors perform all analyses including the factor of finger, and that they report finger-specific statistics and tables on all reported analyses. Indeed, looking at the individual maps, it seems that in the patients, fingers are missing. It is also evident that the thumb seems more distant in controls compared to patients. This has to be tested for.
With regards to the reviewer’s concern about missing fingers – note that the RSA analysis does not depend on any thresholding (i.e., it works with unthresholded data) and missing fingers due to clusters not surviving thresholding will not impact this analysis. A true missing finger would result in a low typicality score.
Following the reviewer’s suggestion, we have included individual participant inter-finger distance plots for both the controls and patients as Figure 3—figure supplement 2 and Figure 3—figure supplement 3, respectively. We believe this is more readily readable and interpretable than a table containing the 10 inter-finger distance scores for all 32 participants. Instead, these values have been made available online on https://osf.io/e8u95/.
Lastly, we have conducted a geodesic distance analysis to assess whether the finger representations of the SCI patients were aligned differently and/or shifted compared to the control participants. Note however that RSA-based inter-finger distance patterns have been shown to depict the invariant representational structure of fingers in S1 and M1 better than the size, shape, and exact location of the areas activated by finger movements (Ejaz et al., 2015). The geodesic cortical distance was calculated between each participant’s peak finger activation and a reference anchor in the S1 foot cortex. We used the blocked design data for this analysis as the travelling wave paradigm does not lend itself to accurately extract peak finger activation locations due to the consecutive movement task. A robust mixed ANOVA with a within-participants factor for finger (5 levels: thumb, index, middle, ring, and little finger) and a between-participants factor for group (2 levels: controls and patients) showed that, as expected, the geodesic distances were significantly different across fingers (i.e., a main effect for fingers; F(1,18.81) = 4.29, p = 0.05). While the peak finger vertexes were located more lateral for the patient compared to the control group (i.e., a main effect for group; F(4,13.93) = 85.66, p < 0.001), the geodesic distance pattern across the fingers was not significantly different across groups (i.e., no significant finger*group interaction; F(4,13.93) = 0.91, p = 0.48). When testing for group differences per finger, the BF only revealed inconclusive evidence (BF > 0.34 and < 1.88; note that we could not run a Bayesian ANOVA due to normality violations). We have added this analysis to Appendix 1.
Revised text Methods:
“We further compared activity levels in finger-specific ROIs in S1 between groups and conducted a geodesic distance analysis to assess whether the finger representations of SCI patients were aligned differently and/or shifted compared to the control group (see Appendix 1).”
Please see Appendix 1, “Geodesic distance analysis” section for the revised text.
– I would suggest that the authors report % signal change both for the averaged map, for each single finger representation, and for the peak voxels of each finger.
We have adjusted our analysis, results, and figures in our main manuscript such that we now report % signal change rather than averaged z-standardised BOLD activity in the revised main manuscript.
– Prior results on SCI patients have to be described in more detail, also with respect to the used methods (TMS, fMRI).
We revised our Introduction accordingly.
– To target no 4 in the public review: Remove analyses.
We respectfully disagree with the reviewer on removing this analysis. We elaborated on the validity of our typicality analysis in response to public review comment 2.6.
– Conduct analyses on representation order and alignment, map size, and similarity and argue accordingly. Also, the conducted ANOVA is not mentioned in the Methods section, where I indeed wondered why a repeated measure ANOVA was conducted and not a mixed ANOVA with the factor "group". How do the authors want to detect group differences when "group" is not a factor in the ANOVA?
We have removed the sentence saying that ‘qualitatively the position, order of finger preference, and extent of the hand maps were generally similar between patients and controls’. We indeed do not support this claim with quantitative analysis. As per the suggestion of reviewer 1, we now state that despite all maps (except patient map 3) displaying aspects of characteristic finger maps, some finger representations were not visible in the thresholded patient and control maps.
As also indicated in our response to comment 2.11, to assess whether the maps of the SCI patients were aligned differently and/or shifted compared to the control participants, we extracted the geodesic distance per finger on a normalised cortical surface from each participant’s peak activated vertex to a reference vertex placed in the S1 foot cortex.
The ANOVA we conducted was an ANOVA with a within-participants factor for finger pair (3 levels: same, neighbouring and non-neighbouring finger pairs) and a between-participants factor for group (2 levels: controls and patients). We clarified this in the revised manuscript.
Revised text Methods:
“We tested whether the somatotopic relationship in the DOCs was different in controls and patients using a robust mixed ANOVA with a within-participants factor for finger pair (3 levels: same, neighbouring and non-neighbouring finger pairs) and a between-participants factor for group (2 levels: controls and patients).”
Revised text Results:
“Overall, split-half consistency was not significantly different between patients and controls, as tested using a robust mixed ANOVA (see Figure 2C; F(1,20.32) = 0.51, p = 0.48).”
– Methods. It is not clear to me how the mean r was calculated. From what I understand, the authors use the higher r-value as a winner-takes-it-all approach to say which finger wins over the other fingers. When I understand correctly, no statistical threshold is applied here. Therefore, am I correct in assuming that a non-thresholded r map was then transferred to FDR correction?
We thank the reviewer for pointing out the lack of clarity. After calculating the r-values using the cross-correlation approach, the r-values were normalized using a Fisher r-to-z transformation. Following this, we used a winner-take-all approach to assign each voxel to one finger/lag exclusively based on the maximum z-value. We then calculated the threshold using the FDR q < 0.05 criterion based on winner-take-all finger/lag z-valued maps in native space (3D) and applied this threshold to the winner-take-all maps on the surface. We have clarified this in the Methods.
Revised text Methods:
“This resulted in 25 r-values per voxel per run that were normalised using a Fisher r-to-z transformation. […] Thresholding was applied to the winner-take-all finger-specific maps and lag-specific maps on the cortical surface using a false discovery criterion q < 0.05 based on the native (3D) values.”
References:
Adams RA, Shipp S, Friston KJ. 2013. Predictions not commands: Active inference in the motor system. Brain Struct Funct. doi:10.1007/s00429-012-0475-5
Akselrod M, Martuzzi R, Serino A, van der Zwaag W, Gassert R, Blanke O. 2017. Anatomical and functional properties of the foot and leg representation in areas 3b, 1 and 2 of primary somatosensory cortex in humans: A 7T fMRI study. Neuroimage 159:473–487. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.06.021
Armenta Salas M, Bashford L, Kellis S, Jafari M, Jo H, Kramer D, Shanfield K, Pejsa K, Lee B, Liu CY, Andersen RA. 2018. Proprioceptive and cutaneous sensations in humans elicited by intracortical microstimulation. eLife 7:1–11. doi:10.7554/eLife.32904
Berlot E, Prichard G, O’Reilly J, Ejaz N, Diedrichsen J. 2019. Ipsilateral finger representations in the sensorimotor cortex are driven by active movement processes, not passive sensory input. J Neurophysiol 121:418–426. doi:10.1152/jn.00439.2018
Besle J, Sánchez-Panchuelo R-M, Bowtell R, Francis S, Schluppeck D. 2013. Single-subject fMRI mapping at 7 T of the representation of fingertips in S1: a comparison of event-related and phase-encoding designs. J Neurophysiol 109:2293–305. doi:10.1152/jn.00499.2012
Bruurmijn MLCM, Pereboom IPL, Vansteensel MJ, Raemaekers MAH, Ramsey NF. 2017. Preservation of hand movement representation in the sensorimotor areas of amputees. Brain 140:3166–3178. doi:10.1093/brain/awx274
Burman DD, Lie-Nemeth T, Brandfonbrener AG, Parisi T, Meyer JR. 2009. Altered finger representations in sensorimotor cortex of musicians with focal dystonia: Precentral cortex. Brain Imaging Behav. doi:10.1007/s11682-008-9046-z
Chand P, Jain N. 2015. Intracortical and thalamocortical connections of the hand and face representations in somatosensory area 3b of macaque monkeys and effects of chronic spinal cord injuries. J Neurosci 35:13475–13486. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2069-15.2015
Chen LM, Qi H-X, Kaas JH. 2012. Dynamic Reorganization of Digit Representations in Somatosensory Cortex of Nonhuman Primates after Spinal Cord Injury. J Neurosci 32:14649–14663. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1841-12.2012
Cramer SC, Lastra L, Lacourse MG, Cohen MJ. 2005. Brain motor system function after chronic, complete spinal cord injury. Brain 128:2941–2950. doi:10.1093/brain/awh648
Da Costa S, Saenz M, Clarke S, van der Zwaag W. 2014. Tonotopic Gradients in Human Primary Auditory Cortex: Concurring Evidence From High-Resolution 7 T and 3 T fMRI. Brain Topogr 28:66–69. doi:10.1007/s10548-014-0388-0
Da Rocha Amaral S, Sanchez Panchuelo RM, Francis S. 2020. A Data-Driven Multi-scale Technique for fMRI Mapping of the Human Somatosensory Cortex. Brain Topogr 33:22–36. doi:10.1007/s10548-019-00728-6
Delhaye BP, Long KH, Bensmaia SJ. 2019. Neural basis of touch and proprioception in primate cortex. Compr Physiol 8:1575–1602. doi:10.1002/cphy.c170033
Dempsey-Jones H, Wesselink DB, Friedman J, Makin TR. 2019. Organized Toe Maps in Extreme Foot Users. Cell Rep 28:2748-2756.e4. doi:10.1016/j.celrep.2019.08.027
Deyoe EA, Carman GJ, Bandettini P, Glickman S, Wieser J, Cox R, Miller D, Neitz J. 1996. Mapping striate and extrastriate visual areas in human cerebral cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 93:2382–2386. doi:10.1073/pnas.93.6.2382
Ejaz N, Hamada M, Diedrichsen J. 2015. Hand use predicts the structure of representations in sensorimotor cortex. Nat Neurosci 18:1034–1040.
Ejaz N, Sadnicka A, Wiestler T, Butler K, Edwards M, Diedrichsen J. 2016. Finger representations in sensorimotor cortex are not disrupted in musicians’ dystoniaSociety for Neuroscience Annual Meeting, San Diego, USA.
Engel SA, Glover GG, Wandell BA. 1997. Retintopic organization in human visual cortex and the spatial precision of functional MRI. Cereb Cortex 7:181–192. doi:10.1093/cercor/7.2.181
Fassett HJ, Turco C V., El-Sayes J, Nelson AJ. 2018. Alterations in motor cortical representation of muscles following incomplete spinal cord injury in humans. Brain Sci. doi:10.3390/brainsci8120225
Fifer MS, McMullen DP, Thomas TM, Osborn LE, Nickl RW, Candrea DN, Pohlmeyer EA, Thompson MC, Anaya M, Schellekens W, Ramsey NF, Bensmaia SJ, Anderson WS, Wester BA, Crone NE, Celnik PA, Cantarero GL, Tenore F V. 2020. Intracortical Microstimulation Elicits Human Fingertip Sensations. medRxiv 2020.05.29.20117374.
Flesher SN, Collinger JL, Foldes ST, Weiss JM, Downey JE, Tyler-Kabara EC, Bensmaia SJ, Schwartz AB, Boninger ML, Gaunt RA. 2016. Intracortical microstimulation of human somatosensory cortex. Sci Transl Medicne 1–11. doi:10.1126/scitranslmed.aaf8083
Florence SL. 1998. Large-Scale Sprouting of Cortical Connections After Peripheral Injury in Adult Macaque Monkeys. Science (80- ) 282:1117–1121. doi:10.1126/science.282.5391.1117
Freund P, Rothwell J, Craggs M, Thompson AJ, Bestmann S. 2011a. Corticomotor representation to a human forearm muscle changes following cervical spinal cord injury. Eur J Neurosci 34:1839–1846. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07895.x
Freund P, Schmidlin E, Wannier T, Bloch J, Mir A, Schwab ME, Rouiller EM. 2006. Nogo-A-specific antibody treatment enhances sprouting and functional recovery after cervical lesion in adult primates. Nat Med. doi:10.1038/nm1436
Freund P, Weiskopf N, Ward NS, Hutton C, Gall A, Ciccarelli O, Craggs M, Friston K, Thompson AJ. 2011b. Disability, atrophy and cortical reorganization following spinal cord injury. Brain 134:1610–1622. doi:10.1093/brain/awr093
Halder P, Kambi N, Chand P, Jain N. 2018. Altered Expression of Reorganized Inputs as They Ascend From the Cuneate Nucleus to Cortical Area 3b in Monkeys With Long-Term Spinal Cord Injuries. Cereb Cortex 28:3922–3938. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhx256
Henderson LA, Gustin SM, Macey PM, Wrigley PJ, Siddall PJ. 2011. Functional reorganization of the brain in humans following spinal cord injury: Evidence for underlying changes in cortical anatomy. J Neurosci 31:2630–2637. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2717-10.2011
Hotz-Boendermaker S, Funk M, Summers P, Brugger P, Hepp-Reymond MC, Curt A, Kollias SS. 2008. Preservation of motor programs in paraplegics as demonstrated by attempted and imagined foot movements. Neuroimage 39:383–394. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.065
Huber E, Lachappelle P, Sutter R, Curt A, Freund P. 2017. Are midsagittal tissue bridges predictive of outcome after cervical spinal cord injury? Ann Neurol 81:740–748. doi:10.1002/ana.24932
Jain N, Qi H-X, Collins CE, Kaas JH. 2008. Large-scale reorganization in the somatosensory cortex and thalamus after sensory loss in macaque monkeys. J Neurosci 28:11042–11060. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2334-08.2008
Jutzeler CR, Freund P, Huber E, Curt A, Kramer JLK. 2015. Neuropathic pain and functional reorganization in the primary sensorimotor cortex after spinal cord injury. J Pain 16:1256–1267. doi:10.1016/j.jpain.2015.08.008
Kambi N, Halder P, Rajan R, Arora V, Chand P, Arora M, Jain N. 2014. Large-scale reorganization of the somatosensory cortex following spinal cord injuries is due to brainstem plasticity. Nat Commun 5:3602. doi:10.1038/ncomms4602
Kieliba P, Clode D, Maimon-mor RO, Makin TR. 2021. Robotic hand augmentation drives changes in neural body representation. Sci Robot 6:1–14. doi:10.1126/scirobotics.abd7935
Kikkert S, Kolasinski J, Jbabdi S, Tracey I, Beckmann CF, Johansen-Berg H, Makin TR. 2016. Revealing the neural fingerprints of a missing hand. eLife 5:e15292. doi:10.7554/eLife.15292
Kokotilo K, Eng J, Curt A. 2009. Reorganization and preservation of motor control of the brain in spinal cord injury: a systematic review. J Neurotrauma 26:2113–2126. doi:10.1089/neu.2008.0688.Reorganization
Kokotilo KJ, Eng JJ, Curt A. 2009. Reorganization and preservation of motor control of the brain in spinal cord injury: A systematic review. J Neurotrauma 26:2113–2126. doi:10.1089/neu.2008.0688
Kolasinski J., Makin TR, Jbabdi S, Clare S, Stagg CJ, Johansen-Berg H. 2016. Investigating the stability of fine-grain digit somatotopy in individual human participants. J Neurosci 36:1113–1127.
Kolasinski James, Makin TR, Logan JP, Jbabdi S, Clare S, Stagg CJ, Johansen-Berg H. 2016. Perceptually relevant remapping of human somatotopy in 24 hours. eLife 5:1–15. doi:10.7554/eLife.17280
Kriegeskorte N, Mur M, Bandettini P. 2008. Representational similarity analysis – connecting the branches of systems neuroscience. Front Syst Neurosci 2:1–28. doi:10.3389/neuro.06.004.2008
Kuehn E, Haggard P, Villringer A, Pleger B, Sereno MI. 2018. Visually-driven maps in area 3b. J Neurosci 38:0491–17. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0491-17.2017
Levy WJ, Amassian VE, Traad M, Cadwell J. 1990. Focal magnetic coil stimulation reveals motor cortical system reorganized in humans after traumatic quadriplegia. Brain Res. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(90)90738-W
London BM, Miller LE. 2013. Responses of somatosensory area 2 neurons to actively and passively generated limb movements. J Neurophysiol 109:1505–13. doi:10.1152/jn.00372.2012
Mancini F, Haggard P, Iannetti GD, Longo MR, Sereno MI. 2012. Fine-grained nociceptive maps in primary somatosensory cortex. J Neurosci 32:17155–17162. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3059-12.2012
Nili H, Wingfield C, Walther A, Su L, Marslen-Wilson W, Kriegeskorte N. 2014. A Toolbox for Representational Similarity Analysis. PLoS Comput Biol 10. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003553
Pfyffer D, Huber E, Sutter R, Curt A, Freund P. 2019. Tissue bridges predict recovery after traumatic and ischemic thoracic spinal cord injury. Neurology 93:e1550–e1560. doi:10.1212/WNL.0000000000008318
Pfyffer D, Vallotton K, Curt A, Freund P. 2021. Predictive Value of Midsagittal Tissue Bridges on Functional Recovery After Spinal Cord Injury. Neurorehabil Neural Repair 35:33–43. doi:10.1177/1545968320971787
Puckett AM, Bollmann S, Barth M, Cunnington R. 2017. Measuring the effects of attention to individual fingertips in somatosensory cortex using ultra-high field (7T) fMRI. Neuroimage 161:179–187. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.08.014
Puckett AM, Bollmann S, Junday K, Barth M, Cunnington R. 2020. Bayesian population receptive field modeling in human somatosensory cortex. Neuroimage 208:116465. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116465
Sanders Z-B, Wesselink DB, Dempsey-Jones H, Makin TR. 2019. Similar somatotopy for active and passive digit representation in primary somatosensory cortex. bioRxiv 1–36. doi:10.1101/754648
Sereno MI, Dale AM, Reppas JB, Kwong KK, Belliveau JW, Brady TJ, Rosen BR, Tootell RBH. 1995. Borders of multiple visual areas in humans revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Science (80- ) 268:889–893. doi:10.1126/science.7754376
Solstrand Dahlberg L, Becerra L, Borsook D, Linnman C. 2018. Brain changes after spinal cord injury, a quantitative meta-analysis and review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 90:272–293. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.04.018
Streletz LJ, Belevich JKS, Jones SM, Bhushan A, Shah SH, Herbison GJ. 1995. Transcranial magnetic stimulation: Cortical motor maps in acute spinal cord injury. Brain Topogr. doi:10.1007/BF01202383
Talavage TM, Sereno MI, Melcher JR, Ledden PJ, Rosen BR, Dale AM. 2004. Tonotopic Organization in Human Auditory Cortex Revealed by Progressions of Frequency Sensitivity. J Neurophysiol 91:1282–1296. doi:10.1152/jn.01125.2002
Taub E, Elbert T, Candia V, Rockstroh B, Rau H, Pantev C, Altenmüller E, Sterr A. 1998. Alteration of digital representations in somatosensory cortex in focal hand dystonia. Neuroreport.
Topka H, Cohen LG, Cole RA, Hallett M. 1991. Reorganization of corticospinal pathways following spinal cord injury. Neurology. doi:10.1212/wnl.41.8.1276
Urbin MA, Royston DA, Weber DJ, Boninger ML, Collinger JL. 2019. What is the functional relevance of reorganization in primary motor cortex after spinal cord injury? Neurobiol Dis 121:286–295. doi:10.1016/j.nbd.2018.09.009
Vallotton K, Huber E, Sutter R, Curt A, Hupp M, Freund P. 2019. Width and neurophysiologic properties of tissue bridges predict recovery after cervical injury. Neurology 92:E2793–E2802. doi:10.1212/WNL.0000000000007642
Wesselink DB, Maimon-Mor R. 2017. Github.
Wesselink DB, van den Heiligenberg FM, Ejaz N, Dempsey-Jones H, Cardinali L, Tarall-Jozwiak A, Diedrichsen J, Makin TR. 2019. Obtaining and maintaining cortical hand representation as evidenced from acquired and congenital handlessness. eLife 8:1–19. doi:10.7554/eLife.37227
Wrigley PJ, Press SR, Gustin SM, Macefield VG, Gandevia SC, Cousins MJ, Middleton JW, Henderson LA, Siddall PJ. 2009. Neuropathic pain and primary somatosensory cortex reorganization following spinal cord injury. Pain 141:52–59. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2008.10.007
Wrigley PJ, Siddall PJ, Gustin SM. 2018. New evidence for preserved somatosensory pathways in complete spinal cord injury: A fMRI study. Hum Brain Mapp 39:588–598. doi:10.1002/hbm.23868
Zeharia N, Hertz U, Flash T, Amedi A. 2015. New whole-body sensory-motor gradients revealed using phase-locked analysis and verified using multivoxel pattern analysis and functional connectivity. J Neurosci 35:2845–2859. doi:10.1523/Jneurosci.4246-14.2015
[Editors' note: further revisions were suggested prior to acceptance, as described below.]
The manuscript has been substantially improved and there remain only some small issues that need to be addressed as described below by Reviewers #1 and #2. Please play particular attention to (1) ensuring the abstract is clear and (2) that the finger specific RSA analysis is included in the supplement. We will not send this out to external review again.
Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):
I appreciate the thorough response from the authors and believe that the revised manuscript presents a much more complete and balanced view. My only remaining comment is that although I find the RSA analysis to be appropriate here, I disagree with the authors' claims that it has become the "most common" / "gold standard" approach to investigating inter-finger overlap / somatotopy. And I'm not confident that you'd find consensus for this position among those in the field. As such, consider walking this claim back a bit.
Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):
Overall
I thank the authors for performing additional analyses that improved the quality of manuscript and significantly strengthened the claims they made.
Concept
I thank the authors for adding more background information on nonhuman primates, behavioral and fMRI human studies to argue what (or what not) to expect in their study results. Nevertheless, I still think that the major question posed in the abstract is still not accurate. They write in the abstract:
It is not clear "whether somatotopic representations can be preserved despite alterations in net activity". This is really confusing because in the manuscript, the authors do not show alterations in net activity. In addition, in response to both reviewer 2 and 3, the authors removed the formulation "preserved" from the discussion. Please also remove this from the abstract (at present it is still used 2 times). In addition, as argued by reviewer 1, if attention is one of the mechanisms that triggers the activity, it is not clear whether attention-related net activity should be higher or lower. I would therefore suggest to be precise also in the abstract and write that they investigate "whether somatotopic representations can be activated topographically despite reduced or absent afferent input" (or similar).
Analyses/Results
I thank the authors for conducting an ANOVA including the factors group and finger pair for the RSA, and for conducting finger-specific analyses using percent signal change. It is interesting to see that there is no interaction between group and finger pair, and that individual fingers do not differ in signal change. The authors then say that they conducted individual finger analyses where the BF revealed inconclusive evidence for the RSA analyses. Later in the response letter, they say that those analyses have been included as figure supplement into the manuscript: however, within the material downloaded, I could not find these figures. If the finger pair-specific RSA values are therefore not yet included in the supplemental material, I would suggest adding those because 1. Prior studies hint towards finger-specific differences and researchers would like to see what this study revealed in this respect, 2. The amplitude analyses provide a hint towards differences for D4 that would be interesting to inspect for the RSA results too.
It is good to see that the typicality results did not change when using data of the control group as a basis. This makes this analysis much more convincing. Given the authors argue that it would be good to always use the same data set to compare patients against, in my view, it would be of great benefit for the community if the data and results could be made publicly available (in particular the data taken from the Wesselink study) so that potentially other researchers can compare their own results against those.
Specifically:
– We removed our statement about RSA being the golden standard to investigate inter-finger overlap, as per the suggestion of Reviewer 1.
Revised text Methods:
“We estimated inter-finger overlap using RSA. Note that it is also possible to estimate somatotopic overlap from travelling wave data using an iterated Multigrid Priors (iMGP) method and population receptive field modelling (Da Rocha Amaral et al., 2020; Puckett et al., 2020).”
– We changed the terminology in our abstract according as per the suggestion of Reviewer 2.
Revised text Abstract:
“However, little is known about whether somatotopically-specific representations can be activated despite reduced or absent afferent hand inputs. […] We found that somatotopic hand representations can be activated through attempted finger movements in absence of sensory and motor hand functioning, and no spared spinal tissue bridges.
– We added a figure supplement to visualise inter-finger distances across finger pairs for controls and SCI patients, as per the suggestion of Reviewer 2.
Revised text Results:
“We then tested whether the inter-finger distances were different across finger pairs between controls and SCI patients using a robust mixed ANOVA with a within-participants factor for finger pair (10 levels) and a between-participants factor for group (2 levels: controls and patients; Figure 3—figure supplement 2).”
– The canonical RDM used for analysis has already been made publicly available on https://osf.io/e8u95/. In the revised manuscript we refer readers to the publicly available congenital amputees dataset (Wesselink et al., 2019) in the Methods section, according to the suggestion of Reviewer 2.
Revised text Methods:
“Controls’ and SCI patients’ typicality scores were compared to those of a group of individuals with congenital hand malformation (n = 13), hereafter one-handers, obtained in another study (data publicly available on https://osf.io/gmvua/; Wesselink et al., 2019).”
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.67713.sa2Article and author information
Author details
Funding
Swiss National Science Foundation (320030_175616)
- Nicole Wenderoth
ETH Zurich Postdoctoral Fellowship Program
- Sanne Kikkert
Swiss National Science Foundation (PCEFP3_181362 / 1)
- Patrick Freund
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Acknowledgements
We thank our participants for taking part in the study. We thank Lydia Kämpf, Nicolin Gauler, and Silvia Hofer for their assistance with data collection, Daan Wesselink for help with data analysis, and Tamar Makin for comments on the manuscript.
Ethics
Participants' informed consent was obtained according to the Declaration of Helsinki prior to study onset. Ethical approval was granted by the Kantonale Ethikkommission Zürich (KEK-2018-00937).
Senior Editor
- Richard B Ivry, University of California, Berkeley, United States
Reviewing Editor
- J Andrew Pruszynski, Western University, Canada
Reviewer
- Sliman J Bensmaia, University of Chicago, United States
Version history
- Preprint posted: February 9, 2021 (view preprint)
- Received: February 19, 2021
- Accepted: October 18, 2021
- Accepted Manuscript published: October 19, 2021 (version 1)
- Version of Record published: November 8, 2021 (version 2)
Copyright
© 2021, Kikkert et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 739
- Page views
-
- 91
- Downloads
-
- 9
- Citations
Article citation count generated by polling the highest count across the following sources: Crossref, PubMed Central, Scopus.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Cell Biology
- Neuroscience
The amyloid beta (Aβ) plaques found in Alzheimer’s disease (AD) patients’ brains contain collagens and are embedded extracellularly. Several collagens have been proposed to influence Aβ aggregate formation, yet their role in clearance is unknown. To investigate the potential role of collagens in forming and clearance of extracellular aggregates in vivo, we created a transgenic Caenorhabditis elegans strain that expresses and secretes human Aβ1-42. This secreted Aβ forms aggregates in two distinct places within the extracellular matrix. In a screen for extracellular human Aβ aggregation regulators, we identified different collagens to ameliorate or potentiate Aβ aggregation. We show that a disintegrin and metalloprotease a disintegrin and metalloprotease 2 (ADM-2), an ortholog of ADAM9, reduces the load of extracellular Aβ aggregates. ADM-2 is required and sufficient to remove the extracellular Aβ aggregates. Thus, we provide in vivo evidence of collagens essential for aggregate formation and metalloprotease participating in extracellular Aβ aggregate removal.
-
- Computational and Systems Biology
- Neuroscience
The cerebellar granule cell layer has inspired numerous theoretical models of neural representations that support learned behaviors, beginning with the work of Marr and Albus. In these models, granule cells form a sparse, combinatorial encoding of diverse sensorimotor inputs. Such sparse representations are optimal for learning to discriminate random stimuli. However, recent observations of dense, low-dimensional activity across granule cells have called into question the role of sparse coding in these neurons. Here, we generalize theories of cerebellar learning to determine the optimal granule cell representation for tasks beyond random stimulus discrimination, including continuous input-output transformations as required for smooth motor control. We show that for such tasks, the optimal granule cell representation is substantially denser than predicted by classical theories. Our results provide a general theory of learning in cerebellum-like systems and suggest that optimal cerebellar representations are task-dependent.