Development: How do babies feel pain?
Adults and older children are able to tell someone when they are in pain. But what about newborn babies who have had no previous exposure to pain? Behavioral and physiological reactions can indicate a baby is in pain. Are these reactions—which can range from crying to increased heart rate during medical procedures—reflex responses, or do infants consciously feel pain as a sensory, emotional and cognitive experience?
Imaging studies in adults reveal that there is a complex network of brain activity that underlies pain, known as the ‘pain matrix’. Use of a technique called functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI)—which measures brain activity by detecting changes in blood oxygenation level—has identified many regions of the brain that are activated during acute pain in adults: these regions include the thalamus, the somatosensory cortex and the amygdala (Denk et al., 2014). However, whether a pain matrix exists in infants has so far received little attention (Williams et al., 2015).
Now, in eLife, Rebeccah Slater and colleagues at the University of Oxford—with Sezgi Goksan as first author—demonstrate for the first time that most (18/20) of the brain regions involved in pain in adults are also activated in healthy full-term newborn babies (Figure 1; Goksan et al., 2015). The most surprising difference was the lack of activation of the amygdala in the babies. This region is involved in emotional responses and develops earlier than other brain regions that are involved in cognition. Furthermore, the finding that the fMRI response in newborn babies occurs at lower sensory thresholds than in adults confirms the heightened sensitivity to pain in newborns that has been reported in previous studies of behavioral responses.
An important question is whether the pain matrix can be used as a universal objective assessment of the pain experienced by individuals. In adults, verbal reports of pain and changes in neural activity are not viewed as the same phenomenon (Robinson et al., 2013). Although activated brain regions correlate with self-reports of pain intensity (Apkarian et al., 2011), both are viewed as providing complementary information because pain experience is not directly reflected in the extent or degree of brain activation.
There has been a major debate for decades as to whether pain is a learned phenomenon (Rodkey and Pillai Riddell, 2013). Goksan et al. lay this issue to rest by showing that no prior experience of pain is necessary to produce sensory and some emotional responses, but the cognitive aspect of pain emerges later in development. Therefore fMRI shows great promise in understanding the ability of the infant brain to process touch and other potentially harmful stimuli.
However, the activity of neurons does not necessarily correspond to the subjective feelings experienced by the individual. Newborn babies have a relatively advanced ‘limbic system’ so are likely to experience some form of emotion, but they are not able to place pain into a cognitive framework in the way that adults can. The fact that Goksan et al. did not observe activity in the orbitofrontal cortex—a region involved in decision-making—in newborn babies is due to the later development of this brain region after birth.
Can fMRI be used to study the vulnerability of premature babies and other critically ill infants to pain? Very premature babies (those born 2 to 4 months early) generally spend weeks to months in neonatal intensive care units in hospitals where they undergo many medical procedures. The infant brain develops rapidly during this time, and extensive exposure to invasive procedures in very premature newborns is associated with altered microstructure and function in the immature brain, even after accounting for how premature the baby was and any health conditions related to their early birth (Brummelte et al., 2012; Zwicker et al., 2013). These adverse effects may be due to changes in blood flow in particular brain regions and changes in neurons caused by overactivation, but it is not clear what mechanisms are involved.
Imaging research will contribute to refining the assessment of pain in infants and potentially may guide clinical practice. Essentially, Goksan et al. do not propose that newborn babies' experience of pain is the same as the adult sensation. Nor do they maintain that this imaging technique should be the new ‘gold standard’ for assessing pain in individuals that cannot speak, or that it represents a more reliable/objective method to evaluate pain. Instead, the work of Goksan et al. provides an important foundation to move forward the study of pain in early life, while being aware of the challenges that come with interpreting functional imaging.
References
-
Procedural pain and brain development in premature newbornsAnnals of Neurology 71:385–396.https://doi.org/10.1002/ana.22267
-
Pain vulnerability: a neurobiological perspectiveNature Neuroscience 17:192–200.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3628
Article and author information
Author details
Publication history
Copyright
© 2015, Ranger and Grunau
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 3,343
- views
-
- 308
- downloads
-
- 14
- citations
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
Functional MRI suggests that infants and adults experience pain in very similar ways.
-
- Neuroscience
When mice run, activity in their primary visual cortex (V1) is strongly modulated. This observation has altered conceptions of a brain region assumed to be a passive image processor. Extensive work has followed to dissect the circuits and functions of running-correlated modulation. However, it remains unclear whether visual processing in primates might similarly change during locomotion. We therefore measured V1 activity in marmosets while they viewed stimuli on a treadmill. In contrast to mouse, running-correlated modulations of marmoset V1 were small and tended to be slightly suppressive. Population-level analyses revealed trial-to-trial fluctuations of shared gain across V1 in both species, but while strongly correlated with running in mice, gain modulations were smaller and more often negatively correlated with running in marmosets. Thus, population-wide fluctuations of V1 may reflect a common feature of mammalian visual cortical function, but important quantitative differences point to distinct consequences for the relation between vision and action in primates versus rodents.