Impaired voice processing in reward and salience circuits predicts social communication in children with autism
Abstract
Engaging with vocal sounds is critical for children’s social-emotional learning, and children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) often ‘tune out’ voices in their environment. Little is known regarding the neurobiological basis of voice processing and its link to social impairments in ASD. Here, we perform the first comprehensive brain network analysis of voice processing in children with ASD. We examined neural responses elicited by unfamiliar voices and mother’s voice, a biologically salient voice for social learning, and identified a striking relationship between social communication abilities in children with ASD and activation in key structures of reward and salience processing regions. Functional connectivity between voice-selective and reward regions during voice processing predicted social communication in children with ASD and distinguished them from typically developing children. Results support the Social Motivation Theory of ASD by showing reward system deficits associated with the processing of a critical social stimulus, mother’s voice, in children with ASD.
Editorial note: This article has been through an editorial process in which the authors decide how to respond to the issues raised during peer review. The Reviewing Editor's assessment is that minor issues remain unresolved (see decision letter).
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.39906.001Introduction
The human voice is a critical social stimulus in children’s environment, and engaging with vocal sounds is important for language (Kuhl et al., 2005a; Christophe et al., 1994) and social-emotional learning (DeCasper and Fifer, 1980) during typical development. However, children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) are often not responsive to voices (Kanner, 1968; Harstad et al., 2016), and it has been hypothesized that voice processing deficits contribute to pronounced social communication difficulties in ASD (Klin, 1991; Kuhl et al., 2005b; Whitehouse and Bishop, 2008). A special case of voice processing impairments in children with ASD is a deficit in processing mother’s voice (Klin, 1991), a biologically salient and implicitly rewarding sound for typically developing (TD) children (Lamb, 1981; Thoman et al., 1977), which is closely associated with cognitive (Kuhl et al., 2005a; Christophe et al., 1994) and social development (DeCasper and Fifer, 1980; Adams and Passman, 1979). Compared to studies of visual face processing (Dalton et al., 2005; Dawson et al., 2002; Dichter et al., 2012; Pierce et al., 2001; Schultz et al., 2000), very little is known regarding the neurobiology of voice processing networks in children with ASD, which is fundamental to human communication.
It remains unknown why children with ASD often do not engage with the voices in their environment. Specifically, it is not known which aspects of voice processing are impaired in children with ASD. One possibility is that sensory deficits negatively affect voice processing and contribute to social communication deficits (Dinstein et al., 2012; Markram et al., 2007; Russo et al., 2010; Marco et al., 2011; Leekam et al., 2007; Woynaroski et al., 2013). A second possibility relates to the motivation to engage with socially relevant stimuli (Chevallier et al., 2012; Dawson et al., 2004; Pelphrey et al., 2011; Clements et al., 2018). The social motivation theory of ASD posits that impairments in representing the reward value of human vocal sounds impedes individuals with ASD from engaging with these stimuli, and contributes to social interaction difficulties (Dawson et al., 2002; Chevallier et al., 2012). While this is a prominent model for considering social communication function in ASD, there has been a dearth of compelling experimental evidence showing aberrant reward processing in response to clinically meaningful social stimuli (Clements et al., 2018).
An important approach for testing theories of ASD is the use of human brain imaging methods and functional circuit analyses. Behavioral studies are limited in their ability to provide details regarding the neural mechanisms underlying distinct aspects of social information processing, and systems neuroscience analyses can uncover important aspects of social information processing that may be impaired in individuals with ASD. For example, the social motivation theory posits that individuals with ASD show reduced engagement and connectivity in the mesolimbic reward system, including the ventral tegmental area (VTA), nucleus accumbens (NAc), orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), and ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), and structures of the salience and affective processing systems, instantiated in the anterior insula and amygdala, during social processing (Chevallier et al., 2012).
Previous brain imaging research of voice processing in adults with ASD has supported the sensory deficit model by showing reduced regional activity in voice-selective superior temporal sulcus (STS) (Gervais et al., 2004; Schelinski et al., 2016), a core region associated with structural analysis of the human voice (Belin et al., 2000). However, several factors have precluded thorough tests of prominent ASD theories in the context of the neurobiology of voice processing. First, there have been few studies examining voice processing in ASD, particularly when compared to the extensive face processing literature (Dalton et al., 2005; Dichter et al., 2012; Pierce et al., 2001; Schultz et al., 2000; Baron-Cohen et al., 1999; Dapretto et al., 2006). Second, previous studies have not employed biologically salient voices (e.g. mother/caregiver), which are thought to be implicitly rewarding (Chevallier et al., 2012), to probe brain circuit function in children with ASD. For example, a recent study in TD children showed that, compared to unfamiliar voices, mother’s voice elicits activation within voice-selective, mesolimbic reward, affective, and salience, and face-processing brain regions, and connectivity between these regions predicts social communication abilities (Abrams et al., 2016). Third, previous studies of voice processing have focused on group differences in brain activity between individuals with ASD and matched controls but have not examined how individual variation in social communication abilities are associated with social brain circuit function in ASD. Finally, although autism has been conceptualized as a disorder of brain connectivity (Uddin et al., 2013a; Wass, 2011), previous brain imaging studies of human voice processing in ASD have focused on regional activation profiles in voice-selective cortex (Gervais et al., 2004; Schelinski et al., 2016) and have not employed a brain networks perspective. Importantly, a brain networks approach goes beyond describing activation in circumscribed brain regions and accounts for the coordinated activity in distributed brain systems during social information processing, and would provide considerable insight into aberrancies in several critical brain systems in ASD (Di Martino et al., 2011; Uddin et al., 2013b; von dem Hagen et al., 2013). For example, a previous resting state fMRI study investigated intrinsic connectivity of voice-selective cortex and showed that children with ASD have reduced connectivity between voice-selective STS and key structures of the mesolimbic reward system, anterior insula, and amygdala (Abrams et al., 2013a). Moreover, the strength of intrinsic connectivity in this network predicted social communication abilities in children with ASD. While intrinsic network findings support the social motivation theory of ASD, a critical question remains: do results from intrinsic connectivity reflect an epiphenomenon, or is aberrant brain connectivity in voice and reward brain systems during the processing of biologically salient and clinically relevant voices a signature of social communication deficits in children with ASD?
Here, we examine social information processing in children with ASD by probing brain circuit function and connectivity in response to human vocal sounds. We examined two aspects of voice processing: (1) unfamiliar voice processing compared to non-social auditory processing (i.e. environmental sounds) and (2) mother’s voice compared to unfamiliar voice processing (Figure 1a). The rationale for this approach is that these two levels of social information processing may reflect distinct neural signatures in voice-selective, salience, and reward processing brain systems in children with ASD. A key aspect of our analysis was to investigate whether brain activity and connectivity in response to these vocal contrasts reflects individual differences in social communication abilities in children with ASD (Lord et al., 2000). A second aspect of the analysis was to build on results from a previous intrinsic connectivity study of the voice processing network in children with ASD (Abrams et al., 2013a) to examine whether stimulus-evoked connectivity patterns within this network during unfamiliar and mother’s voice processing can reliably distinguish children with ASD from TD children and predict social communication abilities in children with ASD.
Results
TD vs. ASD activation differences in response to unfamiliar voices
Direct group comparisons between TD children and children with ASD in response to unfamiliar female voices show that children with ASD have reduced activity in a relatively small set of brain regions confined to lateral temporal cortex (Figure 2A; see Appendix 1—table 1 for effect sizes and Appendix 1—figure 2 for within-group results). Specifically, children with ASD show reduced activity in right hemisphere planum polare (PP), an area of auditory association cortex within the superior temporal gyrus. Within-group signal level analysis showed that TD children have greater activity for unfamiliar female voices, compared to environmental sounds, in this brain region (i.e. positive βs; see Appendix 1—figure 3A) while children with ASD show weaker activity for this same contrast (i.e. negative βs for unfamiliar voices compared to environmental sounds). No brain regions showed greater activity for unfamiliar female voices in the ASD, compared to the TD, group.
TD vs. ASD activation differences in response to mother’s voice
Direct group comparisons between brain responses measured from TD children and children with ASD in response to mother’s voice relative to unfamiliar female voices revealed that children with ASD have reduced activity in several visual processing regions as well as key structures of the medial temporal lobe memory system (Figure 2B; see Appendix 1—table 1 for effect sizes and Appendix 1—figure 4 for within-group results). Specifically, whole-brain analysis revealed that TD children had greater activation compared to children with ASD for mother’s voice in bilateral intercalcarine cortex extending into lingual gyrus. Moreover, children with ASD showed reduced activity compared to TD children in a broad extent of fusiform gyrus bilaterally, including both left-hemisphere occipital regions of fusiform as well as temporal occipital regions in the right-hemisphere. Children with ASD also showed less activity for mother’s voice in right-hemisphere posterior hippocampus, a critical region for learning and memory, as well as precuneus cortex of the default mode network. Signal level analysis shows that TD children have greater activity for mother’s voice compared to unfamiliar female voices in these brain regions (i.e. positive βs; see Appendix 1—figure 3B) while children with ASD show weaker activity for mother’s voice (i.e. negative βs). No brain structures showed greater activity for mother’s voice in the ASD, compared to the TD, group. Moreover, fMRI activation profiles in children with ASD were not related to mother’s voice identification accuracy (see Appendix, fMRI activation and connectivity profiles in children with ASD are not related to mother’s voice identification accuracy).
Brain activity and social communication abilities
Identifying sources of variance in key symptom domains represents an important question for autism research. We performed a whole-brain linear regression analysis using individual social communication scores as a predictor of brain activation. We first examined this relation in the context of general vocal processing using the unfamiliar female voices minus environmental sounds contrast. Results from this analysis show a striking pattern: the strength of activity in a variety of brain systems serving auditory, reward, and salience detection is correlated with social communication abilities in children with ASD (Figure 3A; see Appendix 1—table 2 for effect sizes). Specifically, this pattern was apparent in auditory association cortex of the superior temporal plane, including the PP, but also in the nucleus accumbens of the reward pathway, and anterior insula of the salience network. Scatterplots show that brain activity and social communication abilities vary across a range of values and greater social function, reflected by lower social communication scores, is associated with greater brain activity in these auditory, reward, and salience processing regions. Support vector regression (SVR) analysis (Abrams et al., 2016; Cohen et al., 2010) showed that the strength of activity in these regions was a reliable predictor of social communication function in these children (R ≥ 0.49; p ≤ 0.011 for all regions).
We next examined the question of heterogeneity in the context of mother’s voice processing, and results show a similar pattern: children with ASD with greater social communication abilities showed greater activation for mother’s voice in a wide extent of primary auditory, auditory association, and voice-selective cortex as well as mesolimbic reward, salience detection, and motor regions (Figure 3B). Specifically, this brain-behavior relationship was evident in auditory regions of superior temporal cortex, including medial aspects of bilateral Heschl’s gyrus, which contains primary auditory cortex, right-hemisphere PP of the superior temporal plane, as well as bilateral voice-selective mSTS. This relationship was also observed in regions of the salience network, including dorsal aspects of AI bilaterally and right-hemisphere rostral ACC (rACC), as well as vmPFC of the reward network. SVR results indicated that the strength of activity in these particular brain regions during mother’s voice processing was a reliable predictor of social communication function in these children (R ≥ 0.50; p ≤ 0.009 for all regions).
Connectivity patterns predict group membership
Functional connectivity was examined using a generalized psychophysiological interaction (gPPI) model within an extended voice processing brain network defined a priori from intrinsic connectivity results described in a previous study in children with ASD (Abrams et al., 2013a) (Figure 4A). This approach allows us to systematically build upon our previous findings while preempting task and sample-related biases in region-of-interest (ROI) selection. This extended voice processing network included ROIs in voice-selective STS, structures of the reward and salience networks, amygdala, hippocampus, and fusiform cortex (see Appendix 1—table 3 for details of this network). There were no univariate group differences in individual links during either unfamiliar voice (Figure 4B) or mother’s voice processing (Figure 4C) after correcting for multiple comparisons (FDR, q < 0.05). Support vector classification (SVC) results showed that multivariate connectivity patterns during unfamiliar voice processing were unable to predict group membership above chance (SVC Accuracy = 51.6%, p = 0.31); however, multivariate connectivity patterns during mother’s voice processing accurately predicted TD vs. ASD group membership (SVC Accuracy = 70.4%, p = 0.001). We performed a confirmatory analysis using a different logistic regression classifier (GLMnet, generalized linear model via penalized maximum likelihood) and results were similar to the SVC results (unfamiliar voice processing: 54.8%, p = 0.78 (not significant); mother’s voice: 80.9%, p = 0.010). These SVC results held even after accounting for group differences in mother’s voice identification accuracy (see Appendix, fMRI activation and connectivity profiles in children with ASD are not related to mother’s voice identification accuracy). Results show that patterns of brain connectivity during biologically-salient voice processing, but not unfamiliar voice processing, can distinguish children with ASD from TD children.
Connectivity patterns predict social communication abilities
We next examined the relation between connectivity beta weights in each cell of the connectivity matrix and social communication scores in children with ASD. There were no significant univariate correlations between the strength of brain connectivity during either unfamiliar (Figure 5B) or mother’s voice processing (Figure 5C) and social communication abilities. We then performed support vector regression (SVR) to examine whether multivariate patterns of connectivity during voice processing accurately predict social communication abilities in these children. Given that brain activation results showed that both unfamiliar (Figure 3A) and mother’s voice processing (Figure 3B) explained variance in social communication abilities, we used a combination of connectivity features from both vocal conditions for this analysis. SVR results showed that multivariate connectivity patterns during unfamiliar and mother’s voice processing accurately predict social communication scores in children with ASD (R = 0.42, p = 0.015). We performed a confirmatory analysis using GLMnet and results were similar to the SVR results (social communication prediction: R = 0.76, p < 0.001). Furthermore, when children with below chance accuracy on the mother’s voice identification accuracy were removed from the analysis, this result held and connectivity patterns were still predictive of social communication scores (see Appendix, fMRI activation and connectivity profiles in children with ASD are not related to mother’s voice identification accuracy).
Discussion
It is unknown why children with ASD often ‘tune out’ from the voices of social partners in their environment (Kanner, 1968), including personal relations such as family members and caregivers (Klin, 1991). Here, we identify a striking relationship between individuals’ social communication abilities and the strength of activation in reward and salience processing brain regions, notably NAc and AI, during human voice processing in children with ASD. Multivariate connectivity patterns within an extended voice processing network distinguished children with ASD from their TD peers and predicted social communication abilities in children with ASD. These findings suggest that dysfunction of the brain’s reward system provides a stable brain signature of ASD that contributes to aberrant processing of salient vocal information (Abrams et al., 2013a).
Regional and network features associated with voice processing predict individual differences in social function in children with ASD
Individuals with ASD present with a complex behavioral profile, which includes an array of sensory (Marco et al., 2011), cognitive (Mundy and Newell, 2007), and affective processing differences (Harms et al., 2010) compared to TD individuals. Consensus on the specific factors that most contribute to pronounced social communication difficulties in this population has remained elusive. Our findings showed that both regional and network features associated with voice processing, encompassing voice-selective cortex in the STS and extended voice-processing network that includes auditory, reward, and salience regions, predicted social function in children with ASD. The diversity of this network reflects the complexities of social communication itself, which involves the ability to integrate sensory, affective, mnemonic, and reward information. Importantly, our results unify several important characteristics of ASD in the extant literature, including regional functional aberrancies within specific brain systems and their association with social abilities (Gervais et al., 2004; Kleinhans et al., 2008; Scott-Van Zeeland et al., 2010; Richey et al., 2014; Lombardo et al., 2015), network level dysfunction (Di Martino et al., 2011; Uddin et al., 2013b; von dem Hagen et al., 2013; Abrams et al., 2013a), and heterogeneity of social communication abilities (Lord et al., 2012; Lord et al., 1994). We suggest that social communication function – human’s ability to interact with and relate to others – is a unifying factor for explaining regional activation profiles and large-scale connectivity patterns linking key elements of the social brain.
A voice-related brain network approach for understanding social information processing in autism
Brain network analyses represent an important approach for understanding brain function in autism (Di Martino et al., 2011; Uddin et al., 2013b; von dem Hagen et al., 2013; Abrams et al., 2013a), and psychopathology more broadly (Menon, 2011). These methods, which are typically applied to resting-state brain imaging data, have yielded considerable knowledge regarding network connectivity patterns in ASD and their links to behavior (Abrams et al., 2013a). A central assumption of this approach is that aberrant task-evoked circuit function is associated with clinical symptoms and behavior; however, empirical studies examining these associations have been lacking from the ASD literature. Our study addresses this gap by probing task-evoked function within a network defined a priori from a previous study of intrinsic connectivity of voice-selective networks in an independent group of children with ASD. We show that voice-related network function during the processing of a clinically and biologically meaningful social stimulus predicts both ASD group membership as well as social communication abilities in these children. Our findings bridge a critical gap between the integrity of the intrinsic architecture of the voice-processing network in children with ASD and network signatures of aberrant social information processing in these individuals.
Biologically-salient vocal stimuli for investigating the social brain in autism spectrum disorders
Our results demonstrate that brief samples of a biologically salient voice, mother’s voice, elicit a distinct neural signature in children with ASD. Our findings have important implications for the development of social skills in children with ASD. Specifically, typically developing children prefer biologically salient voices such as a mother’s voice which provide critical cues for social (Adams and Passman, 1979) and language learning (Liu et al., 2003). In contrast, both anecdotal (Kanner, 1968) and experimental accounts (Klin, 1991) indicate that children with ASD do not show a preference for these sounds. We suggest that aberrant function within the extended voice processing network may underlie insensitivity to biologically salient voices in children with ASD, which may subsequently affect key developmental processes associated with social and pragmatic language learning.
The social motivation theory and reward circuitry in children with ASD
The social motivation theory of ASD provides an important framework for considering pervasive social deficits in affected individuals (Dawson et al., 2002; Chevallier et al., 2012). The theory posits that social skills emerge in young children from an initial attraction to social cues in their environment. For example, TD infants are highly attentive to speech despite having no understanding of words’ meanings, and this early attraction to vocal cues may be a critical step in a developmental process that includes speech sound discrimination, mimicry, and, ultimately, language learning and verbal communication (Kuhl et al., 2005b). In contrast, children with ASD often do not engage with the speech in their environment (Kanner, 1968), and a central hypothesis of the social motivation theory is that weak reward attribution to vocal sounds during early childhood disrupts important developmental processes supporting social communication.
Our findings provide support for the social motivation theory by showing a link between social communication abilities in children with ASD and the strength of activity in reward and salience detection systems in response to unfamiliar and mother’s voice. Specifically, children with ASD who have the most severe social communication deficits have the weakest responses in reward and salience detection brain regions to both of these vocal sources. Moreover, network connectivity of an extended voice-selective network, which includes nodes of the salience and reward networks, distinguished ASD and TD children and predicted social communication abilities in children with ASD. These results are the first to show that aberrant function of reward circuitry during voice processing is a distinguishing feature of childhood autism, and may limit the ability of children with ASD to experience vocal sounds as rewarding or salient. Our findings add to a growing literature suggesting that functional connectivity between voice-selective STS and reward and salience processing regions is an important predictor of social skill development in children (Abrams et al., 2016; Abrams et al., 2013a).
Our results highlighting the role of reward and salience in the context of voice processing have implications for clinical treatment of social communication deficits in children with ASD. An important direction for treatment of children with ASD involves the use of teaching strategies (Dawson et al., 2010; Koegel and Koegel, 2006) that focus on motivating children to engage in verbal interactions to improve social communication skills (Koegel et al., 2005; Mundy and Stella, 2000). Findings suggest that clinical efforts to increase the reward value of vocal interactions in children with ASD may be key to remediating social communication deficits in these individuals. Furthermore, neural activity and connectivity measures may represent a quantitative metric for assessing response to clinical treatments focused on verbal interactions.
Limitations
There are limitations to the current work that warrant consideration. First, the sample size is relatively modest compared to recent task-based brain imaging studies of neurotypical adult populations and resting-state fMRI or structural MRI studies in individuals with ASD, however these types of studies do not face the same data collection challenges as task-based studies in clinical pediatric populations (Yerys et al., 2009). Importantly, resting-state and structural imaging studies are unable to address specific questions related to social information processing in ASD, such as biologically salient voice processing, which are critical for understanding the brain bases of social dysfunction in affected children. Indeed, our sample size is larger than, or comparable to, the majority of task-fMRI studies in children with ASD published since 2017, and have more stringent individual-level sampling compared to these studies. This is an important consideration given that the replicability of task fMRI data is not solely contingent on a large sample size but also depends on the amount of individual-level sampling. A recent report examining this question showed that modest sample sizes, comparable to those described in our submitted manuscript, yield highly replicable results with only four runs of task data with a similar number of trials per run as our study (Nee, 2018). In comparison, we required that each child participant had at least seven functional imaging runs of our event-related fMRI task that met our strict head movement criteria. A final limitation of this work is that, consistent with the vast majority of brain imaging studies in children with ASD, we were unable to include lower functioning children with ASD since the scanner environment is ill-suited for these children (Yerys et al., 2009). Further studies with larger samples are needed both to capture the full range of heterogeneity of ASD and to ensure the broader generalizability of the findings reported here.
Conclusion
We identified neural features underlying voice processing impairments in children with ASD, which are thought to contribute to pervasive social communication difficulties in affected individuals. Results show that activity profiles and network connectivity patterns within voice-selective and reward regions, measured during unfamiliar and mother’s voice processing, distinguish children with ASD from TD peers and predict their social communication abilities. These findings are consistent with the social motivation theory of ASD by linking human voice processing to dysfunction in the brain’s reward centers, and have implications for the treatment of social communication deficits in children with ASD. For example, parent training has emerged as a powerful and cost-effective approach for increasing treatment intensity (National Research Council, 2001): treatment delivery in the child’s natural environment promotes functional communication (Delprato, 2001), generalization (Stokes and Baer, 1977), and maintenance of skills over time (Sheinkopf and Siegel, 1998; Moes and Frea, 2002). Findings from the current study, which demonstrate a link between social communication function and neural processing of mother’s voice, support the importance of parent training by suggesting that a child’s ability to focus on, and direct neural resources to, these critical communication partners may be a key to improving social function in affected children.
Materials and methods
Participants
The Stanford University Institutional Review Board approved the study protocol. Parental consent and the child’s assent were obtained for all evaluation procedures, and children were paid for their participation in the study.
A total of 57 children were recruited from around the San Francisco Bay Area for this study. All children were required to be right-handed and have a full-scale IQ > 80, as measured by the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI) (Wechsler, 1999). 28 children met ASD criteria based on an algorithm (Risi et al., 2006) that combines information from both the module 3 of the ADOS-2 (47) and the ADI–Revised (Lord et al., 1994). Specifically, these children showed mild to more severe social communication deficits, particularly in the areas of social-emotional reciprocity and verbal and non-verbal communication, and repetitive and restricted behaviors and interests (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Five children with ASD were excluded because of excessive movement in the fMRI scanner, one child was excluded because of a metal retainer interfering with their brain images, and one child was excluded because their biological mother was not available to do a voice recording. Importantly, children in the ASD sample are considered ‘high-functioning’ and had fluent language skills and above-average reading skills (Table 1). Nevertheless, these children are generally characterized as having communication impairments, especially in the area of reciprocal conversation.
TD children and had no history of neurological, psychiatric, or learning disorders, personal and family history (first degree) of developmental cognitive disorders and heritable neuropsychiatric disorders, evidence of significant difficulty during pregnancy, labor, delivery, or immediate neonatal period, or abnormal developmental milestones as determined by neurologic history and examination. Three TD children were excluded because of excessive movement in the fMRI scanner, one was excluded because of scores in the ‘severe’ range on standardized measures of social function, and four female TD children were excluded to provide a similar ratio of males to females relative to the ASD participants. The final TD and ASD groups that were included in the analysis consisted of 21 children in each group who were matched for full-scale IQ, age, sex, and head motion during the fMRI scan (Table 1). All participants are the biological offspring of the mothers whose voices were used in this study (i.e. none of our participants were adopted, and therefore none of the mother’s voices are from an adoptive mother), and all participants were raised in homes that included their mothers. Participants’ neuropsychological characteristics are provided in Table 1.
Data acquisition parameters
Request a detailed protocolAll fMRI data were acquired at the Richard M. Lucas Center for Imaging at Stanford University. Functional images were acquired on a 3 T Signa scanner (General Electric) using a custom-built head coil. Participants were instructed to stay as still as possible during scanning, and head movement was further minimized by placing memory-foam pillows around the participant’s head. A total of 29 axial slices (4.0 mm thickness, 0.5 mm skip) parallel to the anterior/posterior commissure line and covering the whole brain were imaged by using a T2*-weighted gradient-echo spiral in-out pulse sequence (Glover and Law, 2001) with the following parameters: repetition time = 3576 ms; echo time = 30 ms; flip angle = 80°; one interleave. The 3576 msec TR can be calculated as the sum of: (1) the stimulus duration of 956 msec; (2) a 300 ms silent interval buffering the beginning and end of each stimulus presentation (600 ms total of silent buffers) to avoid backward and forward masking effects; (3) the 2000 ms volume acquisition time; and (4) an additional 20 ms silent interval, which helped the stimulus computer maintain precise and accurate timing during stimulus presentation. The field of view was 20 cm, and the matrix size was 64 × 64, providing an in- plane spatial resolution of 3.125 mm. Reduction of blurring and signal loss arising from field inhomogeneities was accomplished by the use of an automated high-order shimming method before data acquisition.
fMRI Task
Request a detailed protocolAuditory stimuli were presented in 10 separate runs, each lasting 4 min. One run consisted of 56 trials of mother’s voice, unfamiliar female voices, environmental sounds and catch trials, which were pseudo-randomly ordered within each run. Stimulus presentation order was the same for each subject. Each stimulus lasted 956 msec in duration. Prior to each run, child participants were instructed to play the ‘kitty cat game’ during the fMRI scan. While laying down in the scanner, children were first shown a brief video of a cat and were told that the goal of the cat game was to listen to a variety of sounds, including ‘voices that may be familiar,’ and to push a button on a button box only when they heard kitty cat meows (catch trials). The function of the ‘catch trials’ was to keep the children alert and engaged during stimulus presentation. During each run, four or five exemplars of each stimulus type (i.e. nonsense words samples of mother’s and unfamiliar female voices, environmental sounds), as well as three catch trials, were presented. At the end of each run, the children were shown another engaging video of a cat. Although the button box failed to register responses during data collection in four children with ASD and nine TD children, data analysis of the catch trails for 17 children with ASD and 12 TD children showed similar catch trial accuracies between TD (accuracy = 91%) and ASD groups (accuracy = 89%; two-sample t-test results: t(2) = 0.35, p = 0.73). Across the ten runs, a total of 48 exemplars of each stimulus condition were presented to each subject (i.e. 144 total exemplars produced by each of the three vocal sources, including the child’s mother, unfamiliar female voice #1, and unfamiliar female voice #2). Vocal stimuli were presented to participants in the scanner using Eprime V1.0 (Psychological Software Tools, 2002). Participants wore custom-built headphones designed to reduce the background scanner noise to ∼70 dBA (Abrams et al., 2011; Abrams et al., 2013b). Headphone sound levels were calibrated prior to each data collection session, and all stimuli were presented at a sound level of 75 dBA. Participants were scanned using an event-related design. Auditory stimuli were presented during silent intervals between volume acquisitions to eliminate the effects of scanner noise on auditory discrimination. One stimulus was presented every 3576 ms, and the silent period duration was not jittered. The total silent period between stimulus presentations was 2620 ms, and consisted of a 300 ms silent period, 2000 ms for a volume acquisition, another 300 ms of silence, and a 20 ms silent interval that helped the stimulus computer maintain precise and accurate timing during stimulus presentation.
Functional MRI preprocessing
Request a detailed protocolfMRI data collected in each of the 10 functional runs were subject to the following preprocessing procedures. The first five volumes were not analyzed to allow for signal equilibration. A linear shim correction was applied separately for each slice during reconstruction by using a magnetic field map acquired automatically by the pulse sequence at the beginning of the scan. Translational movement in millimeters (x, y, z) was calculated based on the SPM8 parameters for motion correction of the functional images in each subject. To correct for deviant volumes resulting from spikes in movement, we used a de-spiking procedure. Volumes with movement exceeding 0.5 voxels (1.562 mm) or spikes in global signal exceeding 5% were interpolated using adjacent scans. The majority of volumes repaired occurred in isolation. After the interpolation procedure, images were spatially normalized to standard Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space, resampled to 2 mm isotropic voxels, and smoothed with a 6 mm full-width at half maximum Gaussian kernel.
Movement criteria for inclusion in fMRI analysis
Request a detailed protocolFor inclusion in the fMRI analysis, we required that each functional run had a maximum scan-to-scan movement of < 6 mm and no more than 15% of volumes were corrected in the de-spiking procedure. Moreover, we required that all individual subject data included in the analysis consisted of at least seven functional runs that met our criteria for scan-to-scan movement and percentage of volumes corrected; subjects who had fewer than seven functional runs that met our movement criteria were not included in the data analysis. All 42 participants included in the analysis had at least seven functional runs that met our movement criteria, and the total number of runs included for TD and ASD groups were similar (TD = 192 runs; ASD = 188 runs).
Voxel-wise analysis of fMRI activation
Request a detailed protocolThe goal of this analysis was to identify brain regions that showed differential activity levels in response to mother’s voice, unfamiliar voices, and environmental sounds. Brain activation related to each vocal task condition was first modeled at the individual subject level using boxcar functions with a canonical hemodynamic response function and a temporal derivative to account for voxel-wise latency differences in hemodynamic response. Environmental sounds were not modeled to avoid collinearity, and this stimulus served as the baseline condition. Low-frequency drifts at each voxel were removed using a high-pass filter (0.5 cycles/min) and serial correlations were accounted for by modeling the fMRI time series as a first-degree autoregressive process (Friston et al., 1997). We performed whole-brain ANOVAs to separately investigate unfamiliar and mother’s voice processing: (1) the unfamiliar voice analysis used the factors group (TD and ASD) and auditory condition (unfamiliar voices and environmental sounds) and (2) the mother’s voice analysis used the factors group (TD and ASD) and voice condition (mother's voice and unfamiliar voices). These ANOVAs were designed to test specific hypotheses described in the Introduction. Group-level activation was determined using individual subject contrast images and a second-level analysis of variance. The main contrasts of interest were [mother’s voice – unfamiliar female voices] and [unfamiliar female voices – environmental sounds]. Significant clusters of activation were determined using a voxel-wise statistical height threshold of p < 0.005, with family-wise error corrections for multiple spatial comparisons (p < 0.05; 67 voxels) determined using Monte Carlo simulations (Forman et al., 1995; Ward, 2000) using a custom Matlab script (see Source Code). To examine GLM results in the inferior colliculus and NAc, small subcortical brain structures, we used a small volume correction at p<0.05 with a voxel-wise statistical height threshold of p < 0.005. To determine the robustness of our findings, group comparisons were also performed using more stringent height and extent thresholds (Appendix 1—tables 4–5). To provide estimates of effect sizes within specific regions displayed in Figure 2, t-scores from the whole-brain TD vs. ASD group GLM analysis were averaged within each significant cluster. Effect sizes were then computed as Cohen’s d according to Equation 1 below, where t is the mean t-score within a cluster and N is the sample size:
To define specific cortical regions, we used the Harvard–Oxford probabilistic structural atlas (Smith et al., 2004) with a probability threshold of 25%.
Brain-behavior analysis
Request a detailed protocolRegression analysis was used to examine the relationship between brain responses to unfamiliar and mother’s voice and social communication abilities in children with ASD. Social communication function was assessed using the Social Affect subscore of the ADOS-2 (47). Brain-behavior relationships were examined using analysis of activation levels. A whole-brain, voxel-wise regression analysis was performed in which the relation between fMRI activity and social communication scores was examined using images contrasting [unfamiliar female voices > environmental sounds] and [mother’s vs. unfamiliar female voices]. Significant clusters were determined using a voxel-wise statistical height threshold of p < 0.005, with family-wise error corrections for multiple spatial comparisons (p < 0.05; 67 voxels) determined using Monte Carlo simulations (Forman et al., 1995; Ward, 2000). To determine the robustness of our findings, brain-behavior relations were also examined using more stringent height and extent thresholds (Appendix 1—tables 6–7). To provide estimates of effect sizes within regions displayed in Figure 3, t-scores from the whole-brain ASD Social Communication covariate analysis were averaged within each cluster identified in the GLM analysis. Effect sizes were then computed as Cohen’s f according to Equation 2 below, where t is the mean t-score within a cluster and N is the sample size:
Brain activity levels and prediction of social function
Request a detailed protocolTo examine the robustness and reliability of brain activity levels for predicting social communication scores, we used support vector regression (SVR) to perform a confirmatory cross-validation analysis that employs a machine-learning approach with balanced fourfold cross-validation (CV) combined with linear regression (Cohen et al., 2010). In this analysis, we extracted individual subject activation beta values taken from the [unfamiliar female voices > environmental sounds] and [mother’s voice > unfamiliar female voices] GLM contrasts. For the [unfamiliar female voices > environmental sounds] GLM contrast, GLM betas were extracted from right-hemisphere PP and AI as well as left-hemisphere NAc. For the [mother’s voice > unfamiliar female voices] GLM contrast, GLM betas were extracted from left-hemisphere HG, PP, and AI as well as right-hemisphere mSTS, vmPFC, rACC, and SMA. These values were entered as independent variables in a linear regression analysis with ADOS-2 Social Affect subscores as the dependent variable. r (predicted, observed), a measure of how well the independent variable predicts the dependent variable, was first estimated using a balanced fourfold CV procedure. Data were divided into four folds so that the distributions of dependent and independent variables were balanced across folds. Data were randomly assigned to four folds and the independent and dependent variables tested in one-way ANOVAs, repeating as necessary until both ANOVAs were insignificant in order to guarantee balance across the folds. A linear regression model was built using three folds leaving out the fourth, and this model was then used to predict the data in the left-out fold. This procedure was repeated four times to compute a final r(predicted, observed) representing the correlation between the data predicted by the regression model and the observed data. Finally, the statistical significance of the model was assessed using a nonparametric testing approach. The empirical null distribution of r (predicted, observed) was estimated by generating 1000 surrogate datasets under the null hypothesis that there was no association between changes in ADOS social communication subscore and brain activity levels.
Functional connectivity analysis
Request a detailed protocolWe examined functional connectivity between ROIs using the generalized psychophysiological interaction (gPPI) model (McLaren et al., 2012), with the goal of identifying connectivity between ROIs in response to each task condition as well differences between task conditions (mother’s voice, other voice, environmental sounds). We used the SPM gPPI toolbox for this analysis. gPPI is more sensitive than standard PPI to task context-dependent differences in connectivity (McLaren et al., 2012). Unlike dynamical causal modeling (DCM), gPPI does not use a temporal precedence model (x(t + 1)~x(t)) and therefore makes no claims of causality. The gPPI model is summarized in Equation 3 below:
Briefly, in each participant, the regional timeseries from a seed ROI was deconvolved to uncover quasi-neuronal activity and then multiplied with the task design waveform for each task condition to form condition-specific gPPI interaction terms. These interaction terms are then convolved with the hemodynamic response function (HRF) to form gPPI regressors for each task condition. The final step is a standard general linear model predicting target ROI response after regressing out any direct effects of the activity in the seed ROI. In the equation above, and are the time series in the two brain regions, and contains three columns corresponding to each task condition. The goal of this analysis was to examine connectivity patterns within an extended voice-selective network identified in a previous study of children with ASD (Abrams et al., 2013a). This study showed weak intrinsic connectivity between bilateral voice-selective STS and regions implicated in reward, salience, memory, and affective processing. The rationale for the use of an a priori network is it is an established method of network identification that preempts task and sample-related biases in region-of-interest (ROI) selection. This approach therefore allows for a more generalizable set of results compared to a network defined based on nodes identified using the current sample of children and task conditions. The network used in all connectivity analyses consisted of 16 regions. All cortical ROIs were constructed as 5 mm spheres centered on the coordinates listed in Appendix 1—table 3, while subcortical ROIs were constructed as 2 mm spheres.
Functional connectivity, group classification, and prediction of social function
Request a detailed protocolSupport vector classification (SVC) and regression (SVR) were used to examine whether patterns of connectivity within the extended voice processing network could predict TD vs. ASD group membership and social communication abilities in children with ASD, respectively. First, to examine TD vs. ASD group membership, a linear support vector machine algorithm (C = 1) from the open-source library LIBSVM (http://www.csie.ntu.edu.tw/~cjlin/libsvm/) was used to build classifiers to distinguish children with ASD from TD children during unfamiliar voice processing. Individual subject connectivity matrices (16 × 16 ROIs) taken from the [unfamiliar female voices > environmental sounds] gPPI contrast were used as features to train classifiers in each dataset. Classifier performance was evaluated using a four-fold cross-validation procedure. Specifically, a dataset was randomly partitioned into four folds. Three folds of data (training set) were used to train a classifier, which was then applied to the remaining fold (test set) to predict whether each sample in the test set should be classified as ASD or TD. This procedure was repeated four times with each of the four folds used exactly once as a test set. The average classification accuracy across the four folds (cross-validation accuracy) was used to evaluate the classifier’s performance. To further account for variation due to random data partition, we repeated the same cross-validation procedure 100 times with different random data partitions. Finally, the mean cross-validation accuracies from 100 iterations was reported, and its statistical significance was evaluated using permutation testing (1000 times) by randomly permuting subjects’ labels and repeating the same above procedures. The same SVC methods were used to examine whether connectivity features during mothers voice processing could accurately predict TD vs. ASD group membership, however in this analysis individual subject connectivity matrices (16 × 16 ROIs) taken from the [mother’s voice > unfamiliar female voices] gPPI contrast were used as features to train the classifier.
Finally, SVR was used to examine whether connectivity patterns during unfamiliar female and mother’s voice processing could predict social communication scores in children with ASD. SVR methods are the same as those described in Brain Activity Levels and Prediction of Social Function; however, features in this analysis include multivariate connectivity patterns across the extended voice-selective network (16 ROIs). Given that brain activation results showed that both unfamiliar (Figure 3A) and mother’s voice processing (Figure 3B) explained variance in social communication abilities, we used a combination of connectivity features from both vocal conditions for this analysis. Specifically, connectivity features from both the [unfamiliar female voices > environmental sounds] and [mother’s voice > unfamiliar female voices] gPPI contrasts were entered as independent variables in a linear regression analysis with ADOS-2 Social Affect subscores as the dependent variable.
As a confirmatory analysis, and to examine the robustness of SVC and SVR results, we used GLMnet (http://www-stat.stanford.edu/~tibs/glmnet-matlab), a logistic regression classifier that includes regularization and exploits sparsity in the input matrix, on the same 16 × 16 connectivity matrices described for the SVC and SVR analyses above.
Stimulus design considerations
Request a detailed protocolPrevious studies investigating the processing (DeCasper and Fifer, 1980; Adams and Passman, 1979) and neural bases (Imafuku et al., 2014; Purhonen et al., 2004) of mother’s voice processing have used a design in which one mother’s voice serves as a control voice for another participant. However, due to an important practical limitation, the current study used a design in which all participants heard the same two control voices. While we make every effort to recruit children from a variety of communities in the San Francisco Bay Area, some level of recruitment occurs through contact with specific schools, and in other instances our participants refer their friends to our lab for inclusion in our studies. In these cases, it is a reasonable possibility that our participants may have known other mothers involved in the study, and therefore may be familiar with these mothers’ voices, which would limit the control we were seeking in our control voices. Importantly, HIPPA guidelines are explicit that participant information is confidential, and therefore there would be no way to probe whether a child knows any of the other families involved in the study. Given this practical consideration, we concluded that it would be best to use the same two control voices, which we knew were unfamiliar to the participants, for all participants’ data collection.
Stimulus recording
View detailed protocolRecordings of each mother were made individually while their child was undergoing neuropsychological testing. Mother’s voice stimuli and control voices were recorded in a quiet conference room using a Shure PG27-USB condenser microphone connected to a MacBook Air laptop. The audio signal was digitized at a sampling rate of 44.1 kHz and A/D converted with 16-bit resolution. Mothers were positioned in the conference room to avoid early sound wave reflections from contaminating the recordings. To provide a natural speech context for the recording of each nonsense word, mothers were instructed to repeat three sentences, each of which contained one of the nonsense words, during the recording. The first word of each of these sentence was their child’s name, which was followed by the words ‘that is a,’ followed by one of the three nonsense words. A hypothetical example of a sentence spoken by a mother for the recording was ‘Johnny, that is a keebudieshawlt.’ Prior to beginning the recording, mothers were instructed on how to produce these nonsense words by repeating them to the experimenter until the mothers had reached proficiency. Importantly, mothers were instructed to say these sentences using the tone of voice they would use when speaking with their child during an engaging and enjoyable shared learning experience (e.g. if their child asked them to identify an item at a museum). The vocal recording session resulted in digitized recordings of the mothers repeating each of the three sentences approximately 30 times to ensure multiple high-quality samples of each nonsense word for each mother.
Stimulus post-processing
Request a detailed protocolThe goal of stimulus post-processing was to isolate the three nonsense words from the sentences that each mother spoke during the recording session and normalize them for duration and RMS amplitude for inclusion in the fMRI stimulus presentation protocol and the mother’s voice identification task. First, a digital sound editor (Audacity: http://audacity.sourceforge.net/) was used to isolate each utterance of the three nonsense words from the sentences spoken by each mother. The three best versions of each nonsense word were then selected based on the audio and vocal quality of the utterances (i.e. eliminating versions that were mispronounced, included vocal creak, or were otherwise not ideal exemplars of the nonsense words). These nine nonsense words were then normalized for duration to 956 ms, the mean duration of the nonsense words produced by the unfamiliar female voices, using Praat software similar to previous studies (Abrams et al., 2016; Abrams et al., 2008). A 10 msec linear fade (ramp and damp) was then performed on each stimulus to prevent click-like sounds at the beginning and end of the stimuli, and then stimuli were equated for RMS amplitude. These final stimuli were then evaluated for audibility and clarity to ensure that post-processing manipulations had not introduced any artifacts into the samples. The same process was performed on the control voices and environmental sounds to ensure that all stimuli presented in the fMRI experiment were the same duration and RMS amplitude.
Post-scan mother’s voice identification task
Request a detailed protocolAll participants who participated in the fMRI experiment completed an auditory behavioral test following the fMRI scan. The goal of the Mother’s Voice Identification Task was to determine if the participants could reliably discriminate their mother’s voice from unfamiliar female voices. Participants were seated in a quiet room in front of a laptop computer, and headphones were placed over their ears. In each trial, participants were presented with a recording of a multisyllabic nonsense word spoken by either the participant’s mother or a control mother, and the task was to indicate whether or not their mother spoke the word. The multisyllabic nonsense words used in the behavioral task were the exact same samples used in the fMRI task. Each participant was presented with 54 randomly ordered nonsense words: 18 produced by the subject’s mother and the remaining 36 produced by unfamiliar female voices.
Signal level analysis
Request a detailed protocolGroup mean activation differences for key brain regions identified in the whole-brain univariate analysis were calculated to examine the basis for TD > ASD group differences for both [unfamiliar female voices > environmental sounds] (Figure 2A) and [mother’s voice > unfamiliar female voices] contrasts (Figure 2B). The reason for this analysis is that stimulus differences can result from a number of different factors. For example, both mother’s voice and unfamiliar female voices could elicit reduced activity relative to baseline and significant stimulus differences could be driven by greater negative activation in response to unfamiliar female voices. Significant stimulus differences were inherent to this ROI analysis as they are based on results from the whole-brain GLM analysis (Vul et al., 2009); however, results provide important information regarding the magnitude and sign of results in response to both stimulus conditions. Baseline for this analysis was calculated as the brain response to environmental sounds. The coordinates for the ROIs used in the signal level analysis were based on peaks in TD > ASD group maps for the [unfamiliar female voices > environmental sounds] and [mother’s voice > unfamiliar female voices] contrasts. Cortical ROIs were defined as 5 mm spheres, and subcortical ROIs were 2 mm spheres, centered at the peaks in the TD > ASD group maps for the [unfamiliar female voices > environmental sounds] or [mother’s voice >unfamiliar female voices] contrasts. Signal level was calculated by extracting the β-value from individual subjects’ contrast maps for the [unfamiliar female voices > environmental sounds] and [mother’s voice >environmental sounds] comparisons. The mean β-value within each ROI was computed for both contrasts in all subjects. The group mean β and its standard error for each ROI are plotted in Appendix 1—figure 3.
Appendix 1
Acoustical analysis of mother’s voice samples
We performed acoustical analyses of mother’s voice and unfamiliar voice samples to characterize the physical attributes of the stimuli used for fMRI data collection. The goal of these analyses was to determine if differences between vocal samples collected from mothers of children with ASD and those collected from mothers of TD controls could potentially account for group differences in fMRI activity. Human voices are differentiated according to several acoustical features, including those reflecting the anatomy of the speaker’s vocal tract, such as the pitch and harmonics of speech, and learned aspects of speech production, which include speech rhythm, rate, and emphasis (Bricker and Pruzansky, 1976; Hecker, 1971). Acoustical analysis showed that vocal samples collected from mothers of children with ASD were comparable to those collected from mothers of TD controls measured across multiple spectrotemporal acoustical features (p > 0.10 for all acoustical measures; Figure 1B). An additional goal of the fMRI data analysis was to examine individual differences in social communication abilities in children with ASD, and therefore the next analysis focused on whether acoustical features varied as a function of social communication abilities in children with ASD; there was no relationship between acoustical measures and social communication scores (p > 0.25 for all acoustical measures). Finally, acoustical analyses of the unfamiliar voice samples used in all fMRI sessions were qualitatively similar to vocal samples collected from the mothers of TD controls and children with ASD. Together, these results indicate that there are no systematic differences in the acoustical properties of vocal samples collected from participants’ mothers that could potentially bias the fMRI analysis.
Identification of mother’s voice
To examine whether children who participated in the fMRI study could identify their mother’s voice accurately in the brief vocal samples used in the fMRI experiment, participants performed a mother’s voice identification task. All TD children identified their mother’s voice with a high degree of accuracy (mean accuracy = 97.5%; Figure 1C), indicating that brief (< 1 s) pseudoword speech samples are sufficient for the consistent and accurate identification of mother’s voice in these children. 16 of the 21 children in the ASD sample were also able to identify their mother’s voice with a high degree of accuracy (mean accuracy = 98.2%), however the remaining five children with ASD performed below chance on this task. Group comparison revealed that TD children had greater mother’s voice identification accuracy compared to children with ASD (t(40) = 2.13, p=0.039).
An important question is whether the five children with ASD who performed below chance on the mother’s voice identification task might show a distinct behavioral signature that may help explain why these children were unable to identify their mother’s voice in our identification task. While these children did not present with hearing impairments as noted by parents or neuropsychological assessors, who had performed extensive neuropsychological testing on these children prior to the fMRI scan and mother’s voice identification task, a plausible hypothesis is that the five children who were unable to identify their mother’s voice in the task would show greater social communication deficits, or lower scores on measures of cognitive and language function, and/or reduced brain activation in response to unfamiliar or mother’s voice stimuli. To test this hypothesis, we performed additional analyses to examine whether there are any identifying clinical or cognitive characteristics regarding these five children with low mother's voice identification accuracy.
We first examined differences in social communication scores and measures of cognitive and language abilities between children with ASD with low (N = 5) vs. high (N = 16) mother’s voice identification accuracy. Examining the distribution of ADOS Social scores revealed that the five children with low mother’s voice identification accuracy had a wide range of scores from 7 to 16 (please note that ADOS Social Affect is scored in a range between 0–20, with a score of 0 indicating no social deficit, a score of 7 indicating a more mild social communication deficit, and a score of 16 a more severe deficit). Group results for this measure are plotted in Appendix 1—figure 1A (left-most violin plot) and group comparisons between low (‘Low ID’ in green) and high (‘High ID’ in blue) mother’s voice identification groups using Wilcoxon rank sum tests were not significant for ADOS Social scores (p = 0.83). In a second analysis, we examined whether mother’s voice identification accuracy is related to social communication scores. Results from Pearson’s correlation analysis indicates that mother’s voice identification accuracy is not related to ADOS Social scores (R = 0.13, p = 0.59).
We next examined whether there were any differences in standardized IQ scores (Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (Wechsler, 1999)) for children with low and high mother’s voice identification accuracy, which are plotted below (Appendix 1—figure 1A, three right-most violin plots). Group comparisons between low and high mother’s voice identification groups using Wilcoxon rank sum tests were not significant for any of the IQ measures (p > 0.25 for all three measures, uncorrected for multiple comparisons). We then examined whether there were any differences for children with low vs. high mother’s voice identification accuracy in standardized measures of language abilities, including CTOPP Phonological Awareness (Wagner, 1999) and CELF-4 Core Language, Receptive Language, and Expressive Language standard scores (Semel, 2003) (Appendix 1—figure 1B). Group comparison using Wilcoxon rank sum tests were not significant for any of the language measures (p > 0.05 for all four measures, not corrected for multiple comparisons), however there was a trend for reduced Core Language (p = 0.062) and Expressive Language abilities (p = 0.055) in the low (green) mother’s voice identification group.
Together, results from clinical (i.e., social communication), cognitive, and language measures showed that there are no distinguishing features for the children with below chance mother’s voice identification accuracy compared to children with above chance accuracy.
Activation to unfamiliar female voices in TD children and children with ASD
We identified brain regions that showed increased activation in response to unfamiliar female voices compared to non-vocal environmental sounds separately within TD and ASD groups. This particular comparison has been used in studies examining the cortical basis of general vocal processing in neurotypical adult (Belin et al., 2000) and child listeners (Abrams et al., 2016). The TD child sample showed strong activation in bilateral superior temporal gyrus (STG) and sulcus (STS), amygdala, and right-hemisphere supramarginal gyrus of the inferior parietal lobule (IPL; Appendix 1—figure 2A). Children with ASD, however, showed a reduced activity profile in response to unfamiliar female voices, including a reduced extent of bilateral STG and STG and no difference in activity between unfamiliar female voices and environmental sounds in the amygdala (Appendix 1—figure 2B).
Activation to mother’s voice in TD children
We identified brain regions that showed greater activation in response to mother’s voice compared to unfamiliar female voices separately within the TD and ASD groups. By subtracting out brain activation associated with hearing unfamiliar female voices producing the same nonsense words (i.e., controlling for low-level acoustical features, phoneme and word-level analysis, auditory attention), we estimated brain responses unique to hearing the maternal voice. TD children showed increased activity in a wide range of brain systems, including auditory, voice-selective, reward, social, and visual functions (Appendix 1 —figure 3A). Specifically, mother’s voice elicited greater activation in primary auditory regions, including bilateral inferior colliculus (IC), the primary midbrain nucleus of the ascending auditory systems, and Heschl’s gyrus (HG), which includes primary auditory cortex. Mother’s voice also elicited greater activity in TD children in auditory association cortex in the superior temporal plane, including planum polare and planum temporale, with a slightly increased extent of activation in the right-hemisphere. Additionally, mother’s voice elicited greater activity in a wide extent of bilateral voice-selective STS, extending from the posterior-most aspects of this structure (y = −52) to anterior STS bordering the temporal pole (y = 6). Preference for mother’s voice was also evident in the medial temporal lobe, including left-hemisphere amygdala, a key node of the affective processing system, and bilateral posterior hippocampus, a critical structure for declarative and associative memory. Structures of the mesolimbic reward pathway also showed greater activity for mother’s voice, including bilateral nucleus accumbens and ventral putamen in the ventral striatum, orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), and ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC). Mother’s voice elicited greater activity in a key node of the default-mode network, instantiated in precuneus and posterior cingulate cortex, a brain system involved in processing self-referential thoughts. Preference for mother’s voice was also evident in visual association cortex, including lingual and fusiform gyrus. Next, mother’s voice elicited greater activity in bilateral anterior insula, a key node of the brain’s salience network. Finally, preference for mother’s voice was evident in frontoparietal regions, including right-hemisphere pars opercularis [Brodmann area (BA) 44] and pars triangularis (BA 45) of the inferior frontal gyrus, the angular and supramarginal gyri of inferior parietal lobule (IPL), and supplementary motor cortex.
Activation to mother’s voice in children with ASD
Children with ASD showed a smaller collection of brain regions that were preferentially activated by mother’s voice (Appendix 1—figure 4B). This group did not show a preference for mother’s voice in primary auditory regions, including the IC, and activity in auditory cortex was confined to a small extent of left-hemisphere HG. Preference for mother’s voice was also more limited in both auditory association cortex of the superior temporal plane as well as voice selective STS, particularly in the right hemisphere, where only a focal anterior STS (aSTS) cluster showed increased activity for mother’s voice. Children with ASD also did not show a preference for mother’s voice in medial temporal lobe structures, including both amygdala and hippocampus, as well as structures of the mesolimbic reward pathway, default mode network, and occipital regions. Children with ASD did, however, show increased activation to mother’s voice in bilateral anterior insula of the salience network as well as frontoparietal regions, including left-hemisphere BA 44, bilateral supramarginal gyrus, and left-hemisphere angular gyrus.
fMRI activation and connectivity profiles in children with ASD are not related to mother’s voice identification accuracy
Behavioral results indicated that 5 of the 21 children with ASD had below chance-level accuracy on the mother’s voice identification task (Figure 1C; see Results, Identification of Mother’s Voice). An important question is whether the five children with ASD who performed below chance on the mother’s voice identification task might show a distinct neural signature that may help explain why these children were unable to identify their mother’s voice in our behavioral task. A plausible hypothesis is that the five children who were unable to identify their mother’s voice in the task would show reduced brain activation in response to unfamiliar or mother’s voice stimuli. To test this hypothesis, we performed additional analyses to examine whether there are any identifying neural characteristics regarding these five children with low identification accuracy.
We first examined neural response profiles for the five children with low vs. high mother’s voice identification accuracy by plotting ROI signal levels for the contrasts and regions identified in Figure 3A. First, results showed no group differences between children with low vs. high identification accuracy using Wilcoxon rank sum tests for any of the brain regions associated with the [unfamiliar voices vs. non-social environmental sounds contrast] (Appendix 1—figure 5A; p > 0.35 for all three regions, not corrected for multiple comparisons). We then examined low vs. high identification accuracy using Wilcoxon rank sum tests for the brain regions associated with the [mother’s voice vs. unfamiliar voices contrast] (Figure 3B) and again found no group differences (Appendix 1—fFigure 5B; p > 0.45 for all seven regions, not corrected for multiple comparisons).
We examined whether mother’s voice identification accuracy affected results from ADOS covariate analyses in children with ASD (Figure 3). Therefore, additional regression analyses were performed in which ADOS Social Affect scores were the dependent variable and predictors included mother’s voice identification accuracy and betas from ROIs identified in the [unfamiliar female voice minus environmental sounds] contrast (i.e., Figure 3A) or [mother’s voice minus unfamiliar voices] contrast (i.e., Figure 3B). Separate regression models were computed for each ROI in each vocal contrast. Results showed that all ROI signal levels reported in Figure 3 were significant predictors of social communication scores after regressing out mother’s voice identification accuracy (p ≤ 0.005 for all ROIs).
We then examined whether removing the five children with low mother’s voice identification accuracy would affect group GLM and functional connectivity results. We therefore examined a sub-group comprised of the 16 children with ASD who showed above chance identification accuracy and performed whole-brain TD vs. ASD group comparisons, social communication covariate analysis within the ASD group, and functional connectivity analyses, including SVC and SVR. Results for all analyses were similar to those described previously for the entire ASD group. Specifically, whole-brain TD vs. ASD group differences and social communication covariate results were evident in similar brain regions as those described for the larger ASD group. Functional connectivity results also showed the same pattern of results described for the entire ASD group: SVC results showed that connectivity during unfamiliar voice processing could not classify individuals with ASD from TD children (SVC Accuracy = 50.9%, p = 0.41) while connectivity during mother’s voice processing could classify individuals with ASD from TD children (SVC Accuracy = 66.3%, p = 0.014). Furthermore, SVR results showed that connectivity using combined features from both unfamiliar and mother’s voice processing could classify individuals with ASD from TD children (R = 66.3%, p = 0.003). These results indicate that patterns of brain activity and connectivity in children with ASD in response to vocal stimuli were unrelated to behavioral identification of mother’s voice.
Together, results from neural measures of voice processing showed that there are no distinguishing features for the children with below chance mother’s voice identification accuracy compared to children with above chance accuracy.
Data availability
All fMRI activation maps reported in the manuscript will be made available at NeuroVault (https://neurovault.org/collections/4815/). Full single subject raw data will be made public on the NIH NDAR repository, as per NIH rules (procedure is ongoing).
-
NeuroVaultfMRI activation maps reported in 'Impaired voice processing in reward and salience circuits predicts social communication in children with autism'.
References
-
Right-hemisphere auditory cortex is dominant for coding syllable patterns in speechJournal of Neuroscience 28:3958–3965.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0187-08.2008
-
Inter-subject synchronization of brain responses during natural music listeningEuropean Journal of Neuroscience 37:1458–1469.https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.12173
-
BookDiagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5Washington: American Psychiatric Association.
-
Social intelligence in the normal and autistic brain: an fMRI studyEuropean Journal of Neuroscience 11:1891–1898.https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00621.x
-
Contemporary Issues in Experimental Phonetics295–326, Speaker Recognition, Contemporary Issues in Experimental Phonetics, New York United States, Academic, 10.1016/b978-0-12-437150-7.50015-4.
-
The social motivation theory of autismTrends in Cognitive Sciences 16:231–239.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.02.007
-
Do infants perceive word boundaries? an empirical study of the bootstrapping of lexical acquisitionThe Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 95:1570–1580.https://doi.org/10.1121/1.408544
-
Gaze fixation and the neural circuitry of face processing in autismNature Neuroscience 8:519–526.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1421
-
Comparisons of discrete-trial and normalized behavioral language intervention for young children with autismJournal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 31:315–325.https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1010747303957
-
Aberrant striatal functional connectivity in children with autismBiological Psychiatry 69:847–856.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.10.029
-
Reward circuitry function in autism during face anticipation and outcomesJournal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 42:147–160.https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-011-1221-1
-
Improved assessment of significant activation in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI): use of a cluster-size thresholdMagnetic Resonance in Medicine 33:636–647.https://doi.org/10.1002/mrm.1910330508
-
Abnormal cortical voice processing in autismNature Neuroscience 7:801–802.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1291
-
Spiral-in/out BOLD fMRI for increased SNR and reduced susceptibility artifactsMagnetic Resonance in Medicine 46:515–522.https://doi.org/10.1002/mrm.1222
-
BookEarly Warning Signs of Autism Spectrum Disorder: Division of Birth Defects, National Center on Birth Defects and Developmental Disabilities, Centers for Disease Control and PreventionCenters for Disease Control and Prevention.
-
Speaker RecognitionAn interpretive survey of the literature, Speaker Recognition, ASHA Monographs.
-
Reduced neural habituation in the amygdala and social impairments in autism spectrum disordersThe American Journal of Psychiatry pp. 467–475.https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07101681
-
Young autistic children's listening preferences in regard to speech: a possible characterization of the symptom of social withdrawalJournal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 21:29–42.https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02206995
-
Psychosocial Treatments for Child and Adolescent Disorders: Empirically Based Strategies for Clinical Practice633–657, Child-initiated interactions that are pivotal in intervention for children with autism, Psychosocial Treatments for Child and Adolescent Disorders: Empirically Based Strategies for Clinical Practice, Washington, American Psychological Association.
-
BookPivotal Response Treatments for Autism: Communication, Social, and Academic DevelopmentBaltimore: Brookes Publishing.
-
Early speech perception and later language development: implications for the "Critical Period"Language Learning and Development 1:237–264.https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2005.9671948
-
BookDeveloping trust and perceived effectance in infancyIn: Lipsitt L. P, editors. Advances in Infancy Research. Norwood: Ablex. pp. 101–127.
-
Describing the sensory abnormalities of children and adults with autismJournal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 37:894–910.https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0218-7
-
Autism diagnostic Interview-Revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disordersJournal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 24:659–685.https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02172145
-
The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autismJournal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 30:205–223.https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1005592401947
-
BookAutism Diagnostic Observation ScheduleTorrance: Western Psychological Services.
-
Sensory processing in autism: a review of neurophysiologic findingsPediatric Research 69:48R–54.https://doi.org/10.1203/PDR.0b013e3182130c54
-
The intense world syndrome--an alternative hypothesis for autismFrontiers in Neuroscience 1:77–96.https://doi.org/10.3389/neuro.01.1.1.006.2007
-
Large-scale brain networks and psychopathology: a unifying triple network modelTrends in Cognitive Sciences 15:483–506.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.08.003
-
Contextualized behavioral support in early intervention for children with autism and their familiesJournal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 32:519–533.https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1021298729297
-
Attention, joint attention, and social cognitionCurrent Directions in Psychological Science 16:269–274.https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00518.x
-
Autism Spectrum Disorders: A Transactional Developmental Perspective55–77, Joint attention, social orienting, and nonverbal communication in autism, Autism Spectrum Disorders: A Transactional Developmental Perspective, Baltimore, Paul H. Brookes Publishing Company.
-
BookEducating Children with Autism. Committee on Educational Interventions for Children with AutismWashington: National Academies Press.
-
Research review: constraining heterogeneity: the social brain and its development in autism spectrum disorderJournal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 52:631–644.https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02349.x
-
Cerebral processing of mother's voice compared to unfamiliar voice in 4-month-old infantsInternational Journal of Psychophysiology 52:257–266.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2003.11.003
-
Common and distinct neural features of social and non-social reward processing in autism and social anxiety disorderSocial Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 9:367–377.https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nss146
-
Combining information from multiple sources in the diagnosis of autism spectrum disordersJournal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 45:1094–1103.https://doi.org/10.1097/01.chi.0000227880.42780.0e
-
Temporal voice areas exist in autism spectrum disorder but are dysfunctional for voice identity recognitionSocial Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 11:1812–1822.https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsw089
-
Abnormal ventral temporal cortical activity during face discrimination among individuals with autism and asperger syndromeArchives of General Psychiatry 57:331–340.https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.57.4.331
-
Reward processing in autismAutism Research : Official Journal of the International Society for Autism Research 3:53–67.https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.122
-
Home-based behavioral treatment of young children with autismJournal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 28:15–23.https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1026054701472
-
An implicit technology of generalizationJournal of Applied Behavior Analysis 10:349–367.https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1977.10-349
-
Modification of responsiveness to maternal vocalization in the neonateChild Development 48:563–569.https://doi.org/10.2307/1128654
-
Reconceptualizing functional brain connectivity in autism from a developmental perspectiveFrontiers in Human Neuroscience 7:458.https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00458
-
Reduced functional connectivity within and between 'social' resting state networks in autism spectrum conditionsSocial Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 8:694–701.https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nss053
-
Puzzlingly high correlations in fMRI studies of emotion, personality, and social cognitionPerspectives on Psychological Science 4:274–290.https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01125.x
-
BookSimultaneous Inference for fMRI Data, AFNI 3dDeconvolve DocumentationMedical College of Wisconsin.
-
BookThe Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of IntelligenceSan Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.
-
Multisensory speech perception in children with autism spectrum disordersJournal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 43:2891–2902.https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-013-1836-5
Article and author information
Author details
Funding
National Institute of Mental Health (MH102428)
- Daniel Arthur Abrams
Brain and Behavior Research Foundation (NARSAD Young Investigator Grant)
- Daniel Arthur Abrams
Stanford School of Medicine, Stanford Medicine, Stanford University (UL1TR001085)
- Daniel Arthur Abrams
National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences (UL1TR001085)
- Daniel Arthur Abrams
National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders (DC011095)
- Vinod Menon
National Institute of Mental Health (MH084164)
- Vinod Menon
Singer Family Foundation
- Vinod Menon
Simons Foundation (308939)
- Vinod Menon
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by NIH Grants K01 MH102428 (to DAA), and DC011095 and MH084164 (to VM), a NARSAD Young Investigator Grant from the Brain and Behavior Research Foundation (to DAA), Stanford Child Health Research Institute and the Stanford NIH-NCATS-CTSA (to DAA; UL1 TR001085), the Singer Foundation, and the Simons Foundation/SFARI (308939, VM). We thank all the children and their parents who participated in our study, E Adair and the staff at the Stanford Lucas Center for Imaging for assistance with data collection, S Karraker for assistance with data processing, H Abrams and C Anderson for help with stimulus production, and C Feinstein for helpful discussions.
Ethics
Human subjects: The Stanford University Institutional Review Board approved the study protocol (Protocol # 11849). Parental written informed consent and consent to publish were obtained for all participants, and the child's assent was obtained for all evaluation procedures. Children were paid for their participation in the study. All procedures performed were in accordance with ethical standards set out by the Federal Policy for the Protection of Human Subjects (or 'Common Rule', U.S. Department of Health and Human Services Title 45 DFR 46).
Copyright
© 2019, Abrams et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 2,855
- views
-
- 373
- downloads
-
- 28
- citations
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Neuroscience
Motivation depends on dopamine, but might be modulated by acetylcholine which influences dopamine release in the striatum, and amplifies motivation in animal studies. A corresponding effect in humans would be important clinically, since anticholinergic drugs are frequently used in Parkinson’s disease, a condition that can also disrupt motivation. Reward and dopamine make us more ready to respond, as indexed by reaction times (RT), and move faster, sometimes termed vigour. These effects may be controlled by preparatory processes that can be tracked using electroencephalography (EEG). We measured vigour in a placebo-controlled, double-blinded study of trihexyphenidyl (THP), a muscarinic antagonist, with an incentivised eye movement task and EEG. Participants responded faster and with greater vigour when incentives were high, but THP blunted these motivational effects, suggesting that muscarinic receptors facilitate invigoration by reward. Preparatory EEG build-up (contingent negative variation [CNV]) was strengthened by high incentives and by muscarinic blockade, although THP reduced the incentive effect. The amplitude of preparatory activity predicted both vigour and RT, although over distinct scalp regions; frontal activity predicted vigour, whereas a larger, earlier, central component predicted RT. The incentivisation of RT was partly mediated by the CNV, though vigour was not. Moreover, the CNV mediated the drug’s effect on dampening incentives, suggesting that muscarinic receptors underlie the motivational influence on this preparatory activity. Taken together, these findings show that a muscarinic blocker impairs motivated action in healthy people, and that medial frontal preparatory neural activity mediates this for RT.
-
- Neuroscience
Complex macro-scale patterns of brain activity that emerge during periods of wakeful rest provide insight into the organisation of neural function, how these differentiate individuals based on their traits, and the neural basis of different types of self-generated thoughts. Although brain activity during wakeful rest is valuable for understanding important features of human cognition, its unconstrained nature makes it difficult to disentangle neural features related to personality traits from those related to the thoughts occurring at rest. Our study builds on recent perspectives from work on ongoing conscious thought that highlight the interactions between three brain networks – ventral and dorsal attention networks, as well as the default mode network. We combined measures of personality with state-of-the-art indices of ongoing thoughts at rest and brain imaging analysis and explored whether this ‘tri-partite’ view can provide a framework within which to understand the contribution of states and traits to observed patterns of neural activity at rest. To capture macro-scale relationships between different brain systems, we calculated cortical gradients to describe brain organisation in a low-dimensional space. Our analysis established that for more introverted individuals, regions of the ventral attention network were functionally more aligned to regions of the somatomotor system and the default mode network. At the same time, a pattern of detailed self-generated thought was associated with a decoupling of regions of dorsal attention from regions in the default mode network. Our study, therefore, establishes that interactions between attention systems and the default mode network are important influences on ongoing thought at rest and highlights the value of integrating contemporary perspectives on conscious experience when understanding patterns of brain activity at rest.