Isoform-specific roles for AKT in affective behavior, spatial memory, and extinction related to psychiatric disorders
Abstract
AKT is implicated in neurological disorders. AKT has three isoforms, AKT1/AKT2/AKT3, with brain cell type-specific expression that may differentially influence behavior. Therefore, we examined single Akt isoform, conditional brain-specific Akt1, and double Akt1/3 mutant mice in behaviors relevant to neuropsychiatric disorders. Because sex is a determinant of these disorders but poorly understood, sex was an experimental variable in our design. Our studies revealed AKT isoform- and sex-specific effects on anxiety, spatial and contextual memory, and fear extinction. In Akt1 mutant males, viral-mediated AKT1 restoration in the prefrontal cortex rescued extinction phenotypes. We identified a novel role for AKT2 and overlapping roles for AKT1 and AKT3 in long-term memory. Finally, we found that sex-specific behavior effects were not mediated by AKT expression or activation differences between sexes. These results highlight sex as a biological variable and isoform- or cell type-specific AKT signaling as potential targets for improving treatment of neuropsychiatric disorders.
Introduction
Psychiatric disorders, including schizophrenia, major depressive disorder (MDD), and anxiety, are important public health burdens with large societal and economic costs (Wittchen et al., 2011; Vigo et al., 2016). Genetics is known to play a significant role in the manifestation of psychiatric illnesses (Sullivan et al., 2003; Schumacher et al., 2011; Musci et al., 2019). Numerous studies in humans and experimental model systems have identified genetic variations that can promote abnormal neural function underlying the occurrence of these disorders (Renoir, 2014). Detailed molecular studies have also led to the development of many pharmacological treatments for psychiatric illnesses. However, the effectiveness of available therapies is still limited and many patients remain untreated (Gould et al., 2007; Duman and Voleti, 2012). This may be due in part to lack of information about the specificity of neuromolecular signaling pathways involved in the manifestation of individual behaviors and processes associated with the symptomology of psychiatric disorders as well the specific signaling effects within different neural cell types and those impacted by sex. Consequently, further identification of critical signaling pathways are necessary for developing new therapeutic targets and improving efficacy of existing ones.
The protein kinase B (PKB/AKT) family of serine/threonine kinases is involved in numerous neuromolecular signaling processes (Kandel and Hay, 1999; Franke, 2008) and has been implicated in neurological and psychiatric disorders (Saudou et al., 1998; Ikeda et al., 2004; Griffin et al., 2005). For example, Akt1 haplotypes have been identified in schizophrenic patients (Emamian et al., 2004; Ikeda et al., 2004; Schwab et al., 2005; Xu et al., 2007). Additionally, antidepressants, antipsychotics, and mood stabilizers are known to modify AKT activity (Li et al., 2004; Krishnan et al., 2008; Beaulieu et al., 2009; Li et al., 2010; Park et al., 2014). AKT is expressed as three isoforms termed AKT1/PKBα, AKT2/PKBβ, and AKT3/PKBγ in the brain. Each isoform shows significant homology with one another and across species, being highly conserved in both humans and mice. Despite this homology, the isoforms exhibit different expression patterns in the brain and regulate the expression of synaptic plasticity differently (Levenga et al., 2017). Therefore, it is possible that the different isoforms also contribute differentially to cognitive processes affected in schizophrenia and affective disorders, such as memory formation and extinction. Unfortunately, the overwhelming number of human and animal studies have investigated AKT from a general perspective not delineating between isoforms. A few single AKT isoform studies have been performed with conflicting results (Chang et al., 2016; Wang et al., 2017). Additionally, no study to date has simultaneously examined AKT isoform deficiencies using a comprehensive battery of behavior tests. Finally, the few studies that have examined single AKT isoforms have ignored sex as a variable and, therefore, may have missed important sex-linked effects.
To address these gaps in our knowledge of AKT signaling related to psychiatric disorders, we selectively removed AKT isoforms from the mouse brain using complementary genetic approaches, generating mutant mice with single-isoform deficiencies in AKT1, AKT2, or AKT3; selective loss of AKT1 in forebrain excitatory neurons; or double loss of AKT1 and AKT3 isoforms. With these mice, we performed murine behavioral assays that are models for affective behavior, spatial learning and memory, and associative fear memory and extinction. The behavioral results provide multiple lines of evidence demonstrating specific and overlapping roles for AKT isoforms in the activity of neural circuits that may model brain processes impacted in neuropsychiatric disorders. On balance, AKT1 exerts the strongest individual isoform effects on cognition and behavior, influencing males selectively in many of the tests we performed. Like AKT1, AKT2 affects anxiety-like behavior in a sex-specific fashion and impacts contextual memory. AKT3 exerted no effects on its own but in the background of AKT1 deficiency, enhanced deficits we observed in Akt1 mutants. Therefore, this study provides novel evidence demonstrating AKT isoform-specific regulation of neural function in a sex-specific fashion. Combined with the fact that AKT isoforms are expressed in a cell type-specific fashion, these findings improve understanding of how AKT activity is specifically involved in distinct behaviors and neurobiological processes relevant to diagnoses and therapies directed against AKT signaling pathways for treating psychiatric disorders.
Results
Akt deficiency alters anxiety-like behavior in an isoform- and sex-specific manner
To investigate the isoform-specific contributions of AKT and any sex-specific effects on cognitive processes, we began all behavioral studies of Akt mutant mice with a basic locomotor assessment using the open field arena (OFA) test. Besides locomotor activity, OFA assesses anxiety-like behavior by measuring time spent exploring the periphery or exposed center of the arena (Bouwknecht and Paylor, 2008). OFA testing revealed that male Akt1 KO mice spend significantly less time exploring the center compared with wild-type (WT) littermate controls (Figure 1A; t(42)=4.789, p<0.001), indicating increased anxiety-like behavior, which was not due to differences in overall movement (Figure 1A). In contrast, female Akt1 WT and KO mice were indistinguishable (Figure 1B). No genotype effects were observed in Akt2 or Akt3 mutants for both sexes (Figure 1A,B).

Akt deficiency affects the expression of anxiety-related behavior in an isoform- and sex-specific fashion.
Male and female mice with single-isoform deletions of Akt1, Akt2, or Akt3 were assessed for anxiety-related behaviors in the open field arena (OFA) and elevated plus maze (EPM) tests. (A–B) OFA activity shown as percent time spent exploring the center zone out of total arena time in Akt mutant (A) male and (B) female mice. Akt1 KO male mice show reduced center time compared to their WT controls. (C–D) EPM activity shown as percent time spent in the open arms out of total maze time in Akt mutant (C) male and (D) female mice. Akt1 KO and Akt2 KO male mice show reduced open arm time compared with their respective WT controls. Differences in OFA center or EPM open arm times are not explained by locomotor alterations between KO and WT mice. No differences between female KO and WT mice were observed. *p<0.05. N = Akt1 (WT-M = 22, KO-M = 22, WT-F = 16, KO-F = 13); Akt2 (WT-M = 13–14, KO-M = 15–16, WT-F = 12–13, KO-F = 11); Akt3 (WT-M = 19, KO-M = 23, WT-F = 17–18, KO-F = 18).
-
Figure 1—source data 1
Figure 1 source data.
- https://cdn.elifesciences.org/articles/56630/elife-56630-fig1-data1-v2.xlsx
To complement our OFA studies, we also performed elevated plus maze (EPM) testing, which examines anxiety-like behavior and exploratory drive in rodents (Bouwknecht and Paylor, 2008). Time spent exploring the EPM open (exposed) arms is a measure of anxiolytic-like behavior. Consistent with increased anxiety-like behavior, Akt1 mutants also displayed a male-specific reduction in open arm time compared with controls (Figure 1C; t(42)=2.156, p=0.037), which was not due to differences in distance moved (Figure 1C). EPM testing also revealed decreased open arm time in Akt2 mutant males compared with WT controls (Figure 1C; t(28)=2.651, p=0.013) that was not due to differences in overall movement (Figure 1C), indicating increased anxiety-like behavior. In contrast, Akt1 or Akt2 deletion had no effect on female mice (Figure 1D) while Akt3 deletion had no effect on either sex (Figure 1C,D) in the EPM. Together these OFA and EPM data demonstrate that Akt1 and Akt2 deficiency affect the expression of anxiety-related behaviors in a sex-specific fashion.
AKT1 deficiency leads to a mild spatial memory impairment
We next examined isoform-specific contributions of Akt to spatial learning and memory using the Morris Water Maze (MWM) (Morris, 1984). In this assay, mice are trained to find a hidden escape platform in a pool of water using visual cues. Spatial learning is measured by the escape latency curve over the training period. Cognitive flexibility also can be tested in this assay with reversal training, in which mice are re-trained to find the platform in the quadrant of the maze opposite the original training location. Finally, intact vision, required for this task, is assessed in a visible platform training component. No latency differences in each training component were detected for any Akt mutants compared to WT controls (Figure 2), indicating normal spatial learning, reversal learning, and visual acuity. After MWM training, spatial memory efficacy is evaluated using a probe test in which the escape platform is removed, and time spent in the target quadrant where the platform had been located and frequency of crossing the specific platform location are measured. We found no differences in target quadrant time between Akt1 KO and WT mice (Figure 2A,B; males t(16)=1.187, p>0.05; females t(16)=-.489, p>0.05), but Akt1 KO males displayed reduced target platform crossings compared with controls (Figure 2A; t(16)=2.389, p=0.030), indicating a sex-specific deficit in more precise spatial memory. Probe tests revealed no differences in Akt2 KO (Figure 2C,D) or Akt3 KO mice (Figure 2E,F). Combined, these data suggest a subtle requirement for Akt1 in males during spatial memory formation.

Akt deficiency affects spatial memory formation in an isoform- and sex-specific fashion.
Spatial learning and memory were assessed using the Morris Water Maze (MWM) in male and female mice with single-isoform deletions of Akt1, Akt2, or Akt3. Left graph: Learning measured by latency of mice to escape the MWM during training to locate a hidden escape platform (days 1–8), reversal training to locate the hidden escape platform moved to the opposite quadrant (days 9–11), and visible platform training to locate the escape platform marked by a visual cue (days 12–13). Memory measured by percent time spent in the target quadrant where mice were trained to locate the hidden escape (left bar graph) and number of platform location crossings in the target quadrant (right bar graph) during a probe test. MWM performance in (A) Akt1 KO male, (B) Akt1 KO female, (C) Akt2 KO male, (D) Akt2 KO female, (E) Akt3 KO male, and (F) Akt3 KO female mice. Akt1 KO male mice showed a significant reduction in platform crossings compared with WT controls during the probe test. No other differences were detected between Akt isoform KO and WT mice. *p<0.05. N = Akt1 (WT-M = 9 KO-M = 9, WT-F = 9, KO-F = 9); Akt2 (WT-M = 9, KO-M = 11, WT-F = 11, KO-F = 9); Akt3 (WT-M = 10, KO-M = 11, WT-F = 10, KO-F = 9).
-
Figure 2—source data 1
Figure 2 source data.
- https://cdn.elifesciences.org/articles/56630/elife-56630-fig2-data1-v2.xlsx
Akt deficiency impacts conditioned fear in an isoform- and sex-specific manner
To identify further isoform-specific contributions of AKT and sex-specific effects on cognitive processes, we examined associative fear long-term memory (LTM) in Akt mutant mice conditioned to associate an aversive foot-shock to an initially neutral tone. We found no effects on training performance or contextual and cued LTM evaluated 24 hr post-training in Akt1 KO mice (Figure 3A,B). For Akt2 KO mice, contextual LTM was impaired relative to WT littermates in males (Figure 3C; t(28)=-2.474, p=0.020) but not females (Figure 3D). In Akt3 KO mice, we detected no fear conditioning effects (Figure 3E,F).

Single Akt isoform deficiency does not impact cued fear LTM but Akt2 deficiency affects contextual fear LTM in males.
Associative fear conditioning acquisition and long-term memory (LTM) in Akt isoform mutant male and female mice shown as percent time spent freezing. In training, freezing behavior was assessed during the baseline (pre) and 30 s after the first (CS1) and second (CS2) CS-US presentations. Contextual and cued LTM were assessed 24–25 hr after training by measuring freezing behavior during the entire test for contextual LTM and during the baseline (pre) and CS presentations (CS1, CS2) in the cued LTM test. Performance of (A) Akt1 KO male, (B) Akt1 KO female, (C) Akt2 KO male, (D) Akt2 KO female, (E) Akt3 KO male, and (F) Akt3 KO female mice. Akt2 KO male mice showed impaired contextual LTM compared with WT controls. No other significant differences were detected between Akt isoform KO and WT mice. *p<0.05. N = Akt1 (WT-M = 16–17, KO-M = 13–15, WT-F = 10, KO-F = 10); Akt2 (WT-M = 14, KO-M = 14–16, WT-F = 10–11, KO-F = 10); Akt3 (WT-M = 18, KO-M = 18–20, WT-F = 17, KO-F = 16–17).
-
Figure 3—source data 1
Figure 3 source data.
- https://cdn.elifesciences.org/articles/56630/elife-56630-fig3-data1-v2.xlsx
Because AKT mutations and single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNP)s have been linked to psychiatric disorders like schizophrenia (Emamian et al., 2004; Ikeda et al., 2004; Schwab et al., 2005; Xu et al., 2007) and extinction is altered in the disorder (Craske et al., 2018), we also examined extinction learning in Akt isoform mutants after fear conditioning. Interestingly, Akt1 KO mice showed a male-specific enhancement in extinction learning compared with WT controls (Figure 4A,B; males F(1,30)=8.571, p=0.006). No extinction acquisition differences were observed in Akt2 KO (Figure 4C,D) or Akt3 KO (Figure 4E,F) mice. To determine the efficacy of extinction training in Akt mutant mice, we tested the resulting expression of cued fear memory. Consistent with their more rapid extinction learning, extinction LTM was enhanced specifically in Akt1 KO males compared with WT controls (Figure 4A; F(1,30)=9.121, p=0.005). When we tested renewal of extinguished fear by re-exposing mice to the CS in the original training context, Akt1 KO male mice also showed reduced renewal relative to WT controls (Figure 4A; F(1,30)=7.187, p=0.012). With Akt2 KO mice, we detected no differences in extinction LTM from WT controls (Figure 4C,D), but KO males showed enhanced fear renewal (Figure 4C; F(1,26)=5.969, p=0.022). Akt3 KO mice showed no differences from WT controls in extinction LTM or renewal (Figure 4E,F). These data support a role for Akt1 and Akt2 in associative fear memory processes of males.

AKT1 deficiency enhances fear extinction learning and AKT2 removal enhances fear renewal.
Extinction training to the CS was performed over three days (D1–D3) with 20 CS presentations each day. Extinction learning in mice measured as percent freezing during the baseline period prior to the first CS (pre) and during each CS presentation shown as average percent freezing in 4 blocks of 5 CS presentations each for every day of training. Efficacy of extinction learning assessed by testing extinction LTM (ext LTM) and renewal of extinguished cued LTM. Ext LTM measured as percent freezing to 2 CS presentations in a novel context 24 hr after completion of extinction training. Renewal measured as percent freezing to 2 CS presentations in the original training context 10 d after ext LTM testing. Performance of (A) Akt1 KO male, (B) Akt1 KO female, (C) Akt2 KO male, (D) Akt2 KO female, (E) Akt3 KO male, and (F) Akt3 KO female. Akt1 KO males showed faster rates of extinction compared with controls while Akt2 KO males showed increased renewal freezing compared with controls. No other significant differences were detected between Akt isoform KO and WT mice. *p<0.05. N = Akt1 (WT-M = 17, KO-M = 15, WT-F = 10, KO-F = 10); Akt2 (WT-M = 13–14, KO-M = 15–16, WT-F = 9, KO-F = 9); Akt3 (WT-M = 18, KO-M = 20, WT-F = 15–17, KO-F = 15–16).
-
Figure 4—source data 1
Figure 4 source data.
- https://cdn.elifesciences.org/articles/56630/elife-56630-fig4-data1-v2.xlsx
Restoring AKT1 expression in the PFC rescues fear extinction processes in Akt1 KO males
Because extinction memory relies on the prefrontal cortex (PFC) for normal display (Courtney et al., 1998; Fallgatter, 2001; Bussey et al., 2012), we next tested if restoring AKT1 activity in the PFC might rescue the observed extinction effects in male Akt1 KO mice (Figure 4). To do this, we injected the PFC of male WT and Akt1 KO mice with AAV vectors to express either AKT1-GFP or Cre-GFP as a sham control (Figure 5A). We confirmed AAV expression in both the prelimbic (PL) and infralimbic (IL) regions of the PFC (Figure 5B). Furthermore, we confirmed that expression of AKT1 in the PFC of Akt1 KO males (Akt1 KO+AKT1PFC) is restored in neurons (Figure 5C), both excitatory and inhibitory neurons (Figure 5—figure supplement 1). We found no difference in conditioned fear learning or contextual and cued 24 hr memory in Akt1 KO+AKT1PFC mice compared to sham-treated WT and KO males (Figure 5D). However, we found a significant effect on extinction learning (Figure 5E; F(2,34)=5.400, p=0.009). In agreement with our previous experiment (Figure 4), Akt1 KO-sham mice showed enhanced extinction learning compared with WT-sham mice (p=0.009). Akt1 KO+AKT1PFC males showed a significant difference in extinction learning from Akt1 KO-sham mice (p=0.047) but not from WT-sham mice (p=0.875), consistent with the idea that PFC AKT1 activity underlies the extinction effects we observed in Akt1 KO mice. We also found a significant difference in extinction LTM between Akt1 KO-sham and WT-sham groups (Figure 5E; F(2,34)=4.388, p=0.020, post-hoc comparison p=0.035) but no significant difference between WT-sham and Akt1 KO+AKT1PFC groups (post-hoc comparison p=0.985), supporting the notion that PFC-expressed AKT1 rescued Akt1 KO effects on extinction LTM. Finally, we examined extinction renewal following PFC AKT1 expression. We did not observe a significant difference between WT-sham and Akt1 KO+AKT1PFC groups (Figure 5E; F(2,29)=1.018, p=0.374, post-hoc comparison p=0.605). Unexpectedly, we also did not observe a significant difference between WT-sham and Akt1 KO-sham groups (post-hoc comparison p=0.375), as we did with WT and Akt1 KO mice in the previous experiment (Figure 4). However, the combined data support that idea that AKT1-mediated PFC function underlies effects of Akt1 deficiency on the expression of extinction-related behaviors.

Restoration of AKT1 expression in the PFC normalizes extinction learning and LTM in Akt1 KO males.
(A) Experimental design to test the function of AKT1 in the PFC. AKT1 expression was restored using AAV-mediated pan-neuronal (hSyn) co-expression of AKT1 and green fluorescent protein (GFP) in the PFC of Akt1 KO male mice (KO+AKT1PFC). Sham surgeries were performed on Akt1 KO and WT males using hSyn-driven expression of GFP-tagged Cre recombinase (Cre-GFP) to generate control groups (WT-sham, KO-sham). Mice were injected with AAV in the PL at 1.98 mm AP and then tested on associative fear conditioning and extinction beginning 21 days post-injection. (B) Left: Schematic of AAV expression spread (green areas) in the mouse PFC at 1.70 mm AP overlaid for six subjects (N = 2/group). Right: Representative image of AAV expression. Green, GFP; blue, Hoechst. Scale bar, 500 µm. (C) Higher magnification of PFC corresponding to orange boxed area in (B) right image. Staining for AKT1 (red) in the PFC of KO+AKT1PFC mice confirms that AKT1 expression is restored in neurons and colocalizes with GFP (AAV-infected cells). Scale bars, 40 µm. (D) Sham surgery and AKT1 restoration in Akt1 KO mice did not affect fear acquisition or 24 hr contextual and cued LTM. (E) Extinction learning and LTM were restored to WT levels in KO+AKT1PFC mice but not renewal. KO-sham mice displayed reduced freezing compared with either WT-sham or KO+AKT1PFC groups. *p<0.05. N(WT-sham, KO-sham, KO+AKT1PFC)=15,11,11 except for renewal, due to loss of animals between extinction and renewal test phases: N = 13,11,8. Schematics of the mouse brain are guided by Franklin and Paxinos, 2007. PFC, prefrontal cortex; PL, prelimbic cortex; IL, infralimbic cortex; CC, corpus callosum.
-
Figure 5—source data 1
Figure 5 source data.
- https://cdn.elifesciences.org/articles/56630/elife-56630-fig5-data1-v2.xlsx
Restricted Akt1 deficiency affects contextual fear memory but not fear extinction
To determine if systemic or developmental Akt1 deficiency led to the phenotypes we observed in Akt1 KO males, we generated conditional KO mice with Cre-mediated Akt1 removal (Akt1 cKO) from excitatory neurons of the forebrain, including the amygdala and PFC (Figure 6—figure supplement 1) and hippocampus (Levenga et al., 2017), late in development (Hoeffer et al., 2008). We found no differences in OFA center time (Figure 6A) or EPM open arm time (Figure 6B) between male Akt1 cKO and WT mice, suggesting AKT1 activity in forebrain excitatory neurons or late development is not required for normal anxiogenic-like responses. Additionally, Akt1 cKO males performed similarly in the MWM to WT controls (Figure 6C), suggesting AKT1 activity in forebrain excitatory neurons or late development is not required for spatial memory as well. Conditional Akt1 removal also had no effect on associative fear learning and cued LTM (Figure 6D) or on extinction learning and LTM (Figure 6E), although there was a trend for enhanced fear renewal (Figure 6E; F(1, 22)=3.105, p=0.092). Interestingly, contextual LTM was impaired in Akt1 cKO males (Figure 6D; t(22)=2.535, p=0.019), suggesting AKT1 activity in forebrain excitatory neurons or late development of male mice is required for contextual fear memory. Although we did observe some effect on memory formation, remarkably we did not identify a requirement for excitatory neuron AKT1 activity in the normal expression of many behaviors. Taken together with our results from restoring neuronal AKT1 expression in the PFC (Figure 5), these data suggest that AKT1 activity within interneurons is required for normal extinction learning.

Conditional Akt1 deficiency in excitatory neurons impairs contextual fear LTM but does affect anxiety-related behaviors or fear extinction processes.
Behavior of conditional Akt1 KO (Akt1 cKO) male mice generated by removal of Akt1 with a forebrain excitatory neuron-specific Cre driver in floxed Akt1 mice. (A) OFA activity, measured as percent time spent exploring the center zone, and (B) EPM activity, measured as percent time spent in the open arms, were similar between Akt1 cKO and WT mice. Distance moved during OFA or EPM assays also were similar between genotypes. N = 14 WT, 20 cKO. (C) Akt1 cKO mice and WT controls showed similar latencies to escape the MWM during training, reversal, and visible platform phases and similar probe test performance measured as percent time spent and number of platform location crossings in the target quadrant. N = 8 WT, 8 cKO. (D) Akt1 cKO mice showed impaired contextual fear LTM but no difference in fear learning or cued LTM compared with WT controls. N = 11 WT, 13 cKO. (E) No significant difference in extinction learning, extinction LTM (ext LTM) or renewal was seen between Akt1 cKO and WT mice. N = 11 WT, 13 cKO. *p<0.05.
-
Figure 6—source data 1
Figure 6 source data.
- https://cdn.elifesciences.org/articles/56630/elife-56630-fig6-data1-v2.xlsx
AKT1 and AKT3 function may substitute for each other during memory processes
Because AKT1 and AKT3 are expressed in similar cell types in the brain (Levenga et al., 2017), they may provide compensatory activity in the absence of the other. To test this idea, we generated mice with conditional Akt1 removal in the Akt3 KO background (Akt1 cKO Akt3 KO) (Levenga et al., 2017). When we assessed anxiety-like behavior in Akt1 cKO Akt3 KO mice, we found no performance differences in the OFA (Figure 7A) or EPM (Figure 7B) compared to WT controls for either sex. In the MWM, spatial learning was also indistinguishable between Akt1 cKO Akt3 KO and WT mice for both sexes (Figure 7C,D). However, Akt1 cKO Akt3 KO mice did show significantly reduced target quadrant time and target platform crossings compared with WT littermates for both males (Figure 7C; quadrant time t(20)=2.172, p=0.042; crossings t(20)=3.406, p=0.003) and females (Figure 7D; quadrant time t(22)=3.262, p=0.004; crossings t(22)=2.283, p=0.032), indicating pronounced spatial memory deficits with simultaneous Akt1 and Akt3 removal. Similarly in associative fear conditioning, training performance of Akt1 cKO Akt3 KO mice was normal but contextual LTM was impaired for both males (Figure 7E; t(20)=2.213, p=0.039) and females (Figure 7F; t(23)=2.250, p=0.034) compared with WT controls. Taken together with the results in single Akt isoform KO mice (Figures 2–3), these data suggest that AKT3 substitutes for AKT1 activity in female mice while AKT1 can substitute for AKT3 in both sexes during spatial memory tasks. Furthermore, these findings suggest all three AKT isoforms play a role in contextual fear memory of male mice. Interestingly, Akt1 cKO Akt3 KO males and females also showed deficits in cued LTM (Figure 7E,F; males F(1,20)=5.176, p=0.034; females F(1,22)=4.533, p=0.045), suggesting Akt1 and Akt3 can compensate for each other in cued fear memory in both sexes. These data strongly implicate AKT1 and AKT3 in providing overlapping function in the brain. Importantly, they suggest that female behavior and memory are in fact regulated by AKT1 activity but AKT3 can provide compensatory function in females not available in male mice.

Simultaneous removal of AKT1 and AKT3 isoforms from the brain promotes memory deficits in both male and female mice.
Male and female mice with conditional Akt1 removal in the background of Akt3 deficiency (Akt1 cKO Akt3 KO) were assessed in anxiety-related behavior, spatial memory, and associative fear memory tests. No differences were detected between Akt1 cKO Akt3 KO mutant mice and WT controls for both sexes in (A) OFA activity, measured as percent time spent exploring the center zone, and (B) EPM activity, measured as percent time spent in the open arms. Distance moved during OFA or EPM assays were similar between genotypes for both sexes. N = 15 WT-M, 13 cKO/KO-M, 16 WT-F, 15 cKO/KO-F. (C) Male and (D) female Akt1 cKO Akt3 KO mice had similar latencies to escape the MWM during training, reversal, and visible platform phases compared to their respective WT controls but showed significantly reduced target quadrant time and platform crossings. N = 13 WT-M, 9 cKO/KO-M, 12 WT-F, 12 cKO/KO-F. (E) Male and (F) female Akt1 cKO Akt3 KO mice displayed normal fear learning but impaired contextual and cued LTM compared with their respective WT controls. N = 11 WT-M, 11 cKO/KO-M, 14 WT-F, 10–11 cKO/KO-F. *p<0.05.
-
Figure 7—source data 1
Figure 7 source data.
- https://cdn.elifesciences.org/articles/56630/elife-56630-fig7-data1-v2.xlsx
AKT expression under normal and deficiency conditions are regulated similarly between sexes
Our data support the idea that AKT isoforms play differential roles in behavior, and importantly, these requirements are expressed in a sex-specific fashion. Behavioral results are summarized in Table 1. To determine if the basis for these sex differences were due to inherent variation between sexes or a sex-specific compensatory response to Akt isoform removal, we conducted biochemical analyses of AKT isoform expression between the sexes. To do this, we immunoblotted proteins from the brains of Akt isoform mutant mice and WT controls and measured AKT isoform and activation levels. Comparing WT males and females, we identified higher AKT2 levels in the hippocampus of female mice (Figure 8A; t(19)=3.045; p=0.007). No sex differences in protein levels were detected for the other isoforms or for total AKT levels measured with a pan-AKT antibody that recognizes all AKT isoforms (Figure 8A). When AKT activation was measured between the sexes using isoform-specific and pan-AKT serine 473 phosphorylation (pAKT) antibodies, we also found no differences in AKT1, AKT2, and total AKT activation levels (Figure 8A). These data suggest sex differences in brain expression levels of AKT2 but not the fraction of phosphorylated AKT2 while the other isoforms show similar expression and activation between sexes.
Summary of behavioral impacts of Akt isoform deficiency.
Behavioral assay | Genotype and/or viral expression manipulation | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Akt1 KO | Akt2 KO | Akt3 KO | Akt1 KO + virally expressed PFC AKT1 (male only) | Akt1 cKO (male only) | Akt1 cKO Akt3 KO | |
OFA | reduced center time in males | no effect | no effect | n/a | no effect | no effect |
EPM | reduced open arm time in males | reduced open arm time in males | no effect | n/a | no effect | no effect |
MWM | reduced platform crossings during probe test in males | no effect | no effect | n/a | no effect | reduced quadrant time and platform crossings during probe test in males and females |
Contextual fear LTM | no effect | impaired in males | no effect | no effect | impaired | impaired in males and females |
Cued fear LTM | no effect | no effect | no effect | no effect | no effect | impaired in males and females |
Fear extinction learning | enhanced | no effect | no effect | restored to WT levels | no effect | n/a |
Fear extinction LTM | reduced in males | no effect | no effect | restored to WT levels | no effect | n/a |
Fear extinction renewal | reduced in males | enhanced in males | no effect | no statistical difference from WT | no effect | n/a |

AKT expression under normal and deficiency conditions are regulated similarly between sexes.
(A) Western blot analysis of AKT isoforms individually, detected with isoform-specific AKT antibodies, and all together, detected with pan-AKT antibodies, in the hippocampus of male versus female WT mice. Left: Representative blots. Middle: Total levels of each AKT isoform and pan-AKT normalized by GAPDH levels and shown as percent of immunoreactivity in WT males. Right: Phosphorylated (p) levels of individual and all (pan) AKT isoforms normalized by their respective total levels and shown as percent of immunoreactivity in WT males. No differences between the sexes were detected except higher AKT2 levels in female WT mice compared with males. N = AKT1,2, pan-AKT: 10 WT-M, 11 WT-F; AKT3: 9 WT-M, 10 WT-F. (B–C) Western blot analysis of total (pan-AKT) and phosphorylated (pan-pAKT) AKT levels in the hippocampus of (B) male and (C) female mice with single-isoform deletions of Akt1, Akt2, or Akt3 compared with their respective WT controls. Top: Representative blots. Left: Total AKT levels normalized by GAPDH levels and shown as percent of immunoreactivity in WT controls. Right: Pan-pAKT levels normalized by pan-AKT levels and shown as percent of immunoreactivity in WT controls. Both sexes showed reduced total AKT levels with single Akt isoform deletions and an upregulation of pAKT levels with Akt1 deficiency. N = Akt1: 7 WT-M, 7 KO-M, 8 WT-F, 8 KO-F; Akt2: 7 WT-M, 7 KO-M, 6 WT-F, 6 KO-F; Akt3: 5 WT-M, 5 KO-M, 4 WT-F, 5 KO-F. (D) Western blot analysis of pAKT2 levels normalized by total AKT2 levels in the hippocampus of Akt1 KO male (left graph) and female (right graph) mice compared with their respective WT controls and shown as percent of immunoreactivity in WT controls. Left: Representative blots. Both sexes showed increased pAKT2 levels in response to Akt1 deficiency. N = 7 WT-M, 7 KO-M, 8 WT-F, 8 KO-F. (E) Western blot analysis of pAKT1 levels normalized by total AKT1 levels in the hippocampus of Akt3 KO male (left graph) and female (right graph) mice compared with their respective WT controls and shown as percent of immunoreactivity in WT controls. Left: Representative blots. Both sexes showed increased pAKT1 levels in response to Akt3 deficiency. N = 5 WT-M, 5 KO-M, 8 WT-F, 9 KO-F. *p<0.05.
-
Figure 8—source data 1
Figure 8 source data.
- https://cdn.elifesciences.org/articles/56630/elife-56630-fig8-data1-v2.xlsx
We next measured AKT expression and activation in Akt isoform mutants. Using the pan-AKT antibody, we found reduced total AKT levels in all male Akt isoform-specific mutants compared with WT controls (Figure 8B; Akt1 KO t(12)=3.427, p=0.005; Akt2 KO t(12)=2.282, p=0.042; Akt3 KO t(8)=11.79; p<0.0001), with the greatest reduction in Akt3 KO mice. Interestingly, when we examined AKT activation with the pan-pAKT antibody, we found increased pAKT levels in male Akt1 KO mice compared with WT controls (Figure 8B; t(12)=2.541, p=0.026) but no differences in Akt2 KO or Akt3 KO males (Figure 8B). When we performed the same analyses in female Akt isoform-specific mutants and WT controls, we observed decreased total AKT levels in a pattern like that found in males (Figure 8C; Akt1 KO t(14)=3.507, p=0.004; Akt2 KO t(10)=2.253, p=0.048; Akt3 KO t(7)=11.77, p<0.0001). Similarly, we found increased pan-pAKT levels in female Akt1 KO mice compared with WT controls (Figure 8C; t(14)=3.456, p=0.004) and no differences in Akt2 KO or Akt3 KO females (Figure 8C). These data provide evidence for similar levels of total AKT isoform expression and activation between sexes in response to removing one Akt isoform.
We next sought to determine how AKT isoforms were individually regulated in the Akt mutants. Using the isoform-specific pAKT antibodies to assay AKT isoform activation, we found increased pAKT2 levels in both male and female Akt1 KO mice compared with WT controls (Figure 8D; males t(12)=2.645, p=0.021; females t(14)=2.160, p=0.049), contributing to the higher pan-pAKT levels observed in Akt1 KO mice (Figure 8B,C). In Akt2 KO mice, we found no difference in pAKT1 levels in either males or females (data not shown), but pAKT1 levels were significantly higher in both male and female Akt3 KO mice compared with WT controls (Figure 8E; males t(8)=3.130, p=0.014; females t(15)=3.724, p=0.002). Because Akt3 KO and WT mice showed similar pan-pAKT levels (Figure 8B,C), the increased pAKT1 levels may compensate for the absence of pAKT3 in Akt3 KO mice. However, isoform-specific antibodies for AKT3 phosphorylation are not available, so we did not assess pAKT3 levels. These data demonstrate that male and female mice respond similarly to Akt isoform deficiency by activating the other AKT isoforms.
Single Akt isoform deficiency results in minimal changes to some downstream and related AKT signaling pathways
To investigate the molecular mechanisms underlying the behavioral effects of Akt isoform deficiency that we observed, we performed a candidate pathway analysis of AKT signaling in the brain (Figure 9—figure supplement 1). AKT is known to be regulated upstream by protein 3-phosphoinositide-dependent protein kinase 1 (PDK1), a direct target of several neuronal surface signaling complexes (Hoeffer and Klann, 2010). PDK1 activity is regulated by phosphorylation of serine 241 (Scheid et al., 2005). A critical downstream substrate of AKT believed to play a role in neuropsychiatric disorders and be a target of psychotropic drugs used to treat the disorders is glycogen synthase kinase three beta (GSK3β) (Beaulieu et al., 2004; Beaulieu et al., 2009). AKT phosphorylates GSK3β at serine 9, downregulating its activity (Cross et al., 1994; Cross et al., 1995). The extracellular regulated kinase (ERK) pathway also has been implicated in neurological disorders and neural processes like extinction (Radulovic and Tronson, 2010; Vithayathil et al., 2018; Albert-Gascó et al., 2020) and is a parallel signaling pathway to AKT that can have crosstalk (York et al., 2000; Perkinton et al., 2002; Sutton and Chandler, 2002; Jones et al., 2003). ERK activity is regulated by phosphorylation of threonine 202/tyrosine 204 (ERK1) and threonine 185/tyrosine 187 (ERK2), which could be indirectly affected by AKT activity (Ahn et al., 1990; Payne et al., 1991).
Using western blotting, we examined brain tissue from Akt isoform mutant mice to determine if PDK1, GSK3β, and ERK1/2 signaling were affected in an isoform- or sex-specific fashion. Comparing Akt1 KO and WT samples, we found no significant differences in phosphorylated or total levels of the proteins examined from the hippocampus for either sex (Figure 9A,B; Supplementary file 1). Because AKT1 loss affected extinction (Figure 4), we also examined signaling in the PFC of Akt1 KO mice but found no differences in males (Figure 9C; Supplementary file 1). Akt1 KO females, however, showed reduced pGSK3β levels in the PFC compared with WT controls (Figure 9D; t(12)=3.567; p=0.004). Comparing Akt2 KO and WT hippocampal samples, we found no significant differences in the proteins examined for either sex (Figure 9E,F; Supplementary file 1). In the hippocampus of Akt3 KO mice, we previously found reduced pGSK3β levels in males compared with WT controls (Levenga et al., 2017). In females, we found no difference in pGSK3β levels between Akt3 KO and WT hippocampal samples (Figure 9—figure supplement 2). We also examined if interneuronal expression was different between the sexes. Because we posit that interneuronal AKT1 activity in the PFC is required for extinction learning in males based on our results (Figures 4–6), any sex difference in interneuronal expression in the PFC could contribute to the observed results. Using the interneuronal markers parvalbumin (PV) and glutamate decarboxylase 67 (GAD67), we assessed the number of PV-positive interneurons and the level of GAD67 in the WT mouse PFC from both sexes and found no differences between males and females (Figure 9—figure supplement 3). While certainly not a comprehensive examination of Akt isoform deficiency impacts on signaling in the brain, these results may help to explain the isoform- and sex-specific behavioral effects we observed in Akt mutant mice and suggest additional pathways are involved.

Examination of Akt isoform deficiency effects on neuromolecular signaling pathways.
Upstream signaling of AKT was assessed by measuring phosphorylation levels of phosphoinositide-dependent protein kinase 1 (PDK1) at serine 241. Downstream signaling of AKT was assessed by measuring phosphorylation levels of glycogen synthase kinase three beta (GSK3β) at serine 9. Crosstalk between AKT and the extracellular regulated kinase (ERK) pathway was assessed by measuring ERK1 and ERK2 phosphorylation levels at threonine 204/tyrosine 202 and threonine 185/tyrosine 187, respectively. See Figure 9—figure supplement 1 for schematic of pathways. (A) Western blot analysis of phosphorylated (p) proteins normalized by their respective total levels in the hippocampus (HPC) of Akt1 KO males and shown as percent of immunoreactivity in WT controls. Left: Representative blots. N = 7 WT, 7 KO. (B) Western blot analysis of signaling in the HPC of Akt1 KO females. N = 7 WT, 7 KO. (C) Western blot analysis of signaling in the prefrontal cortex (PFC) of Akt1 KO males. pPDK1, pERK1/2: N = 6 WT, 6 KO; pGSK3β N = 8 WT, 8 KO. (D) Western blot analysis of signaling in the PFC of Akt1 KO females showed reduced pGSK3β levels compared with WT controls. pPDK1, pERK1/2: N = 6 WT, 6 KO; pGSK3β N = 7 WT, 7 KO. (E) Western blot analyses of signaling in the HPC of Akt2 KO males and (F) females compared to their respective WT controls. N = 5 WT-M, 5 KO-M, 5 WT-F, 5 KO-F. *p<0.05.
-
Figure 9—source data 1
Figure 9 source data.
- https://cdn.elifesciences.org/articles/56630/elife-56630-fig9-data1-v2.xlsx
Discussion
We provide evidence for Akt isoform-specific functions underlying cognitive processes for affective behavior, spatial and contextual memory formation, and extinction learning and memory. Our study is the first to comprehensively investigate the behavioral consequences of Akt deficiency in the context of sex-specific effects, brain-specific Akt1 deletion, and simultaneous Akt1 and Akt3 removal from the brain. We find that AKT1 loss more broadly affects neural function, impacting anxiety-related behavior, spatial memory, extinction learning, and renewal in Akt1 KO mice but only the males. This may identify a role for Akt1 outside the brain or during development that preferentially affects males. We also demonstrate that Akt2 is required for the display of normal anxiety-like behavior and contextual fear memory. Although we found no evidence for Akt3-specific roles in the assays we performed, Akt3 removal did enhance phenotypes in the Akt1 mutant background, importantly extending them to females. All behavior results are summarized in Table 1. Finally, we found that the loss of individual AKT isoforms in the brain resulted in increased activation of the other isoforms and minimal changes to upstream PDK1, downstream GSK3β, and crosstalk with ERK signaling, with some sex-specific differences in AKT regulation and signaling pathways. Sex differences in neuropsychiatric disorders are well-recognized but poorly understood and understudied. By identifying sex-specific effects on AKT isoform-specific and redundant activity in cognitive processes, our study provides valuable new insight into how the highly influential AKT signaling pathway may affect diagnosis and treatment of neuropsychiatric disorders.
AKT isoforms and behaviors linked to psychiatric disorders
Examining affective behavior, spatial and contextual memory, and extinction in mice has translational relevance to neuropsychiatric disorders. Anxiety is featured in many psychiatric conditions, including MDD, bipolar disorder (BD), and schizophrenia (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Hippocampus-dependent memory functions such as spatial and episodic memory are impaired in schizophrenia (Achim and Lepage, 2003; Titone et al., 2004; Achim and Lepage, 2005; Ragland et al., 2015). Defects in extinction processes are observed in anxiety, depression, and schizophrenia (Elliott et al., 1997; Boks et al., 2000; Maren and Holmes, 2016; Craske et al., 2018). Extinction is also used in cognitive behavioral therapy to treat fear-associated disorders like PTSD and panic disorders (Powers et al., 2017). Therefore, delineating AKT isoform-relevant information about the neural circuitry and molecular signaling underlying these processes modeled in mice may aid therapeutic strategies.
Our study is the first to report on the effects of Akt deficiency in several murine behaviors and expands previous knowledge on the contribution of AKT isoforms. For affective behaviors, no prior study had examined Akt1 KO mice. An earlier study reported higher anxiogenic-like behaviors in Akt2 KO mice, as we report, but did not examine sex-specific effects (Leibrock et al., 2013). In Akt3 KO mice, one earlier study reported increased anxiogenic-like EPM behavior (Bergeron et al., 2017) while another reported hyperactivity but no anxiety-like behavioral alterations (Howell et al., 2017). The discrepancies may result from using smaller sample sizes than our study, which can cause cohort effects, and mixing sexes. For spatial memory, previous studies had tested Akt1 KO mice in the MWM but did not examine sex-based performance comprehensively and reported conflicting results (Balu et al., 2012; Chang et al., 2016; Wang et al., 2017). While our results support Balu et al., 2012, reasons for discrepancies with the other studies are unclear, but in Chang et al., 2016 the unusually superior MWM performance of WT females, achieving maximal training performance after just one day, makes the apparently impaired KO performance difficult to interpret. Only one prior study examined MWM performance in Akt2 KO mice and found no effects (Leibrock et al., 2013), confirmed by our results for both sexes. In Akt3 KO mice, previous MWM studies had been conducted with two reporting similar findings to ours (Bergeron et al., 2017; Howell et al., 2017) and another reporting a mild learning impairment but normal memory (Wang et al., 2017). For associative fear conditioning, unlike our results, a previous study reported impaired contextual fear memory in Akt1 mutants (Balu et al., 2012) but used far fewer mice than our study and did not consider sex-specific effects. Fear extinction in Akt1 mutants and both fear conditioning and extinction in Akt2 mutants had not been studied previously. A prior study examined fear extinction in Akt3 KO mice using a different protocol but found no effects (Howell et al., 2017), in agreement with our findings. Finally, no prior study had examined the behavioral impacts of conditional AKT1 deficiency or loss of two isoforms simultaneously. Our results suggest that AKT1 and AKT3 have overlapping functions but exert their effects in a sex-specific fashion, perhaps pointing to compensatory activities present in females but not males, whereas AKT2 has unique but also sex-specific functions.
These results are important given our previous work and others showing differential AKT isoform expression in the brain (Zeisel et al., 2015; Levenga et al., 2017; DuBois et al., 2019). AKT1 and AKT3 are expressed in overlapping neuronal populations but only AKT1 is found in interneurons (Levenga et al., 2017) while only AKT3 is found in oligodendrocytes (DuBois et al., 2019). This may explain the synergistic effects of Akt1 and Akt3 removal on memory and why Akt1 removal alone was sufficient to affect anxiety-like behavior and fear extinction: GABAergic mechanisms linked to interneuronal function were shown previously to mediate these behaviors (Delamater et al., 2009; Maren et al., 2013). Indeed, our rescue experiment with viral-mediated AKT1 restoration in the PL and IL regions of the PFC of Akt1 KO mice highlight a critical role for AKT1 in these brain regions for extinction processes (Figure 5). This experiment contrasts with the results of our conditional Akt1 KO studies, where Akt1 removal from excitatory neurons of the forebrain (Figure 6—figure supplement 1) did not affect extinction (Figure 6). Our AKT1 restoration experiment does not distinguish between AKT1 activity in excitatory and inhibitory neurons for rescuing extinction phenotypes in Akt1 KO mice (Figure 4); therefore, the results of our conditional Akt1 removal data suggest that AKT1 activity within interneurons is required for normal extinction learning. Future efforts aimed at refining AKT1 activity using interneuronal restoration of AKT1 expression or interneuron-specific Akt1 removal may help to resolve this question. Another implication of our viral rescue result taken together with our conditional Akt1 removal experiments is that AKT1 activity is unlikely to be required during perinatal development to affect cognition and memory (Figures 5 and 6); AKT1 activity appears to be required during active neurobiological processes rather than developmentally. AKT3 may be able to compensate for AKT1 during development but not post-developmentally in active neurobiological processes like cognition and synaptic plasticity (Figure 6 and Levenga et al., 2017). The behavioral effects of Akt2 deficiency are also interesting because AKT2 is not expressed in neurons but rather astrocytes of the mouse brain (Levenga et al., 2017), and little has been known about astrocyte function in cognitive processes. Because initial learning is normal in the assays we performed (Figures 2, 3 and 6), sensory processing is likely intact in the Akt mutants we studied, but our experiments do not distinguish between consolidation or retrieval processes. Future experiments using reversible temporal control of AKT activity may be used to address these questions.
Human and animal studies linking AKT to psychiatric disorders
Abnormal AKT signaling has emerged as a potential mechanism underlying several psychiatric disorders. Schizophrenia, for example, has been associated with SNPs in AKT1 (Ikeda et al., 2004; Tan et al., 2008; Blasi et al., 2011; Karege et al., 2012) and AKT3 (Psychosis Endophenotypes International Consortium et al., 2014), reduced AKT1 levels in patient brains (Emamian et al., 2004), and pathogenic AKT signaling (Liu et al., 2013). Other studies have failed to find statistically robust links between schizophrenia candidate genes like AKT and the disorder (Loh et al., 2013; Purcell et al., 2014; Farrell et al., 2015; Johnson et al., 2017). However, phenotypic and genetic heterogeneity associated with complex brain disorders like schizophrenia, combined with divergent clinical diagnoses, may impede current genomic studies from capturing AKT contributions. Additionally, post-translational modification of AKT is critical for its activity and highly heritable (Hutz et al., 2011) yet seldom considered in genetic association studies. Functional studies provide strong evidence for AKT involvement in schizophrenia. Magnetic resonance imaging studies have linked AKT signaling and SNPs in the pathway to structural and functional brain abnormalities in schizophrenia patients (Jagannathan et al., 2010; Nicodemus et al., 2010; Wolthusen et al., 2015). Antipsychotics for treating schizophrenia, which antagonize dopamine receptor-2 function, activate AKT and inhibit its substrate GSK3 (Emamian et al., 2004; Alimohamad et al., 2005; Li et al., 2007; Beaulieu et al., 2009). Furthermore, genetic and pharmacological manipulations of the AKT/GSK3 pathway affect dopaminergic signaling and schizophrenia-related behaviors (Beaulieu et al., 2004; Emamian et al., 2004; Gould et al., 2007). Atypical antipsychotics also promote AKT activity (Alimohamad et al., 2005; Li et al., 2007). In other disorders, AKT has been implicated by post-mortem brain tissue from suicide victims showing decreased AKT activity (Hsiung et al., 2003), genetic studies associating AKT1 SNPs with BD (Karege et al., 2010; Karege et al., 2012) and depression (Ellsworth et al., 2013; Pereira et al., 2014), and pharmacological studies showing that lithium, used widely to treat schizophrenia and BP (Chalecka-Franaszek and Chuang, 1999; De Sarno et al., 2002; Beaulieu et al., 2004; Beaulieu et al., 2008; Nciri et al., 2013), and the antidepressant ketamine (Park et al., 2014) induce AKT signaling. Altogether, these studies suggest AKT is a key factor in the manifestation of psychiatric disorders and a key mechanism of action by current treatments. Because these disorders are heterogeneous and available therapies still have limited effectiveness, our data identifying specific and redundant roles for AKT isoforms, combined with the cell-specific expression of AKT in the brain (Levenga et al., 2017; DuBois et al., 2019), will be useful for subdividing these disorders to improve diagnostic measures and develop more targeted treatments.
Sex-specific effects of AKT signaling?
Sex-dependent differences in mental health are well-documented. Women are disproportionately impacted by affective disorders (Rubinow and Schmidt, 2019), with higher rates of MDD (Kessler et al., 1993; Weissman et al., 1993; Kessler et al., 2005) and anxiety disorders (Yonkers et al., 2003; Maeng and Milad, 2015). In men, schizophrenia is more prevalent (Abel et al., 2010) but over the course of normal aging, schizophrenia impacts women at higher rates (Häfner, 2003; Meesters et al., 2012). Sex-dependent effects also are evident in treatment profiles for neuropsychiatric disorders. Studies have found that women are more responsive to available treatments for schizophrenia and BD (Seeman, 2012; Crawford and DeLisi, 2016; Alberich et al., 2019). In MDD, women generally report better treatment effects but also worse outcomes during tricyclic treatment (Keers and Aitchison, 2010). In female mice, ketamine exerts more potent and rapid effects in the forced swim test compared with males (Franceschelli et al., 2015). Only one prior study has examined sex-dependent effects of AKT signaling in a neurological context, showing Akt1 KO female but not male mice were resistant to pentylenetetrazol-induced epileptic effects compared with controls (Chang et al., 2016), which is consistent with the generally increased susceptibility of Akt1 KO males to behavioral alterations in the present study. Sex-specific differences in Akt mutant behavior reported here and previously cannot be explained by AKT expression or activation differences between the sexes. Our data show that with the exception of AKT2 total levels, male and female mice exhibit similar AKT isoform levels (Figure 8A), activation under Akt deficiency conditions (Figure 8B,C), and compensatory responses to AKT isoform loss (Figure 8D,E). Because AKT levels and activation are largely similar between males and females, sex-related differences in behavior are likely defined by sex-related differences in AKT signaling and interacting pathways. To begin to address this idea, we examined candidate signaling upstream, downstream, and parallel of AKT (Figure 9 and Levenga et al., 2017). Interestingly, we found reduced phosphorylation of the AKT substrate GSK3β in the PFC of Akt1 KO females. Why this was not observed in males is of significant future interest. It may indicate that for some downstream AKT targets, such as GSK3β, males may recruit additional signaling to modulate their activities. These additional or compensatory pathways that normalize GSK3β signaling in males may contribute to the behavioral differences observed between male and female Akt1 mutant mice (Figures 1–4). By contrast, pGSK3β levels were reduced in the hippocampus of Akt3 KO male mice but unchanged in females compared with WT controls (Figure 9—figure supplement 2 and Levenga et al., 2017). This may indicate that AKT substrates also are regulated in Akt isoform-specific pools, each with specialized regulation. These findings while very preliminary, do indicate that sex-specific differences in AKT-dependent or -interacting signaling pathways exist. Future investigation using more comprehensive and unbiased methodologies like RNAseq or protein mass spectrometry should help identify these sex-specific signaling differences. Our findings greatly extend understanding of sex-specific AKT isoform effects in cognitive processes, which further supports the need for more specialized diagnoses and treatments. Future studies will be important to determine how AKT is differentially expressed and regulated in the brain between males and females.
Conclusion
Neuropsychiatric disorders are major health and economic concerns, with many afflicted individuals going undiagnosed or misdiagnosed and inadequately treated. Given the links between AKT and neuropsychiatric disorders, more studies like ours are needed to increase understanding of how this potent molecular pathway, with multiple isoforms, impacts behaviors and neurobiological processes modeled for these disorders. Because treatment responses can differ based on sex, newer more effective treatments may be developed if sex-dependent differences in critical neuropsychiatric signaling pathways, such as AKT, are better understood.
Materials and methods
Reagent type (species) or resource | Designation | Source or reference | Identifiers | Additional information |
---|---|---|---|---|
Antibody | anti-AKT1 (Rabbit monoclonal) | Cell Signaling | Cat# 2938; RRID:AB_915788 | WB (1:2000) |
Antibody | anti-AKT1 (Rabbit monoclonal) | Cell Signaling | Cat# 75692; RRID:AB_2716309 | IHC (1:100) |
Antibody | anti-AKT1 phospho-S473 (Rabbit monoclonal) | Cell Signaling | Cat# 9081; RRID:AB_11178946 | WB (1:1000) |
Antibody | anti-AKT2 (Rabbit monoclonal) | Cell Signaling | Cat# 2964; RRID:AB_331162 | WB (1:1000); IHC (1:100) |
Antibody | anti-AKT2 phospho-S474 (Rabbit monoclonal) | Cell Signaling | Cat# 8599; RRID:AB_2630347 | (1:1000) |
Antibody | anti-AKT3 (Mouse monoclonal) | Cell Signaling | Cat# 8018; RRID:AB_10859371 | WB (1:2000) |
Antibody | anti-AKT3 (Rabbit monoclonal) | Cell Signaling | Cat# 14982; RRID:AB_2716311 | IHC (1:100) |
Antibody | anti-pan-AKT (Rabbit monoclonal) | Cell Signaling | Cat# 4685; RRID:AB_2225340 | WB (1:5000) |
Antibody | anti-pan-AKT phospho-S473 (Rabbit monoclonal) | Cell Signaling | Cat# 4058; RRID:AB_331168 | WB (1:2000) |
Antibody | anti-GAPDH (Rabbit monoclonal) | Cell Signaling | Cat# 5174; RRID:AB_10622025 | WB (1:20000) |
Antibody | anti-β-actin (Mouse monoclonal) | Cell Signaling | Cat# 3700; RRID:AB_2242334 | WB (1:20000) |
Antibody | anti-PDK1 phospho-S241 (Rabbit polyclonal) | Cell Signaling | Cat# 3061; RRID:AB_2161919 | WB (1:5000) |
Antibody | anti-PDK1 (Rabbit polyclonal) | Cell Signaling | Cat# 3062; RRID:AB_2236832 | WB (1:1000) |
Antibody | anti-GSK3β phospho-S9 (Rabbit monoclonal) | Cell Signaling | Cat# 5558; RRID:AB_10013750 | WB (1:5000) |
Antibody | anti-GSK3β (Rabbit monoclonal) | Cell Signaling | Cat# 9315; RRID:AB_490890 | WB (1:5000) |
Antibody | anti-ERK1/2 phospho-T202/Y204 (Rabbit polyclonal) | Cell Signaling | Cat# 9101; RRID:AB_331646 | WB (1:2000) |
Antibody | anti-ERK1/2 (Rabbit polyclonal) | Cell Signaling | Cat# 9102; RRID:AB_330744 | WB (1:7500) |
Antibody | anti-NeuN (Mouse monoclonal) | Novus | Cat# NBP1-92693; RRID:AB_11036146 | IHC (1:1000) |
Antibody | anti-GAD67 (Mouse monoclonal) | Millipore | Cat# MAB5406 RRID:AB_2278725 | IHC (1:1500) WB (1:10000) |
Antibody | anti-paravalbumin (Mouse monoclonal) | Millipore | Cat# MAB1572; RRID:AB_2174013 | IHC (1:1000) |
Antibody | anti-rabbit Cy3 (Donkey polyclonal) | Jackson ImmunoResearch | Cat# 711-165-152; RRID:AB_2307443 | (1:250) |
Antibody | anti-mouse IgG2b Alexa 647 (Goat polyclonal) | Invitrogen | Cat# A-21242; RRID:AB_2535811 | (1:500) |
Antibody | anti-mouse IgG1 Alexa 488 (Goat polyclonal) | Invitrogen | Cat# A-21121; RRID:AB_2535764 | (1:500) |
Antibody | anti-mouse HRP (Goat polyclonal) | Promega | Cat# W4021; RRID:AB_430834 | (1:5000–20000) |
Antibody | anti-rabbit HRP (Goat polyclonal) | Promega | Cat# W4011; RRID:AB_430833 | (1:5000–20000) |
Other | Hoechst | Thermo Fisher Scientific | Cat# H3569; RRID:AB_2651133 | (1:3000) |
Genetic reagent (Mus musculus), both sexes | Akt1tm1Mbb, C57BL/6 (Akt1 KO) | Jackson Laboratory | Stock # 004912; RRID:IMSR_JAX:004912 | |
Genetic reagent (Mus musculus), both sexes | Akt2tm1.1Mbb, C57BL/6 (Akt2 KO) | Jackson Laboratory | Stock # 006966; RRID:IMSR_JAX:006966 | |
Genetic reagent (Mus musculus), both sexes | Akt3tm1.3Mbb, C57BL/6 (Akt3 KO) | Easton et al., 2005; PMCID:PMC549378 | MGI Cat# 3804003, RRID:MGI:3804003 | Obtained from Birnbaum lab (UPenn) |
Genetic reagent (Mus musculus), both sexes | Akt1tm2.2Mbb,C57BL/6 (Akt1fl/fl) | Jackson Laboratory | Stock #026474; RRID:IMSR_JAX:026474 | |
Genetic reagent (Mus musculus), females only | Tg(CamkIIa-Cre)T29Stl, C57BL/6 (Camk2a-Cre) | Hoeffer et al., 2008; PMCID:PMC2630531 | MGI Cat# 6273652, RRID:MGI:6273652 | Obtained from Kelleher lab (MIT) before the Tonegawa lab submitted a strain to Jax with same name; not same strain as listed at JAX. Expression properties for this line in Hoeffer et al., 2008. |
Recombinant DNA reagent | hSyn-hAKT1-hSyn-eGFP (AAV) | Vector Biolabs | RRID:SCR_011010 AKT1:Genbank RefSeq# BC000479.2 | Commercially produced custom construct |
Recombinant DNA reagent | hSyn-eGFP-Cre (AAV) | Penn Vector Core | RRID:Addgene 105540 | |
Software, algorithm | IBM SPSS Statistics | IBM Analytics | RRID:SCR_002865 | |
Software, algorithm | Prism | GraphPad | RRID:SCR_002798 | |
Software, algorithm | ImageQuant TL | GE Healthcare | RRID:SCR_014246 | |
Software, algorithm | Icy | Institut Pasteur and France-BioImaging | RRID:SCR_010587 | Open source image processing |
Mice
Single-isoform knockout (KO) mice for Akt1, Akt2, and Akt3 as well as Akt1fl/fl Akt3 KO mice were generated on a C57B/l6 background as previously described (Levenga et al., 2017). To generate mice with conditional removal of Akt1 in forebrain excitatory neurons, we bred Akt1fl/fl or Akt1fl/fl Akt3 KO mice with Akt1fl/+ crossed to Camk2a::Cre mice on a C57Bl/6 background (Hoeffer et al., 2008). Because it would have been logistically prohibitive to generate all Akt mutant combinations from a single progenitor line, Akt mutant strains were maintained and tested separately. Mice from all strains were assessed for general health and sensory and locomotor capacities compared to their wild-type (WT) littermates. Apart from previously reported significant size differences in Akt1 and Akt3 mutant backgrounds (Cho et al., 2001; Easton et al., 2005) and lower than expected Mendelian frequencies for Akt1 KO mice (Cho et al., 2001), KO and WT mice were indistinguishable from each other. We also observed no significant differences in mortality during the testing period. Mice were group-housed in the same facility and maintained on a 12:12 hr light:dark schedule with food and water available ad libitum. Mice of both sexes were used in all experiments and tested over multiple independent cohorts at 3–6 months old. All procedures were approved by the University of Colorado, Boulder’s Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee and conformed to the National Institutes of Health’s Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals.
Behavioral assays
View detailed protocolTimeline: A schematic representation of the experimental timeline is shown in Figure 1—figure supplement 1. For Figures 1–4 and 6, mice were tested first in the OFA, then EPM the next day, followed by either MWM testing over 13 days or associative fear memory and extinction testing over 20 days. For Figure 5, mice were allowed to recover from surgery for 21 days and handled for three consecutive days, 30 min each day, immediately prior to fear conditioning. For Figure 7, mice were tested first in the OFA, then EPM the next day, followed by either MWM testing over 13 days or associative fear memory testing. Mice were acclimated to the testing room for 1 hr before each assay. All studies were performed with experimenters blind to genotype.
OFA: Mice were allowed 10 min to explore a white Plexiglas arena (40 × 40 cm2) with 180 lux overhead lighting and 55 dB white noise present for their entire duration in the testing room. Data were collected and analyzed using the Ethovision XT video tracking system (Noldus, Wageningen, Netherlands), with the center zone defined as the area 10 cm from the arena walls.
EPM: Mice were allowed 5 min to explore a white EPM arena (30 cm arm length) under similar testing conditions to OFA and activity was analyzed with Ethovision XT as previously described (Hoeffer et al., 2013).
MWM: Mice were trained over 8 days as previously described (Wong et al., 2015) to locate a hidden escape platform 2–3 cm below the surface of a pool (112 cm diameter) of opaque water using visual cues outside of the pool. After the probe test on day 8, a reversal phase was introduced on days 9–11 in which mice were similarly trained to locate the hidden platform in the opposite quadrant. On days 12–13, mice were tested for visual acuity using a visible escape platform as previously described (Wong et al., 2015). Data were collected and analyzed using Ethovision XT.
Fear conditioning and extinction: Mice were trained on day one with two pairings of a tone (CS, 30 s, 85 dB white noise) and foot-shock (US, 2 s, 0.5-mA) presentation, and freezing behavior was measured to assess fear acquisition. On day 2, contextual and cued long-term memory (LTM) were assessed by re-exposing mice to the training context (white light, grid floor, and peppermint odor) or to the CS in a novel context, respectively, with no US. Order of testing contextual and cued LTM with 1 hr between tests for each mouse was randomly counterbalanced among animals. Memory was measured as percent time spent freezing during the 5 min contextual test or during the two 30 s CS presentations given 1 min apart in the cued test. On day 7, mice received extinction training in the cued LTM testing environment over three consecutive days with a 35 min session each day consisting of 20 (30 s) CS exposures at varying intervals. On day 10, extinction LTM was assessed like cued LTM in a novel context. On day 20, renewal of conditioned fear was performed by re-exposing mice to the CS in the original training context using the same protocol as for training except no US was delivered. Baseline freezing was also monitored prior to the first CS presentation in any session. Context novelty was generated using alternate lighting (red light), walls and flooring (changeable acrylic inserts with different display patterns or textures), and odorants (vanilla or lavender) compared to the training environment.
Intra-PFC AAV injections
Request a detailed protocolAkt1 WT and KO male mice 3–4 months old were anesthetized with isoflurane and stabilized on a stereotaxic apparatus (Kopf Instruments) for bilateral injections of recombinant AAV vectors into the prefrontal cortex (PFC) at Bregma coordinates 1.98 mm anterioposterior, ±0.5 mm mediolateral, and −2.35 mm dorsoventral. Akt1 KO mice were administered AAV (1 µL at 2.5e12 GC/mL over 10 min) to express either human AKT1-GFP (Vector Biolabs, Malvern, PA) or Cre-GFP (Penn Vector Core, Philadelphia, PA) under the human synapsin I promoter (hSyn) in each hemisphere. WT mice received only AAV-hSyn::Cre-GFP injections. Injectors were removed after an additional 10 min post-AAV infusion. Following standard post-operative care, mice remained in their home cages for 21 d post-surgery to allow AAV expression before fear memory and extinction testing. Following behavior tests, the brains from these mice were fixed and sectioned for microscopy using similar procedures described previously (Levenga et al., 2017) to confirm AAV expression in the PFC by GFP visualization and AKT1 immunostaining.
Western blotting
Request a detailed protocolBrain tissues were isolated from a subset of mice after completion of behavioral testing (Figure 1—figure supplement 1) and blotted using procedures described previously to probe phosphorylated and total levels of AKT isoforms, PDK1, GSK3β, and ERK (Levenga et al., 2017).
Immunohistochemistry
Request a detailed protocolFluorescent immunostaining of fixed brain sections from transcardially perfused mice was performed using similar procedures described previously (Levenga et al., 2017) to visualize expression of AKT1, AKT3, NeuN, PV, and GAD67.
Experimental design and statistical analysis
Request a detailed protocolAll data are presented as the mean ± SEM and were statistically evaluated using SPSS (IBM Corporation). Data were analyzed for each sex by Student’s t-test, one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) or one-way repeated-measures ANOVA where appropriate, with genotype as the between-subjects factor (Supplementary file 1). Homogeneity of variance was assessed using Levene’s test. Outliers were excluded based on Grubbs’ method, experimental criteria, or equipment errors (Supplementary file 2). Akt isoform mutants were maintained in separate lines and were not tested together, so Akt isoform experimental groups were compared only with their respective WT littermates and not analyzed together. Because male and female mice were tested separately, we also did not combine sexes for statistical analyses. However, we have included two-way ANOVA with sex and genotype as independent variables for each Akt mutant line (Supplementary file 3) showing that our findings using either statistical approach are largely unchanged. Experiments were designed based on power analyses derived from previously conducted similar experiments (Hoeffer et al., 2008; Hoeffer et al., 2013; Wong et al., 2015). All behavior experiments used a minimum of three independent litters derived from separate dams. Significant ANOVA results were followed by Tukey’s HSD test for multiple group comparisons where appropriate. All statistical tests were two-tailed with p<0.05 considered significant.
Data availability
All data generated or analyzed during this study are included in the manuscript and supporting files. Source data files have been provided for Figures 1-9.
References
-
Sex differences in schizophreniaInternational Review of Psychiatry 22:417–428.https://doi.org/10.3109/09540261.2010.515205
-
Episodic memory-related activation in schizophrenia: meta-analysisBritish Journal of Psychiatry 187:500–509.https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.187.6.500
-
Identification of multiple epidermal growth factor-stimulated protein serine/threonine kinases from swiss 3t3 cellsThe Journal of Biological Chemistry 265:11487–11494.
-
MAP/ERK signaling in developing cognitive and emotional function and its effect on pathological and neurodegenerative processesInternational Journal of Molecular Sciences 21:4471.https://doi.org/10.3390/ijms21124471
-
BookDiagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental DisordersAmerican Psychiatric Association.
-
Akt/GSK3 signaling in the action of psychotropic drugsAnnual Review of Pharmacology and Toxicology 49:327–347.https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.pharmtox.011008.145634
-
Genetic deletion of Akt3 induces an endophenotype reminiscent of psychiatric manifestations in miceFrontiers in Molecular Neuroscience 10:102.https://doi.org/10.3389/fnmol.2017.00102
-
The specificity of neurological signs in schizophrenia: a reviewSchizophrenia Research 43:109–116.https://doi.org/10.1016/S0920-9964(99)00145-0
-
Akt1/PKBα is required for normal growth but dispensable for maintenance of glucose homeostasis in miceJournal of Biological Chemistry 276:38349–38352.https://doi.org/10.1074/jbc.C100462200
-
The role of prefrontal cortex in working memory: examining the contents of consciousnessPhilosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences 353:1819–1828.https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.1998.0334
-
State-of-the-art and future directions for extinction as a translational model for fear and anxietyPhilosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 373:20180432.https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2018.0432
-
Issues related to sex differences in antipsychotic treatmentCurrent Opinion in Psychiatry 29:211–217.https://doi.org/10.1097/YCO.0000000000000243
-
The inhibition of glycogen synthase kinase-3 by insulin or insulin-like growth factor 1 in the rat skeletal muscle cell line L6 is blocked by wortmannin, but not by rapamycin: evidence that wortmannin blocks activation of the mitogen-activated protein kinase pathway in L6 cells between ras and rafBiochemical Journal 303:21–26.https://doi.org/10.1042/bj3030021
-
Renewal and spontaneous recovery, but not latent inhibition, are mediated by gamma-aminobutyric acid in appetitive conditioningJournal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 35:224–237.https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013293
-
Akt3-Mediated protection against inflammatory demyelinating diseaseFrontiers in Immunology 10:1738.https://doi.org/10.3389/fimmu.2019.01738
-
Role for Akt3/Protein kinase bγ in attainment of normal brain sizeMolecular and Cellular Biology 25:1869–1878.https://doi.org/10.1128/MCB.25.5.1869-1878.2005
-
Convergent evidence for impaired AKT1-GSK3beta signaling in schizophreniaNature Genetics 36:131–137.https://doi.org/10.1038/ng1296
-
Electrophysiology of the prefrontal cortex in healthy controls and schizophrenic patients: a reviewJournal of Neural Transmission 108:679–694.https://doi.org/10.1007/s007020170045
-
Evaluating historical candidate genes for schizophreniaMolecular Psychiatry 20:555–562.https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2015.16
-
Intracellular signaling by akt: bound to be specificScience Signaling 1:pe29.https://doi.org/10.1126/scisignal.124pe29
-
Beta-catenin overexpression in the mouse brain phenocopies lithium-sensitive behaviorsNeuropsychopharmacology 32:2173–2183.https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.npp.1301338
-
Gender differences in schizophreniaPsychoneuroendocrinology 28 Suppl 2:17–54.https://doi.org/10.1016/S0306-4530(02)00125-7
-
mTOR signaling: at the crossroads of plasticity, memory and diseaseTrends in Neurosciences 33:67–75.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2009.11.003
-
Association of AKT1 with schizophrenia confirmed in a japanese populationBiological Psychiatry 56:698–700.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.07.023
-
The regulation and activities of the multifunctional serine/threonine kinase akt/PKBExperimental Cell Research 253:210–229.https://doi.org/10.1006/excr.1999.4690
-
Gender differences in antidepressant drug responseInternational Review of Psychiatry 22:485–500.https://doi.org/10.3109/09540261.2010.496448
-
Sex and depression in the national comorbidity survey I: lifetime prevalence, chronicity and recurrenceJournal of Affective Disorders 29:85–96.https://doi.org/10.1016/0165-0327(93)90026-G
-
Prevalence, severity, and comorbidity of 12-month DSM-IV disorders in the national comorbidity survey replicationArchives of General Psychiatry 62:617–627.https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.617
-
Akt2 deficiency is associated with anxiety and depressive behavior in miceCellular Physiology and Biochemistry 32:766–777.https://doi.org/10.1159/000354478
-
In vivo regulation of glycogen synthase kinase-3beta (GSK3beta) by serotonergic activity in mouse brainNeuropsychopharmacology 29:1426–1431.https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.npp.1300439
-
Regulation of mouse brain glycogen synthase kinase-3 by atypical antipsychoticsThe International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology 10:7–19.https://doi.org/10.1017/S1461145706006547
-
The contextual brain: implications for fear conditioning, extinction and psychopathologyNature Reviews Neuroscience 14:417–428.https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3492
-
Schizophrenia spectrum disorders in later life: prevalence and distribution of age at onset and sex in a dutch catchment areaThe American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 20:18–28.https://doi.org/10.1097/JGP.0b013e3182011b7f
-
Developments of a water-maze procedure for studying spatial learning in the ratJournal of Neuroscience Methods 11:47–60.https://doi.org/10.1016/0165-0270(84)90007-4
-
Gene-Environment interactions in psychiatry: recent evidence and clinical implicationsCurrent Psychiatry Reports 21:81.https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-019-1065-5
-
Neuroprotective effects of chronic exposure of SH-SY5Y to low lithium concentration involve glycolysis stimulation, extracellular pyruvate accumulation and resistance to oxidative stressInternational Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology 16:365–376.https://doi.org/10.1017/S1461145712000132
-
Differential effects of antidepressant drugs on mTOR signalling in rat hippocampal neuronsThe International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology 17:1831–1846.https://doi.org/10.1017/S1461145714000534
-
Genetic variant of AKT1 and AKTIP associated with late-onset depression in a brazilian populationInternational Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 29:399–405.https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.4018
-
Core mechanisms of cognitive behavioral therapy for anxiety and depressionPsychiatric Clinics of North America 40:611–623.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psc.2017.08.010
-
Molecular specificity of multiple hippocampal processes governing fear extinctionReviews in the Neurosciences 21:1–18.https://doi.org/10.1515/REVNEURO.2010.21.1.1
-
New frontiers in the neuropsychopharmacology of mental illnessFrontiers in Pharmacology 5:212.https://doi.org/10.3389/fphar.2014.00212
-
Sex differences and the neurobiology of affective disordersNeuropsychopharmacology 44:111–128.https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-018-0148-z
-
Phosphoinositide-dependent phosphorylation of PDK1 regulates nuclear translocationMolecular and Cellular Biology 25:2347–2363.https://doi.org/10.1128/MCB.25.6.2347-2363.2005
-
The genetics of panic disorderJournal of Medical Genetics 48:361–368.https://doi.org/10.1136/jmg.2010.086876
-
Schizophrenia as a complex trait: evidence from a meta-analysis of twin studiesArchives of General Psychiatry 60:1187–1192.https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.60.12.1187
-
Genetic variation in AKT1 is linked to dopamine-associated prefrontal cortical structure and function in humansJournal of Clinical Investigation 118:2200–2208.https://doi.org/10.1172/JCI34725
-
Transitive inference in schizophrenia: impairments in relational memory organizationSchizophrenia Research 68:235–247.https://doi.org/10.1016/S0920-9964(03)00152-X
-
Estimating the true global burden of mental illnessThe Lancet Psychiatry 3:171–178.https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(15)00505-2
-
ERK/MAPK signaling and autism spectrum disordersProgress in Brain Research 241:63–112.https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.pbr.2018.09.008
-
Impaired spatial learning is associated with disrupted integrity of the white matter in Akt3 knockout miceCNS Neuroscience & Therapeutics 23:99–102.https://doi.org/10.1111/cns.12647
-
Sex differences in rates of depression: cross-national perspectivesJournal of Affective Disorders 29:77–84.https://doi.org/10.1016/0165-0327(93)90025-F
-
The size and burden of mental disorders and other disorders of the brain in Europe 2010European Neuropsychopharmacology 21:655–679.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2011.07.018
-
Genetic underpinnings of left superior temporal gyrus thickness in patients with schizophreniaThe World Journal of Biological Psychiatry 1:430–440.https://doi.org/10.3109/15622975.2015.1062915
-
Association of AKT1 gene polymorphisms with risk of schizophrenia and with response to antipsychotics in the chinese populationThe Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 68:1358–1367.https://doi.org/10.4088/jcp.v68n0906
Decision letter
-
Mary Kay LoboReviewing Editor; University of Maryland, United States
-
Kate M WassumSenior Editor; University of California, Los Angeles, United States
In the interests of transparency, eLife publishes the most substantive revision requests and the accompanying author responses.
Acceptance summary:
The role of molecular isoforms, highly similar molecules, in behaviors relevant to psychiatric disorders are not well elucidated. Using a series of genetic and behavioral approaches this research uncovers a new role for sex-specific contributions of molecular isoforms in distinct behaviors related to memory, anxiety, or fear. These findings have implications for understanding molecular mechanisms relevant to biological sex that occur in neuropsychiatric related behaviors.
Decision letter after peer review:
Thank you for submitting your article "Isoform-specific roles for AKT in affective behavior, spatial memory, and extinction related to psychiatric disorders" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by three peer reviewers, and the evaluation has been overseen by a Reviewing Editor and Kate Wassum as the Senior Editor. The reviewers have opted to remain anonymous.
The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.
As the editors have judged that your manuscript is of interest, but as described below that additional experiments are required before it is published, we would like to draw your attention to changes in our revision policy that we have made in response to COVID-19 (https://elifesciences.org/articles/57162). First, because many researchers have temporarily lost access to the labs, we will give authors as much time as they need to submit revised manuscripts. We are also offering, if you choose, to post the manuscript to bioRxiv (if it is not already there) along with this decision letter and a formal designation that the manuscript is 'in revision at eLife'. Please let us know if you would like to pursue this option. (If your work is more suitable for medRxiv, you will need to post the preprint yourself, as the mechanisms for us to do so are still in development.)
Summary:
This study examines AKT isoform-dependent effects on behaviors that are relevant to complex brain diseases. The study reveals differential selection of AKT1, AKT2, and AKT3 isoforms as a novel mechanism underlying anxiety, memory formation, and extinction. The authors further demonstrate that different AKT isoforms can compensate for each other, partially explaining the presence of very mild or null phenotypes. Overall, the manuscript expands upon the current limited knowledge of AKT signaling in maladaptive behaviors.
Essential revisions:
1) A concern with this study is that the results do not present a coherent conclusion, thus, it is not clear what is learned from this study. Please reshape the Introduction to clearly motivate the rationale to study these specific isoforms of AKT with genetic deletion, and what relevance this has for specific psychiatric disease and why the behavioral test performed are relevant. Additional comments below should also be integrated into this revised Introduction.
2) In the Introduction, and some parts of the Discussion, the authors indicate that AKT is associated with MDD, and that antidepressant medications, among other psychotropic agents, modify AKT signaling. Yet, the authors provide only one reference that supports such statement for antipsychotics only (Beaulieu et al., 2008). Please reference other sources to fully support such statements across the paper, particularly for antidepressants in human subjects (for example, Krishnan et al., 2008).
3) The NIH released a statement on the use of animals for research on psychiatric illness (https://grants.nih.gov/grants/guide/notice-files/NOT-MH-19-053.html). The statement emphasizes the importance of wording (including "of" versus "for") in descriptions and interpretations of the types of studies described in the present article. It would seem critical to ensure that the conceptualizations of genetically altered mice, and the behaviors they exhibit when compared to wildtypes, are made in the context of "modeling" endpoints that are suggestive of symptoms that underlie human psychiatric illnesses. Thus, it is imperative that the assertions in the present article are consistent with this NIH statement. As one example, the insinuation that the behaviors described (e.g., OFA, EPM, MWM, fear conditioning/extinction) can accurately assess neuropsychiatric behavior in a mouse (i.e., last paragraph of Introduction) may be viewed as increasingly unacceptable. Therefore, please revise to describe these tests in terms of their objective endpoints.
4) Following the previous point, at the end of the Introduction, the authors indicate that their "findings have significant implications for diagnoses and therapies." Please describe how this statement is consistent with the NIH considerations regarding the use of animal neurobehavioral approaches in basic studies.
5) One of prominent phenotypes in the study is a male-specific enhanced fear extinction in AKT1 KO. This can also be reversed by expressing AKT1 in the PFC. However, one of the main brain parts responsible for fear extinction is the amygdala and there are studies showing the connection between the PFC and amygdala may also play role in fear extinction. In addition, in Figure 6, conditional KO of AKT1 show normal extinction, which is possibly due to the fact that the cre line used in the study has not reported to have expression in the amygdala. This suggests AKT1 in the amygdala plays a role in fear extinction. Please examine whether AKT1 expression in the amygdala of KO mice can reverse fear extinction, or test whether specific loss of AKT1 in the amygdala (by viral shRNA knockdown in WT mice) has any effects on behavior.
6) The authors inject AKT1 into the Prelimbic cortex (PL), which is sufficient to reverse altered fear extinction in KO mice. Importantly, several studies show that the connection between the amygdala and PL mediates fear acquisition and retrieval while the link between the amygdala and infralimbic cortex (IL) are thought to mediate fear extinction (Choi et al., PNAS. 2010. and Laurent et al., Learning and Memory. 2009). Please explain how AKT1 expression in the PL can reverse fear extinction or examine the effects of AKT1 expression in the IL on fear extinction in KO animals.
Please also include a representative example of expression with sufficient display of anatomical landmarks to indicate that expression was restricted to the PL, as well as a schematic of expression spread overlaid for each subject so readers can see the extent of expression for the entire cohort.
7) Across the manuscript, the authors indicate that their findings are "sex-dependent." This is technically inaccurate, given that sex was not incorporated as an independent variable in their statistical analyses. This is particularly problematic in the Discussion section of the paper, since the experimental design does not support such inferences. A similar problem is exhibited for the arguments made across the different AKT isoforms (AKT1, AKT2, AKT3), since "isoform" was also not incorporated as an independent variable. The data should be re-analyzed using a 2-way ANOVA with "sex" and "AKT isoform" as sources of variance. Please also include a discussion on potential sex-dependent downstream players.
8) In the Experimental Design and Statistical analysis section of the paper, the authors indicate that outliers were excluded from the study. The specific number of excluded animals, across each experiment, should be clearly indicated. Also, the current manuscript only includes group numbers. The total number (n) of animals per experimental group needs to be included (a table depicting this information would be helpful, or adding the "n" within each group in the data figures). This is particularly important, given that the degrees of freedom change across experiments (this is confusing, since the authors indicate that mice were used across all experiments).
Because the numbers of animals differ considerable between some of the groups, please also show that the data meet the assumption of homogeneity of variance for ANOVA.
9) In Figure 6, please confirm loss of AKT1 expression by immunohistochemistry to examine the brain regions with AKT1 loss of expression.
10) Given the complexity and length of the behavioral tests, it would be very useful if the authors included a schematic representation of the experimental timeline, depicting the multiple groups of AKT mutants, the rescue experiment, and the timeline of the behavioral tests.
11) While the authors use a genetically targeted approach to study AKT isoforms, these molecules cross-talk with many signaling pathways. Therefore, it is very likely that multiple pathways are de-regulated by AKT KO. Assessing activation of other cross-talk signaling molecules (PI3-kinase, GSK3b, ERK, or CREB) across the multiple AKT mutants (Western blotting) would provide a better understanding of the role that each AKT isoform plays in the AKT-dependent behavioral modifications.
12) Some of the AKT KO mouse lines have previously been shown to be smaller and display growth deficiency (i.e., Dummler et al., 2006-DOI: 10.1128/MCB.00722-06; Cho et al., 2001-DOI: 10.1074/jbc.C100462200). Are the same lines used in the current study. Are the mice smaller? Did the authors check the general health state of the mice? It is suggested is to add a short paragraph in the manuscript that clarifies this point.
[Editors' note: further revisions were suggested prior to acceptance, as described below.]
Thank you for resubmitting your work entitled "Isoform-specific roles for AKT in affective behavior, spatial memory, and extinction related to psychiatric disorders" for further consideration by eLife. Your revised article has been evaluated by Kate Wassum (Senior Editor) and a Reviewing Editor.
The manuscript has been improved but there are some remaining issues that need to be addressed before acceptance, as outlined below:
1) The authors conclude that AKT1 in PFC interneurons is important for fear extinction. They showed that virally expressed AKT1 in the PFC reversed enhanced fear extinction in KO mice (Figure 5). However, the study lacks confirmation of AKT1 expression in interneurons. The authors are able to show double labeling of PV and AKT1 in the PFC in Figure 6—figure supplement 1. Thus, the authors should be able to perform similar labeling to demonstrate that virally expressed AKT1 is present in PFC interneurons in Figure 5C to confirm that AKT1 is indeed expressed in PFC interneurons of KO mice.
2) Describing sex as an "experimental variable" throughout the manuscripts is inaccurate. The authors are to be commended for including both males and females in their work, and indeed, the inclusion of both sexes is extremely important. But not including sex as an independent variable in their statistical analyses (ANOVAs), precludes the authors from suggesting that sex was an experimental variable (this is misleading, and technically incorrect). There is agreement with the justification provided by the authors for not including sex or isoform as a variable in their statistical approaches. For this reason, it is encouraged that the authors remove any statement indicating that their findings are isoform or sex "dependent", and simply rephrase them as sex and isoform-"specific" (because data is compared to a same sex control).
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.56630.sa1Author response
Essential revisions:
1) A concern with this study is that the results do not present a coherent conclusion, thus, it is not clear what is learned from this study. Please reshape the Introduction to clearly motivate the rationale to study these specific isoforms of AKT with genetic deletion, and what relevance this has for specific psychiatric disease and why the behavioral test performed are relevant. Additional comments below should also be integrated into this revised Introduction.
To address the reviewers’ concerns on this important point we have revised the Introduction as well as the Significance Statement to reflect more clearly the goals and knowledge gaps that motivated these studies. AKT has been implicated in neuropsychiatric disorders from human studies but it is a central kinase in many cellular signaling pathways, so understanding the specificity of AKT activity will be important for improving diagnosis and therapeutic interventions. Our study investigated the isoform- and sex-specific functions of AKT in mouse behaviors with underlying neural circuits and activity that may model brain processes impacted in neuropsychiatric disorders. The use of genetic deletion is a standard approach for understanding the contributions of a gene toward a process of interest. While there are other approaches (e.g. gain of function, expression of allelic variants), these approaches are not currently available for mouse model work without additional reagent generation. We did use the approach of conditional cell type-specific Akt isoform removal and viral-mediated gene expression to extend our results beyond null characterization and feel that these studies are very informative above and beyond what is known about AKT function in the context of behavior and how these behaviors may relate to human neuropsychiatric disorder.
2) In the Introduction, and some parts of the Discussion, the authors indicate that AKT is associated with MDD, and that antidepressant medications, among other psychotropic agents, modify AKT signaling. Yet, the authors provide only one reference that supports such statement for antipsychotics only (Beaulieu et al., 2008). Please reference other sources to fully support such statements across the paper, particularly for antidepressants in human subjects (for example, Krishnan et al., 2008).
As suggested by the reviewers, we have included more references for AKT’s involvement in psychotropic agents, including antidepressants. These additional references add to the growing significance of the AKT signaling pathway in psychiatric disorders.
3) The NIH released a statement on the use of animals for research on psychiatric illness (https://grants.nih.gov/grants/guide/notice-files/NOT-MH-19-053.html). The statement emphasizes the importance of wording (including "of" versus "for") in descriptions and interpretations of the types of studies described in the present article. It would seem critical to ensure that the conceptualizations of genetically altered mice, and the behaviors they exhibit when compared to wildtypes, are made in the context of "modeling" endpoints that are suggestive of symptoms that underlie human psychiatric illnesses. Thus, it is imperative that the assertions in the present article are consistent with this NIH statement. As one example, the insinuation that the behaviors described (e.g., OFA, EPM, MWM, fear conditioning/extinction) can accurately assess neuropsychiatric behavior in a mouse (i.e., last paragraph of Introduction) may be viewed as increasingly unacceptable. Therefore, please revise to describe these tests in terms of their objective endpoints.
We understand that the behavioral measures we assessed in mice are endpoints which serve as models for examining effects on neural activity relevant to brain functions affected in psychiatric disorders and apologize for inadvertently using language that was not responsive to the NIH statement and guidance. We have revised the Introduction and wording in other places of the manuscript to be more objective and amenable to NOT-MH-19-053. The goals of our research were to identify behaviors and neurobiological processes impacted by AKT isoform activity and to examine them in the context of potential sex differences, which had been largely overlooked in previous preclinical studies. This has been more clearly delineated in the text.
4) Following the previous point, at the end of the Introduction, the authors indicate that their "findings have significant implications for diagnoses and therapies." Please describe how this statement is consistent with the NIH considerations regarding the use of animal neurobehavioral approaches in basic studies.
We have modified the Introduction to better fit the guidelines outlined in NOT-MH-19-053. In accordance with this notice, our study highlights the findings in the context of behavioral assays as readouts “for” brain function that may model neurobiological processes involved in psychiatric disorders. We delineate the contributions of AKT isoforms to specific brain processes (e.g. hippocampus-dependent long-term memory formation) and identify sex-dependent differences in the requirements for AKT activity.
5) One of prominent phenotypes in the study is a male-specific enhanced fear extinction in AKT1 KO. This can also be reversed by expressing AKT1 in the PFC. However, one of the main brain parts responsible for fear extinction is the amygdala and there are studies showing the connection between the PFC and amygdala may also play role in fear extinction. In addition, in Figure 6, conditional KO of AKT1 show normal extinction, which is possibly due to the fact that the cre line used in the study has not reported to have expression in the amygdala. This suggests AKT1 in the amygdala plays a role in fear extinction. Please examine whether AKT1 expression in the amygdala of KO mice can reverse fear extinction, or test whether specific loss of AKT1 in the amygdala (by viral shRNA knockdown in WT mice) has any effects on behavior.
The particular Cre line used for this study, T29-1 (Tsien et al., 1996), does in fact express quite well in the amygdala. In a previous report, we confirmed its expression in the amygdala (Hoeffer et al., 2008, Figure 1). To provide additional support, we have included immunoblotting and histological data confirming reduced AKT1 expression in the amygdala as well as in the PFC of conditional Akt1 KO (cAkt1 KO) mice driven by the T29-1 line (Figure 6—figure supplement 1). We show further that this line expresses in excitatory neurons and not interneurons of the amygdala and PFC (Figure 6—figure supplement 1). Taken together with our finding that neuronal AKT1 expression in the PFC of Akt1 KO mice reverses their fear extinction phenotype, these data suggest that interneuronal AKT1 in the PFC plays a critical role in fear extinction. Therefore, cAkt1 KO mice showed normal extinction possibly because AKT1 expression remained intact in interneurons of the PFC. We do not dispute the important role of the amygdala in extinction, but our data indicate that AKT1 is not required in excitatory neuronal populations of the amygdala or PFC for the normal display of extinction. To more thoroughly test this idea, we plan to generate interneuron-specific Akt1 KO mice and examine fear extinction in these animals for future studies. We have included this discussion in the revised manuscript. Based on these data, we did not pursue behavioral studies to test the role of AKT1 specifically in the amygdala, although we agree that it would be interesting to examine as part of a larger future study of brain region-specific AKT1 functions.
6) The authors inject AKT1 into the Prelimbic cortex (PL), which is sufficient to reverse altered fear extinction in KO mice. Importantly, several studies show that the connection between the amygdala and PL mediates fear acquisition and retrieval while the link between the amygdala and infralimbic cortex (IL) are thought to mediate fear extinction (Choi et al., PNAS. 2010. and Laurent et al., Learning and Memory. 2009). Please explain how AKT1 expression in the PL can reverse fear extinction or examine the effects of AKT1 expression in the IL on fear extinction in KO animals.
Please also include a representative example of expression with sufficient display of anatomical landmarks to indicate that expression was restricted to the PL, as well as a schematic of expression spread overlaid for each subject so readers can see the extent of expression for the entire cohort.
We apologize that the methods for viral-mediated AKT1 expression was not clearly described in the original submission. Although we targeted the PL region of the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), it was not our intention to imply that viral expression was limited to the PL. At the reviewers’ suggestion, we provide a representative example of AAV expression in the PFC along with a schematic of the expression spread overlaid for available subjects, which we have incorporated into Figure 5. While AAV expression was visually confirmed for every subject, we had only comprehensively imaged a few representative samples. As these experiments were performed over the course of a couple years, the original slides for confirming expression were no longer usable for imaging. We prepared new slides but only some subjects had brain sections with PFC available. To construct the schematic, we compiled images from this subset, which consists of samples from each group. The revised figure shows that both the PL and IL exhibit AAV expression (Figure 5B). We do not distinguish between the requirements for AKT1 activity in PL or IL in this study. Instead, we elected for a more general approach, indeed given some conflicting reports regarding the requirement for the PL and IL in fear extinction processes (Do-Monte et al., 2015; Sierra-Mercado et al., 2011; Marek et al., 2018). Our simplest interpretation of the results from restoring AKT1 expression in the mPFC of Akt1 KO mice is that AKT1 activity is normally required in the mPFC to limit extinction learning. While examining the role of AKT1 activity in specific PFC regions would be an interesting line of inquiry, based on our results from forebrain excitatory neuron-specific removal of AKT1, we posit that the AKT1 activity utilized during extinction learning is primarily required in interneuronal populations of the PFC (Figure 6 and Figure 6—figure supplement 1). The AAV-AKT1 construct we used expresses in cells responsive to the synapsin promoter, so it does not resolve AKT1 activity in excitatory or inhibitory neurons (Figure 5). Therefore, as mentioned for point #5, our future efforts will directly test the idea that interneuronal AKT1 is responsible for normal extinction behavior using genetic approaches to eliminate AKT1 and AAV constructs designed to limit AKT1 expression to interneurons.
7) Across the manuscript, the authors indicate that their findings are "sex-dependent." This is technically inaccurate, given that sex was not incorporated as an independent variable in their statistical analyses. This is particularly problematic in the Discussion section of the paper, since the experimental design does not support such inferences. A similar problem is exhibited for the arguments made across the different AKT isoforms (AKT1, AKT2, AKT3), since "isoform" was also not incorporated as an independent variable. The data should be re-analyzed using a 2-way ANOVA with "sex" and "AKT isoform" as sources of variance. Please also include a discussion on potential sex-dependent downstream players.
This is a very important point, and we carefully considered the implications of our research design and interpretations to address this critique. It appears to us that many labs employ a variety of statistical approaches, some applied correctly, some applied incorrectly. It is also the case that there are often multiple statistical approaches that could be used, and if the “take-home” message doesn’t change it can simply be a matter of preference. The two main issues raised by reviewers for this manuscript focus on how to handle possible sex differences and whether to examine possible Akt isoform differences. Below we present our rationale for the way we conducted the analyses.
1) Sex differences: We do not share the reviewers’ interpretation about the use of “sex-dependent.” Behavioral effects observed in one sex but not another is sex-dependent. However, we understand the concept of including sex as a variable in our statistical analyses. In a previous submission to another journal that included only a subset of these data, we did in fact prepare our manuscript with two-way ANOVA analyses (genotype and sex) but received criticisms about that approach. In that case, the reviewers requested post-hoc analyses, but post-hoc tests are not appropriate when there are only two levels for each variable (e.g. genotype can be only KO/WT and sex can be only M/F). Therefore, we consulted with local biostatistical experts (Dr. Luke Evans and Dr. Marissa Ehringer) and revised our figures based on their directions.
Dr. Ehringer deals with this sex effect issue on a regular basis because there are well-known sex differences in both rodents and humans for alcohol-related behaviors, which is a major area of her research. Given the a priori knowledge of such sex differences (Choleris et al., 2018; Andreano and Cahill, 2009; Wiltgen et al., 2005; File, 2001), analyses are frequently performed separately by sex to simplify the presentation and interpretation of results. It is not uncommon for researchers to actually submit two different papers, one for each sex. In our case, the behaviors we examined are also well-known to show reproducible sex differences in WT (C57BL/6) strains (Archer, 1977; Wiltgen et al., 2001; Bolivar et al., 2001; Voikar et al., 2001; La Buda et al., 2002; Gresack and Frick, 2003; Guo et al., 2004; Berger-Sweeney et al., 2005; Jonasson, 2005; Calderone et al., 2008; An et al., 2011; Davis et al., 2012; Matsuda et al., 2015; Arakawa, 2019). Thus, to avoid complicating the current submission with results of behavioral differences inherent to sex alone, which are already known, and because males and females within each genotype set were tested separately, we opted to perform within-sex t-tests (except for Figure 5, in which the independent variable had three levels). This analysis facilitated presentation of our results for easy comparison between WT and KO genotypes within either male or female groups. It allowed us to show data normalized to WT controls within each sex in order to focus on the effect of Akt isoform manipulation, which was the primary goal of our study. At the reviewers’ suggestion, we also performed two-way ANOVA, and the overall “take-home” message does not change. This analysis is included in Table 4. If all reviewers and the editor strongly feel we should present the results as a two-way ANOVA, we can change our figures. Based on our previous submission experience, we expect this might cause more confusion than the simpler t-test, which we believe is justified based on previously known sex differences and the experimental design.
2) AKT isoform comparisons: We believe including Akt isoform as an independent variable for analyses is not appropriate for four reasons. First, the individual Akt mutant lines are bred and maintained separately. They cannot be reasonably grouped all together because they are maintained on different backgrounds and not simply interchangeable. In order to analyze the data as proposed (all Akt mutants simultaneously), all the mutant Akt alleles would have to be maintained in the same strain. This was not done for practical reasons; it would require thousands of progeny from Akt1+/-Akt2+/-Akt3+/- breeder mice to complete these studies in this way and only 1.6% of mice derived from such a cross would be either pure WT or KO for each isoform group. Second, although all the mice are maintained on a C57Bl/6 background, the different Akt lines are not isogenic. We regularly introduce new C57Bl/6 mice into all our strains to prevent the accumulation of genetic modifiers. Despite this precaution, “WT” mice from each strain demonstrate slight performance differences in some tasks compared with “WT” mice from other Akt isoform strains. There are many possible reasons for this (genetics, rearing, microbiome differences, etc.) but those factors are largely beyond our control. Therefore, it would not be scientifically appropriate to compare KO mice to “WT” mice from a different line or to create a master “WT” group by simply averaging the performance of WT mice from all lines. Third, we used littermates obtained from breeding the individual Akt lines for each experimental group, a practice that is supported by previous studies (Holmdahl and Malissen, 2012). This important element of rigorous experimental design would be lost if we were to combine across all isoform groups. Finally, the experiments in this study were not designed or performed with the goal of comparing each Akt mutant line to one other. Combining all the mutants for simultaneous comparison creates an insurmountable multiple comparison problem that would necessitate testing many more animals, as the power analyses used to design our experiments were based on separate isoform testing. Another way to view the situation is that because the experiments were performed with only one mutant at a time, it would have been reasonable and appropriate to prepare three different manuscripts, one for each isoform, which would be analyzed independently.
At the reviewers’ request, we also included in the revised manuscript a discussion on potential sex-dependent downstream players, which incorporates the findings from our experiments to address point #11 below.
8) In the Experimental Design and Statistical analysis section of the paper, the authors indicate that outliers were excluded from the study. The specific number of excluded animals, across each experiment, should be clearly indicated. Also, the current manuscript only includes group numbers. The total number (n) of animals per experimental group needs to be included (a table depicting this information would be helpful, or adding the "n" within each group in the data figures). This is particularly important, given that the degrees of freedom change across experiments (this is confusing, since the authors indicate that mice were used across all experiments).
Because the numbers of animals differ considerable between some of the groups, please also show that the data meet the assumption of homogeneity of variance for ANOVA.
We apologize that presentation of the experimental details for our statistical analysis was confusing in the original submission. We have now added summary tables with the number of animals per experimental group (also included in the figure legends), number of excluded animals and reason for exclusion, and homogeneity of variance test results across each experiment (Tables 2-4).
9) In Figure 6, please confirm loss of AKT1 expression by immunohistochemistry to examine the brain regions with AKT1 loss of expression.
We previously confirmed that conditional Akt1 removal under the T29-1 Camk2α-Cre driver reduced AKT1 expression in the hippocampus (Levenga et al., 2017). Complementing this report, we now demonstrate using western blotting and immunohistochemistry that AKT1 expression is also reduced in the amygdala and PFC. These data have been added as a supplemental figure (Figure 6—figure supplement 1).
10) Given the complexity and length of the behavioral tests, it would be very useful if the authors included a schematic representation of the experimental timeline, depicting the multiple groups of AKT mutants, the rescue experiment, and the timeline of the behavioral tests.
We thank the reviewers for this very useful suggestion and have amended our Materials and methods section to include a flowchart for the behavioral assays (Figure 1—figure supplement 1).
11) While the authors use a genetically targeted approach to study AKT isoforms, these molecules cross-talk with many signaling pathways. Therefore, it is very likely that multiple pathways are de-regulated by AKT KO. Assessing activation of other cross-talk signaling molecules (PI3-kinase, GSK3b, ERK, or CREB) across the multiple AKT mutants (Western blotting) would provide a better understanding of the role that each AKT isoform plays in the AKT-dependent behavioral modifications.
We agree that loss of AKT likely influences many different signaling pathways and that these may be involved in the behavioral effects we observed. Following the reviewers’ suggestion, we performed western blot analysis of PDK1, GSK3β, and ERK activation to assess candidate signaling upstream, downstream, and parallel of AKT, respectively, in brain tissue from Akt isoform mutants of both sexes. These data are now included as Figure 9 and associated supplemental figures. While we did not identify many differences in this analysis, we did find some exciting isoform- and sex-dependent effects on GSK3β phosphorylation. We hope to investigate other potential signaling differences that may mediate the behavioral effects of Akt isoform deficiency using a more comprehensive approach, such as mass spectrometry, in a follow-up study.
12) Some of the AKT KO mouse lines have previously been shown to be smaller and display growth deficiency (i.e., Dummler et al., 2006-DOI: 10.1128/MCB.00722-06; Cho et al., 2001-DOI: 10.1074/jbc.C100462200). Are the same lines used in the current study. Are the mice smaller? Did the authors check the general health state of the mice? It is suggested is to add a short paragraph in the manuscript that clarifies this point.
We obtained our Akt1 and Akt2 KO lines from JAX, donated by Morris Birnbaum. Our Akt3 KO line was also obtained from the Birnbaum Lab by direct request and is not the same strain described in Dummler et al. 2006. This information has been added to the Key Resources Table. In our hands, both Akt1 and Akt3 KO mice show significantly reduced sizes compared with their WT littermates. The size reduction is slight in Akt1 KO mice and more pronounced in Akt3 KO mice, consistent with previous reports (Cho et al., 2001a (PMID: 11533044); Easton et al., 2005). There are conflicting reports of Akt2 KO mice having a reduced weight (Cho et al., 2001b (PMID: 11387480); Garofalo et al., 2003). In our hands, Akt2 KO mice trend towards a reduced size but the difference is not significant. Akt2 KO mice do develop insulinemia that becomes progressively worse with age (Cho et al., 2001b; Garofalo et al., 2003). Most glucose-related phenotypes in Akt2 KO mice are uncovered following fasting or with aging, but our mice have access to food ad libitum and were tested younger than 16 weeks, before insulin-related phenotypes become severe. Previous studies examined the general health of all these KO mice, and apart from size, found them generally indistinguishable from their WT littermates (Cho et al., 2001a; Cho et al., 2001b; Easton et al., 2005). In addition, we assessed sensory functions (acoustic startle, vision, and pain perception) and found no differences between KO and WT mice. Locomotor assessments, such as distance moved and swim velocity, were included in our behavioral assays to rule out locomotor defects as a potential confound for observed behavioral differences. None were identified. Some of these control data are included in the submitted figures (e.g. distance moved, visual platform performance, baseline freezing). We have added a paragraph about general health to the Materials and methods section to make clear this point.
[Editors' note: further revisions were suggested prior to acceptance, as described below.]
The manuscript has been improved but there are some remaining issues that need to be addressed before acceptance, as outlined below:
1) The authors conclude that AKT1 in PFC interneurons is important for fear extinction. They showed that virally expressed AKT1 in the PFC reversed enhanced fear extinction in KO mice (Figure 5). However, the study lacks confirmation of AKT1 expression in interneurons. The authors are able to show double labeling of PV and AKT1 in the PFC in Figure 6—figure supplement 1. Thus, the authors should be able to perform similar labeling to demonstrate that virally expressed AKT1 is present in PFC interneurons in Figure 5C to confirm that AKT1 is indeed expressed in PFC interneurons of KO mice.
As suggested, we now include images confirming that virally expressed AKT1 in the PFC of Akt1 KO mice is present in interneurons with our AAV construct (Figure 5—figure supplement 1).
2) Describing sex as an "experimental variable" throughout the manuscripts is inaccurate. The authors are to be commended for including both males and females in their work, and indeed, the inclusion of both sexes is extremely important. But not including sex as an independent variable in their statistical analyses (ANOVAs), precludes the authors from suggesting that sex was an experimental variable (this is misleading, and technically incorrect). There is agreement with the justification provided by the authors for not including sex or isoform as a variable in their statistical approaches. For this reason, it is encouraged that the authors remove any statement indicating that their findings are isoform or sex "dependent", and simply rephrase them as sex and isoform-"specific" (because data is compared to a same sex control).
We have changed references of “dependent” to “specific” throughout the text.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.56630.sa2Article and author information
Author details
Funding
National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NS086933)
- Helen Wong
- Josien Levenga
- Lauren LaPlante
- Bailey Keller
- Andrew Cooper-Sansone
- Curtis Borski
- Ryan Milstead
- Charles Hoeffer
National Institute of Mental Health (MH016880)
- Helen Wong
- Charles Hoeffer
Jerome Lejeune Foundation (1805)
- Helen Wong
- Andrew Cooper-Sansone
- Charles Hoeffer
National Institute on Aging (AG 064465)
- Helen Wong
- Lauren LaPlante
- Charles Hoeffer
National Institute on Aging (AG052371)
- Ryan Milstead
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Acknowledgements
These studies were supported by grants from the National Institutes of Health (R01 NS086933-01, R01 AG 064465, T32 MH016880 and T32 AG052371) and funds from the Linda Crnic Institute. We thank Nicole Kethley, Michael Roche, Daniel Peterson, Stephanie Quintana, and Emily Schmitt for technical contributions to this work and Drs. Luke Evans and Peter Cain for useful comments and discussions in the preparation of the manuscript. We dedicate this work in the loving memory of Ms. Lauren LaPlante, our friend and colleague whose contributions were not only critical to this study but also to the well-being of our research group.
Senior Editor
- Kate M Wassum, University of California, Los Angeles, United States
Reviewing Editor
- Mary Kay Lobo, University of Maryland, United States
Version history
- Received: March 4, 2020
- Accepted: December 15, 2020
- Accepted Manuscript published: December 16, 2020 (version 1)
- Version of Record published: January 6, 2021 (version 2)
Copyright
© 2020, Wong et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 3,665
- Page views
-
- 317
- Downloads
-
- 11
- Citations
Article citation count generated by polling the highest count across the following sources: Crossref, PubMed Central, Scopus.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Neuroscience
Decisions about noisy stimuli are widely understood to be made by accumulating evidence up to a decision bound that can be adjusted according to task demands. However, relatively little is known about how such mechanisms operate in continuous monitoring contexts requiring intermittent target detection. Here, we examined neural decision processes underlying detection of 1 s coherence targets within continuous random dot motion, and how they are adjusted across contexts with weak, strong, or randomly mixed weak/strong targets. Our prediction was that decision bounds would be set lower when weak targets are more prevalent. Behavioural hit and false alarm rate patterns were consistent with this, and were well captured by a bound-adjustable leaky accumulator model. However, beta-band EEG signatures of motor preparation contradicted this, instead indicating lower bounds in the strong-target context. We thus tested two alternative models in which decision-bound dynamics were constrained directly by beta measurements, respectively, featuring leaky accumulation with adjustable leak, and non-leaky accumulation of evidence referenced to an adjustable sensory-level criterion. We found that the latter model best explained both behaviour and neural dynamics, highlighting novel means of decision policy regulation and the value of neurally informed modelling.
-
- Genetics and Genomics
- Neuroscience
A hexanucleotide repeat expansion in C9ORF72 is the most common genetic cause of amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS) and frontotemporal dementia (FTD). A hallmark of ALS/FTD pathology is the presence of dipeptide repeat (DPR) proteins, produced from both sense GGGGCC (poly-GA, poly-GP, poly-GR) and antisense CCCCGG (poly-PR, poly-PG, poly-PA) transcripts. Translation of sense DPRs, such as poly-GA and poly-GR, depends on non-canonical (non-AUG) initiation codons. Here, we provide evidence for canonical AUG-dependent translation of two antisense DPRs, poly-PR and poly-PG. A single AUG is required for synthesis of poly-PR, one of the most toxic DPRs. Unexpectedly, we found redundancy between three AUG codons necessary for poly-PG translation. Further, the eukaryotic translation initiation factor 2D (EIF2D), which was previously implicated in sense DPR synthesis, is not required for AUG-dependent poly-PR or poly-PG translation, suggesting that distinct translation initiation factors control DPR synthesis from sense and antisense transcripts. Our findings on DPR synthesis from the C9ORF72 locus may be broadly applicable to many other nucleotide repeat expansion disorders.