Direct neural pathways convey distinct visual information to Drosophila mushroom bodies

  1. Katrin Vogt
  2. Yoshinori Aso
  3. Toshihide Hige
  4. Stephan Knapek
  5. Toshiharu Ichinose
  6. Anja B Friedrich
  7. Glenn C Turner
  8. Gerald M Rubin  Is a corresponding author
  9. Hiromu Tanimoto  Is a corresponding author
  1. Max-Planck Institut für Neurobiologie, Germany
  2. Tohoku University Graduate School of Life Sciences, Japan
  3. Janelia Research Campus, Howard Hughes Medical Institute, United States
  4. Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory, United States

Abstract

Previously, we demonstrated that visual and olfactory associative memories of Drosophila share mushroom body (MB) circuits (Vogt et al., 2014). Unlike for odor representation, the MB circuit for visual information has not been characterized. Here, we show that a small subset of MB Kenyon cells (KCs) selectively responds to visual but not olfactory stimulation. The dendrites of these atypical KCs form a ventral accessory calyx (vAC), distinct from the main calyx that receives olfactory input. We identified two types of visual projection neurons (VPNs) directly connecting the optic lobes and the vAC. Strikingly, these VPNs are differentially required for visual memories of color and brightness. The segregation of visual and olfactory domains in the MB allows independent processing of distinct sensory memories and may be a conserved form of sensory representations among insects.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.14009.001

Introduction

Rewarding or punitive stimuli modulate behavioral responses to sensory stimuli. In insects, such associative modulation takes place in the mushroom body (MB) (Heisenberg, 2003). In the fruit fly, the MB receives distinct dopaminergic inputs that signal reward and punishment, and this valence circuit in the MB is shared for associative memories of different modalities: olfaction, gustation, and vision (Masek et al., 2015Aso et al., 2012; Liu et al., 2012Perisse et al., 2013). The role of MB output during memory acquisition and testing is similar in visual and olfactory memories (Vogt et al., 2014). However, the required subsets of MB Kenyon cells (KCs) are not the same (Aso et al., 2014a; Vogt et al., 2014), raising the possibility that memory-relevant visual and olfactory information may be represented by different KC subsets in the MB.

Studies of the neuronal circuits underlying olfactory learning have benefitted from well-characterized neuronal pathways conveying olfactory information to the MB (Turner et al., 2008; Butcher et al., 2012). Beyond visual associative memory, the MB was shown to be important in various vision-guided behavioral tasks (Liu et al., 1999; Brembs, 2009; Zhang et al., 2007). Yet, how visual information is conveyed and represented in the Drosophila MB is totally unknown. MBs of hymenoptera receive visual afferents in their calyces (Ehmer and Gronenberg, 2002; Gronenberg and Hölldobler, 1999; Paulk and Gronenberg, 2008), while such direct visual input from the optic lobes to the MBs has never been observed in dipteran insects (Mu et al., 2012; Otsuna and Ito, 2006). Therefore, indirect, multisynaptic pathways have been proposed to convey the visual input to the Drosophila MB (Farris and Van Dyke, 2015; Tanaka et al., 2008). In this report, we demonstrate the existence of two visual projection neurons that directly connect the optic lobes to the MB.

Results

To understand the representation of visual memory in the MB, we blocked different subsets of KCs using split-GAL4 drivers and UAS-shits1 (Aso, 2014a) and behaviorally screened the flies for color discrimination memory using an aversive reinforcer (Figure 1C). Memory was consistently impaired when we used drivers labelling the γ-lobe neurons, confirming their importance for visual memory (Figure 1—figure supplement 1A) (Vogt et al., 2014). Strikingly, this screen further suggested a subset of the γ neurons (γd) to be specifically responsible (Figure 1, Figure 1—figure supplement 1A).

Figure 1 with 1 supplement see all
The γd KCs are required for visual memory.

(A) γd neurons labeled by MB607B-GAL4. (B) 3D reconstruction of γd neurons labeled by MB607B-GAL4 (purple) in the entire MB (yellow). Arrow indicates atypical dendritic protrusion of the γd neurons. Scale bars: 50 µm (A) and 20 µm (B). (C) Schematic diagram of color discrimination learning and test. (D) Average time courses of conditioned color avoidance in the test for flies with the blockade of the γd neurons with MB607B-GAL4 (red) and the parental controls (black and gray). (E) Pooled conditioned color avoidance. Blocking the γd neurons with MB607B-GAL4 impairs aversive color discrimination learning (one-way ANOVA, post-hoc pairwise comparison, p<0.05; n = 8–12). (F) The same Shits1 blockade of the γd neurons does not impair immediate aversive olfactory memory (one-way ANOVA, post-hoc pairwise comparison, p>0,05; n = 9–10). Throughout this study, bars and error bars display mean and SEM, respectively.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.14009.002

The γd neurons are embryonic born KCs consisting of ca. 75 cells in the adult brain (Butcher et al., 2012; Aso et al., 2009; Aso et al., 2014b). Two split-GAL4 drivers MB607B and MB419B showed strong expression in the γd neurons but had no detectable expression in other KCs (Figure 1A, Figure 1—figure supplement 1B). The blockade of the γd neurons using these lines severely impaired visual memory (Figure 1D,E, Figure 1—figure supplement 1C). In contrast, olfactory memory of these flies was not significantly affected (Figure 1F, Figure 1—figure supplement 1D). Given that both visual and olfactory memories were reinforced by the same aversive stimulus - electric shock punishment - the selective γd requirement for visual memory suggests that these KCs represent visual stimuli.

We characterized γd cell morphology using the MB607B-GAL4 and MB419B-GAL4 drivers to express axonal and dendritic markers. Their axons run in parallel to those of the other KCs in the peduncle, and project to the dorsomedial tip of the γd lobe (Figure 1B, 2A). The γd neurons are atypical in that their dendrites are highly enriched ventrolaterally outside the main calyx, where olfactory projection neurons (OPNs) terminate (Figure 1B, 2B), and form the ventral accessory calyx (vAC) (Butcher et al., 2012; Aso et al., 2014b). Nevertheless, the γd neurons are equipped with claw-like dendritic endings forming microglomeruli similar to those in the main calyx (Figure 2C,D).

γd neurons are atypical KCs and respond to visual stimuli.

(A–B) Main output and input sites labeled by Syt::GFP (green) and DenMark::mCherry (red) are differentially localized to the dorsal γ lobe and the vAC (arrow). The MB lobe (A) and main calyx (B) are outlined. P: MB peduncle (C–D) The γd dendrites (green) enwrap presynaptic terminals (gray; arrows). A single optical slice of the inset in the projection in C is magnified in D-D’’. P: MB peduncle. Scale bars: 50 µm (A–B); 20 µm (C); 2 µm (D-D’’). (E) Responses to light and odor stimulation in γd KCs measured with whole-cell current-clamp recordings. Data from four representative neurons are shown (each column corresponds to the data from one cell). Voltage traces of individual trials (gray lines, 5–7 trials) are overlaid with the mean (colored line). Raster plots below the traces represent spikes. Stimulus presentation is indicated below each trace (duration = 1 s). For odors, three of five tested odors are displayed (OCT: 3-octanol; MCH: 4-methylcyclohexanol; HEP: 2-heptanone). (F) Responses in two representative α/β KCs. (G) Modality segregation by γd (n = 12 cells) and α/β KCs (n = 11 cells). Each of the pie charts represents 24 (γd) or 22 (α/β) light-cell pairs measured in 6 flies and 60 (γd) or 55 (α/β) odor-cell pairs measured in 3 flies. The distributions of all four response categories are significantly different between γd KCs and α/β KCs with respect to both visual (p<10–5, Fisher’s exact test) and odor responses (p<10–6) See Materials and methods for details.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.14009.004

To examine if the γd neurons are tuned to visual stimuli, we measured electrophysiological responses using whole-cell patch-clamp recordings. In line with the specific requirement of γd neurons for visual conditioning, we found spiking responses to blue and green light stimuli in some of these cells (Figure 2E,G). Stimulating flies with 5 different odors did not lead to excitatory responses of the γd neurons; olfactory stimulation rather evoked slow inhibitory responses, implying the existence of feedforward inhibition through other odor-responsive KC populations (Figure 2E,G). This response profile is in sharp contrast to what we observed with typical KCs (e.g. α/β neurons), many of which responded to odors but none to visual stimuli (Figure 2F,G, [Turner et al., 2008]). Thus, there is clear modality segregation between γd and α/β neurons.

The selective tuning of the γd neurons to visual stimuli prompted us to ask if Drosophila MBs receive direct visual input from the optic lobes in the γd dendrites. Performing an anatomical screen of a GAL4 driver collection (Jenett et al., 2012), we identified two types of neurons with arbors in the optic lobes and projections in the area of the vAC, making them candidates for visual projection neurons (VPNs) to the MB (Figure 3—figure supplement 1). We named these two types of VPNs VPN-MB1 and VPN-MB2.

To precisely map the morphology of these VPNs, we generated split-GAL4 drivers, MB425B and MB334C. These drivers have strong expression in VPN-MB1 and VPN-MB2 with little other expression (Figure 3A,C). MB425B-GAL4 has predominant expression in VPN-MB1 (Figure 3B) projecting from the medulla to the vAC with many cell bodies on the anterior surface of the optic lobe. In contrast, MB334C-GAL4 strongly labels 1–3 cells of VPN-MB2 as well as the MB output neurons MBON-α1 (Figure 3C,D) (Aso et al., 2014a; 2014b). We found that the majority of VPN-MB1 and VPN-MB2 dendrites are localized to the optic lobes (Figure 3E,G). The VPN-MB1 neurons have dendritic arbors in the medulla enriched in layer M8 (Figure 3F). They cover a large field of the medulla with a conspicuous elaboration in the ventral half (Figure 3A,E,F). Single-cell labeling revealed that each VPN-MB1 cell samples input from approximately 20 optic cartridges in the medulla (Figure 3B). Dendrites of a single VPN-MB2 cover a large field of the ventral medulla, arborizing in layer M7 (Figure 3C,D,G,H). The output sites of both VPNs are restricted to the lateral protocerebrum, including the vAC, forming pre-synaptic boutons in microglomeruli (Figure 3E,G; Figure 3—figure supplement 2).

Figure 3 with 3 supplements see all
VPNs directly convey optic lobe inputs to the MB vAC.

(A) VPN-MB1 neurons labeled by MB425B-GAL4. (B) A single VPN-MB1 neuron, generated by heat shock flip out, connects the medulla and the central brain. (C) VPN-MB2 neurons labeled by MB334C-GAL4. (D) VPN-MB2 neurons connect the medulla and the central brain. (E) VPN-MB1 has dendrites (DenMark::mCherry, red) in the ventral medulla and presynaptic terminals in the central brain (Syt::GFP, green). (F) The dendrites of VPN-MB1 (green) arborize in the M8 layer. (G) VPN-MB2 has dendrites (DenMark::mCherry, red) in the ventral medulla and presynaptic terminals in the central brain (Syt::GFP, green). (H) The dendrites of VPN-MB2 (green) arborize in the M7 layer. (I–J) Reconstituted GFP signals visualize contacts between KCs and VPN-MB1 in the vAC. (K–L) Reconstituted GFP signals visualize contacts between KCs and VPN-MB2 in the vAC. J and L are magnifications of the insets in I and K. Scale bars represent 50 µm.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.14009.005

To visualize possible connections between the VPNs and KCs in the vAC, we performed counterstaining of VPNs and the catalytic subunit of PKA that marks KCs (Wolff and Strausfeld, 2015) and found that the VPN terminals are enwrapped by the dendrites of the γd neurons (Figure 3—figure supplement 2). Differential labeling of KCs and the VPNs was consistent with their proposed connection (Figure 3—figure supplement 3). We also detected a GRASP signal between the VPNs and the ventrolateral projection of the γd dendrites (Figure 3I–L).

We examined the requirement of the VPNs as well as OPNs in visual memory by blocking their output using the driver lines MB425B-GAL4, MB334C-GAL4 and GH146-GAL4 (Figure 4A–C). Memory was not significantly altered upon the blockade of the OPNs (Figure 4A). In contrast, the blockade of VPN-MB1, but not VPN-MB2, significantly impaired color discrimination memory (Figure 4B,C). To further substantiate the results with MB425B-GAL4, we employed another driver line (VT008475) that labels similar VPNs to VPN-MB1. These VPNs in VT008475 project to the lateral protocerebrum but do not reach the vAC (Figure 4—figure supplement 1). The blockade of these VPNs left color discrimination memory intact (Figure 4—figure supplement 1). These results strongly suggest the existence of modality-specific input pathways to the MB and that VPN-MB1 is a key component of the visual learning pathway.

Figure 4 with 2 supplements see all
VPN-MB1 and VPN-MB2 convey distinct visual features.

(A) OPNs labeled by GH146-GAL4 are not required for visual color conditioning (one-way ANOVA, p>0.05), n = 8. (B–C) VPN-MB1 (MB425B-GAL4; B), but not VPN-MB2 (MB334C-GAL4; C), are required for color discrimination learning (one-way ANOVA, post-hoc pairwise comparison, p<0.01). n = 9–12. (D) γd neurons labeled by MB419B-GAL4 are required for green intensity learning (one-way ANOVA, post-hoc pairwise comparison, p<0.05), n = 9–11; these neurons are also required for color discrimination learning (Figure 1). (E–F) In contrast to the requirement in color discrimination learning, the blockade of VPN-MB2 (MB334C-GAL4; F), but not VPN-MB1 (MB425B-GAL4; E), significantly impaired intensity discrimination learning (one-way ANOVA, post-hoc pairwise comparison, p<0.05). n = 8–13. (G) Schematic of memory circuits in the MB. Visual and olfactory information is first processed in the optic lobe and antennal lobe, respectively. Components of sensory information (e.g. brightness and color) are separately processed there and conveyed to corresponding KC subtypes in the MB directly through distinct projection neurons Ca: calyx, vAC: ventral accessory calyx. These segregated representations of visual and olfactory information undergo the same dopaminergic (DA) valence modulation to operate acquired behavior (e.g. conditioned avoidance) via shared circuits.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.14009.009

In our standard learning assay, flies discriminate the chromatic information of blue and green LED stimuli. In addition, flies can learn different light intensities of a single chromatic cue (Schnaitmann et al., 2013). We asked if the γd neurons and same VPNs are also required for brightness discrimination learning. We found that while the γd neurons were also necessary for this task, the blockade of VPN-MB1 did not significantly alter performance (Figure 4D,E). In contrast, learning was significantly impaired by blockade of VPN-MB2 (Figure 4F). Because MB334C-GAL4 has expression not just in VPN-MB2 but also in MBON-α1, we tested and ruled out the involvement of MBON-α1 by showing that its blockade did not significantly impair brightness discrimination learning (Figure 4—figure supplement 2). The differential requirements for VPN-MB1 and VPN-MB2 indicate that information about color and intensity of a visual cue is separately conveyed to the MB via distinct VPNs.

Discussion

Previous studies have shown the importance of MB circuits for visual memories and other visually guided behaviors in Drosophila (Vogt et al., 2014; Liu et al., 1999; Zhang et al., 2007); however, the MB had been thought to receive visual input from the optic lobe through an unknown indirect pathway, because direct connections had not been observed (Farris and Van Dyke, 2015; Tanaka et al., 2008). Our identification of direct pathways reveals similarity between dipteran and hymenopteran circuit design for visual processing, and provides experimental evidence for the behavioral roles of these circuits (Figure 4G).

VPN-MB1 and VPN-MB2 receive differential inputs in the medulla and convey distinct visual features - color and brightness - to the MB. The medulla layer M8 where VPN-MB1 arborizes (Figure 3F) contains presynaptic terminals of Tm5 neurons that are involved in color vision (Karuppudurai et al., 2014; Gao et al., 2008). VPN-MB2 is the first Drosophila interneuron shown to convey brightness information, and the position of its dendrites suggests that light intensity may be encoded in the medulla layer M7 (Figure 3H). Interestingly, the dendrites of VPN-MB1 and VPN-MB2 are preferentially distributed in the ventral half of the medulla, an arrangement consistent with the specialization of the ventral retina for color processing of landmarks during foraging (Giger and Srinivasan, 1997Kinoshita et al., 2015; Wernet et al., 2015). The target region of the MB-projecting VPNs is separated from other described VPNs in the central brain, and largely segregated from other VPNs mediating innate spectral processing and motion vision (Mu et al., 2012; Otsuna and Ito, 2006; Zhang et al., 2013a). Parallel processing of different visual features with segregated projections may be a conserved circuit strategy for visual processing across phyla (Livingstone and Hubel, 1988).

Our results provide evidence that the Drosophila MB represents distinct sensory modalities in different KC subsets whose dendrites are segregated in subdomains of the calyx. Olfactory inputs project to the main calyx and visual stimuli to the vAC, while gustatory stimuli have been recently shown to project to yet another calyx domain (Kirkhart and Scott, 2015). In the fly MB, dopamine neurons including those encoding positive and negative valences divide the long axon terminals of KCs into distinct compartments (Aso et al., 2014a; Tanaka et al., 2008). As KCs send parallel axon fibers, a single dopamine neuron locally modulates the corresponding axonal compartment of multiple KCs (Hige et al., 2015Cohn et al., 2016; Boto et al., 2014). Distinct KC subsets, γd and γm for example, can therefore share the same dopaminergic valence modulation, even if these KCs are devoted to different sensory modalities (Figure 4G) (Vogt et al., 2014). These local modulations in turn affect the information conveyed by shared MB output pathways (Figure 4G) (Aso et al., 2014; Vogt et al., 2014). Our results can thus explain the circuit mechanism by which the Drosophila MB processes memories of different modalities with shared modulatory and output pathways (Figure 4G).

There appears to be a close correlation between the ecological specialization of different insects and the organization of their MB calyces (Ehmer and Gronenberg, 2002; Lin and Strausfeld, 2012; Yilmaz et al., 2016; Groh et al., 2014), with the functional subdivision of the calyx reflecting the salient sensory environment. Olfactory processing, which is the dominant sensory modality in Drosophila subject to associative modulation, utilizes ~1800 of the 2000 KCs (Quinn et al., 1974). While fruit flies perform a wide range of behaviors driven by visual input, few could be modified by associative learning (Borst, 2009), given that fewer KCs subserve visual memory formation. The MB calyces of other insects also possess modality segregation, and different sets of KCs are presumably assigned to each sensory space (Gronenberg and Hölldobler, 1999; Kinoshita et al., 2015; Mobbs, 1982). Our results here are the first to show that individual KCs indeed have unimodal responses. The segregated sensory representation in the MB enables independent formation of different sensory memories, while allowing interaction among distinct KC populations that may underlie complex forms of learning involving multimodal integration (Zhang et al., 2013b).

Materials and methods

Flies and genetic crosses

Request a detailed protocol

Flies were reared at 25°C, at 60% relative humidity under a 12-12-hr light-dark cycle on a standard cornmeal-based diet. Flies were sorted by genotype at least two days prior to experiments. Each behavioral experiment used 30–40 flies of mixed gender under dim red light in a custom-made plastic box, containing a heating element on the bottom and a fan for air circulation.

For behavioral experiments, we used F1 progeny of crosses between females of w+;;pJFRC100-20x pJFRC100-20XUAS-TTS-Shibire-ts1-p10 in VK00005 (Pfeiffer et al., 2012) or WT-females and males of genotypes MB607B-GAL4 (Aso et al., 2014b), MB419B-GAL4 (Aso, 2014b), GH146-GAL4 (Stocker et al., 1997), MB425B-GAL4, MB334C-GAL4, MB310C-GAL4 (Aso et al., 2014b), VT008475-GAL4 (VDRC, Vienna, Austria) or Canton-S males. Split-GAL4 lines were generated using described vectors (Pfeiffer et al., 2010): MB425B carries 28F07-p65ADZp in attP40 and 10E05-ZpGdbd in attP2, MB334C carries 52G04-p65ADZp in attP40 and 49F03-ZpGdbd in VK00027. Split-GAL4 lines used in the KC screening (Figure 1—figure supplement 1) and for intensity conditioning (Figure 4—figure supplement 2) are as described in (Aso et al., 2014b). As all transgenes were inserted into the w- mutant genome, the X chromosomes of the shits effector strain was replaced with that of wild-type Canton-S (w+).

Aversive visual conditioning

Request a detailed protocol

We used LEDs to present visual stimuli (green [520 nm] and blue light [465 nm]) from the bottom of the arena as previously described in (Schnaitmann et al., 2013). The intensities were controlled by current and calibrated using a luminance meter BM-9 (Topcon Technohouse Corporation) or a PR-655 SpectraScan Spectroradiometer: 19.4 mW/m2 (blue) and 8.58 mW/m2 (green) (Schnaitmann et al., 2013). To train flies with different light intensities, blue and green visual cues were replaced by different intensities of green light (1:10 ratio; 27.8 mW/m2 (bright-green), and 2.77 mW/m2 (dark-green).

For aversive electric shock conditioning, we used an arena with a transparent shock grid as previously described (Vogt et al., 2014). During the test phase, the shock arena was video recorded from above with a CMOS camera (Firefly MV, PointGrey, Richmond, Canada) controlled by custom-made software (Schnaitmann et al., 2010). Four setups were run in parallel.

Differential conditioning was followed by binary choice without reinforcement (Vogt et al., 2014; Schnaitmann et al., 2010). Briefly, in a single experiment, approximately 40 flies were introduced into the arena using an aspirator. During a training trial, the entire arena was illuminated with alternating visual stimuli (60 s each) with one stimulus paired with aversive reinforcement. A 1-s electric shock (AC 60 V) was applied 12 times spaced over 60 s during presentation of the punished visual stimulus. Training trials were repeated four times per experiment. In the test, administered 60 s after the end of training, flies were allowed to choose between the two visual stimuli, which were each presented in two diagonally opposed quadrants of the arena. The distribution of the flies was video recorded for 90 s at 1 frame per second. No US was presented in the test period; however, a 1-s shock pulse (90 V) was applied 5 s before the beginning of the test to arouse the flies. Two groups were trained with reciprocal pairings and tested consecutively in the same setup, respectively. The difference in visual stimulus preference between the two groups was then used to calculate a learning index for each video frame. Reinforcement was paired with the first visual stimulus in half of the experiments, and with the second in the remaining experiments, to cancel any effect of order. The whole experimental setup was kept at 33°C for the temperature-induced effect of Shits1.

Aversive olfactory conditioning

Request a detailed protocol

Aversive olfactory conditioning was performed as described in (Aso et al., 2014a). A group of about 50 flies in a training tube alternately received octan-3-ol (OCT; Merck, Darmstadt, Germany) and 4-methylcyclohexanol (MCH; Sigma-Aldrich, MO) for 1 min in a constant air stream. OCT and MCH were diluted to 0.6% and 2%, respectively, in paraffin oil (Sigma-Aldrich) and presented in a cup with a diameter of 30 mm. For odor presentation, two of 3/2-way solenoid valves (MFH-3-3/4-S, FESTO, Germany) were used. Each valve was connected to two cups, one of which contains the diluted odor and the other of which contains pure paraffin oil. Four training tubes were connected to the valves. Twelve 1.5 s 90 V electric shocks (DC) were paired with one of the odor presentations. The delivery of electric shocks and the odors was controlled by a custom-made computer program. In the test, the trained flies were allowed to choose between MCH and OCT for 2 min in a T-maze. The odor cups used were identical to the ones for the conditioning. The distribution of the flies was imaged by cameras (FFMV-03M2M, Point Grey), and the preference index was calculated by taking the mean indices of the last 10 s in the 2 min choice. The learning index was then calculated by taking the mean preference of the two reciprocally trained groups. Half of the trained groups received reinforcement together with the first presented odor and the other half with the second odor to cancel the effect of the order of reinforcement. Temperature and humidity were 60% and 33ºC, measured with a digital thermo-hydrometer (6011000, Venta Luftwäscher, Germany). Behavioral experiments were performed in dim red light for training and in complete darkness for test.

Statistics

Statistical analyses were performed with Prism5 software (GraphPad). Groups that did not violate the assumption of normal distribution (Shapiro-Wilk test) and homogeneity of variance (Bartlett’s test) were analyzed with parametric statistics: one-sample t-test or one-way analysis of variance followed by the planned pairwise multiple comparisons (Bonferroni). Experiments with data that were significantly different from the assumptions above were analyzed with non-parametric tests, such as Mann-Whitney test or Kruskal–Wallis test followed by Dunn’s multiple pair-wise comparison (Figure 1—figure supplement 1). The significance level of statistical tests was set to 0.05. Only the most conservative statistical result of multiple pairwise comparisons is indicated. Visual conditioning bar graphs show pooled data over total duration of test. Olfactory conditioning bar graphs show pooled data over last 10 s of test.

Electrophysiology

Request a detailed protocol

In vivo whole-cell recordings from KCs were performed as previously reported (Turner et al., 2008). Specific cell types were visually targeted using GFP signals, with a 60X water-immersion objective (LUMPlanFl/IR; Olympus) attached to an upright microscope (BX51WI; Olympus). γd and α/β KCs were specifically labeled with GFP by crossing flies bearing UAS-2eGFP (Bloomington) with GAL4 lines, MB607B (γd KCs) or MB008B (α/β KCs). Adult F1 females were used at 2–3 days after eclosion. The patch pipettes were pulled for a resistance of 6–7 MΩ and filled with pipette solution containing (in mM): L-potassium aspartate, 125; HEPES, 10; EGTA, 1.1; CaCl2, 0.1; Mg-ATP, 4; Na-GTP, 0.5; biocytin hydrazide, 13; with pH adjusted to 7.3 with KOH (265 mOsm). The preparation was continuously perfused with saline containing (in mM): NaCl, 103; KCl, 3; CaCl2, 1.5; MgCl2, 4; NaHCO3, 26; N-tris(hydroxymethyl) methyl-2-aminoethane-sulfonic acid, 5; NaH2PO4, 1; trehalose, 10; glucose, 10 (pH 7.3 when bubbled with 95% O2 and 5% CO2, 275 mOsm). Whole-cell current-clamp recordings were made using the Axon MultiClamp 700B amplifier (Molecular Devices). Cells were held at around -50 mV by injecting a small hyperpolarizing current, typically less than 2 pA. Signals were low-pass filtered at 5 kHz and digitized at 10 kHz. Spikes were automatically detected by custom-written scripts in Matlab (R2008b, MathWorks) based on their amplitude, after first removing slow membrane potential deflections with bandpass filtering (100 to 1000 Hz); for each recording, we verified the accuracy of this automatic detection algorithm by visual inspection. To detect subthreshold responses, the peak amplitude during a response time window (0 to 0.5 sec after stimulus onset for light stimulation and 0.2 to 3 s for odor stimulation) was calculated using mean voltage traces smoothed with moving average. When amplitudes exceeded 4.2 SDs of the membrane potential fluctuations during baseline (a 1-sec or 3-sec period prior to stimulus onset), we called this an excitatory or inhibitory response. This criterion accurately reflected our visual impression of what was a significant subthreshold response.

For light stimulation, an LED (520 and 468 nm peak wavelength; WP154A4SUREPBGVGAW, Kingbright) was directed at a fly’s head from an angle 45 degrees below and directly in front of the animal, at a distance of 11cm. Light intensities were adjusted to match those used in the behavioral experiment by changing the duty cycle of the LED through Arduino Uno (Arduino). All experiments were performed in a semi-dark room. Odors were presented through a custom-built device as described previously (Honegger et al., 2011). Saturated vapors of pure odorants were diluted with air at a 1: 20 ratio. Final flow rate was 1 L/min. Odors were presented in a pseudo-random order so that no odor was presented twice in succession. The following five chemicals were used as stimuli: 2-heptanone (CAS# 110-43-0), 3-octanol (589-98-0), 4-methylcyclohexanol (589-91-3), isoamyl acetate (123-92-2) and apple cider vinegar (Richfood).

We observed distinct response profiles of γd KCs and α/β KCs with respect to both light and odor responses (Figure 2G). To see if excitatory drive from the two different modalities is segregated between the cell types, we performed similar statistical analysis (Fisher’s exact test) after combining the counts of 'Spike' with 'Excitatory', and 'Inhibition Only' with 'No Response' in Figure 2G. This analysis also showed highly significant differences both in light (p<10–5) and odor responses (p<10–7).

Immunohistochemistry

Request a detailed protocol

Adult fly brains were dissected, fixed, and stained using standard protocols (Aso et al., 2009). MB419B-GAL4, MB607B-GAL4, MB425B-GAL4, MB334C-GAL4, MB310C-GAL4, and VT008475-GAL4 were crossed to UAS-mCD8::GFP (Figure 1, Figure 2, Figure 3, Figure 1—figure supplement 1, Figure 3—figure supplement 1, Figure 3—figure supplement 2, Figure 4—figure supplement 1, Figure 4—figure supplement 2) and stained with anti-GFP antibody (AB) (rabbit anti-GFP polyclonal, Invitrogen, 1:1000) followed by Alexa Fluor 488 (goat anti-rabbit IgG highly cross absorbed, Invitrogen, 1:1000). For neuropil labeling, we used mouse anti-dskl AB (4F3, DSHB, 1:50) followed by Cy3 anti-mouse (Jackson ImmunoResearch, 1:250) or mouse anti-synapsin AB (DSHB, 1:100, (Klagges et al., 1996) followed by Cy3 anti-mouse (Dianova, 1:250) or Alexa 633 anti-mouse (Invitrogen, 1:250). MB419B-GAL4, MB425B-GAL4 and MB334C-GAL4 (Figure 2D,E, 3C,D) crossed to UAS-DenMark::mCherry; UAS-Syt::GFP (Nicolaï et al., 2010) and double labeling of w-;UAS-myr-CD8::Cherry,R13F02LexA/CyO;LexAop-GFP/TM2 crossed to MB425B-GAL4 or MB334C-GAL4 (Figure 3, Figure 3—figure supplement 3) were stained with anti-GFP AB (rat anti-GFP AB, Chromotek, 1:100) followed by Alexa Fluor 488 (anti-rat AB, Invitrogen, 1:250) and anti-dsRed AB (rabbit anti-dsred, Clontech, 1:100) followed by Alexa Fluor 568 (anti-rabbit AB, Invitrogen, 1:250). DC0-positive neurons (PKA-C1, Figure 3—figure supplement 1) were visualized using anti-DC0 AB staining (anti-DC0 rabbit, 1:2000, (Skoulakis et al., 1993) followed by Cy3 anti-rabbit (Jackson ImmunoResearch, 1:200). To visualize connections between VPNs and the vAC we crossed w-; mb247-DsRed; mb247-splitGFP11, UAS-splitGFP1-10 (Pech et al., 2013) to MB425B-GAL4 and MB334C-GAL4. For visualization of reconstituted GFP (Figure 3I–J), we used mouse anti-GFP AB (Clone N86/38, Neuro-Mab, Antibodies Inc., 1:100) followed by goat anti-mouse Alexa 488 (Invitrogen, 1:200). For MB labeling (Figure 3I–J), we used rabbit anti-dsRed AB (Clontech, 1: 100) followed by Cy3 anti-rabbit (Jackson ImmunoResearch, 1:200). For neuropil labeling, we used rat anti-N-cadherin staining (anti-N-cad DN-Ex no.8, Developmental Studies Hybridoma Bank, 1:100) followed by Alexa 633 anti-rat (Invitrogen, 1:200). R7/R8 neurons (Figure 3F,H) were visualized using mouse anti-MAb24B10 AB (DSHB, (Zipursky et al., 1984) followed by Cy3 anti-mouse (Jackson ImmunoResearch, 1:250). Optical sections of whole-mount brains were sampled with a confocal microscope (Olympus FV1000). Images of the confocal stacks were analyzed with the open-source software Fiji (Schindelin et al., 2012) and rendered using Fluorender (Schnaitmann et al., 2010). We applied the 3D mean filter (r = 2 pixels) followed by deconvolution to high magnification image stacks in Figure 3D–D”.using Fiji plugins Iterative Deconvolve 3D.

To obtain single-cell flp-out staining, males of the MB425B-GAL4 were crossed with females of y-w-, hsp70-flp [X]; UAS>CD2 y+>mCD8::GFP/CyO; TM2/TM6b (Wong et al., 2002) to obtain F1 progeny carrying GAL4 insertion, hsp70-flp and UAS>rCD2,y+>mCD8-GFP. Crosses were raised at 25°C. One to six days before eclosion a mild heat shock was given by placing the vial into a 32°C incubator to remove the FLP-out cassette (rCD2, y+) in a subset of the neurons. The duration of the heat shock was 60–90 min. The eclosed flies were then transferred into a fresh vial and 2- to 5-day-old flies were used for dissection.

References

    1. Giger A
    2. Srinivasan M
    (1997)
    Honeybee vision: Analysis of orientation and colour in the lateral, dorsal and ventral fields of view
    The Journal of Experimental Biology 200:1271–1280.
    1. Klagges BR
    2. Heimbeck G
    3. Godenschwege TA
    4. Hofbauer A
    5. Pflugfelder GO
    6. Reifegerste R
    7. Reisch D
    8. Schaupp M
    9. Buchner S
    10. Buchner E
    (1996)
    Invertebrate synapsins: A single gene codes for several isoforms in drosophila
    Journal of Neuroscience 16:3154–3165.
    1. Pfeiffer BD
    2. Truman JW
    3. Rubin GM
    (2012) Using translational enhancers to increase transgene expression in drosophila
    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 109:6626–6631.
    https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1204520109
    1. Quinn WG
    2. Harris WA
    3. Benzer S
    (1974) Conditioned behavior in drosophila melanogaster
    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 71:708–712.
    https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.71.3.708

Article and author information

Author details

  1. Katrin Vogt

    1. Max-Planck Institut für Neurobiologie, Martinsried, Germany
    2. Tohoku University Graduate School of Life Sciences, Sendai, Japan
    Present address
    Harvard University, Center for Brain Science, Cambridge, United States
    Contribution
    KV, Conception and design, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article
    Contributed equally with
    Yoshinori Aso
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  2. Yoshinori Aso

    Janelia Research Campus, Howard Hughes Medical Institute, Ashburn, United States
    Contribution
    YA, Conception and design, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article, Contributed unpublished essential data or reagents
    Contributed equally with
    Katrin Vogt
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0002-2939-1688
  3. Toshihide Hige

    Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory, Cold Spring Harbor, United States
    Present address
    Janelia Research Campus, Howard Hughes Medical Institute, Ashburn, United States
    Contribution
    TH, Conception and design, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article, Contributed unpublished essential data or reagents
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  4. Stephan Knapek

    Max-Planck Institut für Neurobiologie, Martinsried, Germany
    Contribution
    SK, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  5. Toshiharu Ichinose

    1. Max-Planck Institut für Neurobiologie, Martinsried, Germany
    2. Tohoku University Graduate School of Life Sciences, Sendai, Japan
    Contribution
    TI, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  6. Anja B Friedrich

    Max-Planck Institut für Neurobiologie, Martinsried, Germany
    Contribution
    ABF, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  7. Glenn C Turner

    Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory, Cold Spring Harbor, United States
    Present address
    Janelia Research Campus, Howard Hughes Medical Institute, Ashburn, United States
    Contribution
    GCT, Conception and design, Drafting or revising the article
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  8. Gerald M Rubin

    Janelia Research Campus, Howard Hughes Medical Institute, Ashburn, United States
    Contribution
    GMR, Conception and design, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article, Contributed unpublished essential data or reagents
    For correspondence
    rubing@janelia.hhmi.org
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  9. Hiromu Tanimoto

    1. Max-Planck Institut für Neurobiologie, Martinsried, Germany
    2. Tohoku University Graduate School of Life Sciences, Sendai, Japan
    Contribution
    HT, Conception and design, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article
    For correspondence
    hiromut@m.tohoku.ac.jp
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0001-5880-6064

Funding

Howard Hughes Medical Institute

  • Yoshinori Aso
  • Gerald M Rubin

Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (Postdoctoral Fellowship for Research Abroad)

  • Toshihide Hige

Uehara Memorial Foundation (Postdoctoral Fellowship)

  • Toshihide Hige

NIH Blueprint for Neuroscience Research (R01DC010403-01A1)

  • Glenn C Turner

Max-Planck-Gesellschaft

  • Hiromu Tanimoto

Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (TA552/5-1)

  • Hiromu Tanimoto

Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science, and Technology (KAKENHI 26120705)

  • Hiromu Tanimoto

Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science, and Technology (The Strategic Research Program for Brain Sciences)

  • Hiromu Tanimoto

Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (KAKENHI 26250001)

  • Hiromu Tanimoto

Naito Foundation

  • Hiromu Tanimoto

Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science, and Technology (KAKENHI 26119503)

  • Hiromu Tanimoto

Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (KAKENHI 15K14307)

  • Hiromu Tanimoto

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Acknowledgements

We thank Aljoscha Nern for sharing unpublished information on the anatomy of VPNs and critical reading of the manuscript; Irina Sinakevitch for anti-DC0. This work was supported by Max-Planck Gesellschaft (HT), Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (TA 552/5-1 to HT), MEXT/JSPS KAKENHI (26250001, 26120705, 26119503, 15K14307 to HT) and Postdoctoral Fellowship for Research Abroad (TH), the Strategic Research Program for Brain Sciences “Bioinformatics for Brain Sciences” (HT), Naito Foundation (HT), Howard Hughes Medical Institute (GMR and YA), NIH (R01 DC010403-01A1 to GCT), and a Postdoctoral Fellowship from the Uehara Memorial Foundation (TH).

Copyright

© 2016, Vogt et al.

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.

Metrics

  • 5,280
    views
  • 1,318
    downloads
  • 133
    citations

Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Katrin Vogt
  2. Yoshinori Aso
  3. Toshihide Hige
  4. Stephan Knapek
  5. Toshiharu Ichinose
  6. Anja B Friedrich
  7. Glenn C Turner
  8. Gerald M Rubin
  9. Hiromu Tanimoto
(2016)
Direct neural pathways convey distinct visual information to Drosophila mushroom bodies
eLife 5:e14009.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.14009

Share this article

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.14009

Further reading

    1. Neuroscience
    Jinxin Liu, Haoyue He ... Yongbing Deng
    Research Article

    Background:

    Post-stroke epilepsy (PSE) is a critical complication that worsens both prognosis and quality of life in patients with ischemic stroke. An interpretable machine learning model was developed to predict PSE using medical records from four hospitals in Chongqing.

    Methods:

    Medical records, imaging reports, and laboratory test results from 21,459 ischemic stroke patients were collected and analyzed. Univariable and multivariable statistical analyses identified key predictive factors. The dataset was split into a 70% training set and a 30% testing set. To address the class imbalance, the Synthetic Minority Oversampling Technique combined with Edited Nearest Neighbors was employed. Nine widely used machine learning algorithms were evaluated using relevant prediction metrics, with SHAP (SHapley Additive exPlanations) used to interpret the model and assess the contributions of different features.

    Results:

    Regression analyses revealed that complications such as hydrocephalus, cerebral hernia, and deep vein thrombosis, as well as specific brain regions (frontal, parietal, and temporal lobes), significantly contributed to PSE. Factors such as age, gender, NIH Stroke Scale (NIHSS) scores, and laboratory results like WBC count and D-dimer levels were associated with increased PSE risk. Tree-based methods like Random Forest, XGBoost, and LightGBM showed strong predictive performance, achieving an AUC of 0.99.

    Conclusions:

    The model accurately predicts PSE risk, with tree-based models demonstrating superior performance. NIHSS score, WBC count, and D-dimer were identified as the most crucial predictors.

    Funding:

    The research is funded by Central University basic research young teachers and students research ability promotion sub-projec t(2023CDJYGRH-ZD06), and by Emergency Medicine Chongqing Key Laboratory Talent Innovation and development joint fund project (2024RCCX10).

    1. Neuroscience
    Irene Martínez-Gallego, Heriberto Coatl-Cuaya, Antonio Rodriguez-Moreno
    Research Article

    The entorhinal cortex (EC) connects to the hippocampus sending different information from cortical areas that is first processed at the dentate gyrus (DG) including spatial, limbic and sensory information. Excitatory afferents from lateral (LPP) and medial (MPP) perforant pathways of the EC connecting to granule cells of the DG play a role in memory encoding and information processing and are deeply affected in humans suffering Alzheimer’s disease and temporal lobe epilepsy, contributing to the dysfunctions found in these pathologies. The plasticity of these synapses is not well known yet, as are not known the forms of long-term depression (LTD) existing at those connections. We investigated whether spike timing-dependent long-term depression (t-LTD) exists at these two different EC-DG synaptic connections in mice, and whether they have different action mechanisms. We have found two different forms of t-LTD, at LPP- and MPP-GC synapses and characterised their cellular and intracellular mechanistic requirements. We found that both forms of t-LTD are expressed presynaptically and that whereas t-LTD at LPP-GC synapses does not require NMDAR, t-LTD at MPP-GC synapses requires ionotropic NMDAR containing GluN2A subunits. The two forms of t-LTD require different group I mGluR, mGluR5 LPP-GC synapses and mGluR1 MPP-GC synapses. In addition, both forms of t-LTD require postsynaptic calcium, eCB synthesis, CB1R, astrocyte activity, and glutamate released by astrocytes. Thus, we discovered two novel forms of t-LTD that require astrocytes at EC-GC synapses.