Exogenous capture accounts for fundamental differences between pro- and antisaccade performance
Abstract
To generate the next eye movement, oculomotor circuits take into consideration the physical salience of objects in view and current behavioral goals, exogenous and endogenous influences, respectively. However, the interactions between exogenous and endogenous mechanisms and their dynamic contributions to target selection have been difficult to resolve because they evolve extremely rapidly. In a recent study (Salinas et al., 2019), we achieved the necessary temporal precision using an urgent variant of the antisaccade task wherein motor plans are initiated early and choice accuracy depends sharply on when exactly the visual cue information becomes available. Empirical and modeling results indicated that the exogenous signal arrives ∼80 ms after cue onset and rapidly accelerates the (incorrect) plan toward the cue, whereas the informed endogenous signal arrives ∼25 ms later to favor the (correct) plan away from the cue. Here, we scrutinize a key mechanistic hypothesis about this dynamic, that the exogenous and endogenous signals act at different times and independently of each other. We test quantitative model predictions by comparing the performance of human participants instructed to look toward a visual cue or away from it under high urgency. We find that, indeed, the exogenous response is largely impervious to task instructions; it simply flips its sign relative to the correct choice, and this largely explains the drastic differences in psychometric performance between the two tasks. Thus, saccadic choices are strongly dictated by the alignment between salience and behavioral goals.
Editor's evaluation
When subjects are instructed to produce saccades away from suddenly appearing visual targets under time pressure, early saccades tend to be directed incorrectly to the peripheral target, suggesting that exogenous and endogenous signals that are related to the target position and instruction, respectively, compete to control the motor responses. In this study, the authors provide further evidence for the independence of these two processes by showing that they can account for the temporal evolution of correct saccades regardless of the instruction, stimulus luminance or motor bias.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.76964.sa0Introduction
The oculomotor system of primates specifies a new target to look at every 200–250 ms. Two components of visuospatial attention contribute to the underlying selection process: exogenous mechanisms, which respond to the salience of the objects in view (typically dictated by physical properties such as size, luminance, or motion), and endogenous mechanisms, which prioritize those objects according to their relevance to current behavioral goals (Itti and Koch, 2001; Theeuwes, 2010; Wolfe and Horowitz, 2017).
The antisaccade task, in which the subject is required to look away from a lone visual cue, is ideal for examining this distinction because it decouples stimulus encoding and response preparation (Munoz and Everling, 2004). For a correct antisaccade to take place, endogenous, top-down influences must override the natural tendency to look to the cue. Indeed, in both imaging studies in humans (Brown et al., 2007; Anderson et al., 2008) and single-neuron recordings in nonhuman primates (Everling et al., 1998; Everling et al., 1999; Everling and Munoz, 2000), antisaccade performance is generally characterized as a conflict between volitional and sensory-driven, reflexive responses. The capacity to resolve such conflict is considered so fundamental, that reaction time (RT) and accuracy measurements in the antisaccade task are commonly used as clinical markers for cognitive dysfunction (Klein and Foerster, 2001; Klein et al., 2003; Hutton and Ettinger, 2006; Fielding et al., 2009; Johansson et al., 2022).
This work has established key qualitative distinctions between exogenous and endogenous mechanisms. However, resolving their dynamic interactions and specific, real-time contributions to the saccade selection process has been difficult because that requires temporal resolution far greater than can be achieved with traditional psychophysical tasks.
In a recent study (Salinas et al., 2019), we used an urgent version of the antisaccade task (Figure 1a) to dissociate the exogenous and endogenous contributions to performance with millisecond precision. In the urgent task design (Stanford and Salinas, 2021), the go signal that instructs the subject to respond is given before the visual cue that specifies the correct choice (an eye movement away from the cue, in this case). This way, motor plans are initiated early, so participants guess on some trials and make informed choices on others, but critically, each outcome depends fundamentally on how much time the participant has to see the cue before committing to a response, a quantity that we call the raw processing time (rPT). If choice accuracy is then plotted as a function of rPT, the result is a novel psychometric measure — the tachometric curve — that describes the participant’s perceptual performance on a moment-by-moment basis (Stanford and Salinas, 2021).
In the urgent antisaccade experiment (Salinas et al., 2019), the tachometric curve revealed a unique feature: a range of processing times roughly between 90 and 130 ms in which performance plummeted to nearly 0% correct (Figure 2d). In other words, there was a range of conditions (i.e., states of motor preparation) indexed by processing time over which the cue onset would almost inevitably capture the saccade and lead to an error. Thereafter, for rPT ≳140 ms, accuracy increased steadily and reached nearly 100% correct.
We interpreted these data as the direct manifestation of the conflict between the early attentional pull toward the cue, which is salience-driven, involuntary, and transient, and the later intention to look away, which is task-dependent, voluntary, and sustained. This conflict is hardly detectable during active fixation, but becomes obvious when saccade plans are ongoing. As such, it was instantiated within a saccade competition model in which two opposing motor plans race against each other to trigger an eye movement to the right or to the left (Figure 2a–c). As elaborated below, these plans initially advance at randomly drawn rates, but afterward, time permitting, they are steered by perceptual information. The exogenous signal arrives early (∼80 ms after cue onset) and rapidly accelerates the plan toward the cue regardless of task instructions; the endogenous signal arrives (∼25 ms) later to accelerate the correct plan and decelerate the incorrect one as stipulated by the task. These dynamics replicated all aspects of the behavioral data in great detail.
Here, we leverage this quantitative framework to further test a fundamental conclusion derived from that study: that the exogenous modulation of ongoing motor plans is highly stereotyped and largely independent of behavioral context. We use the antisaccade model to predict the performance that should be observed during an urgent prosaccade task (Figure 1b) assuming that this independence hypothesis is correct, and we test the parameter-free predictions in human participants performing both urgent prosaccades and urgent antisaccades. In essence, the exogenous modulation should evolve with the same timecourse regardless of task instructions, and should favor the motor plan toward the cue in the same way whatever the state of development of the competing motor plans.
To anticipate the main findings, the differences between pro- and antisaccade performance are dramatic, but can be largely accounted for by a fixed, exogenously driven response (an exogenous response, for brevity) that is either aligned or misaligned with the endogenously defined goal.
Results
A quantitative framework for urgent anti- and prosaccade performance
In our previous study (Salinas et al., 2019), we developed a neurophysiologically feasible model that quantitatively replicated the rich psychophysical dataset obtained in the compelled antisaccade task (the CAS model). Now, with a minimal modification, we generate a model of the compelled prosaccade task (the CPS model). The modification corresponds to simply making the top-down, endogenous response favor a saccade to the cue instead of away from the cue — without changing anything else.
The CAS model consists of two variables, and , that represent oculomotor activity favoring saccades toward left and right locations (Figure 2a–c, red and black traces). The first one of these motor plans to exceed a fixed threshold level (Figure 2a–c, dashed lines) triggers a saccade, to the left if crosses the threshold first, or to the right if does. In each trial, and start increasing in response to the go signal, initially advancing with randomly drawn buildup rates. This ramping process may end in a random choice (i.e., a guess; Figure 2a) if the gap interval between the go and the cue onset is long and/or one of the initial buildup rates is high enough. Otherwise, time permitting, the arrival of the cue information modifies the ongoing motor plans in two ways.
First, when the cue onset is detected by the oculomotor circuit, the motor competition is biased toward the cue location during a time period that we refer to as the exogenous response interval, or ERI (Figure 2a–c, gray vertical shades). Specifically, this means that, during the ERI, the motor plan toward the cue is first briefly halted and then accelerated (Figure 2b, c, red traces during gray interval), whereas the motor plan toward the opposite, or ‘anti’, location is halted throughout (Figure 2b, c, black traces during gray interval). These modulations of the ongoing motor activity constitute the involuntary, exogenous response.
Then, after the ERI ends, once the cue location has been interpreted in accordance to task rules as ‘opposite to the target’, the motor selection process is steered toward the correct choice. Specifically, the (erroneous) plan toward the cue is decelerated (Figure 2c, red trace after gray interval) and the (correct) plan away from the cue is accelerated (Figure 2c, black trace after gray interval). These modulations informing the correct choice constitute the top-down, endogenous response.
With these elements in place, the CAS model was able to reproduce the full range of psychophysical results obtained in the urgent antisaccade task, both on average and in individual participants (with parameter values adjusted accordingly; Salinas et al., 2019). In particular, it quantitatively matched the full RT distributions for correct and error trials (see Figure 8 and its supplements in Salinas et al., 2019). Moreover, the model simulations accurately replicated the tachometric curve (Figure 2d, top), the function relating choice accuracy to rPT, which is the key behavioral metric in such urgent tasks. In the rest of the paper, we consider the CAS model with parameters fitted to the pooled data from six participants ( see Table 1, high-luminance cue, in Salinas et al., 2019). The tachometric curve simulated for this case (Figure 2d, top) directly illustrates the conflicting interaction between exogenous and endogenous mechanisms that characterizes the CAS task: When the cue is seen for less than 90 ms or so (as in Figure 2a), the saccades are uninformed and the success rate stays near chance. When the cue is seen for more than 140 ms or so (as in Figure 2c), most saccades are informed and the success rate rises steadily above chance and toward 100% correct. But in between, performance drops to nearly 0% correct, as a large fraction of saccades are drawn to the wrong location. Those saccades are captured by the cue (as in Figure 2b).
To generate predictions about performance in prosaccade trials, we modified the CAS model so that the endogenous signal accelerated the motor plan toward the cue and decelerated the competing plan toward the anti location. That is, we simply swapped the target and distracter designations for the two locations without altering any parameter values. In this way, in the resulting CPS model the exogenous modulation during the ERI still boosts the motor plan toward the cue, just as it did originally (Figure 2f, compare to panel b). Crucially, though, those motor plans that are propelled past the threshold, which previously resulted in erroneous captured saccades, now correspond to correct choices. Furthermore, the informed endogenous signal now typically acts to reinforce the motor plan that is already ahead by the end of the ERI (Figure 2g, compare to panel c). All this results in a predicted tachometric curve for prosaccade performance for which the success rate grows steadily with rPT, rising very early and very steeply (Figure 2d, bottom).
Specific model predictions
The fundamental assumption behind the CPS model just described is that the exogenous response is entirely insensitive to behavioral context, so the endogenous signal reinforces the correct choice in exactly the same way in the pro and anti tasks — that is, the acceleration and deceleration terms simply swap their spatial assignments — regardless of the state of development of the motor alternatives. This is one extreme in the spectrum of possible outcomes; opposite to this ‘full-independence’ scenario is the possibility that the exogenous and endogenous modulations are much different in one task than in the other; and, of course, one or both of these mechanisms could also change modestly across conditions. Hence, the more general question is, to what degree are the exogenous and endogenous mechanisms sensitive to behavioral context? We use the CPS model as a benchmark because it instantiates a conceptually simple hypothesis (independence) to yield parameter-free, quantitative predictions — which, as it turns out, to a first approximation are fairly accurate.
The CAS and CPS models make two specific, clear-cut predictions. The first one is that the upswing in performance during prosaccade trials should follow the same timecourse as the initial downswing in performance during antisaccade trials (Figure 3a, b). In other words, the initial departure from chance should exhibit the same dependence on rPT but going in opposite directions, toward 100% versus toward 0% correct. This is a direct consequence of the exogenous response reinforcing the motor plan toward the cue with the same timing and intensity in the two tasks.
The second prediction is about the rise toward asymptotic performance when there is a motor bias, that is, when the participant tends to guess more toward one side than the other. If such a bias exists, then during antisaccade trials in which the cue happens to appear on the preferred guessing side, the rise in success rate is expected to occur late (Figure 3c, red trace). In contrast, during antisaccade trials in which the cue happens to appear opposite to the preferred guessing side, the success rate should rise earlier (Figure 3c, black trace). This difference arises because the exogenous and endogenous modulations are always opposite, so it is more difficult for the endogenous signal to prevent a capture when the exogenous response and the motor bias reinforce each other. On the other hand, during prosaccade trials the rise in success rate is expected to be very similar regardless of where the cue appears relative to the guessing direction (Figure 3d). In this case, the exogenous and endogenous modulations are aligned and can easily overcome any bias in the initial motor plans, so the rise in success rate is always very steep.
These two predictions are complementary in that they probe bottom-up and top-down components of the selection process. The first one tests whether the sensory-driven exogenous response itself changes across tasks, whereas the second one tests whether the endogenously driven recovery depends on the state of development of the competing motor alternatives.
The exogenous response is highly insensitive to behavioral context
To test the model predictions, 10 participants (6 female, 4 male) were recruited. The experimental procedures were largely the same as in the earlier report (Salinas et al., 2019) and are summarized in Materials and methods. Any differences in approach are described there, along with analysis methods specific to this study.
All the participants performed pro and anti trials in two ways: in blocks of same-task trials, and in blocks of randomly interleaved pro- and antisaccades (Materials and methods). The experiment was designed this way because it seemed possible that the two conditions could lead to major differences in performance, via task-switching costs, for instance (Wylie and Allport, 2000; Monsell, 2003). However, the observed differences were, in fact, very small (Figure 4—figure supplement 1) and had no bearing on any of the subsequent analyses. Thus, in what follows, we consider all the trials of each task aggregated across blocked and interleaved conditions.
For the compelled antisaccade task, the pooled tachometric curve (Figure 4a, red trace), generated by combining the data across participants, replicated our earlier result (Salinas et al., 2019). It exhibited the characteristic drop in success rate due to potent oculomotor capture in the range between 90 and 140 ms, approximately, with the subsequent recovery reaching nearly 100% correct after ∼200 ms of cue viewing time. More importantly, for the compelled prosaccade task the shape of the pooled tachometric curve (Figure 4a, blue trace) was as predicted by the model. In this case, the success rate rose steadily, early, and very steeply.
We stress that the tachometric curve is the main metric in urgent tasks because it characterizes performance independently of RT (Stanford et al., 2010; Salinas et al., 2019; Stanford and Salinas, 2021; Seideman et al., 2022) or gap (Figure 4—figure supplement 2). These variables explain very little once rPT has been taken into account.
To evaluate the first prediction quantitatively, we compared the downswing in antisaccade performance to the upswing in prosaccade performance (Figure 4b). First, the pro curve was inverted so that it decreased monotonically, and then a shift analysis was carried out to determine the optimal shift along the rPT axis that would maximize the overlap between the two curves (Materials and methods). Whereas according to the prediction the optimal time shift should be zero (Figure 3b, arrow), experimentally, we found that the optimal shift was, on average, 3 ms within a 95% confidence interval (CI) of [1, 5] ms (Figure 4b, d). That is, in prosaccade trials, the effect of the exogenous response manifested about 3 ms earlier than in antisaccade trials. This time difference was of comparable magnitude across individual participants (Figure 4c), which had optimal shifts in the [−1, 10] ms range (Figure 4d). Also, the result was very similar when green and magenta cues were assigned to pro and anti trials, respectively (participants 1–5; Materials and methods), and when the color assignments were flipped (participants 6–10). In general, the upswing in prosaccade performance had just a slightly earlier onset than the downswing in antisaccade performance.
To confirm these results, we ran a variant of the experiment in which the cue could be of high or low luminance (Materials and methods). We knew that, in the antisaccade task, less luminant cues produce capture at longer processing times (Salinas et al., 2019), so we reasoned that the rise in prosaccade performance should demonstrate the same temporal dependence — if it is driven by the same exogenous signal. The low luminance level was chosen to elicit a robust rightward shift of the anti tachometric curve (Salinas et al., 2019), and the high-luminance level was identical to that in the main experiment. In this case, the cue luminance (high or low) and task type (pro or anti) were randomly and independently chosen in each trial, and data were collected from three participants.
As expected, in antisaccade trials the decrease in cue luminance resulted in a rightward shift of the tachometric curve (Figure 4e, red curves), which was of 35 ms ([33, 38] ms 95% CI), as determined by an optimal-shift analysis. And critically, a comparable delay was observed in prosaccade trials (Figure 4e, blue curves), for which the lower luminance resulted in a shift of 30 ms ([26, 34] ms 95% CI). The results were qualitatively the same for the three participants (Figure 4—figure supplement 3). In conclusion, the data indicate that, although they are not quite identical, the exogenous signal that rapidly propels prosaccade performance above chance must be very similar to the one that drives antisaccade performance below chance.
Visuomotor dynamics revealed by motor biases
The second prediction derived from the model applies when participants exhibit a motor bias, that is, a preference for looking to one side more than the other. Fortuitously, we did not have to manipulate the experimental conditions to induce such a preference because participants adopted one spontaneously.
When we analyzed our participants’ performance as a function of their prior choices, we noticed a trend: their guesses in a given trial were generally toward the location where they should have responded in the previous trial (i.e., the prior target location). The psychometric manifestation of this tendency is a systematic deviation from chance observed when the trials are sorted according to the history of prior target locations. Specifically, the tachometric curve conditioned on a repeated target location (Figure 5a, green trace) demonstrated a positive offset (above chance) in the range of processing times for which participants must guess (rPT ≲80 ms), whereas the tachometric curve conditioned on a switch in target location (Figure 5a, magenta trace) demonstrated a negative offset (below chance) in the same range. This effect was cumulative; the more target repetitions, the larger the offsets (Figure 5, compare across columns). It was also observed in most participants (Figure 5—figure supplement 1) and in both pro and anti trials.
Given this motor bias, we analyzed the tachometric curves conditioned on prior target location and measured the rise point in each case; for anti trials, this is the rPT at which the success rate is halfway between minimum and asymptotic (Figure 5a–c, gray vertical shades), and for pro trials, it is the rPT at which the success rate is halfway between chance and asymptotic (Materials and methods). We found that both aspects of the second model prediction were correct. In the antisaccade task, the rise toward asymptotic performance occurred late when the motor bias was aligned with the cue location (Figure 5a–c, magenta curves) and early when it was aligned with the anti, or target location (Figure 5a–c, green curves); whereas in the prosaccade task, the rise was essentially the same in the two cases (Figure 5d–f). It is notable that the effect in anti trials grew stronger as the bias became more extreme, and it is instructive to calculate by how much.
Consider the tachometric curves conditioned on one previous trial (Figure 5a). As the chance level went from 0.36 ([0.34, 0.39] 95% CI, AB trials) to 0.64 ([0.61, 0.66], AA trials), the rise point of the tachometric curve went from 154 ms ([149, 154] CI, AB trials) to 143 ms ([139, 144], AA trials). Taking the ratio of the differences gives a drop of approximately 39 ms when traversing the full chance range from 0 to 1; or equivalently, if the chance level increases by 0.1, the rise point of the curve should drop by about 3.9 ms. The reason why this number is of interest is that it can also be computed for the simulated data. To do this, we simply ran the model (as in Figure 2c) with different amounts of motor bias, and for each run we tracked the chance level and the rise point of the simulated tachometric curve (Materials and methods). According to this procedure, the predicted change in rise point over the full chance range was 40 ms ([36, 44] 95% CI; Figure 5—figure supplement 1b, blue line and circles), in excellent agreement with the experimental data. Although in this analysis there was considerable variance across individual participants (Figure 5—figure supplement 1), the model prediction was very much in line with the data pooled across participants (Figure 5—figure supplement 1b, compare triangles and blue circles).
Consistency in the strength of exogenous capture across participants
As expected, in the two tasks there were noticeable differences in performance between individual participants, and analysis of this variance provided further support for the two hypotheses underlying our model predictions.
For the antisaccade task, the tachometric curves of the participants were quite similar in their initial departure from chance, as saccades were increasingly captured by the cue, but they were much more variable during the later rise in success rate (Figure 6a). Across the individual curves, the drop point (the rPT for which the success rate is halfway between chance and the minimum; Materials and methods) ranged from 87 to 98 ms, whereas the rise point ranged from 133 to 203 ms. More importantly, the values for the drop point and the rise point were not significantly correlated (Figure 6c; 0.6, permutation test), consistent with the idea that, in each subject, the exogenous response that determines the former is independent of the endogenous process responsible for the latter.
For the prosaccade task, the rise point covered a range from 82 to 95 ms across participants (Figure 6b), similar to that of the drop point in the anti task. More importantly, however, these two sets of values were significantly correlated (Figure 6d; 0.73, 0.004, permutation test). An alternate analysis based on optimal shifts instead of rise and drop points produced nearly identical correlation numbers. In summary, then, participants that showed an earlier departure from chance in one task also showed an earlier departure from chance in the other, consistent with the notion that, for each individual, the exogenous capture is fundamentally the same in the two tasks.
Discussion
We used the contrast between urgent pro- and antisaccade performance to test the idea that exogenous and endogenous contributions to saccadic target selection are largely independent. The cue onset and the endogenously defined goal were spatially congruent in one task but incongruent in the other, and yet, in both cases we observed a similarly steep increase in the frequency of saccades made toward the cue. As a result, there were striking differences in the proportion and timing of the correct responses across conditions (pro versus anti), but this was very much as predicted by our race model.
Dependencies between exogenous and endogenous signals could have plausibly manifested in many ways. The exogenous response in anti trials could have been visibly attenuated, delayed, or less sensitive to luminance relative to that in pro trials (Figure 4); the exogenous capture in anti trials could have been stronger in interleaved as compared to blocked conditions (Figure 4—figure supplement 1); or it could have been much weaker than predicted by the model when the ongoing motor activity pointed away from the cue rather than toward it (Figure 5). A substantial difference in any of these cases would have meant that the exogenous response was subject to internal modulation. Discrepancies were, in fact, very slight, largely consistent with the independence assumption — but this was by no means a foregone conclusion. This is not to say that robust modulation of the exogenous response is impossible, but the results circumscribe considerably the conditions under which it may occur.
Exogenous capture connects numerous visuomotor phenomena
Given our modeling framework, the mechanistic intuition behind these results is straightforward: when saccade-related neural activity is ramping up, the cue onset can accelerate that activity past the threshold beyond which a saccade is inevitably produced — before top-down information can influence the choice. When the cue itself is not the target, as in the antisaccade task, this leads to so-called captured saccades, which are errors. But when the cue itself is the target, as in the prosaccade task, these same ‘captured’ saccades are correct. In either case, the eye movement is elicited very rapidly, after 75–100 ms of viewing time. The quantitative variations in this process across conditions were tiny, consistent with the hypothesis that the resulting bias on the motor selection process is associated with the detection of the cue and is largely independent of its behavioral relevance or meaning. Perhaps an appropriate descriptor for this effect, as it is thought to occur in oculomotor circuits, is ‘exogenous capture’.
Understood this way — as an involuntary and stereotypical bias on motor planning that occurs when a stimulus is detected by oculomotor circuits — exogenous capture potentially encompasses a wide range of oculomotor phenomena (Salinas and Stanford, 2021). These include (1) oculomotor capture itself, which is observed when saccades are overtly directed toward salient distracters (Theeuwes et al., 1998; Theeuwes et al., 1999); (2) express saccades, which are saccades triggered with extremely short latencies (Kalesnykas and Hallett, 1987; Paré and Munoz, 1996; Sparks et al., 2000); (3) exogenous attention, which improves perceptual performance at explicitly cued locations (Carrasco, 2011) and has neural correlates that are highly consistent with our framework (Bisley and Goldberg, 2003; Busse et al., 2008; Herrington and Assad, 2010); and (4) attentional capture, which is typically observed when saccades to a target take longer in the presence of salient distracters (Ruz and Lupiáñez, 2002; Theeuwes, 2010).
Exogenous capture is a potentially useful construct because it represents a specific hypothesis about the neural events that determine, or at least contribute significantly to, all of these behaviors. For example, the variations in RT that define attentional capture can be easily explained by our framework (Oor et al., 2021; Salinas and Stanford, 2021). In essence, when the distracter-driven acceleration of the cue-aligned plan is relatively weak (weaker than in Figure 2c), most saccades are ultimately correct, but their onset is still delayed relative to a control condition with a less salient or absent distracter. That is, there is reason to think that in attentional capture paradigms the exogenous bias is qualitatively identical to that in the model (Figure 2b, c, f, g) but weaker, so its overt manifestation (in the RT distribution) is more subtle.
A key consideration is that, during active fixation, motor plans are strongly suppressed, so establishing a quantitative link between visual transients, motor activity, and probability of success in a task is more difficult than under urgent conditions. When motor plans are already underway, exogenous capture becomes strong and highly reliable, and its impact on performance easier to discern across tasks (Salinas et al., 2019; Oor et al., 2021; Salinas and Stanford, 2021).
Work by Poth, 2021 showed that robust capture phenomena can be generated in urgent tasks that use button presses instead of saccades and impose very different cognitive demands on the choice process. Task conditions akin to pro and anti were distinguished not by different spatial rules, but rather by contextual features that could be congruent (e.g., >>>>>) or incongruent with a central stimulus (e.g., <<><<). The qualitative similarities with our pro- and antisaccade tasks are remarkable, and suggest two critical questions to pursue more broadly: how some visuomotor associations become more reflexive than others, and how established reflexive tendencies are overcome.
No clear target for inhibitory control
Success in the antisaccade task is typically conceptualized as the result of inhibitory control. That is, it is assumed that an inhibitory process suppresses the reflexive reaction of looking at the suddenly appearing visual cue (Munoz and Everling, 2004; Wiecki and Frank, 2013; Coe and Munoz, 2017). But, what exactly is supposed to be inhibited?
Several factors determine success in an antisaccade trial. On average, weaker preparatory activity, due to a late break of fixation or to low urgency to respond, leads to lower probability of capture, consistent with single-neuron recordings in the superior colliculus and the frontal eye field (Everling et al., 1999; Everling and Munoz, 2000). A developing motor plan that, by chance, is spatially incongruent with the cue is less likely to be captured than one that is congruent (Figure 5a–c and Figure 5—figure supplement 1b), also in agreement with neural recordings (Everling et al., 1998; Everling et al., 1999; Everling and Munoz, 2000). The intensity of the exogenous response is certainly critical, with less salient cues leading to later and less pronounced capture (Figure 4e and Figure 4—figure supplement 3; Salinas et al., 2019). But as far as we can tell, there is no mechanism for preventing the exogenous burst; at most it may be slightly modulated by task conditions (Figure 4 and Figure 4—figure supplement 1; Everling et al., 1998; Everling and Munoz, 2000; see also Gu et al., 2016; Salinas and Stanford, 2018, Salinas et al., 2019; Buonocore et al., 2017; Bompas et al., 2020; Salinas and Stanford, 2021). Finally, the probability of a successful antisaccade is higher when the endogenous modulation that steers the motor plans is stronger or arrives earlier. This signal does implicate inhibition because it must decelerate the incorrect motor plan, but according to modeling results, this applies equally to any urgent saccadic choice that is perceptually informed (Stanford et al., 2010; Shankar et al., 2011; Costello et al., 2013; Salinas and Stanford, 2013; Salinas and Stanford, 2018; Salinas and Stanford, 2021; Seideman et al., 2018; Stanford and Salinas, 2021), including prosaccades. Furthermore, endogenous inhibition does not act alone, it complements endogenous excitation that is necessary for accelerating the correct motor plan.
All this indicates that the role of inhibition in antisaccade performance is likely widespread and quite nuanced (see Aron, 2007). In summary, then, we found that the capture mechanism is the same in the urgent pro- and antisaccade tasks, and that it is the consequence of a robust exogenous burst of oculomotor activity that always occurs. Therefore, it remains to be determined whether antisaccade performance depends critically on a specific circuit element being inhibited, but the obvious candidate, the exogenous response itself, is not it.
Materials and methods
In general, methods were the same as in the preceding study (Salinas et al., 2019). Here, we summarize key elements of the approach and detail new procedures that were implemented specifically for this experiment.
Subjects and setup
Request a detailed protocolExperimental subjects were 10 healthy human volunteers, 4 male and 6 female, with a median age of 28 and ranging between 22 and 63 years. They were recruited from the Wake Forest School of Medicine and Wake Forest University communities. All had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. All participants provided informed written consent before the experiment. All experimental procedures were conducted with the approval of the Institutional Review Board of Wake Forest School of Medicine (IRB00035241).
As in the preceding study (Salinas et al., 2019), the experiment took place in a semi-dark room. The participants sat on an adjustable chair, with their chin and forehead supported, and viewed stimuli presented on a VIEWPixx LED monitor (VPixx Technologies Inc, Saint Bruno, Quebec, Canada; 1920 × 1200 screen resolution, 120 Hz refresh rate, 12-bit color) at a distance of 57 cm. Eye position was recorded using an EyeLink 1000 infrared camera and tracking system (SR Research, Ottawa, Canada; 1000 Hz sampling rate). For this experiment, stimulus presentation and data collection were controlled using the Psychtoolbox 3.0 package (Brainard, 1997; Kleiner et al., 2007) and custom Matlab scripts (Goldstein et al., 2022).
Behavioral tasks
Request a detailed protocolThe sequence of events was the same in the pro- and antisaccade tasks, and is described in Figure 1. As in the earlier study (Salinas et al., 2019), the gap values used were −200, −100, 0, 75, 100, 125, 150, 175, 200, 250, and 350 ms, where negative numbers correspond to delays in nonurgent versions of the task (Figure 1—figure supplement 1); that is, compelled and easy, nonurgent trials were interleaved. In each trial, the gap value and cue location (−8° or 8°) were randomly sampled. The cue was a circle of 1.5° diameter. Auditory feedback (a beep) was provided at the end of the trial if the saccadic choice was made within the allowed RT window (425 ms); no sound was played if the limit was exceeded. No feedback was given about the correctness of the spatial choice. The intertrial interval was 350 ms.
Data were collected in blocks of 150 trials, with 2–3 min of rest allowed between blocks. For the main experiment, each participant completed 9 or 10 blocks of prosaccade trials, 9 or 10 blocks of antisaccade trials, and 18 blocks of pro and anti trials interleaved in equal proportions. For half of the participants (P1–P5), the cue was green (RGB vector [0 0.88 0]) in prosaccade trials and magenta (RGB vector [1 0.32 1]) in antisaccade trials. For the other half (P6–P10), the color assignments were swapped. The green and magenta stimuli had the same luminance (48 cd m−2), as determined by a spectrophotometer (i1 Pro 2 from X-Rite, Inc, Grand Rapids, MI). In blocks in which pro and anti trials were interleaved, the color of the fixation point was the same as that of the cue and indicated to the participant the type of trial that was coming up. In blocks of single-task trials, the fixation point was gray (RGB vector [0.25 0.25 0.25]).
Secondary experiment
Request a detailed protocolAdditional data were collected in a secondary experiment in which luminance varied across trials (Figure 4e). One of the previous volunteers and two newly recruited ones participated (all female). The sequence of events in the tasks was exactly the same as in the main experiment. In this case, however, in each trial, the task type (pro or anti) and cue luminance (high or low) were independently sampled, so the four possible combinations were randomly interleaved. The color of the fixation spot, which was always the same as that of the upcoming cue, indicated the correct response: green stimuli corresponded to prosaccades and magenta stimuli to antisaccades. The green and magenta cues of high luminance (48 cd m−2) were the same as in the main experiment. The green (RGB vector [0 0.0733 0]) and magenta (RGB vector [0.0833 0.0267 0.833]) cues of low luminance (0.24 cd m−2) were chosen so that significant rightward shifts of the tachometric curve would be generated (Salinas et al., 2019).
Data analysis
Request a detailed protocolAll results are based on the analysis of urgent trials (gap ≥0) only; easy, nonurgent trials (gap <0) were excluded. All data analyses were carried out using customized scripts written in Matlab (The MathWorks, Natick, MA), as detailed previously (Salinas et al., 2019).
The RT was always measured as the time elapsed between the go signal (fixation offset) and saccade onset (equal to the first time point after the go for which the eye velocity exceeded a threshold of 40°/s). Trials were scored as correct or incorrect based on the direction of the first saccade made after the go signal. Trials with fixation breaks (aborts) or primarily vertical saccades were excluded from analysis. Otherwise, completed trials were scored and included even if they exceeded the allotted time limit.
Processing times were computed as rPT = RT – gap, where all quantities are specific to each trial. To generate tachometric curves, trials were grouped into rPT bins that were shifted every 1 ms, and the numbers of correct and incorrect responses were tallied for each bin. From these, we calculated the fraction of correct responses in each bin and, using binomial statistics, CIs for it. The bin size was 21 ms in all cases except Figure 4—figure supplement 3, where it was set to 41 ms.
To quantify perceptual performance, each tachometric curve was fitted with a continuous analytical function. The fits served to determine key characteristic metrics from the empirical curves, which are inherently noisy. For the prosaccade task, the fitting curve was an increasing sigmoid
where is the curve rise point, that is, the rPT at which the fraction correct is halfway between chance (equal to ) and asymptotic (equal to ). For the antisaccade task, the fitting curve was defined as
where the maximum function, , returns the largest of , , or . Also, and are two sigmoidal curves given by
where tracks the left (decreasing) side of the tachometric curve and tracks the right (increasing) side. In this case, is the curve drop point, that is, the rPT at which the fraction correct is halfway between chance (equal to ) and minimum (equal to ), whereas is the curve rise point, which in this case corresponds to the rPT at which the fraction correct is halfway between the minimum () and asymptotic (). Previously (Salinas et al., 2019), we referred to the antisaccade rise point as the curve centerpoint, but the new nomenclature is more clear and more suitable for comparing results between pro and anti conditions.
For any given tachometric curve, parameter values (, , , etc.) were found that minimized the mean absolute error between the experimental data and the analytical curves using the Matlab function fminsearch. In most cases, chance levels were fixed at 50% correct by setting 0.5 and 0.5, so these parameters did not vary during the minimization. However, when analyzing motor biases (Figure 5), these parameters were allowed to vary.
In this study, we focus on the drop point and the rise points and . CIs for these quantities were obtained by bootstrapping (Davison and Hinkley, 2006; Hesterberg, 2014), as done previously (Salinas et al., 2019). This involved resampling the trial-wise data with replacement, refitting the resulting (resampled) tachometric curves, storing the new parameter values, and repeating the process many times to generate parameter distributions. Reported 95% CIs correspond to the 2.5 and 97.5 percentiles obtained from the bootstrapped distributions based on 1000–10,000 iterations.
In addition to comparisons between , , and values, to evaluate differences in timing between two tachometric curves (as in Figure 4b–d), we also performed a time-shift analysis, which was as follows. First, one curve, , was designated as the reference and remained fixed. Then, transformed versions of the second curve, , were considered that would minimize the absolute difference between it and the reference curve over a range of values (i.e., rPTs). Three transforming operations were considered: a shift along the y-axis (i.e., a change in baseline), a change in gain, and a shift along the x-axis. In practice, values of the parameters , , and were found that minimized the error
where the angle brackets indicate an average over all rPT values in the specified range. For identical tachometric curves f1 and f2 the optimal parameters would be 0, 1, and 0. Although the quantity of interest is the relative time shift between the curves, , the gain factor and change in baseline are included to account for the fact that the two curves may saturate at somewhat different levels, or may be slightly offset in the vertical direction from each other, which would tend to overestimate the magnitude of the optimal shift, that is, the for which the error is minimized. Notably, the effect of and was modest; results varied by approximately 2 ms when the procedure allowed variations in only, in which case and were fixed at 0 and 1, respectively.
This shift analysis applies to curves f1 and f2 that have similar shapes. To compare the early departure from chance in pro- versus antisaccade curves, the prosaccade curve was first inverted (Figure 4b, c) relative to the chance level by applying the transformation . Then the above procedure for determining was carried out. CIs for were again obtained by bootstrapping, that is, by resampling the data with replacement, recalculating the tachometric curves f1 and f2, and rerunning the entire minimization process. In this way, by iterating this resampling procedure many times, distributions of values were generated.
Model simulations
Request a detailed protocolPredictions were made with the CAS and CPS models as explained in Results. The CAS model is the exact same accelerated race-to-threshold model developed previously (for parameter values, see Table 1 in Salinas et al., 2019, high-luminance cue condition). The CAS model thus represents typical, or average, urgent antisaccade performance, as it replicates the psychometric results obtained by pooling all the trials from the six subjects that participated in the prior experiment. The Matlab scripts for simulating the CAS and CPS models differed by a single sign that determined whether the target location was equal to the cue location or opposite to it. Thus, for a given set of parameters, the exogenous and endogenous modulations produced by the two models were the same; the only thing that varied was which plan was accelerated and which decelerated endogenously. To generate predictions, the CPS model was run with the same parameter values as the CAS model.
In standard model simulations with no motor bias, the two initial buildup rates for the competing motor plans were drawn from a bivariate distribution and were assigned randomly to the cue and anti motor plans. Model runs in which a motor bias was included proceeded in the same way, with initial buildup rates drawn from the same bivariate distribution, but these were assigned to the cue and the anti plans with a biased probability. For example, to generate a bias of 0.1 in favor of the cue location, in each simulated trial the larger of the two buildup rates was assigned to the cue side with a 60% probability. Biases of arbitrary magnitude favoring one direction or the other were generated this way.
Data availability
The trial-by-trial behavioral data that support the findings of this study are publicly available from Zenodo, https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.6757621. Matlab scripts for reproducing analysis results are included as part of the shared data package.
-
ZenodoDataset: Exogenous capture accounts for fundamental differences between prosaccade and antisaccade performance.https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.6757621
References
-
The neural basis of inhibition in cognitive controlThe Neuroscientist 13:214–228.https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858407299288
-
Frontoparietal activation with preparation for antisaccadesJournal of Neurophysiology 98:1751–1762.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00460.2007
-
Saccade reorienting is facilitated by pausing the oculomotor programJournal of Cognitive Neuroscience 29:2068–2080.https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01179
-
Visual attention: the past 25 yearsVision Research 51:1484–1525.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2011.04.012
-
Mechanisms of saccade suppression revealed in the anti-saccade taskPhilosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences 372:20160192.https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2016.0192
-
Perceptual modulation of motor--but not visual--responses in the frontal eye field during an urgent-decision taskThe Journal of Neuroscience 33:16394–16408.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1899-13.2013
-
BookBootstrap Methods and Their ApplicationsCambridge: Cambridge University Press.https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511802843
-
Reflex suppression in the anti-saccade task is dependent on prestimulus neural processesJournal of Neurophysiology 80:1584–1589.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1998.80.3.1584
-
Role of primate superior colliculus in preparation and execution of anti-saccades and pro-saccadesThe Journal of Neuroscience 19:2740–2754.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-07-02740.1999
-
Antisaccade performance in patients with multiple sclerosisCortex; a Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior 45:900–903.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2009.02.016
-
A trial-by-trial window into sensorimotor transformations in the human motor peripheryThe Journal of Neuroscience 36:8273–8282.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0899-16.2016
-
Computational modelling of visual attentionNature Reviews Neuroscience 2:194–203.https://doi.org/10.1038/35058500
-
The differentiation of visually guided and anticipatory saccades in gap and overlap paradigmsExperimental Brain Research 68:115–121.https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00255238
-
Development of prosaccade and antisaccade task performance in participants aged 6 to 26 yearsPsychophysiology 38:179–189.
-
Task switchingTrends in Cognitive Sciences 7:134–140.https://doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(03)00028-7
-
Look away: the anti-saccade task and the voluntary control of eye movementNature Reviews Neuroscience 5:218–228.https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1345
-
ConferenceEncoding of salience and informed choice colludes and conflicts across FEF neuronal types in the guidance of urgent saccadic decisions. Program Number P491Society for Neuroscience Abstracts.
-
A review of attentional capture: On its automaticity and sensitivity to endogenous controlPsicológica 23:283–309.
-
The countermanding task revisited: fast stimulus detection is a key determinant of psychophysical performanceThe Journal of Neuroscience 33:5668–5685.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3977-12.2013
-
Under time pressure, the exogenous modulation of saccade plans is ubiquitous, intricate, and lawfulCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology 70:154–162.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2021.10.012
-
Saccade metrics reflect decision-making dynamics during urgent choicesNature Communications 9:2907.https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-018-05319-w
-
A conflict between spatial selection and evidence accumulation in area LIPNature Communications In press.
-
Tracking the temporal evolution of a perceptual judgment using a compelled-response taskThe Journal of Neuroscience 31:8406–8421.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1419-11.2011
-
Perceptual decision making in less than 30 millisecondsNature Neuroscience 13:379–385.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2485
-
Urgent decision making: Resolving visuomotor interactions at high temporal resolutionAnnual Review of Vision Science 7:323–348.https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-vision-100419-103842
-
Our eyes do not always go where we want them to go: Capture of the eyes by new objectsPsychological Science 9:379–385.https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00071
-
Influence of attentional capture on oculomotor controlJournal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance 25:1595–1608.https://doi.org/10.1037//0096-1523.25.6.1595
-
Top-down and bottom-up control of visual selectionActa Psychologica 135:77–99.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.02.006
-
A computational model of inhibitory control in frontal cortex and basal gangliaPsychological Review 120:329–355.https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031542
-
Five factors that guide attention in visual searchNature Human Behaviour 1:0058.https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-017-0058
-
Task switching and the measurement of “switch costs.”Psychological Research 63:212–233.https://doi.org/10.1007/s004269900003
Article and author information
Author details
Funding
National Eye Institute (R01EY025172)
- Terrence R Stanford
- Emilio Salinas
National Institute of Mental Health (R21MH120784)
- Terrence R Stanford
- Emilio Salinas
National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (T32NS073553)
- Allison T Goldstein
The funders had no role in study design, data collection, and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Acknowledgements
We thank Denise Anderson for technical and logistical assistance.
Ethics
All participants provided informed written consent before the experiment. All experimental procedures were conducted with the approval of the Institutional Review Board of Wake Forest School of Medicine (IRB00035241).
Copyright
© 2022, Goldstein et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 470
- views
-
- 105
- downloads
-
- 11
- citations
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Neuroscience
The concept that dimeric protein complexes in synapses can sequentially replace their subunits has been a cornerstone of Francis Crick’s 1984 hypothesis, explaining how long-term memories could be maintained in the face of short protein lifetimes. However, it is unknown whether the subunits of protein complexes that mediate memory are sequentially replaced in the brain and if this process is linked to protein lifetime. We address these issues by focusing on supercomplexes assembled by the abundant postsynaptic scaffolding protein PSD95, which plays a crucial role in memory. We used single-molecule detection, super-resolution microscopy and MINFLUX to probe the molecular composition of PSD95 supercomplexes in mice carrying genetically encoded HaloTags, eGFP, and mEoS2. We found a population of PSD95-containing supercomplexes comprised of two copies of PSD95, with a dominant 12.7 nm separation. Time-stamping of PSD95 subunits in vivo revealed that each PSD95 subunit was sequentially replaced over days and weeks. Comparison of brain regions showed subunit replacement was slowest in the cortex, where PSD95 protein lifetime is longest. Our findings reveal that protein supercomplexes within the postsynaptic density can be maintained by gradual replacement of individual subunits providing a mechanism for stable maintenance of their organization. Moreover, we extend Crick’s model by suggesting that synapses with slow subunit replacement of protein supercomplexes and long-protein lifetimes are specialized for long-term memory storage and that these synapses are highly enriched in superficial layers of the cortex where long-term memories are stored.
-
- Neuroscience
Motivation depends on dopamine, but might be modulated by acetylcholine which influences dopamine release in the striatum, and amplifies motivation in animal studies. A corresponding effect in humans would be important clinically, since anticholinergic drugs are frequently used in Parkinson’s disease, a condition that can also disrupt motivation. Reward and dopamine make us more ready to respond, as indexed by reaction times (RT), and move faster, sometimes termed vigour. These effects may be controlled by preparatory processes that can be tracked using electroencephalography (EEG). We measured vigour in a placebo-controlled, double-blinded study of trihexyphenidyl (THP), a muscarinic antagonist, with an incentivised eye movement task and EEG. Participants responded faster and with greater vigour when incentives were high, but THP blunted these motivational effects, suggesting that muscarinic receptors facilitate invigoration by reward. Preparatory EEG build-up (contingent negative variation [CNV]) was strengthened by high incentives and by muscarinic blockade, although THP reduced the incentive effect. The amplitude of preparatory activity predicted both vigour and RT, although over distinct scalp regions; frontal activity predicted vigour, whereas a larger, earlier, central component predicted RT. The incentivisation of RT was partly mediated by the CNV, though vigour was not. Moreover, the CNV mediated the drug’s effect on dampening incentives, suggesting that muscarinic receptors underlie the motivational influence on this preparatory activity. Taken together, these findings show that a muscarinic blocker impairs motivated action in healthy people, and that medial frontal preparatory neural activity mediates this for RT.