Neural tracking of phrases in spoken language comprehension is automatic and task-dependent
Abstract
Linguistic phrases are tracked in sentences even though there is no one-to-one acoustic phrase marker in the physical signal. This phenomenon suggests an automatic tracking of abstract linguistic structure that is endogenously generated by the brain. However, all studies investigating linguistic tracking compare conditions where either relevant information at linguistic timescales is available, or where this information is absent altogether (e.g., sentences versus word lists during passive listening). It is therefore unclear whether tracking at phrasal timescales is related to the content of language, or rather, results as a consequence of attending to the timescales that happen to match behaviourally relevant information. To investigate this question, we presented participants with sentences and word lists while recording their brain activity with magnetoencephalography (MEG). Participants performed passive, syllable, word, and word-combination tasks corresponding to attending to four different rates: one they would naturally attend to, syllable-rates, word-rates, and phrasal-rates, respectively. We replicated overall findings of stronger phrasal-rate tracking measured with mutual information for sentences compared to word lists across the classical language network. However, in the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) we found a task effect suggesting stronger phrasal-rate tracking during the word-combination task independent of the presence of linguistic structure, as well as stronger delta-band connectivity during this task. These results suggest that extracting linguistic information at phrasal rates occurs automatically with or without the presence of an additional task, but also that IFG might be important for temporal integration across various perceptual domains.
Editor's evaluation
This MEG study elegantly assesses human brain responses to spoken language at the syllable, word, and sentence level. Although prior studies have shown significant cortical tracking of the speech signal, the current work uses clever task manipulation to direct attention to different timescales of speech, thus demonstrating tracking mechanisms that are both automatic and task-dependent operate in tandem during spoken language comprehension.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.77468.sa0Introduction
Understanding spoken language likely requires a multitude of processes (Friederici, 2011; Martin, 2020; Halle and Stevens, 1962). Although not always an exclusively bottom-up affair, acoustic patterns must be segmented and mapped onto internally-stored phonetic and syllabic representations (Halle and Stevens, 1962; Marslen-Wilson and Welsh, 1978; Martin, 2016). Phonemes must be combined and mapped onto words, which in turn form abstract linguistic structures such as phrases (e.g., Martin, 2020; Pinker and Jackendoff, 2005).In proficient speakers of a language, this process seems to happen so naturally that one might almost forget the complex parallel and hierarchical processing which occurs during natural speech and language comprehension.
It has been shown that it is essential to track the temporal dynamics of the speech signal in order to understand its meaning (e.g., Giraud and Poeppel, 2012; Peelle and Davis, 2012). In natural speech, syllables follow each other in the theta range (3–8 Hz; Rosen, 1992; Ding et al., 2017; Pellegrino et al., 2011), while higher-level linguistic features such as words and phrases tend to occur at lower rates (0.5–3 Hz; Rosen, 1992; Kaufeld et al., 2020; Keitel et al., 2018). Tracking of syllabic features is stronger when one understands a language (Luo and Poeppel, 2007; Zoefel et al., 2018a; Doelling et al., 2014) and tracking of phrasal rates is more prominent when the signal contains phrasal information (Kaufeld et al., 2020; Keitel et al., 2018; Ding et al., 2016; e.g., word lists versus sentences). Importantly, phrasal tracking even occurs when there are no distinct acoustic modulations at the phrasal rate (Kaufeld et al., 2020; Keitel et al., 2018; Ding et al., 2016). These results seem to suggest that tracking of relevant temporal timescales is critical for speech understanding.
An observation one could make regarding these findings is that tracking occurs only at the rates that are meaningful and thereby behaviourally relevant (Kaufeld et al., 2020; Ding et al., 2016). For example, in word lists, the word rate is the slowest rate that is meaningful during natural listening. Modulations at slower phrasal rates might not be tracked as they do not contain behaviourally relevant information. In contrast, in sentences, phrasal rates contain linguistic information and therefore these slower rates are also tracked. Thus, when listening to speech one automatically tries to extract the meaning, which requires extracting information at the highest linguistic level (Halle and Stevens, 1962; Martin, 2016). However, it remains unclear if tracking at these slower rates is a unique feature of language processing, or rather is dependent on attention to relevant temporal timescales.
As understanding language requires a multitude of processes, it is difficult to figure out what participants actually are doing when listening to natural speech. Moreover, designing a task in an experimental setting that does justice to this multitude of processing is difficult. This is probably why tasks in language studies vary vastly. Tasks include passively listening (e.g., Kaufeld et al., 2020), asking comprehension questions (e.g., Keitel et al., 2018), rating intelligibility (e.g., Luo and Poeppel, 2007; Doelling et al., 2014), working memory tasks (e.g., Kayser et al., 2015), or even syllable counting (e.g., Ding et al., 2016). It is unclear whether outcomes are dependent on the specifics of the task. There has so far not been a study that investigates if task instructions focusing on extracting information at different temporal rates or timescales have an influence on the tracking that occurs on these timescales. It is therefore not clear whether tracking phrasal timescales is unique for language stimuli which contain phrasal structures, or could also occur for other acoustic materials where participants are instructed to pay attention to information happening at these temporal rates or timescales.
To answer this question, we designed an experiment in which participants were instructed to pay attention to different temporal modulation rates while listening to the same stimuli. We presented participants with naturally spoken sentences and word lists and asked them to either passively listen, or perform a task on the temporal scales corresponding to syllables, words, or phrases. We recorded brain activity using magnetoencephalography (MEG) while participants performed these tasks and investigated tracking as well as power and connectivity at three nodes that are part of the language network: the superior temporal gyrus (STG), the middle temporal gyrus (MTG), and the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG). We hypothesized that if tracking is purely based on behavioural relevance, it should mostly depend on the task instructions, rather than the nature of the stimuli. In contrast, if there is something automatic and specific about language information, tracking should depend on the level of linguistic information available to the brain.
Results
Behaviour
Overall task performance was above chance and participants complied with task instructions (Figure 1; see Figure 1—figure supplement 1 for individual data). We found a significant interaction between condition and task (F(2,72.0) = 11.51, p < 0.001) as well as a main effect of task (F(2,19.7) = 44.19, p < 0.001) and condition (F(2,72.0) = 29.0, p < 0.001). We found that only for the word-combination (phrasal-level) task, the sentence condition had a significantly higher accuracy than the word list condition (t(54.0) = 6.97, p < 0.001). For the other two tasks, no significant condition effect was found (syllable: t(54.0) = 0.62, p = 1.000; word list: t(54.0) = 1.74, p = 0.176). Investigating the main effect of task indicated a difference between all tasks (phrase–syllable: t(18.0) = 3.71, p = 0.003; phrase–word: t(22.4) = −6.34, p < 0.001; syllable–word: t(19.2) = −8.67, p < 0.001).
Mutual information
The overall time–frequency response in the three different regions of interest (ROI) using the top-20 PCA components was as expected, with an initial evoked response followed by a more sustained response to the ongoing speech (Figure 2). From these regions-of-interest, we extracted mutual information (MI) in three different frequency bands (phrasal, word, and syllable). Here, we focus on the phrasal band as this is the band that differentiates word lists from sentences and showed the strongest modulation for this contrast in our previous study (Kaufeld et al., 2020). MI results for all other bands are reported in the supplementary materials.
For the phrasal timescale in STG, we found significantly higher MI in the sentence compared to the word list condition (F(3,126) = 67.39, p < 0.001; Figure 3; see Figure 3—figure supplement 1 for individual data). No other effects were significant (p > 0.1). This finding paralleled the effect found in Kaufeld et al., 2020. For the MTG, we saw a different picture: Besides the main effect of condition (F(3,126) = 50.24, p < 0.001), an interaction between task and condition was found (F(3,126) = 2.948, p = 0.035). We next investigated the effect of condition per task and found for all tasks except the passive task a significant effect of condition, with stronger MI for the sentence condition (passive: t(126) = 1.07, p = 0.865; syllable: t(126) = 4.06, p = 0.003; word: t(126) = 5.033, p < 0.001; phrase: t(126) = 4.015, p = 0.003). For the IFG, we found a main effect of condition (F(3,108) = 21.89, p < 0.001) as well as a main effect of task (F(3,108) = 2.74, p = 0.047). The interaction was not significant (F(3,108) = 1.49, p = 0.220). Comparing the phrasal task with the other tasks indicated higher MI for the phrasal compared to the word task (t(111) = 2.50, p = 0.028). We also found a trend for the comparison between the phrasal and syllable tasks (t(111) = 2.17, p = 0.064), as well as the phrasal and passive tasks (t(111) = 2.25, p = 0.052).
For the word and syllable frequency bands no interactions were found (all p > 0.1; Figure 3—figure supplement 2). For all six models, there was a significant effect of condition, with stronger MI for word lists compared to sentences (all p < 0.001). The main effect of task was not significant in any of the models (p > 0.1; for the MTG syllable level there was a trend: F(3,126) = 2.40, p = 0.071).
When running the power control analysis, we did not find that significant effects in power differences (also see next section for power in generic bands; mostly due to main effects of condition) influenced our tracking results for any of the bands investigated.
Power
We repeated the linear mixed modelling using power instead of MI to investigate if power changes paralleled the MI effects. For the delta band, we found for the STG a main effect of condition (F(1,18) = 6.11, p = 0.024; Figure 4. See Figure 4—figure supplement 1 for individual data) and task (F(3,108) = 3.069, p = 0.031). For the interaction we found a trend (F(3,108) = 2.620, p = 0.054). Overall sentences had stronger delta power than word lists. We found lower power for the phrase compared to the passive task (t(111) = 2.31, p = 0.045) and lower power for the phrase compared to the syllable task (t(111) = 2.43, p = 0.034). There was no significant difference between the phrase and word task (t(111) = 0.642, p = 1.00).
The MTG delta power effect overall paralleled the STG effects with a significant condition (F(1,124.94) = 12.339, p < 0.001) and task effect (F(3,124.94) = 4.326, p = 0.006). The interaction was trend significant (F(3,124.94) = 2.58, p = 0.056). Pairwise comparisons of the task effect showed significantly stronger power for the phrase compared to the passive task (t(128) = 2.98, p = 0.007) and lower power for the phrase compared to the syllable task (t(128) = 3.10, p = 0.024). The passive–word comparison was not significant (t(128) = 2.577, p = 0.109). Finally, for the IFG we only found a trend effect for condition (F(1,123.27)=4.15, p = 0.057), with stronger delta power in the sentence condition.
The results for all other bands can be found in the supplementary materials (Figure 4—figure supplement 2). In summary, no interaction effects were found for any of the models (all p > 0.1). In all bands, power was generally higher for sentences than for word lists. Any task effect generally showed stronger power for the lower hierarchical level (e.g. generally higher power for passive versus word-combination tasks).
Connectivity
Overall connectivity patterns showed the strongest connectivity in the delta and alpha frequency band (Figure 5). In the delta band, we found a main effect of task for the STG–IFG connectivity (F(3,122.06) = 4.1078, p = 0.008; Figure 6; see Figure 6—figure supplement 1 for individual data). Follow-up analysis showed a significant difference between the phrasal and passive tasks with higher connectivity in the phrasal compared to the passive task (t(125) = 3.254, p = 0.003). The other comparisons with the phrasal task were not significant. The effect of task remained significant even when correcting for power differences between the passive and phrasal tasks (F(1,53.02) = 12.39, p < 0.001; note the change in degrees of freedom as only the passive and phrasal tasks were included in this mixed model as any power correction is done on pairs). Initially, we also found main effects of condition for the delta and beta bands for the MTG–IFG connectivity (stronger connectivity for the sentence compared to the word list condition), however after controlling for power, these effects did not remain significant (Figure 6—figure supplement 2).
MEG–behavioural performance relation
We found for the MI analysis a significant effect of accuracy only in the MTG. Here, we found a three-way interaction between accuracy × task × condition (F(2,91.9) = 3.459, p = 0.036). Splitting up for the three different tasks we found only an uncorrected significant effect for the condition × accuracy interaction for the phrasal task (F(1,24.8) = 5.296, p = 0.03) and not for the other two tasks (p > 0.1). In the phrasal task, we found that when accuracy was high, there was a stronger difference between the sentence and the word list condition compared to when accuracy was low, with stronger accuracy for the sentence condition (Figure 7A).
No relation between accuracy and power was found. For the connectivity analysis, we found a significant condition × accuracy interaction for the STG–MTG connection (F(1,80.23) = 5.19, p = 0.025; Figure 7B). Independent of task, when accuracy was low the difference between sentence and word lists was stronger with higher weighted phase lag index (WPLI) fits for the sentence condition. After correcting for accuracy there was also a significant task × condition interaction (F(2,80.01) = 3.348, p = 0.040) and a main effect of condition (F(1,80.361) = 5.809, p = 0.018). While overall there was a stronger WPLI for the sentence compared to the word list condition, the interaction seemed to indicate that this was especially the case during the word task (p = 0.005), but not for the other tasks (p > 0.1).
Age control
Adding age to the analysis did not change any of the original findings (all original effects were still significant). We did however find for the power analysis age-specific interactions with condition and task. Specifically, for both the STG and the MTG we found an interaction between age and condition (F(1,28.87) = 6.156, p = 0.0192 and F(1,31) = 10.31, p = 0.003). In both ROIs, there was a stronger difference between sentences and word lists (higher delta power for sentences) for the younger compared to the older participants (Figure 7—figure supplement 1). In the MTG, there was also an interaction between task and age (F(1,31) = 5.020, p = 0.006). Here, in a follow-up we found that only in the word task there was a correlation between age and power (p = 0.023 uncorrected), but not for the other tasks (p > 0.1).
Number of component control
Overall, the amount of PCA components did not influence any of the qualitative differences in the condition. It did seem however that 10 PCA components were not sufficient to show all original effects with the same power. Specifically, the IFG task and MTG task × condition effect were only trend significant for 10 components (p = 0.06 and p = 0.1, respectively). The other effects did remain significant with 10 components. Using 30 components made some of our effects stronger than with 20 components. Here, the IFG task and MTG task × condition effects had p values of 0.034 and 0.006, respectively. We conclude that the amount of PCAs components did not qualitative change any of our reported effects.
Discussion
In the current study, we investigated the effects of ‘additional’ tasks on the neural tracking of sentences and word lists at temporal modulations that matched phrasal rates. Different nodes of the language network showed different tracking patterns. In STG, we found stronger tracking of phrase-timed dynamics in sentences compared to word lists, independent of task. However, in MTG we found this sentence-improved tracking only for active tasks. In IFG, we also found an overall increase of tracking for sentences compared to word lists. Additionally, stronger phrasal tracking was found for the phrasal-level word-combination task compared to the other tasks (independent of stimulus type; note that for the syllable and passive comparison we found a trend), which was paralleled with increased IFG–STG connectivity in the delta band for the word-combination task. Behavioural performance seemed to relate to MI tracking in the MTG and STG–MTG connections. This suggests that tracking at phrasal timescales depends both on the linguistic information present in the signal, and on the specific task that is performed.
The findings reported in this study are in line with previous results, with overall stronger tracking of low-frequency information in the sentences compared to the word list condition (Kaufeld et al., 2020). Crucially, for the stimuli used in our study it has been shown that the condition effects are not due to acoustic differences in the stimuli and also do not occur for reversed speech (Kaufeld et al., 2020). It is therefore most likely that our results reflect an automatic inference-based extraction of relevant phrase-level information in sentences, indicating automatic processing in participants as they understand the meaning of the speech they hear using stored, structural linguistic knowledge (Martin, 2020; Ding et al., 2016; Har-Shai Yahav and Zion Golumbic, 2021). Overall, it did not seem that making participants pay attention to the temporal dynamics at the same hierarchical level through an additional task – instructing them to remember word combinations at the phrasal rate during word list presentation – could counter this main effect of condition.
Even though there was an overall main effect of condition, task did influence neural responses. Interestingly, the task effects differed for the three ROIs. In the STG, we found no task effects, while in the MTG we found an interaction between task and condition. In the MTG increased phrasal-level tracking for sentences only occurred when participants were specifically instructed to perform an active task on the materials. It therefore seems that in MTG all levels of linguistic information are used to do an active language operation on the stimuli. Importantly, the tracking at the phrasal rate in MTG seemed relevant for behavioural performance when attending to phrasal timescales (Figure 7A). This is in line with previous theoretical and empirical research suggesting a strong top-down modulatory response of speech processing in which predictions flow from the highest hierarchical levels (e.g., syntax) down to lower levels (e.g., phonemes) to aid language understanding (Martin, 2016; Hagoort, 2017; Federmeier, 2007). As in the word list condition no linguistic information is present at the phrasal rate, this information cannot be used to provide useful feedback for processing lower-level linguistic information. Instead, it could have been expected that the same type of increased tracking should have happened at the word rate rather than the phrasal rate for word lists (i.e., stronger word-rate tracking for word lists for the active tasks versus passive task). This effect was not found; this could either be attributed to different computational operations occurring at different hierarchical levels or to signal-to-noise/signal detection issues.
We found that across participants both the MI and the connectivity in temporal cortex influenced behavioural performance. Specifically, MTG–STG connections were, independent of task, related to accuracy. There was higher connectivity between MTG and STG for sentences compared to word lists at low accuracies. At high accuracies, we found that stronger MTG tracking at phrasal rates (measured with MI) for sentences compared to word lists during the word-combination task. These results suggest that indeed tracking of phrasal structure in MTG is relevant to understand sentences compared to word lists. This was reflected in a general increase in delta connectivity differences when the task was difficult (Figure 7B). Participants might compensate for the difficulty using phrasal structure present in the sentence condition. When phrasal structure in sentences are accurately tracked (as measured with MI) performance is better when these rates are relevant (Figure 7A). These results point to a role for phrasal tracking for accurately understanding the higher-order linguistic structure in sentences, though more research is needed to verify this. It is evident that the connectivity and tracking correlations to behaviour do not explain all variation in the behavioural performance (compare Figure 1 with Figure 3). Plainly, temporal tracking does not explain everything in language processing. Besides tracking there are many other components important for our designated tasks, such as memory load and semantic context which are not captured by our current analyses.
It is interesting that MTG, but not STG, showed an interaction effect. Both MTG and STG are strong hubs for language processing and have been involved in many studies which contrasted pseudo-words and words (Hickok and Poeppel, 2007; Turken and Dronkers, 2011; Vouloumanos et al., 2001). It is likely that STG does the more lower-level processing of the two regions, as it is earlier in the cortical hierarchy, thereby being more involved in initial segmentation and initial phonetic abstraction rather than a lexical interface (Hickok and Poeppel, 2007). This could also explain why STG does not show task-specific tracking effects; STG could be earlier in a workload bottleneck, receiving feedback independent of task, while MTG feedback is recruited only when active linguistic operations are required. Alternatively, it is possible that either small differences in the acoustics are detected by STG (even though this effect was not previously found with the same stimuli, Kaufeld et al., 2020), or that our blocked designed put participants in a sentence or word list ‘mode’ which could have influenced the state of these early hierarchical regions.
The IFG was the only region that showed an increase in phrasal-rate tracking specifically for the word-combination task. Note, however, that this was a weak effect, as the comparison between the phrase task and the syllable and passive tasks only reached a trend towards significance. Nonetheless, this effect is interesting for understanding the role of IFG in language. Traditionally, IFG has been viewed as a hub for articulatory processing (Hickok and Poeppel, 2007), but its role during speech comprehension, specifically in syntactic processing, has also been acknowledged (Friederici, 2011; Hagoort, 2017; Nelson et al., 2017; Dehaene et al., 2015; Zaccarella et al., 2017). Integrating information across time and relative timing is essential for syntactic processing (Martin, 2020; Dehaene et al., 2015; Martin and Doumas, 2019), and IFG feedback has been shown to occur in temporal dynamics at lower (delta) rates during sentence processing (Park et al., 2015; Keitel and Gross, 2016). However, it has also been shown that syntactic-independent verbal working memory chunking tasks recruit the IFG (Dehaene et al., 2015; Osaka et al., 2004; Fegen et al., 2015; Koelsch et al., 2009). This is in line with our findings that show that IFG is involved when we need to integrate across temporal domains either in a language-specific domain (sentences versus word lists) or for language-unspecific tasks (word-combination versus other tasks). We also show increased delta connectivity with STG for the only temporal-integration tasks in our study (i.e., the word-combination task), independent of the linguistic features in the signal. Our results therefore support a role of the IFG as a combinatorial hub integrating information across time (Gelfand and Bookheimer, 2003; Schapiro et al., 2013; Skipper, 2015).
In the current study, we investigated power as a neural readout during language comprehension from speech. This was both to ensure that any tracking effects we found were not due to overall signal-to-noise (SNR) differences, as well as to investigate task-and-condition dependent computations. SNR is better for conditions with higher power, which therefore leads to more reliable phase estimations, critical for computing MI as well as connectivity (Zar, 1998). We will therefore discuss the power differences as well as their consequences for the interpretation of the MI and connectivity results. Generally, it seemed that there was stronger power in the sentence condition compared to the word list condition in the delta band. However, the pattern was very different than the MI pattern. For the power, the word list-sentence difference was the biggest in the passive condition. In contrast, for the MI there was either no task difference (in STG) or even a stronger effect for the active tasks (in MTG; note that the power interaction was trend significant STG and MTG). We therefore think it unlikely that our MI effects were purely driven by SNR differences, and our power control analysis is consistent with this interpretation. Instead, power seems to reflect a different computation than the tracking, where more complex tasks generally lead to lower power across almost all tested frequency bands. As most of our frequency bands are on the low side of the spectrum (up to beta), it is expected that more complex tasks reduce the low-frequency power (Jensen and Mazaheri, 2010; Klimesch, 1999). It is interesting to observe that this did not reduce the connectivity for the delta band between IFG and STG, but rather increased it. It has been suggested that low power can potentially increase the available computational space, as it increases the entropy in the signal (Hanslmayr et al., 2012; ten Oever and Sack, 2015). Note that even though we found increased connectivity, we did not see a clear power peak in the delta band. This suggests that we might not be looking at an endogenous oscillator, but rather at connections operation at that temporal scale (potentially being non-oscillatory in nature). Finally, in the power comparisons for the theta, alpha, and beta bands we found stronger power for the sentence compared to the word list condition, which could reflect that listening to a natural sentence is generally less effortful than listening to a word list.
In the current manuscript, we describe tracking of ongoing temporal dynamics. However, the neural origin of this tracking is unknown. While we can be sure that modulations in the phrasal-rate follow changes in the phrasal rate of the acoustic input, it is unclear what the mechanism behind this modulation is. It is possible that there is stronger alignment of neural oscillations with the acoustic input at the phrasal rate (Lakatos et al., 2008; Obleser and Kayser, 2019; Rimmele et al., 2021). However, it could as well be that there is a phrasal timescale or slower operation happening while processing the incoming input (which de facto is at the same timescale as the phrasal structure inferred from the input). This operation, in response to stimulus input, could just as well induce the patterns we observe (Meyer et al., 2019; Zoefel et al., 2018b). Finally, it is possible that there are specific responses as a consequence of the syntactic structure, task, or statistical regularities occurring as specific events at phrasal timescales (Obleser and Kayser, 2019; Ten Oever and Martin, 2021; Frank and Yang, 2018).
It is difficult to decide on the most natural task in an experimental setting, that best reflects how we use language in a natural setting. This is probably why such a vast number of different tasks have been used in the literature. Our study (and many before us) indicates that during passive listening, we naturally attend to all levels of linguistic hierarchy. This is consistent with the widely accepted notion that the meaning of a natural sentence requires composing words in a grammatical structure. For most research questions in language, it therefore is sensible to use a task that mimics this automatic natural understanding of a sentence. Here, we show that automatic understanding of linguistic information, and all the processing that this entails, cannot be countered to substantially change the consequences for neural readout, even when explicitly instructing participants to pay attention to particular timescales.
Materials and methods
Participants
In total, 20 Dutch native speakers (16 females; age range: 18–59; mean age = 39.5) participated in the study. All were right handed, reported normal hearing, had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, and did not have any history of dyslexia or other language-related disorders. Participants performed a screening for their eligibility in the MEG and MRI and gave written informed consent. The study was approved by the Ethical Commission for human research Arnhem/Nijmegen (project number CMO2014/288). Participants were reimbursed for their participation. One participant was excluded from the analysis as they did not finish the full session.
Materials and design
Request a detailed protocolMaterials were identical to the stimuli used in Kaufeld et al., 2020. They consisted of naturally spoken sentences or word lists which consisted of 10 words (see Table 1 for examples). The sentences contained two coordinate clauses with the following structure: [Adj N V N Conj Det Adj N V N]. All words were disyllabic except for the words ‘de’ (the) and ‘en’ (and). Word lists were word-scrambled versions of the original sentences which always followed the structure [V V Adj Adj Det Conj N N N N] or [N N N N Det Conj V V Adj Adj] to ensure that they were grammatically incorrect. In total, 60 sentences were used. All sentences were presented at a comfortable sound level.
Participants were asked to perform four different tasks on these stimuli: a passive task, a syllable task, a word task, and a word-combination task. For the passive task, participants did not need to perform any task other than comprehension – they only needed to press a button to go to the next trial. For the syllable task, participants heard after every sentence two part-of-speech sounds, each consisting of one syllable. The sound fragments were a randomly determined syllable from the previously presented sentence and a random syllable from all other sentences. Participants’ task was to indicate via a button press which of the two sound fragments was part of the previous sentence. For the word task, two words were displayed on the screen after each trial (a random word from the just presented sentence and one random word from all other sentences excluding ‘de’ and ‘en’), and participants needed to indicate which of the two words was part of the sentence before. For the word-combination task, participants were presented with two word pairs on the screen. Each of the four words was part of the just presented sentence, but only one of the pairs was in the correct order. Participants needed to indicate which of the two pairs was presented in the sentence before. Presented options for the sentence condition were always a grammatically and semantically plausible combination of words. See Table 1 for an example of the tasks for each condition (sentences and word lists). The three active tasks required participants to focus on the syllabic (syllable task), word (word task), or phrasal (word combination or also called phrasal task) timescales. Note that different trials within a task were not matched for task difficulty. For example, in the syllable task syllables that make a word are much easier to recognize than syllables that do not make a word. Additionally, trials pertaining to the beginning of the sentence are more difficult than ones related to the end of the sentence due to recency effects.
Procedure
Request a detailed protocolAt the beginning of each trial, participants were instructed to look at a fixation cross presented at the middle of the screen on a grey background. Audio recordings were presented after a random interval between 1.5 and 3 s; 1 s after the end of the audio, the task was presented. For the word and word-combination task, this was the presentation of visual stimuli. For the syllable task, this entailed presenting the sound fragments one after each other (with a delay of 0.5 s in between). For the passive task, this was the instruction to press a button to continue. In total, there were eight blocks (two conditions × four tasks) each lasting about 8 min. The order of the blocks was pseudo-randomized by independently randomizing the order of the tasks and the conditions. For a single participant, we then always presented the same task twice in a row to avoid task-switching costs. As a consequence, condition was always alternated (a possible order of blocks would be: passive-sentence, passive-word list, word-sentence, word-word list, syllable-sentence, syllable-word list, word-combination-sentence, word-combination-word list). Across participants the starting condition was counterbalanced. After the main experiment, an auditory localizer was collected which consisted of listening to 200 ms sinewave and broadband sounds (centred at 0.5, 1, and 2 kHz; for the broadband at a 10% frequency band) at approximately equal loudness. Each sound had a 50 ms linear on and off ramp and was presented for 30 times (with random inter-stimulus interval between 1 and 2 s).
At arrival, participants filled out a screening. Electrodes to monitor eye movements and heart beat were placed (left mastoid was used as ground electrode) at an impedance below 15 kΩ. Participants wore metal free clothes and fitted earmolds on which two of the three head localizers were placed (together with a final head localizer placed at the nasion). They then performed the experiment in the MEG. MEG was recorded using a 275-channel axial gradiometer CTF MEG system at a sampling rate of 1.2 kHz. After every block participants had a break, during which head position was corrected (Stolk et al., 2013). After the session, the headshape was collected using Polhemus digitizer (using as fiducials the nasion and the entrance of the ear canals as positioned with the earmolds). For each participant, an MRI was collected with a 3T Siemens Skyra system using the MPRAGE sequence (1 mm isotropic). Also for the MRI acquisition participants wore the earmolds with vitamin pills to optimize the alignment.
Behavioural analysis
Request a detailed protocolWe performed a linear mixed model analysis with fixed factors task (syllable, word, and word combination) and condition (sentence and word list) as implemented by lmer in R4.1.0. The dependent variable was accuracy. First, any outliers were removed (values more extreme than median ± 2.5 IQR). Then, we investigated what the best random model was, including a random intercept or a random slope for one or two of the factors. The models with varying random factors were compared with each other using an analysis of variance. With no significant difference, the model with the lowest number of factors was included (with minimally a random intercept). Finally, lsmeans was used for follow-up tests using the kenward-roger method to calculate the degrees of freedom from the linear mixed model. For significant interactions, we investigated the effect of condition per task. For main effects, we investigated pairwise comparisons. We corrected for multiple comparisons using adjusted Bonferroni corrections unless specified otherwise. For all further reported statistical analyses for the MEG data, we followed the same procedure (except that there was one more level of task, i.e. the passive task). To avoid exploding the amount of comparisons, we a priori decided for any task effects in the MEG analysis to only compare the individual tasks with the phrase task.
MEG pre-processing
Request a detailed protocolFirst source models from the MRI were made using a surface-based approach in which grid points were defined on the cortical sheet using the automatic segmentation of freesurfer6.0 (Fischl, 2012) in combination with pre-processing tools from the HCP workbench1.3.2 (Glasser et al., 2013) to down-sample the mesh to 4 k vertices per hemisphere. The MRI was co-registered to the MEG using the previously defined fiducials as well as an automatic alignment of the MRI to the Polhemus headshape using the Fieldtrip20211102 software (Oostenveld et al., 2011).
Pre-processing involved epoching the data between −3 and +7.9 s (+3 relative to the longest sentence of 4.9 s) around sentence onset. We applied a dftfilter at 50, 100, and 150 Hz to remove line noise, a Butterworth bandpass filter between 0.6 and 100 Hz, and performed baseline correction (−0.2 to 0 s baseline). Trials with excessive movements or squid jumps were removed via visual inspection (20.1 ± 18.5 trials removed; mean ± standard deviation). Then data were resampled to 300 Hz and we performed ICA decomposition to correct for eye blinks/movement and heart beat artefacts (4.7 ± 0.99 components removed; mean ± standard deviation). Trials with remaining artefacts were removed by visual inspection (11.3 ± 12.4 trials removed; mean ± standard deviation). Then we applied a lcmv filter to transform the data to have single-trial source space representations. A common filter across all trials was calculated using a fixed orientation and a lambda of 5%. We only extracted time courses for our ROI, STG (Friederici, 2011; Park et al., 2015; Fegen et al., 2015; Koelsch et al., 2009), medial temporal gyrus (Pinker and Jackendoff, 2005; Peelle and Davis, 2012; Luo and Poeppel, 2007), and inferior frontal cortex (Ding et al., 2016; Kayser et al., 2015; Har-Shai Yahav and Zion Golumbic, 2021); numbers correspond to label-coding from the aparc parcellations implemented in Freesurfer. These time courses were baseline corrected (−0.2 to 0 s). To reduce computational load and to ensure that we used relevant data within the ROI, we extracted the top 20 PCA components per ROI for all following analyses based on a PCA using the time window of interest (0.5–3.7 s; 0.5 to ensure that all initial evoked responses were not included and 3.7 as it corresponds to the shortest trials). All following analyses were done per ROI. With enough statistical power one would add ROI as a separate factor in the analyses, but unfortunately, we did not have enough power to find a potential three-way interaction (ROI × condition × task). We therefore cannot make strong conclusions about one ROI having a stronger effect than another.
MI analysis
Request a detailed protocolFirst, we extracted the speech envelopes by following previous procedures (Kaufeld et al., 2020; Keitel et al., 2018; Gross et al., 2013; Ince et al., 2017). The acoustic waveforms (third-order Butterworth filter) were filtered in eight frequency bands (100–8000 Hz) equidistant on the cochlear frequency map (Smith et al., 2002). The absolute of the Hilbert transform was computed, we low passed the data at 100 Hz (third order Butterworth) and then down-sampled to 300 Hz (matching the MEG sampling rate). Then, we averaged across all bands.
MI was calculated between the filtered speech envelopes (using a third-order Butterworth filter) and the filtered MEG data at three different frequency bands corresponding to information content at different linguistic hierarchical levels: phrase (0.8–1.1 Hz), word (1.9–2.8 Hz), and syllable (3.5–5.0 Hz). The frequency bands were extracted based on the rate of the linguistic information in the speech signal. We hypothesized that tracking of relevant information should happen at those respective bands. While in our stimulus set the boundaries of the linguistic levels did not overlap, in natural speech the brain has an even more difficult task as there is no one-to-one match between band and linguistic unit (Obleser et al., 2012). Our main analysis focuses on the phrasal band, as that is where our previous study found the strongest effects (Kaufeld et al., 2020), but for completeness we also report on the other bands. MI was estimated after the evoked response (0.5 s) until the end of the stimulus at five different delays (60, 80, 100, 120, and 140 ms) and averaged across delays between the phase estimations of the envelopes and MEG data. A single MI value was generated per condition per ROI by concatenating all trials before calculating the MI (MEG and speech). Statistical analysis was performed per ROI per frequency band.
Power analysis
Request a detailed protocolPower analysis was performed to compare the MI results with absolute power changes. On the one hand, we did this analysis as MI differences could be a consequence of signal-to-noise differences in the original data (which would be reflected in power effects). On the other hand, generic delta power has been associated with language processing (Meyer, 2018; Kazanina and Tavano, 2021). Therefore, we choose to analyse classical frequency bands instead of the stimulus informed ones (as used in the MI analysis) in order to compare these results with other studies. Moreover, as this analysis does not measure tracking to the stimulus (like the MI analysis does) it did not seem appropriate to match the frequency content to the stimulus content. We first extracted the time–frequency representation for all conditions and ROIs separately. To do so, we performed a wavelet analysis with a width of 4, with a frequency of interest between 1 and 30 (step size of 1) and time of interest between −0.2 and 3.7 s (step size of 0.05 s). We extracted the logarithm of the power and baseline corrected the data in the frequency domain using a −0.3 and −0.1 s window. For four different frequency bands (delta: 0.5–3.0 Hz; theta: 3.0–8.0 Hz; alpha: 8.0–15.0 Hz; beta: 15.0–25.0 Hz) we extracted the mean power in the 0.5–3.7 s time window per task, condition, and ROI. Again, our main analysis focuses on the delta band, but we also report on the other bands for completeness. For each ROI, we performed the statistical analysis on power as described in the behavioural analysis.
Connectivity analysis
Request a detailed protocolFor the connectivity analysis, we repeated all pre-processing as in the power analysis, but separately for the left and right hemispheres (as we did not expect connections for PCA across hemispheres), after which we averaged the connectivity measure across hemispheres (using the Fourier spectrum and not the power spectrum). We used the debiased WPLI for our connectivity measure, which ensures that no zero-lag phase differences are included in the estimation (avoiding effects due to volume conduction). All connections between the three ROIs were investigated for the mean WPLI for the four different frequency bands also used in the power analysis in the 0.5–3.7 s time window. Also in this case, the same statistical analysis was applied. Note that not for all frequency bands we found a clear peak in the power signal (only clearly so for the alpha band), this indicates that the connectivity likely does not reflect endogenous oscillatory activity (Donoghue et al., 2020), but might still pertain to connected regions operating at those timescales.
MEG–behavioural performance analysis
Request a detailed protocolTo investigate the relation between the MEG measures and the behavioural performance we repeated the analyses (MI, power, and connectivity) but added accuracy as a factor (together with the interactions with the task and condition factor). As there is no accuracy for the passive task, we removed this task from the analysis. We then followed the same analyse steps as before. Since we reduced our degree of freedom, we could however only create random intercept and not random slope models.
Power control analysis
Request a detailed protocolThe reliability of phase estimations is influenced by the signal-to-noise ratio of the signal (Zar, 1998). As a consequence, trials with generally high power have more reliable phase estimations compared to low power trials. This could influence any measure relying on this phase estimation, such as MI and connectivity (Ince et al., 2017; Bastos and Schoffelen, 2015). It is therefore possible that power differences between conditions lead to differences between connectivity or MI. To ensure that our reported effects are not due to signal-to-noise effects, we controlled any significant power difference between conditions for the connectivity and MI analysis. To do this, we iteratively removed the highest and lowest power trials between the mean highest and mean lowest of the two relevant conditions (either collapsing trials across tasks/conditions or using individual conditions; for the MI analysis we used power estimated within its respective frequency band). We repeated this until the original condition with the highest power had lower power than the other condition. Then we repeated the analysis and statistics, investigating if the effect of interest was still significant. The control analysis is reported along the main MI and connectivity sections.
Other control analysis
Request a detailed protocolWe performed two final control analyses. Firstly, we investigated if age had an influence on any of our primary outcome measures. Secondly, we repeated the analyses using either 10 or 30 PCA components instead of the original 20 components. These controls ensure a robustness check of all the reported results. Note that this study was not intended to investigate age related differences. We therefore only report on interaction effect with our task and condition variables. Any main effect of age is difficult to interpret as it is unclear if the effect pertains to overall age-related differences (or anatomical variation leading to differential MEG responses) or language-related age differences.
Data availability
Data and analysis code are available at https://data.donders.ru.nl/collections/di/dccn/DSC_3027006.01_220 (doi: https://doi.org/10.34973/vjw9-0572).
-
Donders RepositoryTask relevant tracking of hierarchical linguistic structure.https://doi.org/10.34973/vjw9-0572
References
-
A Tutorial Review of Functional Connectivity Analysis Methods and Their Interpretational PitfallsFrontiers in Systems Neuroscience 9:175.https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2015.00175
-
Cortical tracking of hierarchical linguistic structures in connected speechNature Neuroscience 19:158–164.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4186
-
Temporal modulations in speech and musicNeuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 81:181–187.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.02.011
-
Parameterizing neural power spectra into periodic and aperiodic componentsNature Neuroscience 23:1655–1665.https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-020-00744-x
-
The brain basis of language processing: from structure to functionPhysiological Reviews 91:1357–1392.https://doi.org/10.1152/physrev.00006.2011
-
The core and beyond in the language-ready brainNeuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 81:194–204.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.01.048
-
Speech recognition: A model and A program for researchIEEE Transactions on Information Theory 8:155–159.https://doi.org/10.1109/TIT.1962.1057686
-
Oscillatory power decreases and long-term memory: the information via desynchronization hypothesisFrontiers in Human Neuroscience 6:74.https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00074
-
The cortical organization of speech processingNature Reviews. Neuroscience 8:393–402.https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2113
-
Shaping functional architecture by oscillatory alpha activity: gating by inhibitionFrontiers in Human Neuroscience 4:186.https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2010.00186
-
Linguistic Structure and Meaning Organize Neural Oscillations into a Content-Specific HierarchyThe Journal of Neuroscience 40:9467–9475.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0302-20.2020
-
Irregular Speech Rate Dissociates Auditory Cortical Entrainment, Evoked Responses, and Frontal AlphaThe Journal of Neuroscience 35:14691–14701.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2243-15.2015
-
EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: a review and analysisBrain Research. Brain Research Reviews 29:169–195.https://doi.org/10.1016/s0165-0173(98)00056-3
-
Functional architecture of verbal and tonal working memory: an FMRI studyHuman Brain Mapping 30:859–873.https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20550
-
Predicate learning in neural systems: using oscillations to discover latent structureCurrent Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 29:77–83.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2019.04.008
-
A Compositional Neural Architecture for LanguageJournal of Cognitive Neuroscience 32:1407–1427.https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01552
-
The neural oscillations of speech processing and language comprehension: state of the art and emerging mechanismsThe European Journal of Neuroscience 48:2609–2621.https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.13748
-
Synchronous, but not entrained: exogenous and endogenous cortical rhythms of speech and language processingLanguage, Cognition and Neuroscience 35:1089–1099.https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2019.1693050
-
Neural Oscillations in Speech: Don’t be Enslaved by the EnvelopeFrontiers in Human Neuroscience 6:250.https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00250
-
Neural Entrainment and Attentional Selection in the Listening BrainTrends in Cognitive Sciences 23:913–926.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2019.08.004
-
FieldTrip: Open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological dataComputational Intelligence and Neuroscience 2011:156869.https://doi.org/10.1155/2011/156869
-
Neural Oscillations Carry Speech Rhythm through to ComprehensionFrontiers in Psychology 3:320.https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00320
-
Temporal information in speech: acoustic, auditory and linguistic aspectsPhilosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences 336:367–373.https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.1992.0070
-
Neural representations of events arise from temporal community structureNature Neuroscience 16:486–492.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3331
-
Detection of sounds in the auditory stream: event-related fMRI evidence for differential activation to speech and nonspeechJournal of Cognitive Neuroscience 13:994–1005.https://doi.org/10.1162/089892901753165890
Article and author information
Author details
Funding
Max-Planck-Gesellschaft (Lise Meitner Research Group "Language and Computation in Neural Systems")
- Andrea E Martin
Max-Planck-Gesellschaft (Independent Research Group "Language and Computation in Neural Systems")
- Andrea E Martin
Nederlandse Organisatie voor Wetenschappelijk Onderzoek (016.Vidi.188.029)
- Andrea E Martin
The funders had no role in study design, data collection, and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Ethics
Participants performed a screening for their eligibility in the MEG and MRI and gave written informed consent. The study was approved by the Ethical Commission for human research Arnhem/Nijmegen (project number CMO2014/288). Participants were reimbursed for their participation.
Copyright
© 2022, ten Oever et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 1,011
- views
-
- 240
- downloads
-
- 22
- citations
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Neuroscience
Pain is heavily modulated by expectations. Whereas the integration of expectations with sensory information has been examined in some detail, little is known about how positive and negative expectations are generated and their neural dynamics from generation over anticipation to the integration with sensory information. The present preregistered study employed a novel paradigm to induce positive and negative expectations on a trial-by-trial basis and examined the neural mechanisms using combined EEG-fMRI measurements (n=50). We observed substantially different neural representations between the anticipatory and the actual pain period. In the anticipation phase i.e., before the nociceptive input, the insular cortex, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) showed increased activity for directed expectations regardless of their valence. Interestingly, a differentiation between positive and negative expectations within the majority of areas only occurred after the arrival of nociceptive information. FMRI-informed EEG analyses could reliably track the temporal sequence of processing showing an early effect in the DLPFC, followed by the anterior insula and late effects in the ACC. The observed effects indicate the involvement of different expectation-related subprocesses, including the transformation of visual information into a value signal that is maintained and differentiated according to its valence only during stimulus processing.
-
- Medicine
- Neuroscience
Background:
Alcohol use disorder (AUD) is a global health problem with limited therapeutic options. The biochemical mechanisms that lead to this disorder are not yet fully understood, and in this respect, metabolomics represents a promising approach to decipher metabolic events related to AUD. The plasma metabolome contains a plethora of bioactive molecules that reflects the functional changes in host metabolism but also the impact of the gut microbiome and nutritional habits.
Methods:
In this study, we investigated the impact of severe AUD (sAUD), and of a 3-week period of alcohol abstinence, on the blood metabolome (non-targeted LC-MS metabolomics analysis) in 96 sAUD patients hospitalized for alcohol withdrawal.
Results:
We found that the plasma levels of different lipids ((lyso)phosphatidylcholines, long-chain fatty acids), short-chain fatty acids (i.e. 3-hydroxyvaleric acid) and bile acids were altered in sAUD patients. In addition, several microbial metabolites, including indole-3-propionic acid, p-cresol sulfate, hippuric acid, pyrocatechol sulfate, and metabolites belonging to xanthine class (paraxanthine, theobromine and theophylline) were sensitive to alcohol exposure and alcohol withdrawal. 3-Hydroxyvaleric acid, caffeine metabolites (theobromine, paraxanthine, and theophylline) and microbial metabolites (hippuric acid and pyrocatechol sulfate) were correlated with anxiety, depression and alcohol craving. Metabolomics analysis in postmortem samples of frontal cortex and cerebrospinal fluid of those consuming a high level of alcohol revealed that those metabolites can be found also in brain tissue.
Conclusions:
Our data allow the identification of neuroactive metabolites, from interactions between food components and microbiota, which may represent new targets arising in the management of neuropsychiatric diseases such as sAUD.
Funding:
Gut2Behave project was initiated from ERA-NET NEURON network (Joint Transnational Call 2019) and was financed by Academy of Finland, French National Research Agency (ANR-19-NEUR-0003-03) and the Fonds de la Recherche Scientifique (FRS-FNRS; PINT-MULTI R.8013.19, Belgium). Metabolomics analysis of the TSDS samples was supported by grant from the Finnish Foundation for Alcohol Studies.