Homeostatic regulation through strengthening of neuronal network-correlated synaptic inputs
Abstract
Homeostatic regulation is essential for stable neuronal function. Several synaptic mechanisms of homeostatic plasticity have been described, but the functional properties of synapses involved in homeostasis are unknown. We used longitudinal two-photon functional imaging of dendritic spine calcium signals in visual and retrosplenial cortices of awake adult mice to quantify the sensory deprivation-induced changes in the responses of functionally identified spines. We found that spines whose activity selectively correlated with intrinsic network activity underwent tumor necrosis factor alpha (TNF-α)-dependent homeostatic increases in their response amplitudes, but spines identified as responsive to sensory stimulation did not. We observed an increase in the global sensory-evoked responses following sensory deprivation, despite the fact that the identified sensory inputs did not strengthen. Instead, global sensory-evoked responses correlated with the strength of network-correlated inputs. Our results suggest that homeostatic regulation of global responses is mediated through changes to intrinsic network-correlated inputs rather than changes to identified sensory inputs thought to drive sensory processing.
Editor's evaluation
When sensory inputs, such as vision or sound, are chronically disabled, the loss of input activity is counterbalanced by the upregulation of synaptic activity. In this study, the authors have addressed homeostatic responses in adult animals and provide evidence that instead of synapses that directly represent the sensory information, synapses that show correlated intrinsic network activity are the ones that undergo the change upon sensory deprivation. This fundamental and important paper whose key claims are well supported by the data, will be useful to readers in the fields of experience-dependent plasticity, sensory cortical coding, and homeostatic plasticity.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.81958.sa0Introduction
Homeostatic regulation of neuronal firing levels is a critical feature of neural circuits. It allows for prolonged stable function during changing activity levels associated with ongoing Hebbian plasticity, changes in sensory drive or input loss (Keck et al., 2017). The impairment of homeostatic regulation is thought to result in prolonged periods of excessive neuronal hyper- or hypoactivity, which may impair network function (Frere and Slutsky, 2018; Litwin-Kumar and Doiron, 2014; Marder and Prinz, 2002; Tetzlaff et al., 2011; Wu et al., 2020; Zenke et al., 2013). Homeostatic regulation occurs through plastic changes to a broad range of synaptic, intrinsic, cellular, and network-level properties (Barnes et al., 2015; Bridi et al., 2020; Bridi et al., 2018; Gainey and Feldman, 2017; Joseph and Turrigiano, 2017; Keck et al., 2017; Lee and Kirkwood, 2019; Ma et al., 2019; Turrigiano, 2011). These changes must facilitate rebalancing of activity while maintaining the underlying circuits established for ongoing sensory processing (Chen et al., 2013a; Iacaruso et al., 2017), as well as for learned (Jurjut et al., 2017; Lee et al., 2020; Poort et al., 2015) and innate behaviors (Branco and Redgrave, 2020; Evans et al., 2018). Maintaining this balance is particularly challenging with synaptic forms of homeostatic plasticity, where synaptic connections form the basis for circuitry and memory, but may also undergo homeostatic changes in strength that is inversely proportional to changes in activity levels. Previous work in adult animals has shown that synaptic homeostatic changes occur in only a subset of synapses (Barnes et al., 2017) or via additive changes (Goel and Lee, 2007), as opposed to multiplicative changes across the entire population described for some homeostatic mechanisms during development and the critical period (Desai et al., 2002; Kaneko et al., 2008; Turrigiano, 2017; Turrigiano et al., 1998). Identifying the functional properties of the subset of synapses that are homeostatically strengthened in adulthood is essential for understanding how sensory processing and homeostatic regulation are coordinated after the closure of the critical period, when sensory plasticity is reduced (Hensch, 2005; Levelt and Hübener, 2012).
Sensory cortices in adult mice are known to be multimodal and receive multiple sensory inputs (Ibrahim et al., 2016; Iurilli et al., 2012; Knöpfel et al., 2019; Todd et al., 2016; Vann et al., 2009; Yoshitake et al., 2013), as well as intrinsic contextual and intracortical inputs (Fiser et al., 2016; Khan and Hofer, 2018; Miller et al., 2014; Okun et al., 2015; Pakan et al., 2016; Pakan et al., 2018b; Petrus et al., 2015; Roth et al., 2016; Saleem et al., 2018). Large populations of neurons in visual cortex have a strong correlation or coupling with the average network activity, which is thought to be largely non-sensory and represent contextual input that may be associated with behavioral state (Okun et al., 2015; Stringer et al., 2019). High levels of network coupling are correlated with a higher probability of local intracortical connections between neurons (Okun et al., 2015) and could also reflect the presence of other common non-sensory inputs (Yoshimura et al., 2005). Following sensory deprivation, functional strengthening could occur in functional subsets of inputs, such as the remaining sensory inputs, other non-deprived sensory inputs, or network-correlated inputs, either independently or in a combination of subsets; however, it is currently unclear whether the synapses that undergo homeostatic strengthening encode a specific functional input.
Here, we examine the properties of inputs that undergo functional changes following sensory deprivation using repeated two-photon functional imaging of dendritic spines in awake mice (Chen et al., 2013b; Iacaruso et al., 2017). We find that, following deprivation, inputs with activity that is correlated with the network activity undergo functional strengthening, while inputs that have identified sensory-evoked responses do not strengthen, independent of whether they encode a deprived or non-deprived sensory input. Despite the fact that sensory-evoked spine responses are not strengthened, we find that global sensory-evoked responses homeostatically increase following deprivation and are correlated with increases in responses of network-correlated spines on their dendrites. Together, our work suggests that homeostatic synaptic regulation occurs through changes to network-correlated inputs, rather than the sensory responsive inputs that drive ongoing sensory processing.
Results
We used chronic two-photon imaging in awake adult mice expressing the genetically encoded calcium indicator GCaMP6s (see Methods) to examine the functional properties of dendritic spines that undergo functional plasticity following sensory deprivation. Over a span of 4 days, we repeatedly measured functional calcium signals in dendritic spines on the distal tufts (in layer 2/3) of layer 5 pyramidal cells in primary monocular visual cortex before and after sensory deprivation via enucleation of the contralateral eye (Figure 1A–C; Barnes et al., 2015). We extracted spine responses using previously published methods (Chen et al., 2013b; Iacaruso et al., 2017; Wilson et al., 2016) and were able to measure the functional response properties, response amplitude, and response frequency of the same spines over the course of the imaging time series (Figure 1B–C, Figure 1—figure supplement 1A–D).

Functional strengthening of dendritic spines following sensory deprivation.
(A) Experimental timeline. Green circles denote imaging time points, pink arrows denote time of tumor necrosis factor alpha (TNF-α) inhibitor injections, and blue arrow shows time of monocular enucleation (0 hr). (B,C) Maximum intensity projection of activity for a dendritic stretch of a layer 5 pyramidal cell measured before (B, –1 hr) and after (C, 24 hr) deprivation. Upper inset, higher magnification image of highlighted (arrowhead) individual spine, in which circles show the region of interest for the dendritic spine (orange) and the dendrite (green). Lower inset, calcium fluorescence signals measured at the dendritic spine (orange) and adjacent dendrite (green). Axons were removed from the background of the image for clarity. Scale bar, 25% ΔF/F0, 5 s. (D) Percentage of spines that become inactive at each time point in control (black) and deprived (blue) mice. All spines included were active during at least one imaging time point. Asterisks denote comparison of deprived and control values. (E) Single trial (gray) and average (black/blue) events, aligned by peak, of an example spine at –1 hr (left, black) and 48 hr (middle, blue) from deprivation (0 hr), and the overlay of both (right). (F) Percent change in amplitude normalized to baseline (–24 and –1 hr) of calcium transients measured in persistent (active at all time points) dendritic spines in control (black), deprived (blue) and deprived with TNF-α inhibitor (pink) conditions. Deprivation occurs at 0 hr. For all statistics, see Figure 1—source data 1. ***p<0.001, NS, no significance. Error bars, standard error of the mean (SEM).
-
Figure 1—source data 1
Statistical comparisons for Figure 1.
- https://cdn.elifesciences.org/articles/81958/elife-81958-fig1-data1-v2.docx
Functional strengthening of dendritic spines following sensory deprivation
Consistent with previous work (Barnes et al., 2017), we found that approximately 20% of spines in the contralateral visual cortex became inactive 12 hr after monocular deprivation, with the number of inactive spines decreasing over time (Figure 1D, see Methods). In these experiments, inactive spines could represent either inactivity or spine loss, which can be indistinguishable with GCaMP6s labelling. On average, the persistent (active at all time points) spines increased their functional response amplitudes (Figure 1E–F). It is important to note that the reduction of inactive spines over time may reflect spines whose activity decreased below detection thresholds immediately following deprivation and then, following subsequent homeostatic strengthening, were detectable again.
Certain forms of homeostatic synaptic changes are dependent on the cytokine tumor necrosis factor alpha (TNF-α) (Beattie et al., 2002; Stellwagen and Malenka, 2006), so we examined if that was the case for the synaptic strengthening observed here. The increased amplitude of spine responses was blocked by an intraperitoneal injection of a dominant negative form of TNF-α (Figure 1A and F), suggesting this effect is dependent on TNF-α. Following deprivation, we found no changes over time in the average frequency of responses at the population level (Figure 1—figure supplement 1E).
Previous work has shown that spine-specific decreases in response frequency drive synapse-specific increases in response amplitude (Lee et al., 2010). We therefore examined if frequency changes were indicative of amplitude changes in individual spines. We observed no correlation between changes in frequency and amplitude within individual spines (Figure 1—figure supplement 1F), indicating that the changes in frequency are not likely to be driving the changes in amplitude within a single spine.
Identifying functional subsets of dendritic spines in visual cortex
We have previously used structural imaging of spine size as an in vivo proxy for synaptic strength and shown that not all spines undergo a TNF-α-dependent strengthening following deprivation (Barnes et al., 2017). One possibility is that there could be common functional properties of spines that undergo homeostatic strengthening. We therefore characterized the response properties of individual spines prior to any form of deprivation during the baseline time points (–24 and –1 hr). We first measured the responses of spines to visual stimuli: bilateral drifting gratings (Barnes et al., 2015) and a sparse noise stimulus (de Vries et al., 2020; Figure 2A). Spines that had time-locked responses to the visual stimulus at levels above chance (see Methods) (Barnes et al., 2015; Ko et al., 2011) were categorized as ‘visually responsive’ spines (Figure 2B). We found that a majority of spines that were responsive to the sparse noise stimulus also responded to drifting gratings (97%). Therefore, in a subset of experiments, we characterized visually responsive spines using only the drifting gratings stimulus to allow sufficient imaging time to test other stimuli. When we examined the functional strengthening of all of the spines, we found an inverse relationship, such that the spines with the lowest visual responsiveness prior to deprivation underwent the strongest functional strengthening after deprivation (Figure 2C). Consistent with our previous work (Barnes et al., 2017), we observed that only a subset of spines increased their functional responses (Figure 2C).

Identifying functional subsets of dendritic spines in visual cortex.
(A) Schematic of the stimulus orientation and direction (top) and calcium responses of an individual visually responsive spine (red), the average network response (black), an individual network-correlated spine (gray), and an individual unclassified spine (green). Dark gray shading indicates the mouse was stationary in the dark. (B) Distribution of the percentage of visual stimuli that elicit a time-locked response for all deprived spines during baseline (–24 and –1 hr). Gray dashed line indicates the number of time-locked responses to visual stimuli expected by chance (5.5%, see Methods). (C) Percent change in spine amplitude at 48 hr normalized to baseline (–24 and –1 hr) for the entire population of spines in deprived mice as a function of the percent of visual stimuli out of all visual presentations that elicit time-locked responses, measured during baseline (–24 and –1 hr). Gray dashed line indicates chance levels (see Methods) for percentage of visual responses (vertical) and no change in spine amplitude relative to baseline (horizontal). (D) Maximum intensity projection of activity of a dendritic segment with network-correlated (gray), visually responsive (red), and unclassified (green) spines. (E) Schematic of functional property clustering analysis for first iteration of spine0 (top) and shuffled condition (bottom), for the example dendrite in Panel D. Spines match (value of 1) when they have the same functional class (network-correlated [gray], visually responsive [red], unclassified [green]) as spine0. (F) Population data for probability of matching the functional properties of a neighboring spine for the actual data (blue) and randomly shuffled position (black) of a given spine on a dendritic branch. For statistics, see Figure 2—source data 1. Error bars, standard error of the mean (SEM).
-
Figure 2—source data 1
Statistical comparisons for Figure 2.
- https://cdn.elifesciences.org/articles/81958/elife-81958-fig2-data1-v2.docx
We next characterized other functional inputs. Within mouse visual cortex, there are high levels of correlated network activity, which may reflect the intrinsic dynamics (Okun et al., 2015; Stringer et al., 2019). We therefore hypothesized that activity in some spines may reflect this network activity, potentially via local intracortical connections (Okun et al., 2015). We measured the correlation of individual spines’ activity with the ‘network signal’, which we measured as the average activity during all experimental conditions (visual stimulation and darkness) of all the other spines in the same imaging region (Okun et al., 2015; Figure 2A, see Methods). The network signal does not reflect locomotion-related activity, as the mice were stationary during our measurements.
In our population of spines, we found that most spines (89%) had a significant correlation with the network signal. This included the visually responsive spines, all of which had a significant correlation with the network signal (see Methods). As described above, all spines with time-locked responses to visual stimuli at above chance levels (see Methods) were classified as visually responsive spines (24%; Figure 2B), even though their activity also correlated with the network signal. Spines with activity that correlated with the network signal, but did not have visually-evoked responses, were classified as network-correlated (65%). The remainder of spines neither responded to the visual stimuli nor correlated with the network activity, which we refer to as unclassified (11%; Figure 2A). We tested whether these unclassified spines were correlated with other behavioral measures, such as pupil movement, pupil dilation (diameter), and whisker movement, and found no evidence of significant correlations above chance (see Methods). There were no significant differences in the average activity in the three groups of spines prior to deprivation (measured as the area under the curve of the ΔF/F0 fluorescence signal; comparison between network-correlated, visually responsive and unclassified spine activity, one-way ANOVA, p=0.232).
We next examined if input types were clustered on the same dendritic branches (Hedrick et al., 2022). All of the branches that we measured had all three types of spines (visually responsive, network-correlated, unclassified). Within individual dendritic branches, we found that the local spatial organization of functional response properties (visually responsive, network-correlated, unclassified) of the dendritic spines were not different from chance (Figure 2D–F, see Methods).
Functional strengthening occurs in network-correlated spines
We next examined if the spines that were functionally strengthened had common functional properties (Figure 3). Consistent with our earlier finding (Figure 2C), we found no evidence of an increase in the amplitude of the persistent spines identified as visually responsive (Figure 3A), or of the persistent unclassified spines (Figure 3B). However, following deprivation there was an increase in the response amplitude of persistent network-correlated spines (Figure 3C). Overall activity depends on both the amplitude and frequency of responses. While the average frequency of responses did not change following deprivation (Figure 1—figure supplement 1E), we examined if functional groups of spines had changes in response frequency and observed a decrease in the frequency of responses of visually responsive spines over time following deprivation (Figure 3—figure supplement 1A–C). When we examined the integral of the activity trace (which combines amplitude and frequency) over time following deprivation, we observed a decrease for visually responsive spines (Figure 3—figure supplement 1D), no change for unclassified spines (Figure 3—figure supplement 1E), and an increase for network-correlated spines (Figure 3—figure supplement 1F). We also found a strong increase in the fraction of inactive visually responsive spines after deprivation. While a larger fraction of visually responsive spines become inactive, the absolute number of network-correlated spines that became inactive was higher, since network-correlated spines reflect a larger proportion of the total spine population. These results suggest that the increase in spine loss previously reported following enucleation (Barnes et al., 2017) may include both spines that were visually responsive and network-correlated prior to deprivation (Figure 3D–F). A sizeable fraction of visually responsive spines remains active after visual deprivation (Figure 3D). It is important to note that following sensory deprivation, we no longer observed responses to visual stimuli in the visually responsive spines, since the feedforward input had been permanently removed. The responses of the previously categorized visually responsive spines consisted of network responses following deprivation.

Functional strengthening occurs in network-correlated spines.
(A,B,C) Percent change in spine amplitude normalized to baseline (–24 and –1 hr) of calcium transients for persistent (active at all time points) spines that were classified during baseline as visually responsive (A, red), unclassified (B, green), and network-correlated (C, gray), either following enucleation (0 hr) or sham-enucleated controls (black). (D,E,F) Percentage of spines that become inactive that were classified during baseline as visually responsive (D, red), unclassified (E, green), and network-correlated (F, gray), either following enucleation (0 hr) or sham-enucleated controls (black). All spines included are active during at least one imaging time point. (G,H,I) In deprived mice, normalized percent change in calcium transient frequency at 12 hr (relative to baseline: –24 and –1 hr) versus normalized percent change in amplitude at 48 hr (relative to baseline: –24 and –1 hr) for individual persistent (active at all time points) spines classified as visually responsive (G), unclassified (H), and network-correlated (I). For statistics, see Figure 3—source data 1. *p<0.05, ***p<0.001. Error bars, standard error of the mean (SEM).
-
Figure 3—source data 1
Statistical comparisons for Figure 3.
- https://cdn.elifesciences.org/articles/81958/elife-81958-fig3-data1-v2.docx
There was no correlation between amplitude and frequency within individual spines in any of the groups (Figure 3G–I), again suggesting that frequency decrease does not regulate changes in amplitude within individual spines in this paradigm. Overall, these results suggest the network-correlated inputs, and not the identified visually responsive inputs, are homeostatically strengthened following sensory input loss.
Identified sensory responsive spines are not functionally strengthened
Having found that the network-correlated spines increase their activity, we wanted to determine whether this more generally reflected the functional strengthening of all non-deprived inputs. To investigate this issue, we used a multimodal brain area – the retrosplenial cortex (RSC) – which receives measurable auditory responsive and visually responsive inputs (Figure 4A). We again measured responses of dendritic spines on layer 5 cells in the distal tufts in layer 2/3. Using the same classification approach as before (Figure 2, see Methods), we found that the activity of 92% of the spines was correlated with the network signal. Within this group, a subset of spines had time-locked responses to visual (18.7%) or auditory (15.9%) stimuli (Figure 4A) and the rest correlated only with the network signal (57.4%, Figure 4A). The remaining 8% of spines were unclassified (no correlation with the network signal and no sensory-evoked responses). Following reversible visual deprivation via dark exposure for 48 hr (Figure 4B), we observed an increase in the average spine response amplitude across the population. This response was TNF-α-dependent (Figure 4C), similar to what we observed in visual cortex following enucleation (Figure 1). When we examined the changes in the responses of functionally identified spines, we observed an increase in the response amplitude of the network-correlated spines, but not in the visually responsive or unclassified spines (Figure 4D, Figure 4—figure supplement 1A–C), consistent with our results in visual cortex (Figure 2C, Figure 3A–C). We then examined the responses of the auditory responsive spines, which represent an identified non-deprived sensory input. After visual deprivation, the auditory responsive spines did not undergo functional strengthening but maintained their baseline amplitude following visual deprivation (Figure 4E).

Identified sensory responsive spines are not functionally strengthened after sensory deprivation.
(A) Example activity of auditory responsive (orange), visually responsive (red), and network-correlated (gray) spines, as well as following tumor necrosis factor alpha (TNF-α) inhibitor injections (pink). Auditory: black dots above the trace indicate the timing of auditory stimuli. Visual: orientation and direction of presented visual stimuli that evoked a response are shown above the trace. Scale bars, 50% ΔF/F0 and 10 s. (B,F) Experimental timeline for dark exposure (B) or auditory ear plug (F) deprivation experiments showing imaging time points (green circles) and timing of TNF-α inhibitor injections (pink arrows). (C,G) Percent change in amplitude normalized to baseline (–1 and 0 hr) of all spines following visual (C) or auditory (G) deprivation (blue), control (black) or deprivation with TNF-α inhibition (pink) in response to drifting gratings (C) or auditory stimulation (G). (D,H) During presentation of drifting gratings, percent change in amplitude normalized to baseline (–1 and 0 hr) for control visually responsive spines (black), deprived network-correlated spines (gray), or deprived visually responsive spines (red) following visual deprivation (D) or auditory deprivation (H). (E,I) During auditory stimulation, percent change in amplitude normalized to baseline (–1 and 0 hr) for control auditory responsive spines (black), deprived network-correlated spines (gray), or deprived auditory responsive spines (orange) following visual deprivation (E) or auditory deprivation (I). (J) Forty-eight hours after visual deprivation, average global response amplitude (measured in dendritic branches) in response to drifting gratings (red) or auditory stimulation (orange), normalized to the response of the same branches to the same stimulus during baseline (–1 and 0 hr). (K,L,M) Change in activity 48 hr after visual deprivation normalized to baseline (–1 and 0 hr) for parent global dendritic branch activity and for spine activity for visually responsive (K), auditory responsive (L), and network-correlated (M) spines on that branch. For statistics, see Figure 4—source data 1. **p<0.01, ***p<0.001, NS, no significance. Error bars, standard error of the mean (SEM). Asterisks in C–I denote comparisons at 48 hr.
-
Figure 4—source data 1
Statistical comparisons for Figure 4.
- https://cdn.elifesciences.org/articles/81958/elife-81958-fig4-data1-v2.docx
To ensure that the strengthening effects that we observed were not exclusive to visual deprivation, we performed a complementary, but separate, experiment where we deprived mice of auditory input through reversible ear plugging for 48 hr and again measured post-deprivation spine responses in the RSC (Figure 4F). We observed a TNF-α-dependent population increase in spine responses (Figure 4G). Similar to visual deprivation, network-correlated spines increased their responses following auditory deprivation (Figure 4H–I, Figure 4—figure supplement 1D–F), but there was no change in the response strength of either (non-deprived) visually responsive (Figure 4H) or (deprived) auditory responsive spines (Figure 4I). Together, these data suggest that increases in spine response amplitude predominantly occur in the spines whose activity correlated with the network signal and not in the spines which are responsive to the non-deprived or deprived sensory stimulation.
Increased global sensory-evoked responses correlate with increased network-correlated spine responses
Previous work has shown that following visual deprivation, there is a homeostatic increase in the cellular visually-evoked responses relative to baseline (Mrsic-Flogel et al., 2007). Critically, this homeostatic response is hypothesized to occur through a presumed strengthening of the spared visual inputs (Kaneko et al., 2008; Keck et al., 2017; Mrsic-Flogel et al., 2007; Pandey et al., 2022; Turrigiano, 2017). Given that we did not observe increases in the response strength of sensory responsive spines, we examined if the global sensory responses were increased following dark exposure (a reversible deprivation paradigm) in adult mice. As a proxy for spiking, we measured the global dendritic signals from the same dendrites of layer 5 cells located in lower layer 2/3, where we also measured the spine signals, which we refer to as the ‘global dendritic responses’. In sensory cortices under our experimental conditions, the majority of dendritic signals are thought to reflect back-propagating action potentials (Beaulieu-Laroche et al., 2019; Francioni et al., 2019). We found that following visual deprivation via dark exposure, the global dendritic responses in RSC to both visual and auditory stimulation were increased when compared with baseline measurements (Figure 4J), consistent with past results (Mrsic-Flogel et al., 2007). This was despite the fact that neither the visually responsive nor auditory responsive spines increased their responses to the same respective stimuli (Figure 4D–E). Both the global visually-evoked responses and individual visually responsive spines maintained their visual tuning before and after deprivation at levels comparable to control (Figure 4—figure supplement 1—source data 1).
We then examined the relationship between changes in post-deprivation activity relative to pre-deprivation baseline activity for both global dendritic responses and visually-evoked spine responses. Increases in global dendritic responses after deprivation were uncorrelated with the changes in visually-evoked spine responses on those dendrites, making it unlikely that increases in global dendritic responses were driven by increases in the strength of visually-evoked spine responses (Figure 4K). There was also no relationship within a dendrite between changes in global dendritic responses and auditory-evoked spine responses (Figure 4L). Conversely, increases in global dendritic responses were correlated with an increase in the network-correlated spine activity on the same dendrites (Figure 4M). Our results suggest that increases in sensory-evoked global dendritic responses are unlikely to result from increases in the response strength of the identified sensory responsive inputs.
Discussion
Here, we report a TNF-α-dependent increase in the functional responses of dendritic spines in awake mice following sensory deprivation. Surprisingly, when we examine the functional properties of spines that undergo strengthening, we find dendritic spines whose activity is correlated with the network signal are strengthened, but spines identified as sensory responsive are not. This strengthening of network-correlated spines occurs over a similar time course and magnitude in both visual cortex and RSC and in response to different deprivation paradigms. Despite the fact that we do not see an increase in responses of sensory responsive spines, we do observe a homeostatic increase in global sensory-evoked responses following sensory deprivation. This global increase is correlated with an increase in the response strength of network-correlated spines on those branches, but not of identified sensory responsive spines. Our findings suggest that in vivo synaptic homeostasis during adulthood occurs in a functional subset of inputs, specifically those correlated only with network activity, rather than the identified sensory responsive inputs that are thought to drive sensory processing.
Homeostatic regulation of network activity
Previous work has hypothesized that inputs are homeostatically strengthened following sensory deprivation (Keck et al., 2017; Turrigiano, 2017), and that this process may facilitate the homeostatic regulation of cellular activity levels (Barnes et al., 2015; Hengen et al., 2016; Hengen et al., 2013; Keck et al., 2013). Our results suggest that the homeostatic increase in sensory-evoked responses observed after deprivation in adult mice is not associated with increases in the response strength of identified sensory responsive inputs. Instead, we observe a correlation between increases in global sensory-evoked activity levels and increases in the functional response strength of network-correlated spines. Why do we see an increase in global sensory-evoked responses if there is not a change in the functional strength of the sensory responsive inputs? One possibility is that the strengthened network connections serve to amplify intrinsic activity, keeping cells closer to the threshold for action potential generation and thereby making incoming sensory responsive inputs more likely to elicit the observed sensory-evoked increases in post-synaptic activity. In line with this idea, previous work has shown that homeostatic recovery at the level of small networks of cells is mediated by increases in the intrinsic network activity (Barnes et al., 2015) and that intrinsic network activity is amplified within these interconnected neural ensembles (Miller et al., 2014). Together, these findings suggest that the amplification of network activity through a strengthening of network connections may underlie rebalancing of activity in the cortex following sensory deprivation affecting network activity. This provides a mechanism during adulthood by which overall activity can be modulated through the network inputs, without altering the identified sensory inputs and the established circuits thought to drive sensory processing.
If intrinsic network activity is amplified, what might these network-correlated inputs encode? Network-correlated spines do not respond: (1) in the case of visual stimulation to either drifting gratings or sparse noise visual stimuli, which are thought to drive broad visual responses, or (2) in RSC, in the case of auditory stimulation, specifically white-noise bursts. We cannot rule out that these network-correlated spines may respond to more specific visual or auditory stimuli that were not presented, or to sensory-feedback signals from higher brain regions, which may not be time-locked to the visual stimuli. The presence of the unclassified inputs as a category indicates that (1) not all inputs correlate with the network signal by default and (2) network-correlated inputs do not simply encompass inputs that were not identified as sensory responsive. The mouse cortex is known to respond to many non-sensory inputs and the network signal could reflect contextual signals (Khan and Hofer, 2018; Pakan et al., 2016; Pakan et al., 2018b; Tohmi et al., 2014; Yoshitake et al., 2013), spatial information (Fiser et al., 2016; Pakan et al., 2018a; Saleem et al., 2018), intrinsic signals (Miller et al., 2014; Okun et al., 2015; Stringer et al., 2019), or intracortical inputs, which are known to be preferentially strengthened in ex vivo preparations following dark exposure (Petrus et al., 2015). The network signal likely represents some combination of these signals. Given that our data were collected while the mice were stationary, it is unlikely that the network signal is driven by activity related to locomotion.
Synaptic and molecular mechanisms
We find evidence that the functional strengthening of network-correlated spines is dependent on the cell-signalling cytokine TNF-α. Previous work has shown that soluble TNF-α is released from glial cells following reductions in glutamate levels and plays a key role in synaptic strengthening by facilitating the insertion of α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic acid receptors (AMPAR) (Stellwagen and Malenka, 2006). Importantly, the synaptic localization of N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors (NMDAR) and their associated currents are unaffected by changes in TNF-α levels (Beattie et al., 2002; He et al., 2012). The specific actions of TNF-α may in part explain in vivo findings, where pharmacological blockade of TNF-α has shown selective impairments of homeostatic, but not NMDAR-dependent, Hebbian-like plasticity processes (Kaneko et al., 2008). Further work is required to better understand the key neural circuit and molecular features that allow the selective modulation of homeostatic adaption and even more specifically, the network-correlated spines. One possibility is that TNF-α could target specific receptor profiles, potentially through differential synaptic expression of the TNF receptor 1 which is thought to be central to TNF-α-dependent synaptic plasticity (Grell et al., 1995; He et al., 2012); however, this would assume that homeostatic regulation occurs in the layer 5 cells.
Previous work has shown that TNF-α-dependent AMPAR insertion during homeostatic regulation is associated with the glutamate receptor-interacting protein 1 (GRIP1), a synaptic scaffolding protein which facilitates insertion of synaptic AMPARs, thereby increasing the AMPAR current and number (Barnes et al., 2017; Gainey et al., 2009). Increases in GRIP1-dependent AMPAR insertion may be involved in the functional strengthening of spine responses that we observe here, as we have previously demonstrated that AMPAR-mediated synaptic strengthening occurs following monocular enucleation in a TNF-α-dependent manner and involves GRIP1 (Barnes et al., 2017). Alternatively, changes in inhibitory neural circuitry may contribute to the increased functional response strength that we observe in dendritic spines. Previous work has demonstrated that inhibitory synapses located on dendritic spines (also containing excitatory synapses) are eliminated at a higher rate following sensory deprivation (Chen et al., 2012; van Versendaal et al., 2012). The loss of local synapse-specific inhibition could enhance the response strength of individual spines; however, given that these changes in inhibitory synapses are only seen in a subset of spines (~15%), they are unlikely to be solely responsible for the observed amplitude increases in the network-correlated spines we report, which comprise more than 60% of the total spine population.
Finally, it is important to note that while we made these measurements in layer 5 pyramidal cells, the homeostatic changes mediated by TNF-α could occur outside of layer 5, including changes to upstream inputs or changes to the presynaptic responses, either through changes in presynaptic release (Vitureira et al., 2012) or through a change in activity patterns of the presynaptic cell (e.g., bursts compared to single spikes) (Linden et al., 2009). Given that both sensory responsive spines (which do not strengthen) and network-correlated spines (which do strengthen) exhibit only network-correlated responses after sensory deprivation, changes in presynaptic activity patterns of network signals would require targeted changes to the network-corelated spines, but not the previously sensory responsive spines. Further research is necessary to identify the role of presynaptic plasticity in homeostatic synaptic plasticity in vivo.
Network homeostasis and sensory processing
If the network-correlated inputs are homeostatically regulated, what happens to the sensory responsive inputs following deprivation? We found evidence for a reduction in the overall activity of identified sensory responsive spines in visual cortex, with a fraction of visually responsive spines becoming inactive following enucleation. Our previous work has shown that monocular enucleation is associated with structural spine loss, suggesting that part of the functional silencing we observe here may occur through loss of dendritic spines (Barnes et al., 2017). A considerable number of identified sensory responsive spines do remain active, but in both visual cortex and RSC, they undergo no increase in response amplitude following sensory deprivation, independent of whether they are deprived or non-deprived inputs. Thus, we consistently observe that identified sensory responsive inputs are not strengthened in sensory cortices following deprivation, but that many sensory responsive inputs are preserved, which could underlie the stability of visual tuning that has been observed following sensory deprivation (Figure 4—figure supplement 1—source data 1; Rose et al., 2016).
What are the potential advantages of strengthening the network-correlated inputs, rather than the sensory responsive inputs? Through homeostatic regulation of the network activity, feedforward sensory input-output relationships that may encode sensory information and underlie sensory processing are not perturbed during adulthood. Our previous work (Barnes et al., 2017) has suggested that scaling up the strongest sensory inputs could lead to a saturation of these inputs, due to known dendritic non-linearities in the local integration of multiple inputs (Branco and Häusser, 2011; Branco and Häusser, 2010). If sensory responsive inputs are saturated and no longer have distinct weights, information processing in the neuron is reduced (Barnes et al., 2017). By homeostatically regulating the network-correlated inputs, the dynamic range of the sensory responsive inputs – both the remaining deprived sensory responsive inputs and the non-deprived sensory responsive inputs – is unaltered and the cortex can continue to process a wide range of external sensory stimuli (Panzeri et al., 2017; Panzeri et al., 2010).
Methods
Reagent type (species) or resource | Designation | Source or reference | Identifiers | Additional information |
---|---|---|---|---|
Strain, strain background (AAV) | AAV2/1-ef1a-GCaMP6s | FMI vector core | N/A | |
Strain, strain background (Mus musculus) | C57BL/6J | Charles River | N/A | |
Chemical compound, drug | XPRo1595 | Xencor, Inc | N/A |
Mice
Experiments were conducted according to the United Kingdom Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act 1986 or were approved by the Veterinary Department of the Canton of Basel-Stadt, Switzerland. We used 43 adult (P60-120) male and female mice (C57BL/6) and measured 5858 individual spines. All mice were sex and age matched within experimental groups. Mice were housed with littermates (two to six mice depending on litter size) and kept on a 12 hr light-dark cycle at 21°C. Imaging experiments were time matched during the light cycle (except in dark exposure experiments).
Surgery
For in vivo imaging experiments, cranial windows were surgically implanted, as described previously (Holtmaat et al., 2009), over the right hemisphere of visual cortex (coordinates 1.5–2.5 mm lateral of the midline and 0–1 mm anterior of lambda), which corresponds to the monocular region of visual cortex, or over the right hemisphere of RSC (coordinates 0.2–0.8 mm lateral of the midline and 0–1.5 mm anterior of lambda). We made a craniotomy in ketamine/xylazine (0.15 and 0.015 mg/g of body weight, respectively) anesthetized mice and replaced the skull with a glass coverslip that was attached to the bone with dental cement. Mice were injected with AAV2/1-ef1a-GCaMP6s into layer 5 (500 µm deep) before the glass coverslip was positioned. Mice were allowed to recover for at least 28 days after surgery before imaging commenced. For monocular enucleation, we applied lidocaine (Emla cream) to the area around the left (contralateral) eye in anesthetized mice prior to surgical removal of the eye. Control sham-enucleated mice were given time-matched anesthesia. For dark exposure experiments, mice were housed in a dark cabinet for 48 hr. For auditory deprivation experiments, mice were anesthetized with isoflurane and the ear canal was filled with a plastic polymer (Kwik-Sil) (Zhuang et al., 2017) for 48 hr. The polymer was removed prior to testing at the 48 hr time point. Control sham-deprived mice were given time-matched anesthesia. Mice were randomly assigned into deprivation or sham-deprivation groups, such that half of each litter was in each group (except for dark exposure, where assignments were randomly made by cage, so as to not separate littermates). In visual cortex experiments, mice were imaged at –24 and –1 hr prior to deprivation or sham-deprivation and 12, 24, and 48 hr after. For RSC experiments, mice were imaged twice in a baseline session and then 48 hr later following deprivation or sham-deprivation. For experiments with the TNF-α inhibitor, mice were injected with XPro1595 (Barnes et al., 2017; Lewitus et al., 2014) (Xencor, Inc) at a dose of 10 mg/kg at –48 and –1 hr for RSC experiments and at −72, –24, and 24 hr for visual cortex experiments.
Functional imaging and sensory stimulation
Measurements of functional imaging data were conducted as described previously (Barnes et al., 2017; Barnes et al., 2015; Sammons et al., 2018). Functional calcium imaging of volumes of visual cortex or RSC was performed on a custom-built two-photon microscope with a 12 kHz resonance scanner (Cambridge Technology) and a high-power objective Z-piezo stage (Physik Instrumente), using a MaiTai eHP laser with a DeepSee prechirp unit (Newport/Spectra Physics) set to 910 nm and a Nikon 16×, 0.8 NA objective, as described previously (Barnes et al., 2015). Data were acquired with an 800 MHz digitizer (National Instruments) and pre-processed with a custom programmed field programmable gate array (National Instruments). The dynamic range of both the amplifier and the photo-multiplier tube exceeded the digitization range and the data acquisition software automatically detected digital saturation of all pixels. Imaging data were full-frame registered using a custom written registration algorithm. To remove slow signal changes in raw fluorescence traces, the 8th-percentile value of the fluorescence distribution in a ±15 s window was subtracted from the raw fluorescence signal (Dombeck et al., 2007). Intrinsic signal imaging before enucleation was used to localize the monocular visual cortex as described previously (Barnes et al., 2015; Keck et al., 2013), and the RSC was localized using stereotaxic coordinates. Layer 5 dendrites were identified by tracing the dendrites to the cell bodies. Separating layer 2/3 from layer 5 was aided by the fact that expression levels in layer 4 neurons were much lower in our preparation.
Mice were allowed to habituate to the setup while head-fixed and data were included from periods when the mice were stationary. Mice were presented with drifting gratings in eight directions (0–360 degrees in 45 degree steps) presented in a random order, with a spatial frequency of 0.04 cycles/degree and a temporal frequency of 2 Hz. In a subset of mice, we also presented a sparse visual noise stimulus. This consisted of a sequence of sparse noise stimuli that changed every 250 ms. Each sparse noise stimulus was a set of between 8 and 12, black or white squares (of 8 degrees of visual angle) on a gray background. The location of the squares was chosen randomly, but such that squares did not overlap in an individual sparse noise stimulus (de Vries et al., 2020). Auditory stimulation consisted of the presentation of bilateral white noise bursts at 65 dB presented 30 cm from the mouse’s ears, with timing according to a uniform distribution (minimum interval of 3 s and maximum interval of 5 s). Prior to experiments, we chose a noise volume that was audible to the mice (Meyer et al., 2018). Mice were also imaged for a 6 min period in the dark, without auditory stimulation. The same stimulus paradigms were used at each time point before and after enucleation, dark exposure, auditory deprivation, or sham-deprivation.
Spine signal extraction
Calcium responses from individual spines were isolated from global dendritic signals using a subtraction procedure described previously (Chen et al., 2013b; Iacaruso et al., 2017; Wilson et al., 2016). To measure spine signals, circular ROIs were first drawn over individual dendritic spines to measure spine fluorescence and compute ΔF/F0_spine. We then drew circular ROIs of the same size as the spine ROI around the adjacent parent dendrite to the spine of interest in order to calculate the local dendritic signal, ΔF/F0_dendrite. Plotting ΔF/F0_spine against ΔF/F0_dendrite reveals two components of spine signals, a dendrite-related component and a spine-specific component. The dendrite-related component was removed from the spine signals by subtracting a scaled version of the dendritic signal, ΔF/F0_spine_specific = ΔF/F0_spine − α ΔF/F0_dendrite, where α equals the slope of a robust regression and was determined using the MATLAB function ‘robustfit’ of ΔF/F0_spine versus ΔF/F0_dendrite. To track the activity of the same dendritic spines over many days, individual single time series stacks were first full-frame registered using the motion correction software ‘Moco’ (Dubbs et al., 2016). Stacks were then visually inspected for movement in the z-axis and discarded if a stable dendrite was not evident. Registered stacks were then concatenated across multiple imaging sessions and a running average (100 frames) was generated. The running average stack was then horizontally stitched to the raw stack and used as a template to register the raw data across multiple imaging sessions again using ‘Moco’ and ‘Manual landmark selection’ image registration software (ImageJ). Stacks were then visually inspected for active dendrites, the area around the active dendrite was cropped and this sub-section of the original stack was again motion corrected using ‘Moco’. Dendritic sections were excluded if there was evidence of crossing axons or dendrites. Calcium responses from individual spines were analyzed using peak detection to determine the onset of a spine response greater than a 15% ΔF/F0 threshold above baseline (threshold calculated as 3.5× RMS of spine response traces). Spine response frequency was calculated as the number of spine response peaks above threshold per second for the entire imaging session. Spine amplitude was calculated as the average area under the spine response peak which was above threshold, measured across the entire imaging session, except in RSC experiments (Figure 4, Figure 4—figure supplement 1) where auditory and visual spine response amplitude was measured during auditory and visual stimulation, respectively (or as noted in figure legends). Spine activity integral was the summed area under the curve of all spine responses during the entire imaging session. All data were analyzed blind to time and experimental condition by two independent analysts and differences of more than 10% in responses were investigated. Spines were considered active during a time point if they had at least one response above threshold. Spines were considered inactive during a time point if they had no responses above threshold. Persistent spines were active at all time points. All included spines were active during at least one time point. The orientation selectivity index was calculated as in previous work for cellular responses (Barnes et al., 2015; Ko et al., 2011). Global dendritic signals were measured for each spine in the dendritic area adjacent to that spine. This approach avoids the ambiguity associated with determining whether or not dendritic branches are contiguous when they enter and exit the imaging depth plane.
Functional spine classification
To define functional properties of spines in visual cortex, we first classified spines as visual or not based on their responses to visual gratings and a sparse noise stimulus. Spines were determined to be visually responsive if they responded to either set of visual stimuli. First, to determine if a spine responds to the sparse noise stimulus, we used analyses as described previously (de Vries et al., 2020). Briefly, for each frame of the stimulus, we calculated the ΔF/F0 for 0.47 s after the frame (spanning 7 frames) where F0 is the response of the spine in the preceding 1.0 s (15 frames). As had been previously described, we calculated the derivative of the ΔF/F0 signal immediately after the stimulus presentation and then again 100 ms later and fit a two-dimensional Gaussian-noise distribution, where outliers outside of four standard deviations were considered responsive trials (de Vries et al., 2020).
We next determined if spines were responsive to the visual gratings (Barnes et al., 2015; Ko et al., 2011). We first computed the correlation of the spine calcium fluorescence signal with a binarized visual stimulation signal that corresponds to the timing of the stimulus. In cases where there was a significant positive correlation with this signal, we then used a false positive threshold analysis to determine the extent to which individual spines had responses that were time-locked to the visual stimulus by chance. To set a threshold for visual responsivity, we used epochs of spine ΔF/F0 signals collected when the mouse was stationary in the dark and a dummy binary visual stimulation signal identical to that used to detect visual spine correlations. Time-locked responses to the stimulus, occurring within 500 ms of the onset of the stimulus (Barnes et al., 2015; Ko et al., 2011), were measured in the dark with respect to the dummy visual signal for each spine to determine the false positive rate (mean population false positive rate = 5.5 ± 0.1%). These values were then compared to the percentage of actual visual responses measured during grating presentations. All of the spines (100%) that were identified as visually responsive by our initial correlation analysis had a greater number of stimulus-locked visual responses than the false positive threshold. Because we found few spines that did not respond to both visual stimuli, we only used gratings in later experiments, as the sparse noise stimulus takes approximately 1 hr to present.
To classify network-correlated spines, we calculated the correlation coefficient between individual spine ΔF/F0 signals and a network average ΔF/F0 signal comprising of the average of all spine signals during all conditions in the imaged cortical region, excluding the spine of interest (based on Okun et al., 2015). Spine ΔF/F0 signals were determined to be correlated with the network signal if the correlation value was positive and significant (p<0.05). Spines whose activity had positive and significant correlations with the network activity, but not the visual stimulus, were classified as network-correlated spines. Spines were classified as visually responsive using the criteria above, even though their activity also had significant correlations with the network activity (all visually responsive spines also had positive and significant network correlations). Spines whose activity had no significant correlations with either network or visual signals were labelled unclassified.
Using the same visual stimulation threshold criteria mentioned above, 99% of the spines that were identified as network-correlated, but not visually responsive, had fewer stimulus-locked visual responses than our false positive threshold, and non-significant visual correlations. The 1% of identified network-correlated spines (10 spines) with stimulus-locked responses above the false positive threshold were excluded from further analysis. We repeated this entire process in RSC, but also included a binary signal corresponding to the presentation of auditory stimuli. Unclassified spines in both brain regions had no significant correlation with the external stimuli or network signal. Few unclassified spines had significant correlations with either whisker movement (3.2%) or pupil position or diameter (1.4%).
To test for spatial clustering of dendritic spine functional types, we calculated the probability of spines ‘n’ positions away from a spine0 matching in functional property (adapted from a previous analysis; Barnes et al., 2017). The average probability of a functional match was calculated for spines 1,2,3,…,n spines away from spine0, where a match was given a value of 1 and a non-match a value of 0. We repeated this measure for each spine on a branch serving as spine0. We then randomly shuffled spine positions, repeated our analysis to generate a shuffled condition for each branch and compared the shuffled distribution to the experimentally observed distribution.
For within spine comparisons of normalized frequency and amplitude (Figure 1—figure supplement 1F, Figure 3G–I), we compared frequency at 12 hr and amplitude at 48 hr, since a decrease in synaptic frequency preceded an increase in the strength in past work (Lee et al., 2010).
Statistics
Statistical analyses were performed in either MATLAB or SigmaPlot. Data were tested for equal variance and normality (Shapiro-Wilk test) and then comparisons were made using parametric or non-parametric tests, as appropriate: Student’s t-test, paired Student’s t-test, chi-square test, one-way ANOVA with Holm-Sidak post hoc test, repeated measures ANOVA with Holm-Sidak post hoc test or a two-way ANOVA with Holm-Sidak post hoc test. Statistical tests were two-sided. Correlation coefficients were calculated with a Pearson’s correlation coefficient or a Spearman’s rank correlation. A power analysis was performed to ensure we used a sufficient sample size. Specific statistical tests used for all figures along with the number of samples can be found in the source data files.
Data and code availability
The pre-processed raw data can be accessed at https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.7399601. Data that has not been pre-processed is available upon request to any interested party, due to size constraints, by emailing georg.keller@fmi.ch, who will provide temporary transfer access for downloading the data. No proposal is required to access the data and there are no restrictions on who can access the data. Software for controlling the two-photon microscope and pre-processing of the calcium imaging data is available on https://sourceforge.net/projects/iris-scanning/.
Data availability
The pre-processed raw data can be accessed at https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.7399601. Data that have not been pre-processed are available upon request to any interested party, due to size constraints, by emailing georg.keller@fmi.ch, who will provide temporary transfer access for downloading the data. No proposal is required to access the data and there are not restrictions on who can access the data. Software for controlling the two-photon microscope and pre-processing of the calcium imaging data is available on https://sourceforge.net/projects/iris-scanning/ (copies archived at swh:1:rev:1f1b5616aaaf6cf862d2f4b467b65b479a2b0416, swh:1:rev:48e0b534668ef3ecb7c2580c00cdbcf650d69043, and swh:1:rev:d3b0d1a0482b5b929621f7e6e99c0e9ff2eee11f).
-
ZenodoHomeostatic regulation through strengthening of neuronal network correlated synaptic inputs.https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.7399601
References
-
The single dendritic branch as a fundamental functional unit in the nervous systemCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology 20:494–502.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2010.07.009
-
The neural basis of escape behavior in vertebratesAnnual Review of Neuroscience 43:417–439.https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-100219-122527
-
Critical periods for experience-dependent synaptic scaling in visual cortexNature Neuroscience 5:783–789.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn878
-
Moco: fast motion correction for calcium imagingFront Neuroinform 10:6.https://doi.org/10.3389/fninf.2016.00006
-
Experience-dependent spatial expectations in mouse visual cortexNature Neuroscience 19:1658–1664.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4385
-
Synaptic scaling requires the glur2 subunit of the AMPA receptorThe Journal of Neuroscience 29:6479–6489.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3753-08.2009
-
Multiple shared mechanisms for homeostatic plasticity in rodent somatosensory and visual cortexPhilosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences 372:20160157.https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2016.0157
-
Learning binds new inputs into functional synaptic clusters via spinogenesisNature Neuroscience 25:726–737.https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-022-01086-6
-
Critical period plasticity in local cortical circuitsNature Reviews. Neuroscience 6:877–888.https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1787
-
Learning enhances sensory processing in mouse V1 before improving behaviorThe Journal of Neuroscience 37:6460–6474.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3485-16.2017
-
Integrating hebbian and homeostatic plasticity: the current state of the field and future research directionsPhilosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences 372:20160158.https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2016.0158
-
Contextual signals in visual cortexCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology 52:131–138.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2018.05.003
-
Mechanisms of homeostatic synaptic plasticity in vivoFrontiers in Cellular Neuroscience 13:520.https://doi.org/10.3389/fncel.2019.00520
-
Investigating learning-related neural circuitry with chronic in vivo optical imagingBrain Structure & Function 225:467–480.https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-019-02001-9
-
Critical-Period plasticity in the visual cortexAnnual Review of Neuroscience 35:309–330.https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-113813
-
An adaptive role of TNFα in the regulation of striatal synapsesThe Journal of Neuroscience 34:6146–6155.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3481-13.2014
-
Thalamic activity that drives visual cortical plasticityNature Neuroscience 12:390–392.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2284
-
Formation and maintenance of neuronal assemblies through synaptic plasticityNature Communications 5:5319.https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms6319
-
Action and learning shape the activity of neuronal circuits in the visual cortexCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology 52:88–97.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2018.04.020
-
Sensory neural codes using multiplexed temporal scalesTrends in Neurosciences 33:111–120.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2009.12.001
-
Thalamic nuclei convey diverse contextual information to layer 1 of visual cortexNature Neuroscience 19:299–307.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4197
-
Synaptic scaling mediated by glial TNF-alphaNature 440:1054–1059.https://doi.org/10.1038/nature04671
-
Synaptic scaling in combination with many generic plasticity mechanisms stabilizes circuit connectivityFrontiers in Computational Neuroscience 5:47.https://doi.org/10.3389/fncom.2011.00047
-
Too many cooks? intrinsic and synaptic homeostatic mechanisms in cortical circuit refinementAnnual Review of Neuroscience 34:89–103.https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-060909-153238
-
The dialectic of hebb and homeostasisPhilos Trans R Soc Lond, B, Biol Sci 372:258.https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2016.0258
-
What does the retrosplenial cortex do?Nature Reviews. Neuroscience 10:792–802.https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2733
-
Homeostatic synaptic plasticity: from single synapses to neural circuitsCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology 22:516–521.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2011.09.006
-
Orientation selectivity and the functional clustering of synaptic inputs in primary visual cortexNature Neuroscience 19:1003–1009.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4323
-
Changes in properties of auditory nerve synapses following conductive hearing lossThe Journal of Neuroscience 37:323–332.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0523-16.2016
Decision letter
-
Yukiko GodaReviewing Editor; Okinawa Institute of Science and Technology, Japan
-
Lu ChenSenior Editor; Stanford University, United States
-
Yukiko GodaReviewer; Okinawa Institute of Science and Technology, Japan
-
Keith B HengenReviewer; Washington University, United States
Our editorial process produces two outputs: (i) public reviews designed to be posted alongside the preprint for the benefit of readers; (ii) feedback on the manuscript for the authors, including requests for revisions, shown below. We also include an acceptance summary that explains what the editors found interesting or important about the work.
Decision letter after peer review:
Thank you for submitting your article "Homeostatic regulation through the strengthening of neuronal network-correlated synaptic inputs" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by 3 peer reviewers, and the evaluation has been overseen by a Reviewing Editor and Lu Chen as the Senior Editor. The following individuals involved in the review of your submission have agreed to reveal their identity: Yukiko Goda (Reviewer #1); Keith B Hengen (Reviewer #3).
The reviewers have discussed their reviews with one another, and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this to help you prepare a revised submission.
Essential revisions:
All three reviewers have found the work to be important and of considerable general interest. Please carefully address individual points raised by the three reviewers, which can be dealt with by text edits or minor figure modifications. In particular, the fact that the present study used adult animals and that homeostatic plasticity in the adult brain could follow different rules from juvenile animals should be stressed.
Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):
1) Figure 2C – It would be important to include also a histogram of % visual stimuli eliciting time-locked responses for all deprived spines following deprivation as a measure of how well the system is responding to deprivation. In other words, is there an effect of enucleation on the reliability with which visual stimuli trigger responses?
2) Figure 2 – Related to the point above, only panel B appears to show results pertaining to post-enucleation, which is quite confusing. One should reconsider the panel order, perhaps moving panels B and C with both plots including the respective control and deprived data.
3) Figure 3C – Some network spines do show loss of activity upon sensory deprivation. Given that the proportion of network spines is 65% compared to 24% of visual spines, to begin with, one would expect that increase in the proportion of inactive spines by 10% for network spines (Figure 3F) represents a larger total number of spines than 20% increase for visual spines (Figure 3D). Could the increase in normalized amplitude shown by network spines be a consequence of the reduced activity of some of the network spines and secondary to the direct effects of loss of visual inputs? This point requires consideration/discussion. It would be informative to include a blind assessment of relative proportions of visual, network, and unclassified spines following enucleation and a comparison of the numbers to those of baseline in a graphical representation.
Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):
1. Methods: "Intrinsic signal imaging before enucleation was used to localize the visual cortex…" Were the recorded spines/dendrites in binocular or monocular V1? Baseline visual stimulation (gratings) was bilateral? It seems surprising that each spine's ocular dominance was not determined at baseline – please explain. I would expect different reactions to enucleation in a neuron that was mainly driven by the missing eye vs the intact eye.
2. In the example trace Figure S1B, please label all spine events that were counted for the frequency analysis (Figure S1E).
3. Figure 1E Are these spine calcium transients' responses to a visual stimulus (aligned to stimulation time) or are they 'spontaneous' events aligned by peak or max. slope? If the latter, please don't call them responses but find a more neutral term (e.g. EPSCaTs).
4. Figure 2B: "When we examined the functional strengthening of the maintained visually responsive inputs, we found an inverse relationship, such that the spines with the lowest visual responsiveness prior to deprivation underwent the strongest functional strengthening after deprivation." Why is there a data point at zero visual responses, wouldn't that be an unclassified or network spine?
5. Figure 3 G-I: It is not well explained why you show this lack of correlation and what can be concluded from it. To me, frequency changes point to presynaptic plasticity (either release at the observed synapse or spike frequency of the presynaptic neuron), and amplitude changes to a postsynaptic mechanism. It seems you have both, but rarely in the same spines. I don't like the binning – why not show a cloud of all spines and the correlation coefficient? Try drawing a conclusion from these plots.
6. Figure 4 B: Please show raw traces under conditions of TNF α block. Was the amplitude of Ca transients at baseline affected by the inhibitor? It is my understanding that you use the blocker mainly as a diagnostic criterion for "true homeostatic plasticity", as you draw no further conclusions (e.g. about the potential involvement of glia cells).
7. Figure 4J: How many branches were analyzed? (N missing in Table S4).
8. L.337: "Further work is required to better understand the key neural circuit and molecular features that allow TNF-α to selectively modulate homeostatic adaption and even more specifically, the network correlated spines." You see the enhanced activity of network-correlated spines, but homeostatic plasticity could well happen elsewhere, e.g. in L4. It cannot be concluded that TNF-α selectively acts on the spines you happen to look at (a.k.a. streetlight effect). It would be good to spell out that the locus of homeostatic plasticity could be upstream of the synapses under scrutiny (unless you have good arguments that plasticity is local).
Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):
1. Do the authors have the capacity to separate V1m and V1b in their imaging sessions? The responses to monocular deprivation (via enucleation) in V1m should be purely homeostatic, while the effect in V1b is most likely a combination of homeostatic and Hebbian. The TNF α experiments suggest that a significant component of the data is homeostatic (assuming all TNF α plasticity is homeostatic), but it would be powerful to show that the key effects are evident in V1m. This is certainly not necessary for the paper, but if it were possible, it would be quite nice to see and would complement the TNF experiments.
2. Line 64: change "Following deprivation" to "Following sensory deprivation". At this point, there's been no definition of deprivation so readers from outside the field might be confused.
3. Figure 4: the yellow and gray bars (e.g., 4B) are very confusing – my first impression was that they indicated normal vision and 48h dark deprivation. Then, in F through I, I read the figure as if animals were simultaneously deprived of vision and auditory input (note that there is both a gray bar and plugged ears), but the legend says, "timeline for dark exposure (B) OR auditory ear plug (F)", which seems to suggest that these were not performed concurrently.
4. The authors cite Barnes 2017 and Bridi 2018 as the basis of the hypothesis that subsets of spines are subject to compensatory changes. Barnes 2017 clearly shows this, as well as being in adult animals. Bridi 2018 is based on CP animals and, as far as I can tell, doesn't make any claims about synaptic specificity: they measured extracellular spikes and mEPSCs (in whole cell patch), which cannot resolve synaptic subsets. Their key finding (to my eye) is a novel mechanism for compensatory plasticity, not synaptic specificity:
"In sum, our manipulations of visual input combined with manipulations of spontaneous neuronal activity reveal that (i) changes in overall neuronal firing rate alone are not sufficient to mediate the lowered modification threshold for LTP/LTD nor the increased mEPSC amplitude observed after visual deprivation, (ii) changes in mEPSC amplitude induced by visual deprivation reflect Hebbian potentiation of excitatory synapses enabled by the reduced synaptic modification threshold, and (iii) changes in mEPSC amplitude induced by extreme reductions in neuronal activity reflect NMDAR-independent synaptic scaling."
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.81958.sa1Author response
Essential revisions:
All three reviewers have found the work to be important and of considerable general interest. Please carefully address individual points raised by the three reviewers, which can be dealt with by text edits or minor figure modifications. In particular, the fact that the present study used adult animals and that homeostatic plasticity in the adult brain could follow different rules from juvenile animals should be stressed.
We thank the reviewers for their positive and helpful comments. We agree that the age of the animals is a key consideration for our results and we have changed the manuscript to emphasize this point. We have first made sure that the age of the animals is clear throughout the manuscript (abstract, introduction, results, and discussion). We have also highlighted the specifics of adult plasticity, and its relation to juvenile plasticity, in the introduction and discussion to provide context for our findings, as well as how our work is complementary with previous studies in adult animals.
We have addressed the comments of the individual reviewers below.
Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):
1) Figure 2C – It would be important to include also a histogram of % visual stimuli eliciting time-locked responses for all deprived spines following deprivation as a measure of how well the system is responding to deprivation. In other words, is there an effect of enucleation on the reliability with which visual stimuli trigger responses?
We have double checked and there are no visually-evoked responses following deprivation. It is important to clarify that the visual cortex experiments in our study were done in monocular visual cortex, which we have now specified throughout the manuscript. Because all visual spines also have significant network signals (Figure 2), the spines that were identified as visually responsive during baseline (prior to sensory deprivation), only have network responses following visual deprivation, since there is no longer visual input from the contralateral eye. None of the spines have visually-evoked responses above chance after deprivation. We have specified this in the text now. Lines:
“It is important to note that following sensory deprivation, we no longer observed responses to visual stimuli in the visually responsive spines, since the feedforward input had been permanently removed. The responses of the previously categorized visually responsive spines consisted of network responses following deprivation.”
2) Figure 2 – Related to the point above, only panel B appears to show results pertaining to post-enucleation, which is quite confusing. One should reconsider the panel order, perhaps moving panels B and C with both plots including the respective control and deprived data.
We have made this adjustment and switched the order of the panels 2B and 2C, which we agree makes the manuscript easier to follow.
3) Figure 3C – Some network spines do show loss of activity upon sensory deprivation. Given that the proportion of network spines is 65% compared to 24% of visual spines, to begin with, one would expect that increase in the proportion of inactive spines by 10% for network spines (Figure 3F) represents a larger total number of spines than 20% increase for visual spines (Figure 3D). Could the increase in normalized amplitude shown by network spines be a consequence of the reduced activity of some of the network spines and secondary to the direct effects of loss of visual inputs? This point requires consideration/discussion. It would be informative to include a blind assessment of relative proportions of visual, network, and unclassified spines following enucleation and a comparison of the numbers to those of baseline in a graphical representation.
We thank the reviewer for raising this important point. We have adjusted the text to reflect this point that while the relative fraction of visual spines that become inactive is higher than the relative fraction of inactive network spines, the absolute number of network spines that become inactive is larger. Lines:
“While a larger fraction of visually responsive spines become inactive, the absolute number of network-correlated spines that became inactive was higher, since network-correlated spines reflect a larger proportion of the total spine population. These results suggest that the increase in spine loss previously reported following enucleation (Barnes et al., 2017) may include both spines that were visually responsive and network-correlated prior to deprivation (Figure 3D-F).”
Secondly, the increase in the number of inactive network spines and subsequent strengthening only occurs in deprived animals, so even if the effect is secondary to visual deprivation, our data indicate that it is triggered by deprivation. We consistently see that there are not systematic changes in density/size/functional response strength of spines without the visual deprivation in our previous work (Barnes et al., 2017), as well as in this study.
As for the proportion of spines in each category following enucleation, the spines fall into only two categories, network and unclassified, since we do not observe visually-evoked responses following enucleation, as we are recording in monocular visual cortex. We do not see a difference in the unclassified spines following deprivation (0% of unclassified spines (categorized prior to deprivation) correlate with the network signal after deprivation), so there is no change in categorization, other than the visual spines would now be categorized as network-correlated.
Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):
1. Methods: "Intrinsic signal imaging before enucleation was used to localize the visual cortex…" Were the recorded spines/dendrites in binocular or monocular V1? Baseline visual stimulation (gratings) was bilateral? It seems surprising that each spine's ocular dominance was not determined at baseline – please explain. I would expect different reactions to enucleation in a neuron that was mainly driven by the missing eye vs the intact eye.
We apologize for the confusion on this point and have now clarified in the manuscript that the visual cortex experiments in the study were done in monocular visual cortex. All visual stimulation was bilateral, which we have now clarified in the methods. Given that we were measuring in monocular visual cortex, we did not measure ocular dominance. Furthermore, given that we see no visually-evoked responses to bilateral visual stimulation following sensory deprivation, we think that it is unlikely that spines are responding to inputs from the intact ipsilateral eye, consistent with expectations in monocular visual cortex.
2. In the example trace Figure S1B, please label all spine events that were counted for the frequency analysis (Figure S1E).
We thank the reviewer for this helpful suggestion and have denoted events that were included in our analysis with an asterisk above the trace.
3. Figure 1E Are these spine calcium transients' responses to a visual stimulus (aligned to stimulation time) or are they 'spontaneous' events aligned by peak or max. slope? If the latter, please don't call them responses but find a more neutral term (e.g. EPSCaTs).
This is a great point and we have changed the figure to refer to them as spine events. These examples include all spine events, which were aligned by the peak. We have clarified this in the figure legend.
4. Figure 2B: "When we examined the functional strengthening of the maintained visually-responsive inputs, we found an inverse relationship, such that the spines with the lowest visual responsiveness prior to deprivation underwent the strongest functional strengthening after deprivation." Why is there a data point at zero visual responses, wouldn't that be an unclassified or network spine?
We thank the reviewer for catching this mistake. This figure includes all spines, not just the visually responsive ones, and we have changed this text accordingly.
5. Figure 3 G-I: It is not well explained why you show this lack of correlation and what can be concluded from it. To me, frequency changes point to presynaptic plasticity (either release at the observed synapse or spike frequency of the presynaptic neuron), and amplitude changes to a postsynaptic mechanism. It seems you have both, but rarely in the same spines. I don't like the binning – why not show a cloud of all spines and the correlation coefficient? Try drawing a conclusion from these plots.
We have included this comparison, as previous work has indicated that frequency and amplitude could be modulated within a single synapse (Lee et al., 2010). In other words, the frequency of responses in a spine goes down (likely presynaptically mediated) and then the response amplitude of that spine increases. The correlation we are measuring here tests that directly and finds that decreases in frequency in an individual spine is unlikely to be driving the increases in amplitude. We have included this conclusion in the manuscript now. Lines:
“Previous work has shown that spine specific decreases in response frequency drive synapse-specific increases in response amplitude (Lee et al., 2010). We therefore examined if frequency changes were indicative of amplitude changes in individual spines. We observed no correlation between changes in frequency and amplitude within individual spines (Figure 1 – —figure supplement 1F), indicating that the changes in frequency are not likely to be driving the changes in amplitude within a single spine.”
6. Figure 4 B: Please show raw traces under conditions of TNF α block. Was the amplitude of Ca transients at baseline affected by the inhibitor? It is my understanding that you use the blocker mainly as a diagnostic criterion for "true homeostatic plasticity", as you draw no further conclusions (e.g. about the potential involvement of glia cells).
We have added these traces to Figure 4B. The baseline amplitude of Ca transients is not altered by the TNF-α inhibitor (TNF-α 79.2 ± 4.2 vs Control 81.8 ± 3.1 % ∆F/F, p>0.5, t-test). As the reviewer suggests, we have used this experiment to determine whether or not our effect is dependent on TNF-α, which is often used as an indicator of synaptic scaling. It is still unclear exactly how TNF-α interacts with synaptic strengthening, but we have included the known role of glia in this interaction in the discussion.
7. Figure 4J: How many branches were analyzed? (N missing in Table S4).
In order to measure the global dendritic signals, we measured the dendrite signal adjacent to the dendritic spine. Thus, the number of spine and dendrite measurements are the same. This particular approach is critical to avoid any ambiguity that occurs in identifying contiguous dendritic branches when a dendrite exits and re-enters the imaging plane. We have clarified this point in the methods. Lines:
“Global dendritic signals were measured for each spine in the dendritic area adjacent to that spine. This approach avoids the ambiguity associated with determining whether or not dendritic branches are contiguous when they enter and exit the imaging depth plane.”
8. L.337: "Further work is required to better understand the key neural circuit and molecular features that allow TNF-α to selectively modulate homeostatic adaption and even more specifically, the network correlated spines." You see the enhanced activity of network-correlated spines, but homeostatic plasticity could well happen elsewhere, e.g. in L4. It cannot be concluded that TNF-α selectively acts on the spines you happen to look at (a.k.a. streetlight effect). It would be good to spell out that the locus of homeostatic plasticity could be upstream of the synapses under scrutiny (unless you have good arguments that plasticity is local).
We agree with the reviewer and have adjusted the language about TNF-α and added text to highlight that the locus of plasticity could be outside of layer 5. Lines:
“Finally, it is important to note that while we made these measurements in layer 5 pyramidal cells, the homeostatic changes mediated by TNF-α could occur outside of layer 5, including changes to upstream inputs or changes to the presynaptic responses, either through changes in presynaptic release (Vitureira et al., 2012) or through a change in activity patterns of the presynaptic cell (e.g., bursts compared to single spikes) (Linden et al., 2009). Given that both sensory responsive spines (which do not strengthen) and network-correlated spines (which do strengthen) exhibit only network-correlated responses after sensory deprivation, changes in presynaptic activity patterns of network signals would require targeted changes to the network-corelated spines, but not the previously sensory responsive spines. Further research is necessary to identify the role of presynaptic plasticity in homeostatic synaptic plasticity in vivo.”
Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):
1. Do the authors have the capacity to separate V1m and V1b in their imaging sessions? The responses to monocular deprivation (via enucleation) in V1m should be purely homeostatic, while the effect in V1b is most likely a combination of homeostatic and Hebbian. The TNF α experiments suggest that a significant component of the data is homeostatic (assuming all TNF α plasticity is homeostatic), but it would be powerful to show that the key effects are evident in V1m. This is certainly not necessary for the paper, but if it were possible, it would be quite nice to see and would complement the TNF experiments.
We again apologize for this confusion. We have now clarified throughout the manuscript that the visual cortex experiments in our study were conducted in monocular visual cortex and so all of the TNF-α effects in visual cortex are in monocular visual cortex.
2. Line 64: change "Following deprivation" to "Following sensory deprivation". At this point, there's been no definition of deprivation so readers from outside the field might be confused.
We have changed this as suggested.
3. Figure 4: the yellow and gray bars (e.g., 4B) are very confusing – my first impression was that they indicated normal vision and 48h dark deprivation. Then, in F through I, I read the figure as if animals were simultaneously deprived of vision and auditory input (note that there is both a gray bar and plugged ears), but the legend says, "timeline for dark exposure (B) OR auditory ear plug (F)", which seems to suggest that these were not performed concurrently.
We thank the reviewer for pointing out this confusing issue. The dark exposure and auditory ear plug were not performed concurrently, but are separate sets of experiments. We have changed Figure 4 and Figure 4 – —figure supplement 1 so that the background colors are different for the two experiments. We have also clarified in the text that these are separate experiments.
4. The authors cite Barnes 2017 and Bridi 2018 as the basis of the hypothesis that subsets of spines are subject to compensatory changes. Barnes 2017 clearly shows this, as well as being in adult animals. Bridi 2018 is based on CP animals and, as far as I can tell, doesn't make any claims about synaptic specificity: they measured extracellular spikes and mEPSCs (in whole cell patch), which cannot resolve synaptic subsets. Their key finding (to my eye) is a novel mechanism for compensatory plasticity, not synaptic specificity:
"In sum, our manipulations of visual input combined with manipulations of spontaneous neuronal activity reveal that (i) changes in overall neuronal firing rate alone are not sufficient to mediate the lowered modification threshold for LTP/LTD nor the increased mEPSC amplitude observed after visual deprivation, (ii) changes in mEPSC amplitude induced by visual deprivation reflect Hebbian potentiation of excitatory synapses enabled by the reduced synaptic modification threshold, and (iii) changes in mEPSC amplitude induced by extreme reductions in neuronal activity reflect NMDAR-independent synaptic scaling."
We thank the reviewer for this helpful point and we have taken this point out of the manuscript.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.81958.sa2Article and author information
Author details
Funding
Wellcome Trust (212264/Z/18/Z)
- Tara Keck
European Research Council (homeostasis_in_vivo)
- Tara Keck
- Georg B Keller
Royal Society (Wolfson Research Award)
- Tara Keck
Novartis Stiftung für Medizinisch-Biologische Forschung
- Georg B Keller
Edmond J. Safra Philanthropic Foundation
- Samuel J Barnes
Imperial College London
- Samuel J Barnes
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication. For the purpose of Open Access, the authors have applied a CC BY public copyright license to any Author Accepted Manuscript version arising from this submission.
Acknowledgements
We thank Claire Cheetham, Claudia Clopath, Matthias Heindorf, Nicholas A Lesica, and Tobias Rose for comments on the manuscript and helpful discussions. This work was supported by the Edmond J Safra Foundation (SJB), the European Research Council (TK (homeostasis_in_vivo), GBK), the Royal Society (TK), the Wellcome Trust (TK, 212264/Z/18/Z), the Novartis Research Foundation (GBK), the Swiss National Science Foundation (GBK), and the UK Dementia Research Institute at Imperial College London (SJB), which receives its funding from DRI Ltd, funded by the UK Medical Research Council, Alzheimer’s Society, and Alzheimer’s Research UK. XPRo1595 was provided by Xencor, Inc.
Ethics
Experiments were conducted in strict accordance with the United Kingdom Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act 1986, and were approved by the UCL Animal Welfare and Ethical Review Body (AWERB) and by the Veterinary Department of the Canton of Basel-Stadt, Switzerland.
Senior Editor
- Lu Chen, Stanford University, United States
Reviewing Editor
- Yukiko Goda, Okinawa Institute of Science and Technology, Japan
Reviewers
- Yukiko Goda, Okinawa Institute of Science and Technology, Japan
- Keith B Hengen, Washington University, United States
Publication history
- Received: July 18, 2022
- Preprint posted: August 5, 2022 (view preprint)
- Accepted: November 30, 2022
- Accepted Manuscript published: December 14, 2022 (version 1)
- Version of Record published: December 30, 2022 (version 2)
Copyright
© 2022, Barnes et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 986
- Page views
-
- 150
- Downloads
-
- 0
- Citations
Article citation count generated by polling the highest count across the following sources: Crossref, PubMed Central, Scopus.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Neuroscience
In value-based decision making, options are selected according to subjective values assigned by the individual to available goods and actions. Despite the importance of this faculty of the mind, the neural mechanisms of value assignments, and how choices are directed by them, remain obscure. To investigate this problem, we used a classic measure of utility maximization, the Generalized Axiom of Revealed Preference, to quantify internal consistency of food preferences in Caenorhabditis elegans, a nematode worm with a nervous system of only 302 neurons. Using a novel combination of microfluidics and electrophysiology, we found that C. elegans food choices fulfill the necessary and sufficient conditions for utility maximization, indicating that nematodes behave as if they maintain, and attempt to maximize, an underlying representation of subjective value. Food choices are well-fit by a utility function widely used to model human consumers. Moreover, as in many other animals, subjective values in C. elegans are learned, a process we find requires intact dopamine signaling. Differential responses of identified chemosensory neurons to foods with distinct growth potentials are amplified by prior consumption of these foods, suggesting that these neurons may be part of a value-assignment system. The demonstration of utility maximization in an organism with a very small nervous system sets a new lower bound on the computational requirements for utility maximization and offers the prospect of an essentially complete explanation of value-based decision making at single neuron resolution in this organism.
-
- Neuroscience
While there is a wealth of knowledge about core object recognition—our ability to recognize clear, high-contrast object images—how the brain accomplishes object recognition tasks under increased uncertainty remains poorly understood. We investigated the spatiotemporal neural dynamics underlying object recognition under increased uncertainty by combining MEG and 7 Tesla (7T) fMRI in humans during a threshold-level object recognition task. We observed an early, parallel rise of recognition-related signals across ventral visual and frontoparietal regions that preceded the emergence of category-related information. Recognition-related signals in ventral visual regions were best explained by a two-state representational format whereby brain activity bifurcated for recognized and unrecognized images. By contrast, recognition-related signals in frontoparietal regions exhibited a reduced representational space for recognized images, yet with sharper category information. These results provide a spatiotemporally resolved view of neural activity supporting object recognition under uncertainty, revealing a pattern distinct from that underlying core object recognition.