Group Size: The balance of the sexes
Two is company, three is a crowd – but not for everyone. Animal group sizes vary considerably both across and within species. Explaining why this variation exists is a central question in sociobiology (Clutton-Brock, 2021; Rubenstein and Abbot, 2017). Social companions provide real perks; for instance, cooperative breeders share the costs of parental care. But individuals generally have their own interests at heart, and so the risk of competition is never far away. This is especially true for animals that are cooperative breeders but form breeding groups in which members are not related, such as ostriches (Struthio camelus). In such groups, individuals only benefit from fitness through their own offspring, so the size of these groups is the result of a balancing act between two major social forces: cooperation and competition.
In the wild, the sizes of ostrich breeding groups living under similar ecological conditions vary widely, from single pairs to groups of up to 20 individuals. Moreover, these groups often differ in their ratio of males to females. This heterogeneity in group composition may be a clue to understanding variations in breeding group size that are independent of ecology: the benefits of cooperation and costs of competition are not always shared equally between the members of a group (Jolles et al., 2020; Ward and Webster, 2016), leading to different social preferences. In other words, the optimal group size and sex ratio for reproductive success may be different for male and female ostriches. Now, in eLife, Charlie Cornwallis and colleagues from Lund University, the University of Stellenbosch and the Directorate of Animal Sciences – with Julian Melgar as first author – report on how manipulating both group size and sex composition of ostrich breeding groups can be used to test this prediction (Melgar et al., 2022).
Melgar et al. took ostriches from a breeding farm in South Africa and formed breeding groups of different sizes and compositions in separate enclosures. Each group contained 1, 3, 4, or 6 females, and 1 or 3 males, comparable to the natural variation in groups in the area. Melgar et al. also manipulated one of the likely benefits of cooperative breeding in ostriches: shared incubation. By studying optimal group size – defined as the group size at which an individual produces the most chicks – and how it changes with the presence or absence of shared incubation, it was possible to test whether the benefits of cooperative incubation differed between males and females.
Each of the breeding groups underwent two conditions. In one condition, recently laid eggs were removed and placed in incubators for hatching; in the other, the eggs were left for the ostriches to incubate. For males, both treatments had the same result: the number of chicks per male always increased with the number of females in the group, and always decreased when the number of males increased (Figure 1A). These results indicate that, for males, optimal group size for reproductive success depends more on the costs of competition with other males than on the benefits of shared incubation. For females, however, the results looked different. When the eggs were removed, the size and composition of the group did not have a clear effect on female reproductive success. However, when the eggs were incubated by the ostriches, female reproductive success depended on the number of males and other females in the group. This result indicates that incubation sharing is a relevant factor explaining optimal group size for females.

Ostrich group size and composition which maximizes reproductive success during incubation.
(A) In groups with a single male, both males (black) and females (brown or tan) do better (indicated by a 1 on the podium) with more females around. (B) In groups with three males, the males do better with many (six) females around (left). However, when there are three males, females do best when they are either one of many (six) females in the group (left) or when they are the only female (right); meaning that these two combinations share the number 1 position.
Interestingly, males had a clear optimal group size: more females were always better for reproductive success. In contrast, for females more females were only the best solution if there was a single male in the group (Figure 1A). In groups with three males, on the other hand, females did better when they were either the only female, or one of six (Figure 1B). The low performance of females in intermediate groups could be partly explained by sexual conflict. In intermediate groups with three males, females were interrupted more often while they were incubating, leading to eggs being broken following sexual harassments by ‘slacking’ males (i.e. males that spent less time incubating than females). For females, reproductive success is thus maximized across multiple group sizes and compositions due to the opportunity for females to share incubation costs and minimize sexual harassment.
Melgar et al. also measured how much time ostriches spent incubating eggs. Unsurprisingly, they found that the larger the group size, the larger the proportion of time eggs were incubated, leading to greater hatching success. More importantly, individuals could benefit from this increase in total incubation time without having to increase their own time spent on the task – demonstrating that, for ostriches, shared incubation is an important benefit of breeding in larger groups.
Natural variation in the social organization of cooperative breeders is abundant but causal demonstrations of mechanisms to explain this variation are rare (Balshine et al., 2001; Clutton-Brock, 2021; Rubenstein and Abbot, 2017; Shen et al., 2017). Using ostriches, a key model system for cooperative breeding (Bertram, 2014), Melgar et al. have taken a crucial step in understanding variation in the size of breeding groups. By experimentally manipulating group size and composition as well as a key benefit of cooperative breeding, they have revealed how sex differences in cooperation and competition can play an important role in explaining natural breeding group variation.
Of course, some questions remain to be answered. The optimal group sizes observed in the enclosures were generally larger than those observed in the wild, which could be explained by additional selection pressures that were excluded from these experiments. Future studies could improve on the design by allowing for natural variation in food availability and the presence of nest predators. Larger groups are more conspicuous and may therefore suffer more nest predation and suffer more under competition for food, creating a shift to smaller optimal group sizes (Krause and Ruxton, 2002). In addition, sex is unlikely the only trait to generate differences in optimal group size between individuals. Traits such as age, dominance, and personality are also thought to be significant (Jolles et al., 2020). Identifying the traits that causally influence optimal group size in cooperative breeders through carefully-designed experiments is an intriguing direction for future studies.
References
-
Correlates of group size in a cooperatively breeding cichlid fish (Neolamprologus pulcher)Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 50:134–140.https://doi.org/10.1007/s002650100343
-
BookThe Ostrich Communal Nesting SystemPrinceton University Press.https://doi.org/10.2307/j.ctt7ztm99
-
The role of individual heterogeneity in collective animal behaviourTrends in Ecology & Evolution 35:278–291.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2019.11.001
-
The ecology of cooperative breeding behaviourEcology Letters 20:708–720.https://doi.org/10.1111/ele.12774
Article and author information
Author details
Publication history
Copyright
© 2022, Kurvers and Snijders
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 455
- views
-
- 55
- downloads
-
- 0
- citations
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Ecology
- Evolutionary Biology
While host phenotypic manipulation by parasites is a widespread phenomenon, whether tumors, which can be likened to parasite entities, can also manipulate their hosts is not known. Theory predicts that this should nevertheless be the case, especially when tumors (neoplasms) are transmissible. We explored this hypothesis in a cnidarian Hydra model system, in which spontaneous tumors can occur in the lab, and lineages in which such neoplastic cells are vertically transmitted (through host budding) have been maintained for over 15 years. Remarkably, the hydras with long-term transmissible tumors show an unexpected increase in the number of their tentacles, allowing for the possibility that these neoplastic cells can manipulate the host. By experimentally transplanting healthy as well as neoplastic tissues derived from both recent and long-term transmissible tumors, we found that only the long-term transmissible tumors were able to trigger the growth of additional tentacles. Also, supernumerary tentacles, by permitting higher foraging efficiency for the host, were associated with an increased budding rate, thereby favoring the vertical transmission of tumors. To our knowledge, this is the first evidence that, like true parasites, transmissible tumors can evolve strategies to manipulate the phenotype of their host.
-
- Ecology
Environmental factors can influence ecological networks, but these effects are poorly understood in the realm of the phylogeny of host-parasitoid interactions. Especially, we lack a comprehensive understanding of the ways that biotic factors, including plant species richness, overall community phylogenetic and functional composition of consumers, and abiotic factors such as microclimate, determine host-parasitoid network structure and host-parasitoid community dynamics. To address this, we leveraged a 5-year dataset of trap-nesting bees and wasps and their parasitoids collected in a highly controlled, large-scale subtropical tree biodiversity experiment. We tested for effects of tree species richness, tree phylogenetic, and functional diversity, and species and phylogenetic composition on species and phylogenetic diversity of both host and parasitoid communities and the composition of their interaction networks. We show that multiple components of tree diversity and canopy cover impacted both, species and phylogenetic composition of hosts and parasitoids. Generally, phylogenetic associations between hosts and parasitoids reflected nonrandomly structured interactions between phylogenetic trees of hosts and parasitoids. Further, host-parasitoid network structure was influenced by tree species richness, tree phylogenetic diversity, and canopy cover. Our study indicates that the composition of higher trophic levels and corresponding interaction networks are determined by plant diversity and canopy cover, especially via trophic links in species-rich ecosystems.