Laminar microcircuitry of visual cortex producing attention-associated electric fields
Abstract
Cognitive operations are widely studied by measuring electric fields through EEG and ECoG. However, despite their widespread use, the neural circuitry giving rise to these signals remains unknown because the functional architecture of cortical columns producing attention-associated electric fields has not been explored. Here, we detail the laminar cortical circuitry underlying an attention-associated electric field measured over posterior regions of the brain in humans and monkeys. First, we identified visual cortical area V4 as one plausible contributor to this attention-associated electric field through inverse modeling of cranial EEG in macaque monkeys performing a visual attention task. Next, we performed laminar neurophysiological recordings on the prelunate gyrus and identified the electric-field-producing dipoles as synaptic activity in distinct cortical layers of area V4. Specifically, activation in the extragranular layers of cortex resulted in the generation of the attention-associated dipole. Feature selectivity of a given cortical column determined the overall contribution to this electric field. Columns selective for the attended feature contributed more to the electric field than columns selective for a different feature. Last, the laminar profile of synaptic activity generated by V4 was sufficient to produce an attention-associated signal measurable outside of the column. These findings suggest that the top-down recipient cortical layers produce an attention-associated electric field that can be measured extracortically with the relative contribution of each column depending upon the underlying functional architecture.
Editor's evaluation
By combining rare EEG and laminar recordings in monkeys, Westerberg and colleagues studied the neural correlates of the well-known attention-related N2pc signal and found that it is due to the activation of extra-granular layers of cortex. Further, this effect was stronger for columns that were more feature selective. These findings are extremely important and a unique contribution to the literature on the neurobiology of attention.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.72139.sa0Introduction
Research into extracortical electric fields provides fundamental insights into the mechanisms of human perception, cognition, and intention. For instance, event-related potential (ERP) components like the N2pc (Eimer, 1996; Luck and Hillyard, 1994; Woodman and Luck, 1999) and Pd (Hickey et al., 2009) reliably index selective attention in humans and monkeys, alike. However, the interpretation of these extracortical measures of attention is severely limited by uncertainty about the exact neural processes that generate these signals (Nunez and Srinivasan, 2006). Understanding what brain processes an electric field indicates requires knowing how it is generated (e.g., Cohen, 2017).
One avenue to localize neural generators of electric fields is through inverse source localization (Michel et al., 2004; Grech et al., 2008). However, the results are indefinite and cannot offer conclusive answers. Moreover, these methods do not allow for the probing of the underlying neural circuitry. For example, most EEG signals are hypothesized to be generated by interlaminar interactions in cortical columns (Nunez and Srinivasan, 2006). Columnar microcircuits are ubiquitous across the brain (Douglas et al., 1989; Douglas and Martin, 1991), having a well-defined anatomical structure (Mountcastle, 1997; Kaas, 2012) and consistent physiological activation pattern (Bastos et al., 2012 but see Godlove et al., 2014). The canonical cortical microcircuit offers a framework in which to interpret columnar dynamics in sensory or cognitive tasks, yet the relationship between this functional architecture and electric fields related to cognition commonly measured in humans is unexplored.
Electric fields measured at the surface of the brain (ECoG) and scalp (EEG) are theorized to be generated by dipoles in cortex. However, measuring current dipoles requires sampling electrical potentials across all the layers of the cerebral cortex. Such laminar neurophysiological measurements are rare and unsystematic in humans. Work in rodents has uncovered intriguing insights into cortical laminar microcircuits underlying evoked EEG signals, but all of these were limited to sensory responses (Jellema et al., 2004; Bruyns-Haylett et al., 2017; Nass et al., 2021). Fortunately, macaque monkeys produce homologues of the attention-associated EEG signals (N2pc: Woodman et al., 2007; Cohen et al., 2009; Purcell et al., 2013; Pd: Cosman et al., 2018). Laminar neurophysiological measurements (Schroeder et al., 1998; Maier et al., 2010; Buffalo et al., 2011; Hansen et al., 2011; Self et al., 2013; Godlove et al., 2014; Engel et al., 2016; Klein et al., 2016; Hembrook-Short et al., 2017; Nandy et al., 2017; Trautmann et al., 2019; Westerberg et al., 2019; Tovar et al., 2020; Ferro et al., 2021) and EEG (Schmid et al., 2006; Woodman et al., 2007; Sandhaeger et al., 2019) are well established in macaques. However, despite many studies linking intra- and extracortical signals (Schroeder et al., 1992; Whittingstall and Logothetis, 2009; Musall et al., 2014; Snyder and Smith, 2015), to date, little is known about the laminar origins of ERPs in primates.
Here, we show that visual cortex generates dipoles through layer-specific transsynaptic currents that give rise to electric fields that track the deployment of selective attention. These dipoles were generated by the extragranular compartments of cortex, indicating these cognitive operations likely arise from top-down interactions. Moreover, functional architecture – in the form of feature columns – was associated with the relative contribution of individual, local cortical columns to the global electric field. These results are the first to our knowledge to describe laminar specificity in synaptic activations contributing to the generation of electric fields associated with cognitive processing.
Results
Attention task
To investigate extracortical manifestations of attention-associated electric fields, we trained macaque monkeys to perform a visual search task (Figure 1A). Three macaque monkeys (designated Ca, He, and Z) performed visual search for an oddball color target (red or green), presented within an array of five or seven uniform distractors (green or red) (N sessions for each monkey: Ca 21, He 9, Z 18). A fourth monkey (P) performed visual search for an oddball shape (T or L) presented within an array of up to seven uniform distractors (L or T) (N sessions: monkey P, 22). Each animal performed well above chance (chance level for monkeys Ca, He: 16.6%; P, Z: 12.5%) (behavioral accuracy in color search: Ca 88%, He 81%, Z 85%, shape search monkey P 66%). We sampled cortical neural signals during the color pop-out search to be certain of which item received the benefit of attention in the array. We used the more difficult search data to determine the generality of our findings. Two different recording types were used, necessitating four monkeys total, as described below and summarized in Supplementary file 1.

EEG traces and inverse source localization for the N2pc index of attention in monkeys.
(A) EEG was recorded from electrodes arranged according to the 10–20 system in monkeys performing visual search by shifting gaze to a colored oddball stimulus (monitor diagrams show two example arrays). Blue and red shading highlights mapping between visual hemifield and cerebral hemisphere. (B) Trial-averaged P5 and P6 EEG traces from monkey Z following presentation of search arrays with the target in either the right (blue) or left (red) visual hemifield as well as the difference (orange). The voltage difference between the target in the left versus right hemifields reveals the N2pc ~150 ms after array presentation. The N2pc was significant (dependent samples t test between polarizations averaged between 125 and 250 ms after array presentation (t(35) = 2.42, p = 0.02)). (C) Inverse solution of current distribution consistent with difference in voltage distribution during the N2pc (113–182 ms) when the target was in the left hemifield versus right hemifield using sLORETA. Current density is displayed over the three-dimensional (3D) boundary element model derived from a magnetic resonance scan of monkey Z. Data was clipped below the 85% maximum value for display purposes. Cyan disks indicate EEG electrode positions. Current density is concentrated beneath electrode P6 caudal to the lunate sulcus and in area V4 on the prelunate gyrus. Results are reproduced for a second monkey in Figure 1—figure supplement 1.
Inverse modeling of attention-associated extracortical electric fields points to visual cortex
Once animals could perform visual search, we implanted an array of electrodes approximating the human 10–20 system in monkeys P and Z (Figure 1A). Using these electrodes, we observed extracortical electric dynamics in both monkeys. An index of attention known as the N2pc manifests during visual search. The N2pc electric field indexes attention allocation in this task. The magnitude of the N2pc was largest over occipital sites (Figure 1B, Figure 1—figure supplement 1), consistent with previous reports in humans and macaques (Luck and Hillyard, 1994; Eimer, 1996; Woodman and Luck, 1999; Hopf et al., 2000; Woodman et al., 2007; Cohen et al., 2009; Purcell et al., 2013). We used sLORETA inverse modeling for source localization. Previous source estimates for the N2pc identified the human homologue of V4 (Luck and Hillyard, 1990; Luck and Hillyard, 1994; Hopf et al., 2000). These findings are consistent with numerous reports that areas in mid-level visual cortex in monkeys produce robust attention signals (Moran and Desimone, 1985; Luck et al., 1997a; McAdams and Maunsell, 1999; Reynolds et al., 1999; Fries et al., 2001; see Roe et al., 2012 for review) across cortical layers (Engel et al., 2016; Nandy et al., 2017). Consistent with these earlier studies, the inverse model showed that current sources include V4 on the prelunate gyrus (Figure 1C, Figure 1—figure supplement 1). However, the modeled current sources also included other cortical regions, as is common for inverse solutions. Notably, the inverse solution identifies V1 to be about as strong as V4 in contributing to the N2pc, which is unlikely given current knowledge on attentional modulation for each area (Motter, 1993; Luck et al., 1997a; Kastner et al., 1999; Buffalo et al., 2011). Given the primary feature used in the search task was color, we investigated the laminar profile of attention-associated electric field generation in V4 where color is better represented (Roe et al., 2012).
V4’s laminar microcircuit produces dipoles that predict the attention-associated electric field
Guided by magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), linear multielectrode arrays (LMAs) were inserted into area V4 of monkeys Ca and He. LMAs were placed perpendicular to the cortical surface, spanning supragranular (L2/3), granular (L4), and infragranular (L5/6) cortical layers (Figure 2—figure supplement 1). We confirmed that attentional modulation of spiking activity could be observed during pop-out search performance consistent with previous reports (Westerberg et al., 2020a). Moreover, the laminar profile of attentional modulation matched that of attentional modulation in a different task with spiking activity in the middle layers being the most highly enhanced with attention (Figure 2; Nandy et al., 2017). Critically, while attentional modulation is present in the laminar data prior to the emergence of extracortical attention-associated fields such as the N2pc, that cross-laminar modulation persists through this interval.

Laminar profile of local field potential (LFP) and multiunit (MUA) attentional target selection during visual search task performance across monkeys Ca and He (n = 2).
Responses were averaged across sessions (n = 30) at each of the depths (n = 15) relative to the L4/5 boundary (magenta to green). Difference between target (attended) and distractor (unattended) responses represented by the fill color corresponding to the recording channels’ laminar compartment. Top line of each trace combination is the attended condition, bottom trace is the unattended condition. Significant differences in magnitude of attention effect, averaged 150–190 ms after search array onset, across laminar compartment were detected through an ANOVA for both LFP (F(2, 442) = 22.43, p = 5.2e–10) and MUA (F(2, 442) = 3.87, p = 0.022). Note the effect of attention in the MUA was largest in the middle layers (ML2/3 = 2.68, ML4 = 3.50, ML5/6 = 2.38), consistent with previous reports (Nandy et al., 2017). Time of the N2pc as measured throughout the main text (150–190 ms following array onset) indicated with orange.
Simultaneous with LMA recording, an extracortical electric signal was recorded immediately above V4 – critically the recording took place outside of the cortical column itself. Current source density (CSD) was derived from the local field potentials (LFPs) sampled across V4 layers. To relate the extracortical signal (Figure 3A) to synaptic currents estimated as CSD (Figure 3B–D), we employed information theory to capture multivariate factors and nonlinearities between signals (Shannon, 1948; Cover and Thomas, 2006). Importantly, information theory analyses are model independent (Timme and Lapish, 2018). Information theory is thus superior to standard linear models since these models cannot capture all potential relationships between signals. The relationship between the extracortical signal and CSD was assessed in four distinct steps, as illustrated by a representative session (Figure 3E–F, Figure 3—figure supplement 1). We use the interval of the N2pc to determine whether laminar circuitry in V4 can contribute to the attention-associated electric fields. This interval occurred before the median response times for each monkey contributing laminar V4 data ([median± standard deviation]: monkey Ca 227 ± 49 ms, He 225 ± 44 ms).

Extracortical attention-associated signal and simultaneously recorded V4 synaptic currents during representative session.
(A) Extracortical event-related potential (ERP) voltages after search array presentation, averaged over all trials when the target was presented contra- (solid) or ipsilateral (dashed) to the electrode. Inset magnifies the N2pc interval defined as the difference in potentials 150–190 ms after the array appeared (orange highlight). (B) Simultaneous current source density (CSD) when the target appeared in the population receptive field of the column. Dashed lines indicate boundaries between supragranular (L2/3), granular (L4), and infragranular (L5/6) layers. CSD values were interpolated and smoothed along depth for display only. Current sinks have hotter hues, and current sources, cooler. The earliest sink arises in putative L4, likely from rapid feedforward transmission, followed by intense, prolonged sinks in L2/3 accompanied by weaker source in L5/6. (C) CSD evoked by distractor in the receptive field has similar pattern. (D) Subtraction of CSD responses to target versus distractor in receptive field. The only statistically significant differences (determined through a t test across time-depth with p < 0.05, outlined by magenta line) were due to a current sink in L2/3 that arose gradually ~100 ms after array presentation. This relative sink was associated with a weak relative source in L5/6. (E) Simultaneous mutual information between CSD and the extracortical signal for L2/3 (blue), L4 (purple), and L5/6 (green). Times with significant mutual information were computed through Monte Carlo shuffle simulations (MCS). N2pc interval is highlighted. Intervals with significant mutual information persisting for at least 10 ms are indicated by horizontal bars. No mutual information with EEG was observed in L4. (F) Information transmission about target position from V4 CSD to the extracortical signal. Conventions as in E.
First, we employed Monte Carlo simulations of the mutual information analysis to verify that the extracortical signal exhibited significantly enhanced information about target position during the time window of the N2pc. Second, we measured target information across the layers of V4 during the N2pc interval. This analysis revealed enhanced information in L2/3 and L5/6 but not in L4. Third, we computed the mutual information between the extracortical signal and CSD during the N2pc window, irrespective of target position. This analysis showed a significant relationship between the extracortical signal and the CSD in L2/3 and L5/6 but not in L4. Fourth, we measured the transmitted information about target location from CSD to extracortical signal during the N2pc interval (Timme and Lapish, 2018). This analysis demonstrated significant information transmission to the extracortical signal from L2/3 and L5/6, but not L4.
Averaged across sessions, we observed that the electric field during the N2pc interval (Figure 4A) was associated with a consistent CSD pattern (Figure 4B). This relationship was observed in each monkey (Figure 4—figure supplement 1). Presentation of the search array in any configuration elicited an early current sink in L4, followed by a prolonged sink in L2/3 that was associated with a briefer source in L5/6.

Grand average demonstrating the link between V4 current source density (CSD) and the extracortical attention-associated electric field.
Conventions as in Figure 3. (A) Average event-related potential (ERP) across all sessions and animals with the target contra- (solid) or ipsilateral (dashed). The N2pc interval is indicated by orange shading. (B) Average V4 CSD with the target in (top) or out of the receptive field (RF) (center) with the difference between the two at the bottom. (C) Grand average information transmission about target position from V4 layers to the extracortical signal as a function of time (left). Average +2 SEM of information transmission during the N2pc window (right). Panel below shows that information transmission from L2/3 and in L5/6 was significantly greater than that from L4 (t test p < 0.05). Timepoints with significant information transmission were assessed through Monte Carlo simulations during >75% of sessions. Intervals with significance persisting for at least 10 ms are indicated by horizontal bars, color coded for each laminar compartment (bottom).
We next computed information transmission about target location from the CSD to the extracortical signal for each session (Figure 4C). All cortical layers provided significant information transmission in >75% of sessions during the N2pc window (150–190 ms following array onset). However, the magnitude of transmitted target information was significantly greater in L2/3 and L5/6 relative to L4 (L2/3-L4: t(29) = 2.15, p = 0.040; L5/6-L4: t(29) = 2.20, p = 0.036). The magnitude of information transmission was not significantly different between L2/3 and L5/6 (t(29) = 0.21, p = 0.84). Across sessions, the three other information theoretic analyses were consistent with the example session (Figure 3—figure supplement 1). Moreover, significant information transmission during the N2pc was observed in each monkey (Figure 4—figure supplement 1).
To verify the results, we applied the information theoretic analysis over a longer interval (Figure 4—figure supplement 2). Importantly, we found no signal differences or significant information transmission in the 100 ms pre-array baseline period as expected with baseline correction. We also evaluated the interval 200–250 ms following array presentation and found a polarization reversal in the extracortical signal likely corresponding to the Pd (Cosman et al., 2018). We observe persistent current differences in the extragranular CSD during this interval sufficient to contribute to the extracortical signal. However, we observed no statistically significant information theoretic relationship between the CSD and extracortical signal during this interval. The absence of a relationship could indicate no actual association or be a consequence of the reduced trial count due to the clipping of signals at saccade initiation. This uncertainty prevents further consideration of this interval in these data.
Last, we performed two additional analyses to determine whether the observed information theoretic relationship is confounded by spurious factors. First, we measured the contribution of V4 neuron selectivity for stimulus color. We computed information transmission separately for trials with a red stimulus and with a green stimulus in the receptive field (RF). In the population average of the two calculations for each session, we observed significant information transmission during the N2pc (Figure 4—figure supplement 3). Hence, the relationship between V4 activity and the EEG does not depend on color specificity. Second, we measured the contribution of microsaccades, which have been linked to attentional modulation in V4 (Lowet et al., 2018). We computed information transmission separately for trials without microsaccades (Figure 4—figure supplement 4). In the population average of the two calculations for each session, we observed significant information transmission during the N2pc. Hence, microsaccade production was not responsible for the observed information theoretic associations between signals. The outcomes of these control analyses engender more confidence that the current dipole in V4 generated by the L2/3 CSD sink and the L5/6 CSD source contributes to the N2pc measured in the extracortical electric field.
Columnar feature selectivity influences contribution to N2pc
Given the columnar organization of color tuning of V4 neurons (Figure 5A; Roe et al., 2012; Zeki, 1973; Zeki, 1980; Tootell et al., 2004; Conway and Tsao, 2009; Kotake et al., 2009), we investigated the association between the N2pc and the CSD in columns with different color preferences. To quantify color selectivity through depth, we computed the response ratio between red and green stimuli (Figure 5B). Responses were measured as power in the gamma range (30–150 Hz) because this signal reflects local circuit interactions (Ray and Maunsell, 2011) and feature selectivity in visual cortex (Berens et al., 2008) and is more reliably measured than spiking activity across all LMA contacts. This analysis collapses across differences in color tuning across layers, so although the interlaminar specificity of gamma activity is not fully understood, recent work indicates that laminar gamma power can reliably reflect feature selectivity in a spatially specific fashion (Westerberg et al., 2021b).

Contribution of columnar feature selectivity to the N2pc.
Conventions as in Figure 3. (A) Visual search array configurations used for color selectivity analyses. (B) Laminar profiles of red/green color selectivity across all sessions. The hue of each point across cortical depth signifies the value of a color selectivity index (CSI), derived from local gamma power. CSI values < 0 (>0) indicate preference for green (red). CSI is smoothed across adjacent channels for display. Sessions are sorted from left to right based on a column color selectivity index (CCSI) that estimates each column’s combined selectivity. A bar plot of session-wise CCSI is plotted below. Asterisks indicate columns with significant color-selectivity (Wilcoxon signed rank, p < 0.05). Asterisk color indicates monkey (Ca cyan; He magenta). (C) Average event-related potentials (ERPs) for trials when a red (top) or green (bottom) target or distractor appeared in the receptive field (RF) of the 17 color selective columns. Conventions as in Figure 3. (D) Difference in current source density (CSD) when the target relative to distractor appeared in the columnar population RF when a red (top) or green (bottom) stimulus appeared in the RF (n = 17). (E) Average ERP for trials when the preferred color (top) or non-preferred color (bottom) target or distractor appeared in the RF (n = 17). Conventions as in Figure 3. (F) Difference in CSD when the target relative to distractor appeared in the RF with the preferred (top) or non-preferred (bottom) color. (G) Average difference in information transmission between laminar CSD and N2pc when preferred relative to non-preferred stimulus color appeared in RF. Conventions as before. More information was transmitted when a stimulus of the preferred color appeared in the RF. (H) Correlation between difference in information transmission across color columns and CCSI for each session for L2/3 (blue, top), L4 (purple, center), and L5/6 (green, bottom). Spearman correlation reported in lower right of each plot with data from all 30 sessions. Color-specific information transmission scaled with magnitude of color selectivity. (I) Information transmission for columns with (solid, n = 17) and without (dashed line, n = 13) feature selectivity for L2/3 (top), L4 (middle), and L5/6 (bottom). Intervals with significant differences are plotted below at two alpha levels for a two-sample t test (filled: 0.05; unfilled: 0.1). Bars plot average with upper 95% confidence interval of information transmission during the N2pc for columns with (left) and without (right) feature selectivity. Significant differences are indicated with a bracket and p value from a two-sample t test.
To identify columns with significant selectivity for either red or green, we performed Wilcoxon signed rank tests between the distribution of ratios in each column against bootstrapped null distributions. Each bootstrapped null distribution contained 15 randomly selected ratios from the full dataset (450 experimental values) from which 1000 distributions were generated. The bootstrapped distributions represent the range of possible values observed across V4, but do not capture any difference in the homogeneity of feature selectivity within a column.
We found that more than half of V4 columns show selectivity for red or green stimuli (monkey Ca 12/21 [57.1%], He 5/9 [55.6%]). We computed the information transmission of target position for each color tuned column separately for trials when the preferred or the non-preferred color was in the column’s population RF. Across sessions with different target and distractor colors, we observed no difference in the amplitude of the extracortical signal during the N2pc (paired sample t(16) = 0.40, p = 0.69) (Figure 5C) nor the laminar CSD (L2/3: t(16) = –0.85, p = 0.41; L4: t(16) = 0.75, p = 0.46; L5/6: t(16) = 0.36, p = 0.72) (Figure 5D). However, information transmission during the N2pc was greater when a preferred rather than a non-preferred color was in the RF (Figure 5G). This difference was significant in L2/3 and L5/6 but not in L4 (t test across time with at least 10 ms having p < 0.05) and is evident in single sessions (Figure 5—figure supplement 1).
We investigated whether the magnitude of information transmission varied with degree of color preference. In session-wise correlations of the difference in information transmission between preferred and non-preferred colors at the time of peak information transmission (160–180 ms) as a function of columnar color selectivity index (CCSI), we found a significant relationship for L2/3 (Spearman’s R = 0.50, p = 0.005) and L5/6 (R = 0.51, p = 0.004) but not L4 (Figure 5H).
We also tested whether feature selective columns, on average, transmitted more information than their non-feature-selective counterparts. We found that feature selective columns, in all laminar compartments, transmitted significantly more information (Figure 5I) (two-sample t test: L2/3, p = 0.044; L4, p = 0.023; L5/6, p = 0.009). As such, we wanted to determine if this was due to a lack of attentional modulation in the non-selective columns. This was not the case, we observed that non-selective columns were modulated with attention. Attentional modulation was observed in both the CSD in L2/3 and L5/6 (one-sample t test: L2/3: t(64) = –6.01, p = 9.8e–8; L4: t(64) = –0.18, p = 0.86; L5/6: t(64) = 5.24, p = 1.9e–6) as well as across all layers in the population spiking activity (one-sample t test: L2/3: t(64) = 8.00, p = 3.7e–11; L4: t(64) = 9.66, p = 4.1e–14; L5/6: t(64) = 7.58, p = 1.8e–10) during the N2pc interval (averaged 150–190 ms following array onset) (Figure 5—figure supplement 2).
Importantly, we tested whether the N2pc varied across sessions with or without color-selective columns sampled. We found no difference between N2pc polarization (150–190 ms after the array) between sessions with (n = 17) or without (n = 13) sampling of color selective columns (two sample t test: t(28) = –0.75, p = 0.46). This invariance is expected because extracortical EEG spatially integrates signals from multiple cortical columns.
Translaminar currents in V4 recapitulate the N2pc
CSD is computed by differentiating between LFPs to eliminate volume-conducted signals that do not arise from local circuit activity. Using an inverse procedure (i.e., summing the CSD), it is possible to estimate the LFP without contamination by volume-conducted activity (Nicholson and Llinas, 1971; Kajikawa and Schroeder, 2011). We used this approach to compute an estimated extracortical ERP. Specifically, we computed the sum of currents produced by a cortical column to estimate the extracortical signal at a position directly above. The resultant potential (ERPcal) distinguished the target from a distractor in the RF throughout the N2pc (Figure 6). In other words, the summed potential generated by currents along V4 columns differentiates between attention conditions simultaneous with the extracortically measured attention-associated signal. Note that the shape of the observed extracortical ERP (ERPobs) differs from the estimated extracortical ERPcal. This is expected because the ERPobs reflects several more variables such as volume-conducted contributions of nearby columns as well as the filtering and attenuating effects of the tissue and cranium above the gray matter (Nunez and Srinivasan, 2006). Moreover, the ERPcal does not reflect the potential contributions of other visual areas. Given these expected differences, it is remarkable how well the difference in ERPcal predicts the timing of the attention-associated electric field.

Comparing an estimated field potential generated from the current source density (CSD) across the cortical columns to the actually observed extracortical event-related potential (ERP).
Black lines indicate the empirically measured event-related potential (ERPobs, top), averaged across sessions. The pink line indicates the estimated ERP calculated from the synaptic currents across V4 columns, averaged across sessions (ERPcal, center). Synaptic currents at each electrode are measured and divided by the Euclidean distance of the electrode from the extracortical surface (see Materials and methods; Nicholson and Llinas, 1971; Kajikawa and Schroeder, 2011). ERP for target present in the receptive field (RF) versus target opposite the RF is shown as solid and dashed lines, respectively (example array for each condition shown at top right). Clouds around ERPcal lines indicate 95% confidence intervals across sessions for each condition. Note that despite differences in overall waveshape (which are likely due to the fact that V4 is not the only contributor to the attention-independent, visually evoked ERP), the timing of differences within signal types can be compared. The congruence in polarization of the difference in potentials is of similar note.
Discussion
Bioelectric potentials have practical and clinical applications when their generators are known. For example, the electrocardiogram is useful in medicine because the physiological process associated with each phase of polarization is understood. Likewise, the electroretinogram is useful because the cell layers associated with each polarization are understood. In contrast, human ERP components indexing cognitive operations will have limited and only fortuitous utility until their neural generators are known.
The ERP indices of attention such as the N2pc or Pd are commonly used to assess the deployment of attention in human participants, but can also be observed in macaque monkeys, enabling systematic concurrent EEG and intracranial neurophysiological recordings. Our objective was to identify the neural generator of the attention-associated electric fields that comprise ERPs like the N2pc. Using inverse modeling of cranial surface EEG and laminar resolved CSD in a cortical area, we demonstrate that translaminar synaptic currents in visual cortical area V4 contribute to the generation of attention-associated electric fields during visual search. The dipole resulting in this electric field stemmed from layer-specific interactions in extragranular (top-down recipient) cortical layers. Unexpectedly, we discovered that the contribution of a cortical column to the overlying electric field depended on whether the visual feature in the RF matched the selectivity of the column – an important consideration in the mechanism producing EEG potentials that may not be observable through the macroscopic EEG signal alone.
The attention-associated electric field measured in our task is most likely representative of the commonly measured N2pc component of the EEG ERP. Given our findings regarding the functional architecture comprising attention-associated electric fields, it is conceivable that the N2pc arises from multiple, anatomically distinct cortical areas. That is, given the ubiquity of columnar architecture in sensory cortex and the specificity of visual feature representations to different cortical areas, electric dipoles formed across visual cortical layers could come about across multiple visual cortical areas with the relative contribution of each depending on the feature being attended to. This realization could help reconcile conflicting interpretations of the cognitive states and operations that are supposed to be indexed by the N2pc (Eimer, 1996; Kiss et al., 2008; Pagano and Mazza, 2012; Foster et al., 2020). It may also help account for variability in the N2pc as a function of attentional target presence in the lower versus upper visual hemifield (Luck et al., 1997b) given the positioning of retinotopic representations along the cortical surface (Gattass et al., 1988) – a potential focus for future study. Moreover, contributions from areas other than V4 are plausible because previous neurophysiological studies in macaques demonstrate attentional selection signals during visual search in the temporal (e.g., Sato, 1988), parietal (e.g., Bisley and Mirpour, 2019), and frontal (e.g., Thompson et al., 2005; Zhou and Desimone, 2011) lobes. Of particular note, neuroimaging studies in humans indicate a contribution to the N2pc from posterior parietal cortex (Hopf et al., 2000). In the same vein, FEF neurons locate the target among distractors as early as, or even before, the N2pc arises (Cohen et al., 2009; Purcell et al., 2013). Given the interconnectivity of FEF and V4 (Schall et al., 1995; Ungerleider et al., 2007; Gregoriou et al., 2012; Ninomiya et al., 2012), the frontal lobe could thus be the functional origin of an attentional selection signal communicated to V4 and other posterior areas (Armstrong and Moore, 2007; Ekstrom et al., 2009; Gregoriou et al., 2009; Gregoriou et al., 2012; Marshall et al., 2015; Popov et al., 2017), which in turn generate the N2pc (Westerberg and Schall, 2021a).
Our discovery that dipoles established by synaptic currents in visual cortical columns underlie the generation of the attention-associated electric fields is consistent with the observation that ERP components such as the N2pc are largest over the occipital lobe in humans (Luck and Hillyard, 1990; Luck and Hillyard, 1994) and macaques (Woodman et al., 2007), which is also observed with MEG (Hopf et al., 2000). Our investigation to identify interlaminar interactions producing the N2pc has offered unexpected insights into the underlying neural circuitry. Visual cortical area V4 contains a functional map of hue along its surface (Tanigawa et al., 2010) with individual columns comprising the map preferring the same color (Zeki, 1973; Zeki, 1980; Tootell et al., 2004; Conway and Tsao, 2009; Kotake et al., 2009; Westerberg et al., 2021b). We replicated the finding of columns specified by color-feature selectivity and discovered that the contribution of a column to the global electric field was greater for the preferred feature. Specifically, columns that preferred green (or red) contributed more to the electric field when the item in the RF was green (or red) rather than red (or green). This circuitry likely supports the decoding of visual features like color from EEG (Sandhaeger et al., 2019; Sutterer et al., 2021). The biophysical implications of this unexpected finding are illustrated in Figure 7, which portrays how an attention-associated electric field like the N2pc can arise from mosaics of different cortical columns. While all columns with the attended target in their RF contribute to the N2pc, columns with RF enclosing a target with preferred features establishes stronger dipoles than do columns representing other features or visual field locations. If target position or feature change, then other columns contribute the strongest dipoles.

Feature mosaic hypothesis.
(A) A map of preferred color in area V4 derived from optical imaging (Tanigawa et al., 2010) with corresponding color columns in area V4. (B) Relative contributions of color selective cortical columns to the N2pc when a red (left) or green (right) target appears in the receptive field (RF). Intensity of pyramidal neuron activity is indicated by saturation in the diagram. The mesoscopic columns produce electric fields (dashed lines) that sum to produce the equivalent event-related potential (ERP).
However, these findings leave several unanswered questions. First, the details of the attentional mechanism enacted in the cortical microcircuit and in turn manifested in the EEG remain unknown. In this study, the task design required both spatial and feature-based attention. Existing models of attention put forward hypotheses for the findings reported here. For example, multiplicative gain would predict a larger increase in response for preferred versus non-preferred stimuli when attended in the RF (e.g., McAdams and Maunsell, 1999). Alternatively, feature similarity gain would predict that regardless of what is in the RF, red-preferring columns would increase activity and green-preferring would decrease activity when attending red and vice versa (e.g., Martinez-Trujillo and Treue, 2005). Further work should be undertaken to disentangle the contributions of spatial and feature attention to this attention-associated field generation. Second, we do not know whether the spatial shifts in the voltage distribution entailed by the mosaic hypothesis can be resolved on the human scalp due to smearing of the signals as they propagate through the skull and scalp (Nunez and Srinivasan, 2006). However, given decoding of features from EEG can be achieved (Sandhaeger et al., 2019; Sutterer et al., 2021), that would suggest some spatial information is preserved. Additionally, we do not know if this observation generalizes to other cortical areas or other ERP components. However, the discovery has this general implication: A given ERP can arise from qualitatively different neural circuit configurations. This implication entails specific limits on the nature of mechanistic inferences available from ERP measures.
Other aspects of the data deserve further consideration. First, our information theoretic analyses, while yielding clear results through rigorous means, produced values that were of noticeably smaller magnitude than what has been reported in previous studies (e.g., Optican and Richmond, 1987; Reich et al., 2001; Timme and Lapish, 2018). We are not concerned about this difference because no previous study has performed these measures on the relationship between intracortical synaptic currents and extracortical electric fields, so we have no strong prior about the magnitude of information theoretic results to expect. Also, the previous information theoretic analyses have been applied most commonly to measure relationships between pairs of single units, but we are comparing mesoscopic currents with macroscopic EEG, which is likely comprised of the activity of many cortical columns beyond that being concurrently measured intracortically.
Second, the polarity of the N2pc measured concurrently with intracortical laminar activity was opposite what has been previously described in macaque monkeys (Woodman et al., 2007). We believe this is an unfortunate consequence of differences in the referencing arrangements between the original and the present study. The previous work sampled EEG from an electrical lead embedded in the outer skull referenced to either linked ears or a frontal, extracranial electrode. We sampled EEG from LMA contact(s) outside the skull referenced to a rod supporting the LMA, which extended into the brain. As such, imbalanced measurement of the electric fields across the putative generator could lead to the inverted ERP polarity. We should also acknowledge possible differences caused by the presence of the craniotomy over the lunate gyrus. This curiosity can be resolved by sampling EEG from the cranial surface before and after a craniotomy with the alternative referencing arrangements. Regardless of the explanation, though, our findings of a strong association between V4 laminar substrates and the N2pc do not depend on the EEG polarization.
As a final note, it is important to consider what comes next for this program of research. Two avenues seem promising. In this study we are limited in that we only observed relationships between otherwise unaltered signals. While causal manipulations to neural circuits in cognitive tasks come with their own limitations (e.g., you are no longer observing the normal functioning system and consequent behavior), much could be gleaned about relative contributions from direct stimulation or inactivation of the putative circuitry generating these electric fields. In a similar vein, a biophysical modeling approach will yield more information on the relationships of attention-associated signals across scales. That is, by now knowing something about the circuit and mechanism yielding these attention-associated electric fields, we are able to use biophysically plausible computational models to gain further insight through simulations. Both approaches seem well suited to build on the findings detailed in this study. Ultimately the goal through these means and beyond should be to bridge the gap between what we know of the neurophysiology of attention at the microscopic scale to human attention-associated signals such as the N2pc.
Materials and methods
Reagent type (species) or resource | Designation | Source or reference | Identifiers | Additional information |
---|---|---|---|---|
Biological sample (Macaca radiata) | Bonnet macaque; Ca, He | Wake Forest University, NC, USA | V4 laminar neurophysiology subjects | |
Biological sample (Macaca radiata) | Bonnet macaque; P | University of Colorado, CO, USA | 10/20 EEG subject | |
Biological sample (Macaca mulatta) | Rhesus macaque; Z | Lovelace Biomedical: http://www.lovelacebiomedical.org/ | 10/20 EEG subject | |
Software, algorithm | MATLAB | Mathworks: https://www.mathworks.com/ | Analysis software | |
Software, algorithm | CURRY | Compumedics Neuroscan: http://www.compumedicsneuroscan.com/ | Analysis software | |
Software, algorithm | Brainstorm | Brainstorm: http://www.neuroimage.usc.edu/brainstorm | Analysis software | |
Software, algorithm | TEMPO | Reflective computing: http://www.greatislandsoftware.com/ | Behavioral control software | |
Other | S-probe | Plexon: http://www.plexon.com/ | Recording electrode array | |
Other | Electrophysiology equipment; MAP | Plexon: http://www.plexon.com/ | 10/20 EEG recording system | |
Other | Electrophysiology equipment; RZ2; PZ5 | Tucker-Davis Technologies: http://www.tdt.com/ | V4 laminar neurophysiology recording system | |
Other | Eye tracker; Eye Link II | SR Research: http://www.sr-research.com/ | Monocular eye tracking system | |
Other | Ceramic screws | Thomas Recording: http://www.thomasrecording.com/ | ||
Other | Dental acrylic | Lang Dental: http://www.langdental.com/ | ||
Other | Recording chamber | Crist Instrument: http://www.cristinstrument.com/ |
Animal care
Request a detailed protocolProcedures were in accordance with National Institutes of Health Guidelines, Association for Assessment and Accreditation of Laboratory Animal Care Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals, and approved by the Vanderbilt Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee following United States Department of Agriculture and Public Health Services policies. Animals were socially housed. Animals were on a 12 hr light-dark cycle and all experimental procedures were conducted in the daytime. Each monkey received nutrient-rich, primate-specific food pellets twice a day. Fresh produce and other forms of environmental enrichment were given at least five times a week.
Surgical procedures
Request a detailed protocolTwo male macaque monkeys (Macaca mulatta monkey Z, 12.5 kg; Macaca radiata monkey P, 9 kg) were implanted with head posts and skull-embedded EEG arrays using previously described techniques (Woodman et al., 2007). One monkey (monkey P) was implanted with a subconjunctive eye coil. Two male macaque monkeys (Macaca radiata; monkey Ca, 7.5 kg; He, 7.3 kg) were implanted with head posts and MR compatible recording chambers with craniotomy over V4. Anesthetic induction was performed with ketamine (10 mg/kg). Monkeys were then catheterized and intubated. Surgeries were conducted aseptically with animals under O2, isoflurane (1–5%) anesthesia. EKG, temperature, and respiration were monitored. Postoperative antibiotics and analgesics were administered. Further detail is documented elsewhere (Woodman et al., 2007; Westerberg et al., 2020a; Westerberg et al., 2020b).
Magnetic resonance imaging
Request a detailed protocolAnesthetized animals were placed in a 3 T MRI scanner. T1-weighted three-dimensional (3D) MPRAGE scans were acquired with a 32-channel head coil equipped for SENSE imaging. Images were acquired using 0.5 mm isotropic voxel resolution with parameters: repetition 5 s, echo 2.5 ms, flip angle 7 degrees.
Visual search tasks
Request a detailed protocolMonkeys performed a color pop-out (monkeys Ca, He, and Z) or T/L (monkey P) search. Search arrays were presented on a CRT monitor at 60 Hz, at 57 cm distance. Stimulus generation and timing were done with TEMPO (Reflective Computing). Event times were assessed with a photodiode on the CRT. We used isoluminant red and green disks on a gray background (pop-out) or uniform gray T’s and L’s on a black background (T/L). Target feature varied within session for monkeys Ca, He, and Z. Monkey P identified the same target on any given session (T or L) but changed specific targets session to session. Trials were initiated by fixating within 1 (monkeys Ca and He) or 2 (monkeys P and Z) degrees of visual angle (dva) of a fixation dot. Time between fixation and array onset was at least 500 ms (monkey P: 500–1000 ms; Z: 500 ms; Ca and He: 750–1250 ms). For monkeys experiencing a range of fixation periods (monkeys Ca, He, P), a nonaging foreperiod function was used to determine the fixation period on a trial-by-trial basis. Arrays comprised of six (monkeys Ca and He) or eight (monkeys P and Z) items. Monkeys P and Z experienced invariable array eccentricity (10 dva) and item size (monkey P: 1.3 × 1.3 dva; Z: 1 × 1 dva). Two items were positioned on the vertical meridian, two on the horizontal, and the four remaining items equally spaced between. Monkeys Ca and He viewed items where size scaled with eccentricity at 0.3 dva per 1 dva eccentricity so that they were smaller than the average V4 RF (Freeman and Simoncelli, 2011). The angular position of items relative to fixation varied session to session so that one item was positioned at the center of the RF. Items were equally spaced relative to each other and located at the same eccentricity. Each trial, one array item was different from the others. Monkeys saccaded to the oddball within 1 (monkeys Ca and He) or 2 s (monkeys P and Z) and maintained fixation within 2–5 dva of the target for more than 400 ms (monkeys Ca, He, and Z: 500 ms; monkey P: 400–800 ms). Note that monkeys Ca, P, and Z were trained to versions of their respective search tasks that included catch trials where no target was present and they were tasked to remain fixating. Monkey He did not experience catch trials. Juice reward was administered following successful completion of the trial. The target item had an equal probability of being located at any of the six or eight locations. Eye movements were monitored at 1 kHz or 250 Hz using a corneal reflection system (monkeys Ca, He, and Z) or a scleral search coil (monkey P), respectively. Microsaccades were detected using an automatic algorithm (Otero-Millan et al., 2014). If the monkey failed to saccade to the target, they experienced a timeout (1–5 s).
10-20 EEG recordings
Request a detailed protocolTwo monkeys with intact skulls (i.e., lacking craniotomies) were implanted with an array of electrodes approximating the human 10–20 system locations (monkey P: FpFz, C3, C4, P3, P4, OL, OR, Oz; monkey Z: FpFz, Fpz, F3, F4, FCz, Cz, C3, C4, Pz, P5, P6, POz, O1, O2, Oz) (Jasper, 1958). Referencing was done using either the FpFz electrode (monkey P) or through linked ears (Z). The impedance of the individual electrodes was confirmed to be between 2 and 5 kOhm at 30 Hz, resembling electrodes used for human EEG. EEG was recorded using a Multichannel Acquisition Processor (Plexon) at 1 kHz and filtered between 0.7 and 170 Hz. Data was aligned to array onset and baseline corrected by subtracting the average activity during the 50 ms preceding the array onset from all timepoints. Data was clipped 20 ms prior to saccade to eliminate eye movement artifacts.
Simultaneous laminar V4 and extracortical recordings
Request a detailed protocolThe extracortical electric fields and laminar V4 neurophysiology were acquired at 24 kHz using a PZ5 and RZ2 (Tucker-Davis). Electric signals between 0.1 Hz and 12 kHz were observable with this system. V4 data was collected from two monkeys (monkey Ca: left hemisphere; He: right) across 30 sessions (monkey Ca: 21; monkey He: 9) using 32-channel linear electrode arrays with 0.1 mm interelectrode spacing (Plexon) introduced through the intact dura mater each session. Recordings were conducted with the electrode in a tube-grounded, reference-grounded configuration which grounds the stainless-steel support tube of the electrode and grounds the reference of the headstage. Arrays spanned layers of V4 with a subset of electrode contacts deliberately left outside of cortex. The extracortical electric field was derived from the most superficial electrode outside the brain (above the dura mater) using the same tube-grounded, referenced-grounded configuration and filtered between 1 and 100 Hz. CSD was computed from the raw signal by first extracting the LFP (signal filtered between 1 and 100 Hz, identical to the extracortical signal) and then taking the second spatial derivative along electrodes (Nicholson and Freeman, 1975; Schroeder et al., 1998; Mehta et al., 2000; Westerberg et al., 2019) and converting voltage to current (Logothetis et al., 2007). We computed the CSD by taking the second spatial derivative of the LFP:
where x is the extracellular voltage at time t measured at an electrode contact at depth d and z is the interelectrode distance and σ is conductivity. Both EEG and CSD were baseline corrected at the trial level by subtracting the average activation during the 300 ms preceding array onset from the response at all timepoints. Extracortical electric field potentials and CSD profiles were clipped 10 ms prior to saccade at the trial level to eliminate the influence of eye movements.
Population spiking was measured and analyzed to supplement primary LFP and CSD results. Multiunit activity was derived through well-documented means (Legatt et al., 1980) and has been demonstrated to be effective across multiple brain areas (Logothetis et al., 2001; Roelfsema et al., 2004; Self et al., 2013; Shapcott et al., 2016; Tovar et al., 2020; Westerberg et al., 2020a; Xing et al., 2009). The broadband neural signal was lowpass filtered at 3 kHz, highpass filtered at 300 Hz, full-wave rectified, and lastly, lowpass filtered at 150 Hz. This signal reliably reflect neural population dynamics (Trautmann et al., 2019). Additionally, multiunit activity in V4 has been shown to reliably reflect attentional modulation (Mehta et al., 2000; Nandy et al., 2017).
Laminar alignment
Request a detailed protocolOrthogonal array penetrations were confirmed online through a reverse-correlation RF mapping procedure (Nandy et al., 2017; Westerberg et al., 2019; Cox et al., 2019a; Cox et al., 2019b; Dougherty et al., 2019; Figure 2—figure supplement 1). RFs were found to represent portions of visual space consistent with previous reports of V4 (Gattass et al., 1988). Positions of recording sites relative to V4 layers were determined using CSD (Schroeder et al., 1998; Nandy et al., 2017). Current sinks following visual stimulation first appear in the granular input layers of cortex, then ascend to the supragranular compartment. Previously described observations of laminar V4 CSD include a sink in the infragranular layers following the ascent to the supragranular layers (Nandy et al., 2017), an observation we do not observe in our data. This is likely because we used task-evoked CSD for alignment with stimulus presentation persisting throughout the measurement interval whereas the descending sink observation was found with very brief stimulus presentations. It is likely that the strength of the persistent supragranular sink is masking the previously reported infragranular sink (Mitzdorf, 1985). We computed CSD and identified the granular input sink session-wise. Sessions were aligned by this input sink. ‘L4’ refers to granular input layer, ‘L2/3’ – supragranular layers, and ‘L5/6’ – infragranular layers. Each laminar compartment was assigned the same number of recording sites to alleviate biases during analysis.
Inverse modeling
Request a detailed protocolInverse modeling of 10–20 EEG recordings was performed in CURRY 8 (Compumedics Neuroscan). 3D head reconstruction was created for each monkey (P and Z) using the boundary element method (Hämäläinen and Sarvas, 1989). This method takes into account individual monkey’s surface morphologies to create models of cortex surface, inner and outer skull, and skin boundaries. This model was used in conjunction with EEG to compute a voltage distribution over the cortical surface. We calculated the current density with sLORETA, which calculates a minimum norm least squares that divides current by the size of its associated error bar, yielding F scores of activation. sLORETA produces blurred but accurate localizations of point sources (Pascual-Marqui, 2002). Other algorithms such as minimum norm and SWARM were modeled as well, with agreement between models sufficient not to change any conclusions.
Information theory analyses
Request a detailed protocolInformation theory (Shannon, 1948) analyses were chosen for several reasons. First, information theory analyses yield results in terms of ‘bits’ which can be used to directly compare effect sizes across measurement methods (e.g., CSD, extracortical signal, and array composition [directed spatial attention]). Next, these analyses are inherently multivariate and able to capture linear and nonlinear relationships. Furthermore, information theory is model independent and does not necessitate a specific hypothetical structure in order to detect relationships between signals. This combination allows us to detect relationships between the extracortical signal and CSD signal that might not be linear and therefore would not be captured by linear models or correlation analyses. We chose to measure pairwise mutual information and information transmission to gauge the relationships between our three ‘signals’ (e.g., extracortical, CSD, and array composition [directed spatial attention]). Mutual information is the reduction in uncertainty in one variable afforded by another known variable. That is, mutual information is greater when you know the state of one variable covaries with the state of the other variable. If the two variables do not correspond well, mutual information is low. Therefore, the reduction in uncertainty is formalized as ‘information’ which is relayed in bits. Mathematically, mutual information is captured by the following equation (Cover and Thomas, 2006; Beer and Williams, 2015):
where H(X) and H(X|Y) are the entropy X and X given Y, respectively. Entropy for a signal (S) is computed by:
where p(s) is the probability distribution for signal s and i is the signal state. Therefore, mutual information can be computed probabilistically by:
where p(x), p(y) are the probability distributions for X and Y, and p(x,y) is the joint probability distribution of X and Y across signal states i and j for X and Y, respectively.
While mutual information describes the relationship between the two signals (for our purpose: CSD and the extracortical signal, CSD and directed spatial attention, or the extracortical signal and directed spatial attention), it does not allow for the evaluation of two signals regarding a third (e.g., CSD and the extracortical signal regarding directed spatial attention). For analyses where we want to understand information regarding the allocation of directed attention from the synaptic currents in V4 to the extracortical signal, we use a modified equation rooted in the same entropy/mutual information principles. In computing information transmission, we are interested in the information about X (directed spatial attention), transferred from Y (CSD in V4) to Z (extracortical signal) formalized as:
where past and future describe the timepoints when the data is taken from. The information transmission (IT) is taken as the difference between two minimum information calculations. The minimum information (Imin) is computed regarding the combination of the individual signals (S1 and S2) at the specified intervals as:
where p(x) is the probability distribution for signal X and x are the possible states of X. By taking into account different timepoints for the signals, we can interpret this computation as the information about X (directed spatial attention) shared by Ypast (e.g., earlier CSD in V4) and Zfuture (e.g., later extracortical signal) that was not already in Zpast (e.g., earlier extracortical signal).
Above information theory analyses were performed using the Neuroscience Information Theory Toolbox (Timme and Lapish, 2018). Pairwise mutual information and information transmission were computed at each timepoint across trials for each session using default parameters. Five uniform count bins were used for data binning; 10 ms was used for time lag for information transmission. Only correct trials were included. Information theory measures were computed for each millisecond for the entire interval displayed in each analyses’ respective figure panel. CSD for each laminar compartment was computed by taking the average activity of five sites at the trial level included in each laminar compartment. For mutual information between target position and the extracortical signal, target position was binary where target was either contra- or ipsilaterally presented. For computations within V4, target position was binary where target was either in the RF or positioned opposite the RF; 5000 Monte Carlo simulations were used to generate a distribution of null model values which experimental values were compared to (α = 0.05).
Feature selectivity
Request a detailed protocolFor each recording site within a column, gamma power (30–150 Hz) (Maier et al., 2010) responses were computed when either a red distractor was presented to the RF of the column or when a green distractor was present to the RF. Responses were taken as the average activation 75–200 ms following array onset. An index was computed from these responses by subtracting the 2 and dividing by their sum (CSI). Values were therefore bounded between –1 and 1 where larger magnitude indicates greater selectivity for green (toward –1) or red (toward 1). CCSI was computed as the average of CSIs along the entire column. We performed Wilcoxon signed rank tests on the distribution of CSIs across the recording sites of a given cortical column to determine whether a column was significantly color selective (α = 0.05). The selective columns were included in feature selectivity analyses where the preferred color and non-preferred color were defined as the color that elicited greater and lesser responses, respectively.
Estimating field potential from CSD
Request a detailed protocolWe calculated the ERP at arbitrary positions from the measured laminar CSD (ERPcal) using a previously described model (Nicholson and Llinas, 1971; Kajikawa and Schroeder, 2011).
where ERPcal at depth i (dt) for each timepoint (t) is taken as the sum of CSD at depths j (dj) for each timepoint divided by the Euclidean distance to account for the diminishing impact of local currents on more distant field potentials. The factor A acts only as a scaling factor and we cannot accurately estimate the magnitude of the 1D CSD-derived waveform, so we eliminate this parameter from the calculation. This omission is consistent with previous reports (Kajikawa and Schroeder, 2011) and limits our comparisons of observed ERP and ERPcal to only shape. However, magnitude differences can be observed between conditions for ERPobs and ERPcal, independently. Also, for our purposes, we set h to 0 as we assume that our observed CSD and the calculated ERP are in the same vertical plane.
Data availability
Data supporting the findings documented in this study are freely available online through Dryad at https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.djh9w0w15.
-
Dryad Digital RepositoryData from: Laminar microcircuitry of visual cortex producing attention-associated electric fields.https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.djh9w0w15
References
-
Information processing and dynamics in minimally cognitive agentsCognitive Science 39:1–38.https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12142
-
The neural instantiation of a priority mapCurrent Opinion in Psychology 29:108–112.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.01.002
-
On the origin of event-related potentials indexing covert attentional selection during visual searchJournal of Neurophysiology 102:2375–2386.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00680.2009
-
Where Does EEG Come From and What Does It Mean?Trends in Neurosciences 40:208–218.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2017.02.004
-
Prefrontal Control of Visual DistractionCurrent Biology 28:414–420.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2017.12.023
-
Spiking Suppression Precedes Cued Attentional Enhancement of Neural Responses in Primary Visual CortexCerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y 29:77–90.https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhx305
-
Temporal dynamics of binocular integration in primary visual cortexJournal of Vision 19:1–13.https://doi.org/10.1167/19.12.13
-
Binocular Modulation of Monocular V1 NeuronsCurrent Biology 29:381–391.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.12.004
-
A Canonical Microcircuit for NeocortexNeural Computation 1:480–488.https://doi.org/10.1162/neco.1989.1.4.480
-
A functional microcircuit for cat visual cortexThe Journal of Physiology 440:735–769.https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1991.sp018733
-
The N2pc component as an indicator of attentional selectivityElectroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 99:225–234.https://doi.org/10.1016/0013-4694(96)95711-9
-
Modulation of the contrast response function by electrical microstimulation of the macaque frontal eye fieldThe Journal of Neuroscience 29:10683–10694.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0673-09.2009
-
Selective modulation of cortical state during spatial attentionScience (New York, N.Y.) 354:1140–1144.https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aag1420
-
Covert Spatial Attention Speeds Target IndividuationThe Journal of Neuroscience 40:2717–2726.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2962-19.2020
-
Modulation of oscillatory neuronal synchronization by selective visual attentionScience (New York, N.Y.) 291:1560–1563.https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1055465
-
Visuotopic organization and extent of V3 and V4 of the macaqueThe Journal of Neuroscience 8:1831–1845.
-
Microcircuitry of agranular frontal cortex: testing the generality of the canonical cortical microcircuitThe Journal of Neuroscience 34:5355–5369.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5127-13.2014
-
Review on solving the inverse problem in EEG source analysisJournal of Neuroengineering and Rehabilitation 5:25.https://doi.org/10.1186/1743-0003-5-25
-
High-frequency, long-range coupling between prefrontal and visual cortex during attentionScience (New York, N.Y.) 324:1207–1210.https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1171402
-
Realistic conductivity geometry model of the human head for interpretation of neuromagnetic dataIEEE Transactions on Bio-Medical Engineering 36:165–171.https://doi.org/10.1109/10.16463
-
Examining local network processing using multi-contact laminar electrode recordingJournal of Visualized Experiments 8:2806.https://doi.org/10.3791/2806
-
Electrophysiological indices of target and distractor processing in visual searchJournal of Cognitive Neuroscience 21:760–775.https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21039
-
Neural sources of focused attention in visual searchCerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y 10:1233–1241.https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/10.12.1233
-
The ten-twenty electrode system of the international federationElectroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 10:370–375.
-
Organization of color-selective neurons in macaque visual area V4Journal of Neurophysiology 102:15–27.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.90624.2008
-
Electrophysiological evidence for parallel and serial processing during visual searchPerception & Psychophysics 48:603–617.https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03211606
-
Neural mechanisms of spatial selective attention in areas V1, V2, and V4 of macaque visual cortexJournal of Neurophysiology 77:24–42.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1997.77.1.24
-
Distinct superficial and deep laminar domains of activity in the visual cortex during rest and stimulationFrontiers in Systems Neuroscience 4:31.https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2010.00031
-
BookThe feature similarity gain model of attention: unifying multiplicative effects of spatial and feature-based attentionIn: Itti L, Rees G, Tsotsos JK, editors. Neurobiology of Attention. Academic Press. pp. 300–304.https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-012375731-9/50053-7
-
Effects of attention on orientation-tuning functions of single neurons in macaque cortical area V4The Journal of Neuroscience 19:431–441.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-01-00431.1999
-
Intermodal selective attention in monkeys. II: physiological mechanisms of modulationCerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y 10:359–370.https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/10.4.359
-
EEG source imagingClinical Neurophysiology 115:2195–2222.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2004.06.001
-
Selective attention gates visual processing in the extrastriate cortexScience (New York, N.Y.) 229:782–784.https://doi.org/10.1126/science.4023713
-
The columnar organization of the neocortexBrain: A Journal of Neurology 120 ( Pt 4):701–722.https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/120.4.701
-
Effects of neural synchrony on surface EEGCerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y 24:1045–1053.https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhs389
-
Theory of current source-density analysis and determination of conductivity tensor for anuran cerebellumJournal of Neurophysiology 38:356–368.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1975.38.2.356
-
Segregated pathways carrying frontally derived top-down signals to visual areas MT and V4 in macaquesThe Journal of Neuroscience 32:6851–6858.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6295-11.2012
-
BookElectric Fields of the Brain: The Neurophysics of EEGOxford University Press.https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195050387.001.0001
-
Unsupervised clustering method to detect microsaccadesJournal of Vision 14:18.https://doi.org/10.1167/14.2.18
-
Standardized low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (sLORETA): technical detailsMethods and Findings in Experimental and Clinical Pharmacology 24 Suppl D:5–12.
-
FEF-Controlled Alpha Delay Activity Precedes Stimulus-Induced Gamma-Band Activity in Visual CortexThe Journal of Neuroscience 37:4117–4127.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3015-16.2017
-
Formal and attribute-specific information in primary visual cortexJournal of Neurophysiology 85:305–318.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.2001.85.1.305
-
Competitive mechanisms subserve attention in macaque areas V2 and V4The Journal of Neuroscience 19:1736–1753.
-
Effects of attention and stimulus interaction on visual responses of inferior temporal neurons in macaqueJournal of Neurophysiology 60:344–364.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1988.60.1.344
-
Topography of visual cortex connections with frontal eye field in macaque: convergence and segregation of processing streamsThe Journal of Neuroscience 15:4464–4487.
-
Simultaneous EEG and fMRI in the macaque monkey at 4.7 TeslaMagnetic Resonance Imaging 24:335–342.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mri.2005.12.024
-
Subcortical contributions to the surface-recorded flash-VEP in the awake macaqueElectroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 84:219–231.https://doi.org/10.1016/0168-5597(92)90003-t
-
A spatiotemporal profile of visual system activation revealed by current source density analysis in the awake macaqueCerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y 8:575–592.https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/8.7.575
-
A Mathematical Theory of CommunicationBell System Technical Journal 27:379–423.https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1538-7305.1948.tb01338.x
-
Stimulus-dependent spiking relationships with the EEGJournal of Neurophysiology 114:1468–1482.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00427.2015
-
Decoding chromaticity and luminance from patterns of EEG activityPsychophysiology 58:e13779.https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13779
-
Functional organization for color and orientation in macaque V4Nature Neuroscience 13:1542–1548.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2676
-
Neuronal basis of covert spatial attention in the frontal eye fieldThe Journal of Neuroscience 25:9479–9487.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0741-05.2005
-
A Tutorial for Information Theory in NeuroscienceENeuro 5:ENEURO.0052-18.2018.https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0052-18.2018
-
Search for color “center(s)” in macaque visual cortexCerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y 14:353–363.https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhh001
-
Cortical Connections of Area V4 in the MacaqueCerebral Cortex 18:477–499.https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhm061
-
Performance Monitoring during Visual PrimingJournal of Cognitive Neuroscience 32:515–526.https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01499
-
Neural mechanism of priming in visual searchAttention, Perception & Psychophysics 83:587–602.https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-020-02118-8
-
Spatial Spread of the Local Field Potential and its Laminar Variation in Visual CortexJournal of Neuroscience 29:11540–11549.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2573-09.2009
-
Colour coding in rhesus monkey prestriate cortexBrain Research 53:422–427.https://doi.org/10.1016/0006-8993(73)90227-8
Decision letter
-
Supratim RayReviewing Editor; Indian Institute of Science, India
-
Chris I BakerSenior Editor; National Institute of Mental Health, National Institutes of Health, United States
-
Steven J LuckReviewer
-
Anirvan S NandyReviewer; Yale School of Medicine, United States
Our editorial process produces two outputs: i) public reviews designed to be posted alongside the preprint for the benefit of readers; ii) feedback on the manuscript for the authors, including requests for revisions, shown below. We also include an acceptance summary that explains what the editors found interesting or important about the work.
Decision letter after peer review:
Thank you for submitting your article "Laminar microcircuitry of visual cortex producing attention-associated electric fields" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by 3 peer reviewers, one of whom is a member of our Board of Reviewing Editors, and the evaluation has been overseen by Chris Baker as the Senior Editor. The following individuals involved in review of your submission have agreed to reveal their identity: Steven J. Luck (Reviewer #2); Anirvan S Nandy (Reviewer #3).
The reviewers have discussed their reviews with one another, and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this to help you prepare a revised submission.
Essential revisions:
1. Effects of attention in V4 generally start earlier (~100 ms). It is unclear why no effect is observed during earlier time periods in these data. To make better comparison with previous studies (such as Nandy et al., 2017), the authors should show the average PSTHs in supragranular, granular and infragranular layers during both target-out versus target-in conditions. Interestingly, Nandy and colleagues found largest changes in firing rates in the granular layer. To better understand the ERP outside the cortex, the authors should also show the average LFPs in the three layers, for target-in and target-out conditions. It is surprising that MI analysis reveals no significant information about the target in granular layer – given that some attentional effects are seen in upstream areas such as V1 and V2.
2. Eye position analysis: my understanding is that the animals could make a saccade as soon as the arrays were displayed. Given that the main effect of attention is observed after ~150-200 ms, the potential effect of saccade preparation could be important. There could also be small eye movements before the saccade. Given that the RFs were quite foveal for one monkey and not too far from the fixation window, and the effect of attention appears to be quite late, detailed analysis of eye position and microsaccades is needed to rule out the possibility of differences in eye movements between target in and target-out conditions influencing the results. A timeline and some analysis of eye movement patterns would be appropriate. The authors should also clearly mention the mean and SD of the saccade onset.
3. Attention studies typically keep the stimulus in the RF the same to tease out the effect of attention from stimulus selectivity. Ideally, the comparison should be between the two green (or red) in RF conditions as shown in Figure 4A. However, these results are shown only after pooling across all color selective columns. This comparison should be shown from Figure 2 itself (i.e., Figure 2C should have green in the RF and red target outside).
4. Information has been well characterized in a large number of previous studies (generally yielding values between a few bits/s, see for example, Reich et. al, 2001, JNP). Here, the absolute value of mutual information seems rather low. This may be due to the way the information is computed. A discussion about these reasons would be useful for scientists interested in information-theoretic measures.
5. Dependence on feature preference: The effect of spatial and feature attention is well studied. (A multiplicative gain model of spatial attention would predict a larger increase in firing rates and perhaps other signals such as CSD) for preferred versus non-preferred signals. Feature similarity gain model would predict the red preferring columns to increase their activity and green preferring columns to reduce their activity when the animal is attending to the feature red, irrespective of which stimulus is in the receptive field. Here, the task is a pop-out task which likely has both a spatial and feature attention component. The authors should discuss their findings in these contexts. Further, the authors should discuss whether their findings could just be a reflection of the magnitude of the change (which could be larger for preferred versus non-preferred stimulus). The information-theoretic measure should ideally not depend on the absolute magnitude, but these quantities often get biased in non-trivial ways based on the magnitude. Does information transmission depend on the magnitudes of firing rates/CSDs?
6. For columns that were not feature selective, is there an effect of attention? Does the magnitude of N2pc change depend on color selectivity? I think that should be the case based on Figure 4H and 4I, but a plot and/or some quantification would be useful.
7. The most challenging aspect of the study is to provide a solid link from the intracortical activity to the voltage on the cortical surface, and then to the monkey scalp ERPs, and finally to human ERPs. Toward that end, the present study relied entirely on correlational evidence, rather than experimental manipulations. That's quite appropriate for a first step, but it must be considered an important limitation on the conclusions that can be drawn. It would be wonderful if future research took the next step of providing experimental evidence.
8. There are also some troubling aspects of the existing evidence. The scalp ERP effect in this study, and the prior work from this group, is a positive voltage over the contralateral hemisphere, whereas in humans the voltage is negative. This may well reflect the orientation of the relevant cortical surface in monkeys versus humans. However, the voltage on the cortical surface in the present study was negative contralateral to the target, not positive. Unless this opposite voltage on the cortical surface relative to the scalp reflects something about the reference site for the cortical surface electrode, then this makes it difficult to link the intracortical effects and cortical surface effects to the scalp ERP effects. Also, the CSD was negative in the upper layers and positive in the lower layers, again suggesting that the voltage should be negative contralateral to the target on the surface. Ironically, this polarity is what would be expected from the human brain, where a contralateral negativity is observed. The oddity seems to be the contralateral positivity in the monkey scalp data. Also, the cortical surface voltage exhibits a polarity reversal at approximately 180 ms, which is not seen in the intracortical CSD. One possible explanation for the discrepancy is that the scalp voltage likely comes from multiple brain areas besides V4. If, for example, areas on the ventral surface of the occipital and temporal lobes produce stronger scalp voltages than V4 under the present conditions, the opposite orientation of these areas relative to the cortical surface would be expected to produce a positive voltage at the scalp electrodes. The manuscript notes that multiple areas probably contribute to the scalp ERPs and argues that the pattern of intracortical CSD results obtained in V4 will likely generalize to those areas. That seems quite plausible. Moreover, the results are interesting independent of their link to scalp ERPs. Thus, the present results are important even if the scalp polarity issue cannot be definitively resolved at this time.
9. There are also some significant concerns about the filters. The high-pass cutoff was high enough that it could have produced artifactual opposite polarity deflections in the data. If causal filters were applied (e.g., in hardware during the recordings), these artifactual deflections would have been after rather than before the initial deflection, possibly explaining the polarity reversal at 180 ms. If noncausal filters were applied in software, this would be a larger problem and could produce artifacts at both the beginning and end of the waveform. Moreover, the filters were different for the CSD data and the extracortical voltages, which is somewhat problematic for the information theoretic comparisons of these two data sources (but is likely to reduce rather than inflate the effects).
The filter for the intracranial recordings was listed as "0.1-12kHz". Was the high pass cutoff really at 0.1 kHz (100 Hz), or was it supposed to be 0.1 Hz to 12 kHz? A cutoff at 100 Hz would make it impossible to see field potentials corresponding to the N2pc. For the extracortical electrode, the 1 Hz cutoff is still quite high. I think you'd need to show how it impacts an N2pc-like artificial waveform (e.g., one half cycle of a 5 Hz sine wave) so that the effects of the filter on the observed data can be estimated. Also, the authors might want to apply offline filters so that the same effective bandpass is used for the extracortical voltage and the intracortical CSD. (This could be shown in a supplemental figure.)
10. The method section states correctly that "current sinks following visual stimulation first appear in the granular input layer of the cortex, then ascend and descend to extra granular compartments". However in the example CSDs shown in Figure 2, Figure 3, Figure S3 there is no visible current sink in the infra-granular layers. Instead, the identified infra-granular layers show a prolonged current source (e.g. Figure S5B,C), which is unexpected. Can the authors comment on this discrepancy?
11. The example RF profile shown in Figure S5A, although aligned, looks a little strange in that the RFs taper off rapidly in the infra-granular layer. Is this the best representative example? It will be important to see other examples of RF alignment.
12. The study used LFP power in the gamma range to compute the response ratio between red and green stimuli. LFPs measured across the cortical depth are highly correlated, and so would gamma power estimated from the LFPs. Given this, how meaningful is the laminar analysis shown in Figure 4B? How confidently can it be established that the LFP derived gamma power estimates have laminar specificity?
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.72139.sa1Author response
Essential revisions:
1. Effects of attention in V4 generally start earlier (~100 ms). It is unclear why no effect is observed during earlier time periods in these data. To make better comparison with previous studies (such as Nandy et al., 2017), the authors should show the average PSTHs in supragranular, granular and infragranular layers during both target-out versus target-in conditions. Interestingly, Nandy and colleagues found largest changes in firing rates in the granular layer. To better understand the ERP outside the cortex, the authors should also show the average LFPs in the three layers, for target-in and target-out conditions. It is surprising that MI analysis reveals no significant information about the target in granular layer – given that some attentional effects are seen in upstream areas such as V1 and V2.
We have created a new figure showing multiunit activity and LFP across the layers in both attention conditions. It is included here for convenience. Accompanying text has been added to the Results and Discussion sections to address the reviewers’ comments.
The timing of differentiation between attended and unattended in the population spiking activity is evident in both MUA and LFP. We note that the largest magnitude difference in population spiking between attention conditions was observed in the middle layers, consistent with Nandy et al., 2017. We wish to highlight two observations.
First, with respect to the timing of attentional modulation, it should be noted that the attention task used in our study (pop-out visual search) is different from that used by Nandy et al., 2017, Neuron (cued change detection). The timing of “effects of attention” vary according to stimulus properties and task demands (the number of publications demonstrating this is too long to list). Hence, we do not expect equivalence between the times we measure and times Nandy et al. measure. Nonetheless we are happy to include the requested supplementary figure with that caveat in mind.
Second, with respect to the surprising observation of a relationship between activity in the granular layer and the extracortical signal, we think it is important to remember that these information theoretic analyses are not simply correlational. That is, attentional modulation might be observed in both signals, but if the covariation of these signals trial-to-trial does not exist, then we would not expect a relationship in the mutual information analysis.
2. Eye position analysis: my understanding is that the animals could make a saccade as soon as the arrays were displayed. Given that the main effect of attention is observed after ~150-200 ms, the potential effect of saccade preparation could be important. There could also be small eye movements before the saccade. Given that the RFs were quite foveal for one monkey and not too far from the fixation window, and the effect of attention appears to be quite late, detailed analysis of eye position and microsaccades is needed to rule out the possibility of differences in eye movements between target in and target-out conditions influencing the results. A timeline and some analysis of eye movement patterns would be appropriate. The authors should also clearly mention the mean and SD of the saccade onset.
The reviewer makes a valuable observation. Saccades will influence the electrical signals, something we are quite familiar with (e.g., Godlove et al., 2011, J Neurophysiol). In an effort to combat this, we have two points worth noting. First, as was the case in the initial submission (which remains the same in the revision), we have clipped signals on a trial-by-trial basis prior to eye movements. By doing so, we cannot have an influence of the motor-related polarization of the task-demanded eye movement on the data.
Second, we have prepared a microsaccade analysis – and accompanying newly added supplementary figure included here for convenience – to determine whether they might be driving the results. To do this, we identified trials where microsaccades occurred using a well-regarded microsaccade detection algorithm (Otero-Millan et al., 2014, J Vis). We then reperformed the information theoretic analysis across sessions after removing trials where microsaccades were detected. Briefly, we found that the information theoretic relationship persists in the absence of trials where microsaccades occurred. We believe this serves as evidence that microsaccades are not responsible for the information theoretic findings.
To address the reviewer’s last point, we have included response time data (defined as the saccade onset latency) in the Results.
3. Attention studies typically keep the stimulus in the RF the same to tease out the effect of attention from stimulus selectivity. Ideally, the comparison should be between the two green (or red) in RF conditions as shown in Figure 4A. However, these results are shown only after pooling across all color selective columns. This comparison should be shown from Figure 2 itself (i.e., Figure 2C should have green in the RF and red target outside).
We have clarified prior to Figure 4 that we used all trials including both colors in each of the attention conditions. That is, while the cartoon in Figure 2 shows only green-attended and red-unattended conditions, green-unattended and red-attended conditions were also included in this analysis. As the proportion of red-target and green-target trials was matched, this first analysis was designed in such a way that the influence of stimulus color should be minimized, yet all trials could still contribute to the calculation. We have included a new supplementary figure (included here for convenience) which is what we believe the reviewer requests. In this addition, we perform the information theoretic computation on only stimulus matched conditions. Briefly, we find that this approach does not seem to alter the temporal profile of information theoretic findings.
4. Information has been well characterized in a large number of previous studies (generally yielding values between a few bits/s, see for example, Reich et. al, 2001, JNP). Here, the absolute value of mutual information seems rather low. This may be due to the way the information is computed. A discussion about these reasons would be useful for scientists interested in information-theoretic measures.
We agree that the exact magnitude of our information theoretic analyses in curious. And while these methods have been widely characterized – they have not been characterized, to our knowledge, in relating intracortical laminar currents to extracortical field potentials. As such, we do not have a strong prior as to what we should expect magnitude-wise. We have expanded the discussion to note this observation and provide potential reasons as to why this might be the case. The conclusion being that further application of these methods to these datatypes is necessary to really gain a fuller sense of what should and shouldn’t be expected.
5. Dependence on feature preference: The effect of spatial and feature attention is well studied. (A multiplicative gain model of spatial attention would predict a larger increase in firing rates and perhaps other signals such as CSD) for preferred versus non-preferred signals. Feature similarity gain model would predict the red preferring columns to increase their activity and green preferring columns to reduce their activity when the animal is attending to the feature red, irrespective of which stimulus is in the receptive field. Here, the task is a pop-out task which likely has both a spatial and feature attention component. The authors should discuss their findings in these contexts. Further, the authors should discuss whether their findings could just be a reflection of the magnitude of the change (which could be larger for preferred versus non-preferred stimulus). The information-theoretic measure should ideally not depend on the absolute magnitude, but these quantities often get biased in non-trivial ways based on the magnitude. Does information transmission depend on the magnitudes of firing rates/CSDs?
The relationship of these findings to the specificities of attentional mechanisms and models is indeed intriguing. As the reviewer suggested, this task likely engages both spatial and feature attention – however, the design was not such that they can be disentangled wholly. We have added text to the Discussion to reflect this consideration. As for the potential influence of response magnitude changes on the information theoretic analyses – the exact parameters were chosen to mitigate concerns about magnitude. That is, we chose a uniform count binning procedure on the data which eliminates potential issues such as outliers driving relationships as well as the changes in variability associated with increases in magnitude. Moreover, the uniform count binning procedure results with states rather than magnitudes which again mitigates response-magnitude-driven effects.
6. For columns that were not feature selective, is there an effect of attention? Does the magnitude of N2pc change depend on color selectivity? I think that should be the case based on Figure 4H and 4I, but a plot and/or some quantification would be useful.
These questions have been addressed in a newly added supplementary figure as well as quantification in the Results. Briefly, we did find an effect of attention non-selective columns. Also, we found the magnitude of N2pc did not depend on color-selectivity of the intracortical recording. The results were reported as:
“We also tested whether feature selective columns, on average, transmitted more information than their non-feature-selective counterparts. […] This invariance is expected because extracortical EEG spatially integrates signals from multiple cortical columns.”
7. The most challenging aspect of the study is to provide a solid link from the intracortical activity to the voltage on the cortical surface, and then to the monkey scalp ERPs, and finally to human ERPs. Toward that end, the present study relied entirely on correlational evidence, rather than experimental manipulations. That's quite appropriate for a first step, but it must be considered an important limitation on the conclusions that can be drawn. It would be wonderful if future research took the next step of providing experimental evidence.
We appreciate the reviewer noting that this manuscript is a valuable step in linking attention-associated electrophysiological signals across species. We also recognize that there is much work to be done in this domain. As requested, we have added to the Discussion the limitation of this type of study as well as what should be considered valuable next steps in this program of research.
8. There are also some troubling aspects of the existing evidence. The scalp ERP effect in this study, and the prior work from this group, is a positive voltage over the contralateral hemisphere, whereas in humans the voltage is negative. This may well reflect the orientation of the relevant cortical surface in monkeys versus humans. However, the voltage on the cortical surface in the present study was negative contralateral to the target, not positive. Unless this opposite voltage on the cortical surface relative to the scalp reflects something about the reference site for the cortical surface electrode, then this makes it difficult to link the intracortical effects and cortical surface effects to the scalp ERP effects. Also, the CSD was negative in the upper layers and positive in the lower layers, again suggesting that the voltage should be negative contralateral to the target on the surface. Ironically, this polarity is what would be expected from the human brain, where a contralateral negativity is observed. The oddity seems to be the contralateral positivity in the monkey scalp data. Also, the cortical surface voltage exhibits a polarity reversal at approximately 180 ms, which is not seen in the intracortical CSD. One possible explanation for the discrepancy is that the scalp voltage likely comes from multiple brain areas besides V4. If, for example, areas on the ventral surface of the occipital and temporal lobes produce stronger scalp voltages than V4 under the present conditions, the opposite orientation of these areas relative to the cortical surface would be expected to produce a positive voltage at the scalp electrodes. The manuscript notes that multiple areas probably contribute to the scalp ERPs and argues that the pattern of intracortical CSD results obtained in V4 will likely generalize to those areas. That seems quite plausible. Moreover, the results are interesting independent of their link to scalp ERPs. Thus, the present results are important even if the scalp polarity issue cannot be definitively resolved at this time.
We thank the reviewer for expressing that the results are important whether this polarity difference can be resolved. This is an interesting observation and quite important to consider carefully. First, it is worth reiterating that the referencing setup in our ‘10/20’ monkeys was different than that for the monkeys where intracranial recordings took place. Specifically, the 10/20 recordings were more similar to our previous reports of monkey EEG (e.g., Woodman et al., 2007, PNAS; Cohen et al., 2009, J Neurophysiol; Purcell et al., 2013, J Neurophysiol). Recordings from these monkeys used either a frontal EEG electrode (approximately FpFz) or linked ears for referencing. These yielded the positive-going N2pc and contrast the negative-going N2pc found in humans. The V4 laminar recordings – and their accompanying extracortical signal – used a different referencing setup that we believe is the most likely candidate for the observed difference. Specifically, these recordings used a tied ground-reference setup which incorporated the support rod of the linear multielectrode array. This support rod extended into the brain meaning we had a neural tissue grounded signal and that the reference spanned the neural generator. Therefore, if we are not measuring both sides of the electric field across the generator equally, we can observe an inverted signal. Unfortunately, we cannot observe the 10/20 EEG distribution with an intracranial reference. Ideally, this could be resolved by an experiment where referencing setups are tested before and after performing craniotomy with a series of reference locations used to understand where exactly this flipping of polarization takes place. We have added this consideration to the Discussion and more thoroughly detailed the referencing setups in the Methods.
9. There are also some significant concerns about the filters. The high-pass cutoff was high enough that it could have produced artifactual opposite polarity deflections in the data. If causal filters were applied (e.g., in hardware during the recordings), these artifactual deflections would have been after rather than before the initial deflection, possibly explaining the polarity reversal at 180 ms. If noncausal filters were applied in software, this would be a larger problem and could produce artifacts at both the beginning and end of the waveform. Moreover, the filters were different for the CSD data and the extracortical voltages, which is somewhat problematic for the information theoretic comparisons of these two data sources (but is likely to reduce rather than inflate the effects).
The filter for the intracranial recordings was listed as "0.1-12kHz". Was the high pass cutoff really at 0.1 kHz (100 Hz), or was it supposed to be 0.1 Hz to 12 kHz? A cutoff at 100 Hz would make it impossible to see field potentials corresponding to the N2pc. For the extracortical electrode, the 1 Hz cutoff is still quite high. I think you'd need to show how it impacts an N2pc-like artificial waveform (e.g., one half cycle of a 5 Hz sine wave) so that the effects of the filter on the observed data can be estimated. Also, the authors might want to apply offline filters so that the same effective bandpass is used for the extracortical voltage and the intracortical CSD. (This could be shown in a supplemental figure.)
In revisiting the description of the recording system and filters, we see how some information was conveyed poorly. The language describing the recording in the original submission suggested that online filters were applied to the data as it was being recorded. This was not the case. We have changed that language so that it reads as the data was being collected at a sampling frequency sufficient to observe data between 0.1 Hz and 12 kHz rather than the data being filtered between 0.1 Hz and 12 kHz. Related, the reviewer is correct on the typo regarding units. We did mean 0.1 Hz rather than 0.1 kHz; this has been corrected in the revision. Also, it appears that the description of the processing sequence regarding CSD was ambiguous in the original submission. The CSD underwent the same offline, bandpass filtering procedure (1-100 Hz) as the extracortical signal. We have clarified the Methods accordingly.
Additionally, we have conducted the filtering simulation to see how our filter choice affects a signal representative of the N2pc – as suggested by the reviewer. We have included the result as Author response image 1. Using the same filtering parameters used throughout the manuscript on a 5 Hz half sine wave, we find that the filtering process modestly attenuates the frequency band and period where we expect the N2pc to exist. We have plotted the figure with similar conventions to the manuscript so that the time period and polarization component representative of the N2pc is highlighted.
10. The method section states correctly that "current sinks following visual stimulation first appear in the granular input layer of the cortex, then ascend and descend to extra granular compartments". However in the example CSDs shown in Figure 2, Figure 3, Figure S3 there is no visible current sink in the infra-granular layers. Instead, the identified infra-granular layers show a prolonged current source (e.g. Figure S5B,C), which is unexpected. Can the authors comment on this discrepancy?
We have clarified the Methods to reflect the observations of our data and why they may differ from previous reports. We believe the discrepancy is likely due to the stimulus conditions used to evoke the CSD profile. Specifically, the descending infragranular sink in visual cortical columns has most commonly been described when CSD was computed while monkeys view briefly presented flashes or stimuli (e.g., Schroeder et al., 1998, Cereb Cortex). However, our study uses task evoked CSD to perform the alignment. Importantly, this means there is a persistent stimulus in the receptive field. We believe this persistent stimulus, rather than a flashed stimulus, leads to a persistent, strong sink in the superficial layers of cortex which would mask any current sink present in the infragranular layers (Mitzdorf, 1985, Physiol Rev). This is an observation we made in previous reports (Task evoked CSD: Westerberg et al., 2019, J Neurophysiol vs. Flash evoked CSD: Maier et al., 2010, Front Syst Neurosci), albeit in V1 instead of V4. Given the latency offset between putative granular and supragranular sinks, that we observe receptive fields below the putative granular input sink, and the demonstrable multiunit activation as indicated by the newly included Figure S2, we have no reservations in our assessment of the position of the electrode relative to the layers across sessions.
11. The example RF profile shown in Figure S5A, although aligned, looks a little strange in that the RFs taper off rapidly in the infra-granular layer. Is this the best representative example? It will be important to see other examples of RF alignment.
The attenuation observed in the lower layers is largely due to overall decreased gamma power in the lower layers of cortex as compared to upper and middle layers (Maier et al., 2010, Front Syst Neurosci). At the reviewer’s request, we have added an additional panel to the noted supplementary figure which shows additional laminar receptive field profiles using the evoked LFP so that they are more directly comparable to those shown in Nandy et al., 2017, Neuron.
12. The study used LFP power in the gamma range to compute the response ratio between red and green stimuli. LFPs measured across the cortical depth are highly correlated, and so would gamma power estimated from the LFPs. Given this, how meaningful is the laminar analysis shown in Figure 4B? How confidently can it be established that the LFP derived gamma power estimates have laminar specificity?
An astute observation – there are two aspects to consider. The existence of color-feature columns has been well-documented in V4 (e.g., Zeki, 1973, Brain Res; Zeki, 1980, Nature; Tootell et al., 2004, Cereb Cortex; Conway and Tsao, 2009, PNAS; Kotake et al., 2009, J Neurophysiol; Westerberg et al., 2021, PNAS). This manuscript did not need the evaluation of interlaminar differences in color selectivity to address the question at hand – the top of Figure 4B only serves as a step to the bottom of Figure 4B which provides the measurements used for the subsequent analyses. Thus, the estimation of color selectivity from gamma was sufficient to capture a general sense of the color selectivity of the column. Second, we recently published a manuscript which directly addresses the laminar specificity of gamma with respect to feature selectivity. Westerberg et al., 2021, PNAS uses a spatially localized form of gamma to evaluate color-feature selectivity along V4 columns. In that manuscript, we find a high degree of consistency along the layers of cortex using the gamma signal. Notably, we compared the gamma signal to the population spiking and found a high degree of coherence between selectivity in those two measures as a function of cortical depth. Given the secondary nature of the interlaminar feature selectivity to this submitted manuscript and the detailed report of laminar feature selectivity using the same dataset in another manuscript, we are inclined to leave the analysis reported here as is with adjustments to the text that note these considerations now included in the Results.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.72139.sa2Article and author information
Author details
Funding
National Eye Institute (F31EY031293)
- Jacob A Westerberg
National Eye Institute (P30EY008126)
- Alexander Maier
- Geoffrey F Woodman
- Jeffrey D Schall
National Eye Institute (R01EY019882)
- Geoffrey F Woodman
- Jeffrey D Schall
National Eye Institute (R01EY008890)
- Jeffrey D Schall
National Eye Institute (R01EY027402)
- Alexander Maier
Office of the Director (S10OD021771)
- Alexander Maier
- Geoffrey F Woodman
- Jeffrey D Schall
National Eye Institute (T32EY007135)
- Jacob A Westerberg
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by NIH through the NEI (P30EY008126, R01EY019882, R01EY008890, R01EY027402) and the Office of the Director (S10OD021771). JAW was supported by fellowships from NEI (F31EY031293 and T32EY007135). The authors would like to thank I Haniff, M Feurtado, M Maddox, S Motorny, D Richardson, L Toy, B Williams, R Williams for technical support. The authors would like to thank B Purcell, P Weigand for collecting data, and S Errington, B Herrera, K Lowe, T Reppert, J Riera, A Sajad, E Sigworth for useful conversations regarding the work.
Ethics
Procedures were in accordance with National Institutes of Health Guidelines, Association for Assessment and Accreditation of Laboratory Animal Care Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals, and approved by the Vanderbilt Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (Protocol M1700067) following United States Department of Agriculture and Public Health Services policies.
Senior Editor
- Chris I Baker, National Institute of Mental Health, National Institutes of Health, United States
Reviewing Editor
- Supratim Ray, Indian Institute of Science, India
Reviewers
- Steven J Luck
- Anirvan S Nandy, Yale School of Medicine, United States
Publication history
- Received: July 13, 2021
- Preprint posted: September 11, 2021 (view preprint)
- Accepted: January 25, 2022
- Accepted Manuscript published: January 28, 2022 (version 1)
- Version of Record published: February 15, 2022 (version 2)
Copyright
© 2022, Westerberg et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 912
- Page views
-
- 161
- Downloads
-
- 4
- Citations
Article citation count generated by polling the highest count across the following sources: Crossref, PubMed Central, Scopus.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Neuroscience
Deep brain stimulation targeting the posterior hypothalamus (pHyp-DBS) is being investigated as a treatment for refractory aggressive behavior, but its mechanisms of action remain elusive. We conducted an integrated imaging analysis of a large multi-centre dataset, incorporating volume of activated tissue modeling, probabilistic mapping, normative connectomics, and atlas-derived transcriptomics. Ninety-one percent of the patients responded positively to treatment, with a more striking improvement recorded in the pediatric population. Probabilistic mapping revealed an optimized surgical target within the posterior-inferior-lateral region of the posterior hypothalamic area. Normative connectomic analyses identified fiber tracts and functionally connected with brain areas associated with sensorimotor function, emotional regulation, and monoamine production. Functional connectivity between the target, periaqueductal gray and key limbic areas – together with patient age – were highly predictive of treatment outcome. Transcriptomic analysis showed that genes involved in mechanisms of aggressive behavior, neuronal communication, plasticity and neuroinflammation might underlie this functional network.
-
- Neuroscience
Prediction errors are differences between expected and actual sensory input and are thought to be key computational signals that drive learning related plasticity. One way that prediction errors could drive learning is by activating neuromodulatory systems to gate plasticity. The catecholaminergic locus coeruleus (LC) is a major neuromodulatory system involved in neuronal plasticity in the cortex. Using two-photon calcium imaging in mice exploring a virtual environment, we found that the activity of LC axons in the cortex correlated with the magnitude of unsigned visuomotor prediction errors. LC response profiles were similar in both motor and visual cortical areas, indicating that LC axons broadcast prediction errors throughout the dorsal cortex. While imaging calcium activity in layer 2/3 of the primary visual cortex, we found that optogenetic stimulation of LC axons facilitated learning of a stimulus-specific suppression of visual responses during locomotion. This plasticity – induced by minutes of LC stimulation – recapitulated the effect of visuomotor learning on a scale that is normally observed during visuomotor development across days. We conclude that prediction errors drive LC activity, and that LC activity facilitates sensorimotor plasticity in the cortex, consistent with a role in modulating learning rates.