Unusual prism adaptation reveals how grasping is controlled
Abstract
There are three main theories on how human grasping movements are controlled. Two of them state that grip aperture and the movement of the hand are controlled. They differ in whether the wrist or the thumb of the hand is controlled. We have proposed a third theory, which states that grasping is a combination of two goal-directed single-digit movements, each directed at a specific position on the object. In this study, we test predictions based on each of the theories by examining the transfer of prism adaptation during single-digit pointing movements to grasping movements. We show that adaptation acquired during single-digit movements transfers to the hand opening when subsequently grasping objects, leaving the movement of the hand unaffected. Our results provide strong evidence for our theory that grasping with the thumb and index finger is based on a combination of two goal-directed single-digit movements.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.21440.001Introduction
Two-digit grasping is a prototype of human motor coordination. Yet, there is an ongoing debate about how such movements are controlled (see for example Kleinholdermann et al., 2007; Smeets et al., 2010; van de Kamp and Zaal, 2007; Zaal and Bongers, 2014). Three main theories have been put forward. Jeannerod (1984) proposed that grasping consists of a transport component (movement of the wrist) that brings the hand to a location near the object and a grip component that ensures that the distance between the digits (grip aperture) changes in a manner that is appropriate for the size of the object. Others argue that the thumb is transported to a position on the object, with the index finger moving relative to the thumb to match grip aperture to the object’s size (Mon-Williams and McIntosh, 2000; Wing and Fraser, 1983). Finally, we have proposed that the trajectories of the tips of the digits when grasping is simply the combination of the trajectories of the same tips in goal-directed single-digit movements (‘pointing’), whereby each tip is moved to a specific position on the object (Schot et al., 2011; Smeets and Brenner, 1999; Smeets and Brenner, 2001; Verheij et al., 2012; Voudouris et al., 2013).
We have previously shown that people can simultaneously adapt movements of the finger and thumb of one hand to opposite prismatic displacements (Schot et al., 2014). In the current study, we let people grasp a cube and a cuboid before and after a period in which their single-digit movements were adapted in opposite directions. The three theories on the control of grasping make different predictions for how aftereffects of opposite adaptation during the single-digit movements will transfer to the grip aperture and the grip position during subsequent grasping movements (Table 1).
If, as Jeannerod (1984) suggested, grasping is controlled by planning the movement of the wrist and the opening and closing of the grip aperture, one would not expect opposite adaptation acquired during single-digit movements to transfer to grasping: the aftereffects of the opposite displacements of the wrist when adapting single-digit movements of the index finger and thumb will cancel each other during subsequent movements of both together when grasping, leaving the grip position unaltered. The grip aperture will also not be influenced by adapting pointing movements because it is irrelevant for single-digit movements.
If it is not the wrist movement that is controlled, but the movement of the thumb, as proposed by Mon-Williams and McIntosh (2000) and Wing and Fraser (1983), adaptation of single-digit thumb movements should lead to an aftereffect in the transport of the thumb. This aftereffect will be in the opposite direction than the visual displacement of the thumb in the adaptation phase. The movement of the index finger relative to the thumb will remain unaffected, again because grip aperture is irrelevant during a single-digit movement. The prediction is thus that grip position will show the aftereffect adaptation of the thumb, without any alteration of grip aperture.
Our own theory, based on the control of single-digit movements, predicts transfer of adaptation during pointing movements to the grip aperture, but not to the grip position, because when single-digit movements that are adapted in opposite directions are executed simultaneously, the aftereffects will cancel each other for the grip position, but will summate for grip aperture. So, if movements of the index finger are performed under leftward visual displacement and movements of the thumb are performed under rightward visual displacement during adaptation (thumb right), the predicted aftereffect is a rightward deviation of the index finger and a leftward deviation of the thumb, leading to a larger grip aperture. When the visual displacements are reversed, grip aperture should decrease.
Results
The adaptation of pointing with the tip of each of the two single digits (central part of Figure 1A) was similar to the equivalent adaptation in our previous experiment (Schot et al., 2014). After removing the prisms, the digits’ movements during grasping showed a clear aftereffect of the adaptation during pointing. Interpreted in terms of a transport- and grip-component, we found a clear aftereffect of adapting to prisms during single-digit pointing movements on the grip aperture on subsequent grasping trials. In the thumb right adaptation, the target for pointing with the tip of the thumb was displaced to the right, and that for pointing with the tip of the index finger was displaced to the left. Adapting to these displacements made the thumb move more leftward and the index finger more rightward to a certain visual location. When done simultaneously, this should move the digits further apart. In accordance with our theory that grasping can be regarded as simultaneous movements of the individual digits, grip aperture did increase after thumb right adaptation (and decrease after thumb left adaptation), both for the small and the large block (Figure 1B). As the two digits were always adapted in opposite directions, we expected their average (which we use as a measure of grip position) to remain unaffected by the adaptation. Our results show that this is the case (Figure 1C).
-
Figure 1—source data 1
- https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.21440.004
Statistical analysis of the effects of session, adaptation phase, and target block size on grip aperture showed a significant phase by session interaction (F(1,7)=7.3, p=0.03) indicating that grip aperture indeed changed differently after thumb left and thumb right adaptation (Figure 1B). This difference is 1.2 cm. Besides the phase by session interaction, there were also significant effects of block size (the target cuboid was grasped with a 2.3 cm larger grip aperture than the target cube; F(1,7)=324.1, p<0.001) and of session (F(1,7)=24.8, p=0.002). Statistical analysis of the effects of session, adaptation phase, and target block size on the grip position did not show any significant effects (all p>0.1, Figure 1C). The difference corresponding to the non-significant tendency towards an adaptation phase by session interaction for grip position was only 0.1 cm, an order of magnitude smaller than the 1.2 cm that we found for grip aperture.
To give an indication of the relationship between our measure of grip aperture and the time course of changes in grip aperture during the movement, we plotted the time course of grip aperture during the last movement towards the target cube in one of the pre-adaptation phases and during the first movements towards the target cube in both post-adaptation phases for one representative participant (inset in Figure 1B). During the first movement towards the small block after thumb left adaptation (solid red curve), this participant’s digits came so close together that they touched each other, giving her haptic feedback that they should be further apart. By the time her digits crossed the far end of the board, she had already partly corrected her movement. Thus, our measure of grip aperture gives an underestimation of the initial aftereffect. For thumb right adaptation something similar may have happened in some trials when the digits move so far apart that grip aperture reaches its anatomical limit, but this is not the case for the example in the dashed blue curve in the figure.
We designed our experiment around a set of predictions that could distinguish between three theories of grasping (Table 1), ignoring the joints and muscles involved in making the movements. One might argue that the ability to adapt in opposite directions with two digits is related to transfer more easily occurring from proximal to distal muscles than from distal to proximal muscles (Krakauer et al., 2006). We have ascertained that adaptation does not only transfer to grip formation, which primarily involves distal muscles, but also transfers to grip position, which primarily involves proximal muscles. We did this by adapting both digits in the same direction (see Appendix 1 ‘Transfer to grip position’). Although such transfer is not distinctive for our theory, it shows that adapting single-digit pointing movements does not only influence grip aperture, but involves the complete reach-to-grasp movement.
Some readers of our original adaptation study (Schot et al., 2014) have argued that the opposite adaptation of the two digits might be related to the eye rather than to the digit that was adapted, as we used a fixed digit-eye pairing during adaptation. If this objection would be substantiated, it might also affect the interpretation of the present study. We therefore decided to repeat the original experiment (with new participants in a new set-up, full description in Appendix 2 ‘Effect of digit-eye pairing’) but using monocular single-digit pointing to measure the aftereffect. We did so using both the digit-eye pairing that we used in the adaptation phase and the opposite pairing. We replicated the results that we originally obtained with binocular viewing for monocular viewing. Most importantly, the aftereffect for each digit was equally large for both eyes, irrespective of whether the eye that was viewing was the one used to adapt the digit.
Discussion
We show that adapting the thumb and index finger in opposite directions during single-digit movements gives rise to an aftereffect in grip aperture but not in grip position during subsequent grasping movements. Thus, the pattern of results is only consistent with the predictions based on our theory that grasping can be regarded as movements of the individual digits: adapting each digit during single-digit pointing movements influences the grip aperture during grasping movements. The two other theories predict quite different patterns of results (Table 1). Transfer of prism adaptation has been shown to be absent or only partial when tested with a different movement speed (Kitazawa et al., 1997), throwing style (Martin et al., 1996) or load (Fernández-Ruiz et al., 2000). This shows that adaptation can be very specific to a particular movement (van der Kooij et al., 2016). The fact that we find transfer from single-digit pointing to the grip aperture in grasping is therefore not at all trivial but suggests that grasping is indeed the combination of two single-digit movements.
All three theories limit themselves to describing the movements of the task-relevant elements. They differ in what they consider to be the relevant elements for grasping. None of them is concerned with how the movements are achieved in terms of the roles of all muscles and joints involved, so none predicts how the many task-irrelevant elements are orchestrated (Latash et al., 2002; van Beers et al., 2013). Only the transport-grip theory is concerned with the movement of the wrist (used as a proxy for the position of the hand), which is therefore task-relevant according to that theory. We have previously noted that using the wrist as a proxy for grip position makes it necessary to add a third controlled variable because the position to which the wrist should be transported to grasp an object depends on the orientation of the hand and the size of the object (Smeets and Brenner, 1999). We therefore use ‘grip position’ (the average of the positions of the two digits) as a proxy for hand position.
How the wrist, elbow and shoulder move (or, more generally, how the redundancy problem is solved) is beyond the scope of any of the mentioned theories. We therefore interpret our findings in terms of the movements of the endpoints of the digits, completely ignoring how these movements are achieved in terms of muscles and joints. In doing so, we ignore the evidence that adaptation is specific for the joints involved in the pointing movements (Krakauer et al., 2006). Such evidence makes our findings even more surprising, because as can be seen in the example in Video 1, the participants mainly performed the pointing movements and their corrections from the elbow and shoulder, whereas changes in grip aperture must involve changing the angles of the digits relative to the hand. This suggests that the transfer that we found to grip aperture in grasping is not based on transfer of a joint-specific adaptation, but on a higher-level control of the trajectory of the digit in space.
Finding transfer from transporting the separate digits in pointing to grip aperture in grasping (Figure 1B) contradicts the idea that grip aperture is controlled separately from transport of the hand or thumb (see Table 1). Finding that grip position was not systematically further to the left after thumb right adaptation (Figure 1C) contradicts the theory that the thumb determines the transport of the hand. Both the presence of an effect on grip aperture and the lack of effect on grip position are consistent with similar but independent adaptation of the movements of finger and thumb. Finding that the grip aperture can increase as well as decrease depending on the direction of the adaptation shows that the effect is due to the adaptation itself, rather than for instance uncertainty about the size of the object leading to a larger hand opening.
The observation that the digits sometimes touch each other during the grasping movement (as in the example in the inset of Figure 1B) is also consistent with independent adaptation of the movements of finger and thumb. With independent control of the thumb and index finger, it is possible that people plan digit trajectories that cannot be executed simultaneously for anatomical reasons. This seems indeed to have happened in this study. Neither of the other theories can account for the digits touching each other during the grasping movement because if the aperture is controlled it is only expected to decrease (after reaching maximal grip aperture) until it matches the size of the object.
Together, these results provide strong evidence that grasping can be considered to be built up of the same movements as single-digit pointing: grasping is the combination of two goal-directed single-digit movements (Smeets and Brenner, 1999).
Materials and methods
The methods employed in this study, are very similar to those of our original adaptation study (Schot et al., 2014). The main difference is the use of grasping instead of single-digit pointing in the pre- and post-adaptation phases. This study was part of a research programme that has been approved by the Ethische Commissie Bewegingswetenschappen (ECB 2006–02). All participants gave their informed consent before participating in this study.
Procedure
Request a detailed protocolEight participants (25–29 years of age, two males) took part in the experiment after giving informed consent. They wore PLATO shutter glasses. Movements of Infrared emitting diodes attached to the fingernails of the thumb and index finger were recorded at 250 Hz using an Optotrak 3020 system. A target object was attached to a wooden board at one of three possible target locations (5 cm apart). The board obstructed the participants’ vision of the hand until just before contact with the target object.
The experiments consisted of three phases: pre- and post-adaptation phases which each consisted of 30 grasp trials, and an adaptation phase which consisted of 30 pointing trials with the thumb and 30 pointing trials with the index finger. Three objects were used: a starting cube (2.3 cm), a target cube (2.3 cm), and a target cuboid (2.3 × 2.3×4.6 cm). The starting cube and the target cube were used in all three phases. The target cuboid was only used in the pre- and post-adaptation phases. It was included to force participants to use visual information to guide their grip aperture and to ensure that any observed adaptation effects are not specific to the object used in the adaptation phase.
Participants started all trials by grasping the starting cube with the glasses shut (Figure 2, Video 1). In the pre- and post- adaptation phases (i.e., on grasp trials), participants viewed the target binocularly without prism glasses. Once the shutter glasses opened, they grasped either the target cube or the target cuboid by its left and right surfaces. The cuboid was oriented such that it was grasped by the surfaces that were twice as far apart as those of the cube. In the adaptation phase (i.e., on single-digit pointing trials), participants viewed the target monocularly through 10 diopter prism glasses that were worn over the shutter glasses. The prism in front of the left eye shifted the image of the target about 5 cm to the right. The prism in front of the right eye shifted the image of the target about 5 cm to the left. Before each pointing trial, participants were told either to touch the left side of the target cube with their thumb or to touch the right side of the cube with their index finger. Once the shutter glasses in front of the corresponding eye opened, participants moved the appropriate digit to the appropriate side of the target cube.
The experiment consisted of two sessions that were performed on separate days with their order counterbalanced across participants. In one session, vision was displaced to the right when touching the cube with the index finger and to the left when touching the cube with the thumb (thumb left). In the other session, the displacements were reversed (thumb right). Within each session, trials were presented in pseudo-random order that ensured that all trial-types were distributed evenly: in the pre- and post-adaptation phase, each combination of target size and target location was presented once every six trials; in the adaptation phase, each combination of digit and target location was presented once every six trials.
Data analysis
Request a detailed protocolTo make sure that we had a measure that is not influenced by movement corrections based on visual feedback, we based our analysis for both index finger and thumb on the marker position 1 cm before it crossed the far edge of the board. We calculated the grip aperture by taking the difference between these positions for the two digits and the grip position by taking the average of these positions. As we used three target positions, grip position was expressed relative to the target’s position.
We evaluated the adaptation effect by comparing the median value in the pre-adaptation phase with the median value in the post-adaptation phase, both for grip aperture and grip position. To test the predictions that follow from the different theories on grasping (Table 1), we performed two repeated measures ANOVAs with phase (pre-adaptation, post-adaptation), session (thumb left, thumb right), and target block size (small, large) as independent variables. One ANOVA had grip position as the dependent variable and the other had grip aperture as the dependent variable. A significant phase by session interaction would indicate that the adaptation during the pointing trials transfers to the grasp trials.
Appendix 1
Transfer to grip position
Our theory predicts that adapting pointing movements of both digits in the same direction will transfer to the grip position in grasping. To test this, we performed a simplified version of the main experiment.
Methods
The simplification consisted of using binocular vision throughout the experiment with leftward deviating prisms in front of both eyes during the adaptation phase, and using only a single target object. We furthermore reduced the number of trials in all phases of the experiment and limited the number of participants to six. All participants performed the experiment with their right hand, which was the dominant hand for five of them.
Results
We found the expected rightward after-effect in the grip position of grasping when removing the leftward deviating prisms (Appendix 1—figure 1).
Appendix 2
Effect of digit-eye pairing
Some readers of our original adaptation study (Schot et al., 2014) have argued that because we used a fixed digit-eye pairing during adaptation, the opposite adaptation of the two digits might be related to the eye rather than to the digit that was adapted. Although we regard it as unlikely that such an association could have influenced our results (as we used binocular vision for determining the aftereffect), we thought it would be wise to provide explicit experimental evidence against this explanation, because our interpretation of the transfer to grip aperture relies on the specificity for the digit rather than the viewing eye.
Our claim of simultaneous opposite visuomotor adaptation of the digits of a hand is independent of the type of perturbation and the exact nature of the goal-directed movements. We therefore took the opportunity to modify these aspects to ensure that the effects that we found are not specific to the task that we used as well as to make the protocol less labor-intensive than the original study (Schot et al., 2014). Instead of using prisms that displaced the view of physical targets that are touched at their sides, we used virtual targets in a horizontal plane and a cursor that provides erroneous terminal feedback that we expected people to adapt to. In order to evaluate any role of digit-eye pairing, we used monocular vision throughout the experiment. By adapting with a fixed pairing of digit and eye (with associated sign of the shift of the feedback), but testing with all four combinations of digits and eyes, we can determine whether the adaptation is indeed fixed to the digit (generalizing across eyes), and not specific to the adapted pairing of digit and eye (in which case we should observe no aftereffect for the digit-eye pairings that were not used during adaptation).
Methods
Eight colleagues who were naïve with respect to the purpose of the experiment participated in this experiment. The set-up that we used was similar to that used in an earlier experiment (Kuling et al., 2016). The visual targets were black 2 cm diameter disks on a grey background. They were 20 cm from the starting position in the sagittal direction, and either aligned laterally with the starting position or 5 cm to the left or to the right. The target and background were projected onto a horizontal screen that was viewed by looking into a horizontal mirror below it. A board was positioned below the mirror, at the level of the image of the projection screen, so that the targets appeared to be lying on this board. Participants moved the index finger or thumb of their unseen hand to the visual targets. Movements were recorded by an Optotrak 3020. To initiate a trial, the participant positioned the specified digit on a physical, 4 cm diameter ring (the starting position) that was easy to find by touch alone on the otherwise flat, untextured surface. Participants wore Plato shutter spectacles that always occluded one of the eyes.
The experiment consisted of 2 sessions, one for each pairing of digit and eye. Each session consisted of 60 pre-adaptation trials (5 for each of the 3 positions, 2 eyes and 2 digits), 90 adaptation trials (15 for each of the 3 positions, and 2 combinations of eye and digit) and 60 post-adaptation trials that were identical to the pre-adaptation trials. Each trial started with a written instruction that specified which digit (index finger or thumb) had to move to the starting position. One of the Plato spectacles’ shutters was clear and the other opaque. In the adaptation block, this was consistently coupled to the digit that was to be moved. In the other two blocks, it was random. The target appeared 500 ms after the relevant digit arrived at the starting position. Participants had to move to the target in a single smooth movement. Once the digit stopped moving (less than 1 mm displacement within 300 ms) its position was noted. 500 ms later the target disappeared. In adaptation trials, a 1.4 cm diameter red disk provided feedback about the digit’s position during this last 500 ms. The feedback was 3 cm to the left or 3 cm to the right of the digit’s actual position. Whether it was to the left or to the right was coupled to the digit that was moving (and thus also to the viewing eye). The pairing differed between the two sessions.
Results and conclusion
We found, again, that the two digits could adapt simultaneously in opposite directions (Appendix 2—figure 1A). Thus, we were able to replicate the results of Schot et al. (2014) with different movements and a different perturbation (only feedback about the hand was displaced; when wearing prisms all visual information is displaced). Most importantly, the after-effect of the adaptation was just as clear in the trials with the digit-eye pairing that was never used during adaptation (open bars Appendix 2—figure 1B) as in the trials with the same digit-eye pairing as during adaptation (solid bars in Appendix 2—figure 1B). The lateral errors appear to be slightly lower than zero before the adaptation (leftmost part of Appendix 2—figure 1A). This is most likely a transfer of the after-effect of the previous session because the break between the two sessions was only a few minutes. The mean lateral error was indeed negligible for the first session that participants performed (95% confidence interval: −0.14 to 0.26 cm) but was −0.37 cm for the second session (95% confidence interval: −0.58 to −0.16 cm). As the order of the two kinds of sessions was randomized across participants, this transfer of after-effect did not influence the results.
We conclude that the simultaneous adaptation of digits of the same hand in opposite directions is indeed linked to the digit, rather than to the eye or a specific combination of digit and eye.
References
-
Learning motor synergies makes use of information on muscular loadLearning & Memory 7:193–198.https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.7.4.193
-
The timing of natural prehension movementsJournal of Motor Behavior 16:235–254.https://doi.org/10.1080/00222895.1984.10735319
-
Prism adaptation of reaching movements: specificity for the velocity of reachingJournal of Neuroscience 17:1481–1492.
-
Grasping trapezoidal objectsExperimental Brain Research 180:415–420.https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-007-0867-6
-
Motor control strategies revealed in the structure of motor variabilityExercise and Sport Sciences Reviews 30:26–31.https://doi.org/10.1097/00003677-200201000-00006
-
A test between two hypotheses and a possible third way for the control of prehensionExperimental Brain Research 134:268–273.https://doi.org/10.1007/s002210000479
-
Grasping and hitting moving objectsExperimental Brain Research 212:487–496.https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-011-2756-2
-
Simultaneous adaptation of the thumb and index finger of the same hand to opposite prism displacementsJournal of Neurophysiology 111:2554–2559.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00326.2013
-
Independent movements of the digits in graspingExperimental Brain Research 139:92–100.https://doi.org/10.1007/s002210100748
-
Similarities between digits' movements in grasping, touching and pushingExperimental Brain Research 203:339–346.https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-010-2236-0
-
Random walk of motor planning in task-irrelevant dimensionsJournal of Neurophysiology 109:969–977.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00706.2012
-
Prehension is really reaching and graspingExperimental Brain Research 182:27–34.https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-007-0968-2
-
Temporally stable adaptation is robust, incomplete and specificEuropean Journal of Neuroscience 44:2708–2715.https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.13355
-
Ultra-fast selection of grasping pointsJournal of Neurophysiology 110:1484–1489.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00066.2013
-
The contribution of the thumb to reaching movementsThe Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A 35:297–309.https://doi.org/10.1080/14640748308402135
Article and author information
Author details
Funding
Nederlandse Organisatie voor Wetenschappelijk Onderzoek (453-08-004)
- Jeroen BJ Smeets
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Acknowledgements
This research was funded by NWO/MaGW [grant number 453-08-004]
Ethics
Human subjects: This study was part of a research programme that has been approved by the Ethische Commissie Bewegingswetenschappen (ECB 2006-02). All participants gave their informed consent before participating in this study.
Copyright
© 2017, Schot et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 1,058
- views
-
- 154
- downloads
-
- 12
- citations
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Developmental Biology
- Neuroscience
The blood-brain barrier (BBB) controls the movement of molecules into and out of the central nervous system (CNS). Since a functional BBB forms by mouse embryonic day E15.5, we reasoned that gene cohorts expressed in CNS endothelial cells (EC) at E13.5 contribute to BBB formation. In contrast, adult gene signatures reflect BBB maintenance mechanisms. Supporting this hypothesis, transcriptomic analysis revealed distinct cohorts of EC genes involved in BBB formation and maintenance. Here, we demonstrate that epigenetic regulator’s histone deacetylase 2 (HDAC2) and polycomb repressive complex 2 (PRC2) control EC gene expression for BBB development and prevent Wnt/β-catenin (Wnt) target genes from being expressed in adult CNS ECs. Low Wnt activity during development modifies BBB genes epigenetically for the formation of functional BBB. As a Class-I HDAC inhibitor induces adult CNS ECs to regain Wnt activity and BBB genetic signatures that support BBB formation, our results inform strategies to promote BBB repair.
-
- Computational and Systems Biology
- Neuroscience
Hypothalamic kisspeptin (Kiss1) neurons are vital for pubertal development and reproduction. Arcuate nucleus Kiss1 (Kiss1ARH) neurons are responsible for the pulsatile release of gonadotropin-releasing hormone (GnRH). In females, the behavior of Kiss1ARH neurons, expressing Kiss1, neurokinin B (NKB), and dynorphin (Dyn), varies throughout the ovarian cycle. Studies indicate that 17β-estradiol (E2) reduces peptide expression but increases Slc17a6 (Vglut2) mRNA and glutamate neurotransmission in these neurons, suggesting a shift from peptidergic to glutamatergic signaling. To investigate this shift, we combined transcriptomics, electrophysiology, and mathematical modeling. Our results demonstrate that E2 treatment upregulates the mRNA expression of voltage-activated calcium channels, elevating the whole-cell calcium current that contributes to high-frequency burst firing. Additionally, E2 treatment decreased the mRNA levels of canonical transient receptor potential (TPRC) 5 and G protein-coupled K+ (GIRK) channels. When Trpc5 channels in Kiss1ARH neurons were deleted using CRISPR/SaCas9, the slow excitatory postsynaptic potential was eliminated. Our data enabled us to formulate a biophysically realistic mathematical model of Kiss1ARH neurons, suggesting that E2 modifies ionic conductances in these neurons, enabling the transition from high-frequency synchronous firing through NKB-driven activation of TRPC5 channels to a short bursting mode facilitating glutamate release. In a low E2 milieu, synchronous firing of Kiss1ARH neurons drives pulsatile release of GnRH, while the transition to burst firing with high, preovulatory levels of E2 would facilitate the GnRH surge through its glutamatergic synaptic connection to preoptic Kiss1 neurons.