Subcortico-amygdala pathway processes innate and learned threats
Abstract
Behavioral flexibility and timely reactions to salient stimuli are essential for survival. The subcortical thalamic-basolateral amygdala (BLA) pathway serves as a shortcut for salient stimuli ensuring rapid processing. Here, we show that BLA neuronal and thalamic axonal activity in mice mirror the defensive behavior evoked by an innate visual threat as well as an auditory learned threat. Importantly, perturbing this pathway compromises defensive responses to both forms of threats, in that animals fail to switch from exploratory to defensive behavior. Despite the shared pathway between the two forms of threat processing, we observed noticeable differences. Blocking β-adrenergic receptors impairs the defensive response to the innate but not the learned threats. This reduced defensive response, surprisingly, is reflected in the suppression of the activity exclusively in the BLA as the thalamic input response remains intact. Our side-by-side examination highlights the similarities and differences between innate and learned threat-processing, thus providing new fundamental insights.
Editor's evaluation
This study presents valuable insights into the circuits that are common for innate and acquired threats. The evidence supporting the conclusions is convincing, and the use of state-of-the-art methodology for the study of neural circuits, including chemogenetics, optogenetics, and fiber photometry, is appropriate. This work will be of interest to neuroscientists studying defensive behaviors as well as those in the field of multisensory thalamic integration.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.85459.sa0Introduction
Survival is the direct product of maximizing gains and avoiding harms. This is achieved by integrating brain circuitry that ensures survival through environment exploration while maintaining threat detection and avoidance (Blanchard et al., 2001; Evans et al., 2019; Gross and Canteras, 2012; Headley et al., 2019; Orsini and Maren, 2012; Silva et al., 2016). Such capacity can be acquired through learning, a mechanism widely believed to rely on synaptic plasticity (Mongeau et al., 2003; Johansen et al., 2011; Quirk et al., 1995; Rogan et al., 1997; Tierney, 1986). However, specific sensory stimuli could be innately appetitive or aversive and evoke approach or defensive behaviors, respectively (Pereira and Moita, 2016).
Rapid and continuous integration of innate and learned mechanisms promotes survival. This integration is ideally carried out as a timely and appropriate reaction to sensory stimuli. While cortical processing is necessary for cognitively demanding tasks, it is not well suited for rapid threat detection and response. However, subcortical processing serves as a neural shortcut that can crudely and rapidly elicit defensive behaviors, bypassing the more deliberate and intricate cortical processing. This efficient processing is partially explained by the fact that subcortical areas are among the earliest brain areas that receive sensory information (Carr, 2015; McFadyen et al., 2020; Pessoa, 2008; Pessoa and Adolphs, 2010).
It is well established that the subcortical circuit from the multisensory thalamus, lateral thalamus (LT) to the basolateral amygdala (BLA) is necessary for the acquisition and the recall of auditory learned threat conditioning in rodents (Barsy et al., 2020; Edeline and Weinberger, 1992; Iwata et al., 1986; Lee et al., 2021; LeDoux et al., 1984; Romanski and LeDoux, 1992a; Romanski and LeDoux, 1992b; Romanski et al., 1993; Taylor et al., 2021). However, the role of the subcortical LT-BLA pathway is underemphasized and understudied in processing innate threats (Kang et al., 2022).
Therefore, we sought to examine this pathway for processing innate threats as well, with the view that a side-by-side comparison may lead to a new mechanistic insight into the similarities and differences between circuits processing innate and learned threats.
Here, we show that, as with the learned threat conditioning, the LT-BLA pathway is essential for processing the innately aversive looming stimulus. Inactivation of either the BLA or the BLA-projecting neurons in the LT was sufficient to impair defensive responses to innate and learned threats. Additionally, fiber photometry from the BLA neurons or the LT axons projecting to the BLA showed a rapid rise in their activity to both forms of stimuli. However, the activity was reduced as mice showed habituation to the aversive stimuli. Despite similarities in processing the innate and learned threat, we found that propranolol, a β-adrenergic receptor blocker, specifically impairs the innate defensive response, while the response to the learned threat remains intact.
Results
BLA activity is required for processing a visual innately aversive threat and aversive conditioning
Threat conditioning is an associative learning paradigm where an initially neutral tone (conditioned stimulus [CS]) is repeatedly paired with an aversive footshock (unconditioned stimulus [US]). The next day, mice, upon exposure to the CS, show freezing responses (conditioned response [CR]), indicating successful learning of the association (Blair et al., 2001; Pape and Pare, 2010).
As for the innately aversive threat, we used the looming stimulus, an overhead expanding black disk that is thought to mimic an approaching aerial predator (Yilmaz and Meister, 2013). Unlike the tone used in threat conditioning, the looming stimulus triggers defensive responses without prior learning. The defensive response may vary from freezing to escapes and tail rattling (Yilmaz and Meister, 2013; Salay et al., 2018). In this study, we tested mice in an arena devoid of shelter (Shang et al., 2018). This setup promotes freezing as the dominant defensive response, comparable to the freezing response observed in threat conditioning. Although the mice showed extended freezing beyond the looming stimulus period, we observed rapid habituation to the repeated presentation of the loom (Figure 1—figure supplement 1A–C).
It is well established that the BLA activity is required to process learned threats (Maren et al., 1996; Maren, 1999; Anglada-Figueroa and Quirk, 2005; Johansen et al., 2014). To test whether the BLA is required for visually evoked innate defensive response, we applied a loss-of-function approach by transiently inactivating the BLA. We infected the BLA pyramidal neurons with hM4Di (Gi-coupled human muscarinic M4 designer receptor exclusively activated by a designer drug [iDREADD]) tagged with m-Cherry fluorescent proteins. We validated that the efficacy of iDREADDs’ agonist, clozapine-N-oxide (CNO), mediated inhibition by performing optical stimulation of LT axons and in vivo electrophysiology recording in the BLA (Figure 1A). Intraperitoneal injection of CNO reduced the light-evoked BLA activity significantly (Figure 1B and Figure 1—figure supplement 2A; Armbruster et al., 2007; Stachniak et al., 2014). Behaviorally, the iDREADD-mediated BLA silencing significantly reduced the defensive responses of the animals to the looming stimulus (Figure 1C and D and Figure 1—figure supplement 2B–G).
Specifically, BLA inhibition resulted in failure of switching from rearing behavior to freezing, which is a typical looming stimulus-evoked behavior (Figure 1E and Figure 1—figure supplement 2E-G). The following day, the same mice were subjected to the threat conditioning protocol in the presence of CNO (Figure 1F). Consistent with previous reports (Anglada-Figueroa and Quirk, 2005; Johansen et al., 2014; Maren, 1999; Maren et al., 1996), transient inactivation of the BLA during the conditioning reduced the freezing response to the CS during the conditioning as well as the recall periods (Figure 1G and H and Figure 1—figure supplement 2H and I). Neither the injection of CNO nor the expression of iDREADDs alone interfered with the expression of the defensive responses to innately aversive and learned threats. In addition, mice with a permanent lesion in the BLA showed a similar deficit in defensive responses (Figure 1—figure supplement 3). Thus, the neuronal activity within the BLA is not only required for encoding and processing the learned threat but it is also essential for processing an innately aversive threat. This, to our knowledge, is the first direct evidence documenting that the BLA is required for processing an innate visual threat cue.
The selective lesion of the BLA-projecting LT neurons impairs the defensive responses to the looming stimulus and the aversive conditioning
The subcortical pathway comprising the LT inputs to the BLA is known to be essential for the acquisition of threat conditioning (Romanski and LeDoux, 1992a; Romanski and LeDoux, 1992b; LeDoux et al., 1984; Campeau and Davis, 1995; Barsy et al., 2020; Taylor et al., 2021; Lee et al., 2021). Therefore, we tested whether these inputs are also required for processing innately aversive threats. We selectively lesioned BLA-projecting LT neurons by injecting retroAAV2-Cre in the BLA and a mixture of DIO-GFP and DIO-taCaspase3 in the LT (Yang et al., 2013; Figure 2A). Additionally, in two separate control groups, we injected retroAAV2-Cre in the BLA and DIO-GFP in the LT (GFP group) or a mixture of DIO-GFP and DIO-taCaspase in the LT (sham group). Our retrograde labeling was largely confined within the LT (Figure 2B and D).
Among mice with the selective lesion of the BLA-projecting LT neurons, we observed few GFP+ neurons (Figure 2C, E and F), demonstrating the efficiency of the approach. Furthermore, mice with the selective lesion showed a significant reduction in their defensive response to the looming stimulus as opposed to the two control groups (Figure 2G and Figure 2—figure supplement 1A–C). Similar to the BLA inhibition experiment, the selective lesion caused a similar failure in switching from exploratory to defensive behavior upon looming stimulus exposure (Figure 2G and Figure 2—figure supplement 1A and B). Likewise, the defensive responses to the learned aversive cue were reduced during the conditioning as well as the recall sessions (Figure 2H and Figure 2—figure supplement 1D and E). Together these data, in line with a recent report (Kang et al., 2022), demonstrate that the activity of the BLA-projecting LT neurons is required for the defensive responses not only to a learned but also to an innately aversive threat.
The BLA receives monosynaptic inputs from the temporal associative cortex (TeA) and auditory cortex (AuC). Since the LT is a major input to the TeA and the AuC, we tested whether the effect we observed in the selective lesion of the LT projecting cells could be an indirect effect via interrupting the cortical inputs to the BLA (Barsy et al., 2020). To this end, we lesioned the cortical regions (TeA and AuD) that are known to receive LT projections and in turn project to the BLA (Figure 2—figure supplement 2A and B). Mice with cortical lesions had a similar level of freezing in response to looming (Figure 2—figure supplement 2C) and conditioned stimuli (Figure 2—figure supplement 2D and E). Our results are in agreement with previous studies showing that selective optical inhibition of LT-BLA axons, which spares the disynaptic input from the LT-TeA/AuC-BLA, impairs defensive responses to the innate and learned threat stimuli (Barsy et al., 2020; Kang et al., 2022).
The axons of the BLA-projecting LT neurons are activated by the looming stimulus and show an increase in CS-evoked response following aversive conditioning
In the preceding section, we demonstrated that the activity of the BLA-projecting LT neurons is essential for processing innate and learned threats. Therefore, we expect an increase in the activity of the LT input to the BLA that is time-locked to the threat signals. For this purpose, we took advantage of fiber photometry in freely moving mice. Virus expressing the genetically encoded Ca2+ indicator GCaMP7s (Dana et al., 2019) was injected into the LT, and a fiber optic was implanted above the dorsal tip of the BLA (Figure 3A–C and Figure 3—figure supplement 1A). The axonal activity of the LT neurons serves as a proxy for their release of neurotransmitters into the BLA. The time-locked GCaMP activity of the LT projections to the onset of the looming stimulus was evident. As mice showed habituation to the stimuli, GCaMP activity diminished, with later stimuli eliciting neither defensive behavior nor time-locked GCaMP activity in the LT inputs (Figure 3D–J).
Notably, the white looming stimulus, as opposed to a black looming stimulus, did not evoke defensive behavior (Yilmaz and Meister, 2013), nor did it trigger GCaMP activity (Figure 3E–I). This, along with the result from the habituation sessions, indicates that the increased activity of the LT inputs is not merely the product of the sensory property of the looming stimulus, but it reflects the saliency and aversiveness of the stimulus, as well.
We next monitored the activity of the LT inputs during aversive conditioning, the recall sessions, and post-extinction training (Figure 3K). As expected from a multisensory brain region (Bordi and LeDoux, 1994; Linke et al., 1999; Linke, 1999), the LT inputs were activated from the tone onset (Figure 3L and M). The amplitude of the activity remained unchanged for the subsequent CS presentations, despite mice showing an increased CS-evoked freezing to these stimuli (Figure 3L). Interestingly, during the recall session 24 hr later, we observed a significant increase in CS-evoked GCaMP activity, which was not evident during the conditioning (Figure 3L and M). In addition, upon extinction training, the CS-evoked activity returned to its preconditioning level (Figure 3L and M and Figure 3—figure supplement 1B). Moreover, the footshock induced a time-locked increase in GCaMP activity, which was reduced in amplitude with subsequent US delivery (Figure 3N). From these experiments, we conclude that the LT neurons directly convey the signals for innately aversive as well as for learned threats to the BLA.
Contralateral disconnection of the LT-BLA pathway impairs the defensive responses to the looming stimulus and the aversive conditioning
Our inactivation experiments demonstrate that the BLA (Figure 1) as well as the BLA-projecting neurons in the LT (Figure 2) are necessary for the processing of the learned and innate aversive threat responses. However, the previous experiments on their own cannot distinguish whether the two regions function in series, with the BLA receiving the threat signals from the LT (as indicated by GCaMP activity in the LT inputs) (Figure 3); or, the LT and the BLA function in parallel, with the BLA receiving the signal from other sources. To address this issue, we used an asymmetrical disconnection approach (LeDoux et al., 1986; Iwata et al., 1986; Eldridge et al., 2016; Barker et al., 2017; Torromino et al., 2019), where we inhibited the activity of the LT and the BLA contralaterally. This approach is suited to test whether the direct connection between two regions is required for a particular function (Figure 4A and B). The prerequisite is the connections should be ipsilateral and not reciprocal, as it is the case for the LT and the BLA (LeDoux et al., 1986; LeDoux et al., 1990).
For this purpose, we applied a reversible disconnection between the LT-BLA pathway by expressing hM4Di in the LT and the BLA contralateral to each other (Figure 4A and B and Figure 4—figure supplement 1A and B). This manipulation spared the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) (Figure 4—figure supplement 1C). Electrophysiologically, upon CNO injection, the responses of the BLA neurons to the optical stimulation of LT neurons co-expressing ChrimsonR and hM4Di was greatly reduced (Figure 4C and D). Behaviorally, CNO-induced inactivation of the contralateral regions significantly reduced the defensive responses to the looming stimulus (Figure 4E and Figure 4—figure supplement 2B-H) and the recall session of the aversive conditioning (Figure 4F and Figure 4—figure supplement 2I and J). The locomotor activity and the anxiety-like behaviors, as measured by line crosses and time spent in the center of the looming arena, however, remained intact (Figure 4—figure supplement 2A). CNO injection in mice expressing mCherry in the contralateral LT and BLA did not reduce freezing responses in either of the behavioral tasks (Figure 4E and F). More importantly, unilateral inactivation of the LT and the BLA was not sufficient to block the defensive responses (Figure 4E and F). Similar results were obtained when we performed an irreversible disconnection of the LT-BLA pathway (Figure 4—figure supplement 2K–N). These experiments suggest that the direct projection from the LT to the BLA is required for the processing of innately aversive threats as well as learned threats.
The LT lesion blocks the BLA neuronal activation by the looming and conditioned stimuli as well as reduces the response to a footshock
The disconnection of the LT-BLA pathway impairs the defensive responses to both forms of threat cues (Figure 4), suggesting that this pathway is the main root by which the BLA receives the aversive signals. If so, upon the LT lesion, the BLA responses to the aversive stimuli must largely disappear. We co-injected AAV vectors expressing DIO-ta-Capsase3 and Cre recombinase in the LT and GCaMP8m in the BLA (Zhang et al., 2021). GCaMP signal was collected through a fiber optic implanted above the tip of the BLA (Figure 5A and Figure 5—figure supplement 1A). The control group underwent the same procedure except that no Cre recombinase was injected.
In the LT-lesioned mice, the BLA response to the black looming stimuli, along with the behavioral defensive responses, largely disappeared (Figure 5B–F). In the non-lesioned mice, where defensive responses to the looming stimuli remained intact, we observed a timed-locked GCaMP activity to the stimuli in the BLA (Figure 5B–E). After habituation, the behavioral and neuronal responses to the black looming stimulus in the control group were comparable to those lesioned (Figure 5B–G). Moreover, the white looming stimulus did not produce a noticeable activation of the BLA neurons, nor did it elicit a defensive response (Figure 5—figure supplement 1B–D).
We next monitored the activity of the BLA during the auditory threat conditioning and the recall in the LT-lesioned and non-lesioned mice (Figure 5H). The LT-lesioned mice, as expected, did not produce a conditioning response to the tone (Figure 5—figure supplement 1F and G). Accordingly, the CS failed to activate the BLA in these mice (Figure 5I–K and Figure 5—figure supplement 1E), while the US-evoked response was significantly reduced (Figure 5L). In the non-lesioned mice, on the other hand, we observed tone-evoked response in the BLA, but with a latency that cannot be fully explained by the slow kinetics of the calcium indicator. As the conditioning progressed, we observed the appearance of an additional, smaller and shorter latency tone-evoked component. In the recall session, the tone-evoked response in the BLA was significantly larger in amplitude and shorter in latency than observed at the end of the conditioning session in the previous day (Figure 5J). Upon extinction, the tone-evoked response, along with the defensive behavior, was significantly reduced (Figure 5I and J and Figure 5—figure supplement 1G).
Blocking β-adrenergic receptors reduces the defensive response to the innately aversive threat
Previous studies have shown that innately aversive stimuli such as fox urine (Hu et al., 2007; Liu et al., 2010) or cat fur odor (Do Monte et al., 2008) mediates defensive responses through the activation of the β-adrenergic receptor. Therefore, we considered that the threat response triggered by looming stimulus may rely on the activity of these receptors. To test this, prior to exposure to the looming stimulus mice were injected with propranolol, a β-adrenergic receptor blocker (Figure 6—figure supplement 1A–C). The defensive responses to the looming stimulus in these mice largely disappeared, while the exploratory behavior remained intact (Figure 6—figure supplement 1B). On the other hand, sotalol, a peripherally acting β-adrenergic blocker, had no impact on defensive responses, suggesting that propranolol reduced the looming stimulus-evoked defensive responses by acting on the central nervous system (Figure 6—figure supplement 1B).
To further examine the contribution of the subcortical pathway in the processing of innate threats, we injected propranolol in mice expressing GCaMP in either LT axons projecting to the BLA or in the BLA pyramidal neurons, followed by repeated exposure to looming stimuli (Figure 6A and Figure 6—figure supplement 1K). At the cellular level, looming stimuli failed to elicit a significant increase in the BLA activity, which reflects the reduced freezing response in mice injected with propranolol (Figure 6B–D). Surprisingly, the stimulus-evoked LT axonal activity remained undisturbed despite the lack of defensive response (Figure 6E–G). Of note, 48 hr later, when these mice were retested, defensive response, as well as time-locked BLA activity to the looming stimulus, was restored (Figure 6B–E).
To our surprise, propranolol did not impair the aversive conditioning responses when injected either before the conditioning or before the recall session (Figure 6—figure supplement 1D–I). In addition, propranolol injections prior to the recall session perturbed neither BLA (Figure 6—figure supplement 2A–D) nor LT axonal activity (Figure 6—figure supplement 2E–G).
Discussion
The thalamic-BLA pathway and its intricate microcircuitry have been described for their function in processing the auditory CS and aversive US (Barsy et al., 2020; Janak and Tye, 2015; Rogan et al., 1997; Rogan et al., 2005). Yet, their role in processing unimodal innate threats remains under investigated (Kang et al., 2022). Here, we demonstrate that transient or permanent inactivation of the BLA (Figure 1) or BLA-projecting LT neurons (Figure 2) not only impairs threat learning but also abolishes all the measured defensive responses to an innate threat. More specifically, upon exposure to an innate visual threat, animals with a compromised LT-BLA pathway fail to switch from exploratory to defensive behaviors, displaying neither freezing nor escape reaction (Figures 1, 2 and 4). Furthermore, the LT axons projecting to the BLA (Figure 3F) and the neurons within the BLA produce time-locked activity to each looming stimulus in mice showing defensive responses (Figure 5C). With repeated exposure to looming stimuli, the neuronal activity, along with defensive responses, gradually fades. The habituation to looming stimuli was rapid (within-session) and long-lasting (for the entire duration of the experiment) (Figures 3 and 5). A recent study has shown that upon repeated exposure to a looming stimulus neuronal response within deeper layers of superior colliculus (SC) undergo rapid visual habituation. A form of short-term depression has been proposed to underlie the fast habituation (Lee et al., 2020). This phenomenon may underlie the within-session habituation that we observed here. This is consistent with the gradual reduction in the activity of the BLA-projecting neurons within LT, a region downstream of the deep layers of the SC (Benavidez et al., 2021; Linke et al., 1999). The long-lasting habituation, on the other hand, cannot be explained by such a visual adaptation or habituation that lasts on the order of seconds to minutes and not days (Boehnke et al., 2011; Lee et al., 2020; Wark et al., 2009). The long-term form of the habituation largely occurs upstream of the BLA as observed in the reduced activity in the LT projections to the BLA (Figure 3F and G). If, as we suspect, the signal for the looming stimulus to the LT originates from the SC (see below), the long-term form of the habituation may, at least in part, be processed within the LT. Further studies are necessary to test this possibility.
Although reduced defensive response to an innately aversive signal such as a looming stimulus has been categorized as habituation, we observed physiological similarities between habituation to a looming stimulus and extinction to a cued conditioning stimulus. Both phenomena reduce the heightened evoked activity of the LT projections as well as the BLA neurons. Additionally, while mice injected with propranolol did not show defensive response to a looming stimulus, 2 d later in the absence of the drug the same mice showed robust freezing response to the stimulus. This is similar to a study reporting that mice injected with propranolol before a cued fear recall have reduced freezing response to the cue, while in the following day the mice show a heightened freezing response (Leal Santos et al., 2021); unlike this study, however, we did not observe a physiological or behavioral effect of propranolol on cued conditioning (see below for discussion). The cellular and circuit mechanisms of extinction have been extensively studied (Herry et al., 2006; Orsini and Maren, 2012). These studies may provide mechanistic insight into the habituation that we have observed here. Further investigations in this line may have fundamental and translational value.
An overlap in processing innate and learned threats is not unique to the LT-BLA pathway. The lateral habenula and the central amygdala control the processing of a number of innate and learned threats (Fadok et al., 2017; Lecca et al., 2017; Lecca et al., 2020; Matsumoto and Hikosaka, 2007; Root et al., 2014; Mondoloni et al., 2022; Tovote et al., 2016; Sachella et al., 2022; Isosaka et al., 2015). Since both types of threats share a similar repertoire of defensive responses, such as freezing and escaping, the wiring economy favors the layout where there is closer physical proximity between the two circuits, as we observed here (Klyachko and Stevens, 2003; Stevens, 2012).
Direct and indirect evidence shows that the LT-BLA pathway processes other forms of innate threats as well. Recently, studies have demonstrated that inactivation of thalamic-BLA pathway reduces freezing response to intense sound and looming stimuli (Kang et al., 2022), as well as innately aversive ultrasound activates the BLA (Mongeau et al., 2003; Shukla and Chattarji, 2022). Moreover, the use of live predators as an innate threat has further supported this notion by showing that the BLA lesion in rodents eliminates defensive responses to the threat (Bindi et al., 2018; Martinez et al., 2011).
Since the LT is widely regarded as an auditory relay region (Rogan and LeDoux, 1995; Rogan et al., 1997; Weinberger, 2011), its role as the main source of innately aversive visual signal to the BLA may seem unexpected. However, it has been known that the LT receives auditory, somatosensory, visual, and multimodal information (Bordi and LeDoux, 1994; Linke et al., 1999; Linke, 1999). The lateral posterior nucleus of the thalamus (LP) has been proposed as another direct source conveying a looming stimulus signal to the BLA (Wei et al., 2015). Our reversible disconnection of the LT-BLA pathway, which largely spares the LP, argues otherwise. The LT lesion not only abolishes the defensive responses to the looming stimulus but also largely eliminates the stimulus-evoked activity in the BLA (Figure 5).
Although the input source of the looming stimulus to the LT is unknown, we speculate the SC could be a likely candidate as it encodes threat and escape behavior to looming stimuli (Evans et al., 2018) and sends monosynaptic connections to the LT (Linke et al., 1999). Additionally, it has been shown that the SC may trigger defensive responses to the looming stimulus by conveying the signals to the periaqueductal gray (PAG) (Evans et al., 2018) or indirectly to the central nucleus of the amygdala (CeA) (Shang et al., 2015; Zhou et al., 2019). It is relevant to note that the BLA conveys learned aversive signals directly to the CeA (Fadok et al., 2017) and indirectly through the CeA to the PAG (Tovote et al., 2016). It will be of particular interest to test whether the information signaling the innate threat is communicated through the same channel. As the SC and the BLA, the two essential regions in processing the innate visual threat, share similar downstream targets, their specific contribution to the process deserves further inquiry.
Consistent with the notion that the innate and learned aversive stimuli are conveyed through the LT inputs to the BLA, the activities of these two regions largely mirrored each other; however, noticeable differences were observed. For example, in line with previous studies on innate defensive response to predators odors (Hayley et al., 2001; Hu et al., 2007; Do Monte et al., 2008; Liu et al., 2010), in mice injected with propranolol, the defensive responses to the looming stimulus diminished significantly (Figure 6B and E); the stimulus-induced activity, however, was reduced only in the BLA (Figure 6C), with the response of the axons of the BLA-projecting LT neurons remaining unchanged (Figure 6F). This indicates that the looming stimulus conveyed through the thalamic input is essential but not sufficient to activate BLA neurons and trigger defensive responses, and the activation of β-adrenergic receptors by the release of norepinephrine is required. The neuromodulator may enhance the excitability of the pyramidal neurons directly by downregulating potassium channels in these neurons (Faber et al., 2008) or indirectly by reducing excitability of inhibitory neurons (Tully et al., 2007). Given the time-locked neuronal activity to the stimuli, the modulation of excitability, a comparably slow process, may be achieved through a tonic release of norepinephrine. As for the LT, to the best of our knowledge, there is little published work regarding its modulation by norepinephrine or even if β-adrenergic receptors are expressed in this region. Therefore, at this stage, we have no grounds to speculate about the ineffectiveness of propranolol in blocking the LT projection response to the looming stimulus. Here, we should point out that administration of propranolol prior to the conditioning or recall had no effect on freezing response. The literature on the role of norepinephrine in cued conditioning is mixed. While intraperitoneal injection of propranolol prior to a cued memory recall reduces freezing to the tone in rats (Rodriguez-Romaguera et al., 2009), the drug may reduce (Leal Santos et al., 2021) or have no effect (Cain et al., 2004) on the freezing response in mice. The differences in species or strains used or experimental parameters may contribute to the variability in the effect of the drug in freezing response.
Regarding the processing of the learned threat, again, we observed some differences between the LT projections to the BLA and the BLA itself. As animals learned the CS-US association, we observed an enhanced short-onset auditory response in the BLA (Figure 5J; Quirk et al., 1995). We did not detect a similar conditioning-correlated change in the activity of the LT axons (Figure 3L). It must be noted that because of our use of calcium indicators, we cannot exclude millisecond changes in the auditory response latency in the LT axons. However, previous works using sub-millisecond single-unit recording also showed similar patterns (Barsy et al., 2020; Bordi and LeDoux, 1992). The BLA also differed from the incoming LT projections in its response to the CS after an extensive extinction protocol. While activity of the LT axons returned to its preconditioning value, the CS-evoked activity of the BLA neurons after the extinction was significantly reduced compared to its value prior to the conditioning. This strongly suggests that other inputs to the BLA contribute to such a pronounced reduction. Feedforward inhibition of excitatory neurons in the BLA through synaptic potentiation (Polepalli et al., 2010) or dopaminergic modulation (Bissière et al., 2003) of the local inhibitory neurons may to some extent dampen the CS-evoked response after the extinction. Also, it has been proposed that norepinephrine can promote extinction through the activation of the infralimbic region, which in turn blunts the BLA activity (Giustino and Maren, 2018; Uematsu et al., 2017). It is pertinent to mention that we have used an extensive multiple-session extinction training. Recently, it has been shown that such an extensive extinction protocol may involve a different mechanism by which the original fear memory is erased, and the extinguished CS becomes habituated (An et al., 2017).
Although not the focus of this work, we observed several intriguing physiological features during the conditioning and recall sessions. The conditioning increases the tone-induced activity in the BLA and reduces the response time onset (Barsy et al., 2020; Bordi et al., 1993). The increased response and decreased time onset were significantly more pronounced on the recall day. We observed a similar pattern in the LT input where there was a significant enhanced activity during the recall session, which was not visible during the conditioning. This is in line with previous studies using single-cell imaging in the BLA (Grewe et al., 2017). This significant change between the last trial of the conditioning and the first trial of the recall is puzzling. We speculate that a lack of a detectable increase in the CS-evoked activity at the later stages of the conditioning could be caused by a masking effect from a transient increase in firing rate during the conditioning. Although we did not observe a difference in the overall baseline activity during the conditioning (data not shown), downregulation as well as upregulation of the basal neuronal activity of subpopulations of neurons during the conditioning has been reported. This counteracting phenomenon could produce a net effect of no change in the baseline activity at the population level, while a subset of neurons with increased basal firing rate, possibly caused by enhanced excitability, may undergo plasticity. The plasticity within this population, however, will be masked during the conditioning by the transient increase in the basal firing rate. Alternatively, synaptic potentiation may occur at the dendritic compartments, which through local inhibitory circuits is uncoupled from somatic activity.
The LT-BLA pathway typically has been evaluated in relation to associative learnings (Janak and Tye, 2015; Tye et al., 2008), especially associative learned threats (Barsy et al., 2020; Taylor et al., 2021). Recent works on associative learned threats have particularly solidified the importance of associative plasticity in the LT (Barsy et al., 2020; Taylor et al., 2021). A recent study has further documented the critical role of the LT in processing different forms of innate threat (Kang et al., 2022). Our main aim in this work was to investigate the similarities and differences in processing an innate threat, which relies on pre-wired circuits, and a learned threat, which requires synaptic plasticity. By conducting a side-by-side comparison within the same animals, we not only gained new insights about shared and distinct features of processing the two forms of threats in the LT-BLA pathway, but also learned that, despite being monosynaptically connected, the LT and the BLA differ in important ways, as we detailed in our discussion. This provides new avenues for further investigation.
Methods
Animals
All the procedures were performed on C57BL/6JRJ wildtype (Janvier, France). Mice were naïve and acclimated to the vivarium for at least a week before the beginning of the experiment. The mice were 6–8 weeks old at the beginning of the experimental procedures. Animals were group-housed (3–4 per cage) with enriched conditions in a 12 hr light/dark cycle (the light switches on at 6 AM) with constant level of humidity and temperature (22 ± 1). Food and water were provided ad libitum. Behavioral experiments were conducted between 11 AM and 10 PM at Aarhus University at the Biomedicine department, Ole Worms Allé 8, Aarhus 8000. All the experimental procedures were conducted according to the Danish Animal Experiment Inspectorate.
Stereotaxic surgery and virus expression
Mice were anesthetized using isoflurane (IsoFlo vet 100%, Zoetis), and standard surgical procedures were used to expose the skull. For most of the experiments, stereoscope lights were not used during the surgical procedures because they reduced the behavioral responses (Figure 1—figure supplement 4). Before the surgery, the mice were injected subcutaneously with buprenorphine 0.3 mg/mL (Temgesic, 0.1 mg/kg).
Electrolytic-induced lesion of the BLA
Mice were lesioned bilaterally in the BLA at the following coordinates: anteroposterior (AP): –1.4 mm; mediolateral (ML): ±3.6 mm; dorsoventral (DV): −3.85/–4.1/–4.35 mm from the skull. The electrolytic lesion was performed with a concentric bipolar electrode (50691, Stoelting, USA) by delivering a constant direct current at each location (0.6 mA for 15 s). In another group of mice, the electrode was placed at the same location without delivering any current (sham surgery).
Lesion of the BLA
Mice were bilaterally injected with a mixture of AAV-5/2-hEF1α-dlox-(pro)taCasp3_2A_TEVp(rev)-dlox (titer: 4.7 × 10–12 vg/mL) in AAV-1/2-hCMV-chI-Cre (titer: 1.0 × 10–13 vg/mL, ratio 7:1.5). The volume of injection was 0.5 µL per hemisphere at the following coordinates AP: –1.6 mm; ML: ±3.45 mm; DV: −3.5/–4.1 mm from the skull. Control mice were injected with AAV-5/2-hEF1α-dlox-(pro)taCasp3_2A_TEVp(rev)-dlox at the same coordinates.
Chemogenetics inhibition of the BLA
Mice were bilaterally injected with AAV-5/2-mCaMKIIα-hM4D(Gi)_mCherry (titer: 8.9 × 10–12 vg/mL) or with AAV-5/2-mCaMKIIα-mCherry (titer: 6 × 10–12 vg/mL, diluted 1:1 in PBS). The injection volume was 1 µL per hemisphere at the following coordinates for the BLA: AP: –1.6 mm; ML: ±3.45 mm; DV: −3.5/–4.1 mm from the skull.
Lesion of the TeA and AuC
Mice were bilaterally injected with a mixture of AAV-5/2-hEF1α-dlox-(pro)taCasp3_2A_TEVp(rev)-dlox (titer: 4.7 × 10–12 vg/mL) in AAV-1/2-hCMV-chI-Cre (titer: 1.0 × 10–13 vg/mL, ratio 7:1.5). The volume of injection was 0.2 µL per hemisphere at the following coordinates at AP: –2.85 mm; ML: ±4.44 mm; DV: –1.6 mm from the skull. Control mice were injected with AAV-5/2-hEF1α-dlox-(pro)taCasp3_2A_TEVp(rev)-dlox.
Selective lesion of the LT-projecting neurons to the BLA
Animals were injected bilaterally in the LT with a mixture of AAV-5/2-hEF1α-dlox-(pro)taCasp3_2A_TEVp(rev)-dlox in AAV-5/2-hSyn1-dlox-EGFP(rev)-dlox titer: 1.1 × 10–13 vg/mL, ratio 7:2 at the following coordinates at AP: –3.15 mm; ML: ±1.85 mm; DV: −3.4/–3.5 mm from the skull. The same mice were injected bilaterally with AAV-retro/2-hCMV-chI-Cre (titer: 4.4 × 10–12 vg/mL) in the BLA using the same coordinates mentioned earlier. One control group was injected with AAV-5/2-hEF1α-dlox-(pro)taCasp3_2A_TEVp(rev)-dlox in AAV-5/2-hSyn1-dlox-EGFP(rev)-dlox (ratio 7:2) in the LT. Another control group was injected with AAV-retro/2-hCMV-chI-Crein the BLA and with AAV-5/2-hSyn1-dlox-EGFP(rev)-dlox in the LT using the same dilution mentioned above.
LT-BLA disconnection experiment
Mice were injected contralaterally in one LT and one LA (randomized hemispheres). In both locations, a mixture of AAV-5/2-hEF1α-dlox-(pro)ta-Casp3_2A_TEVp(rev)-dlox in AAV-1/2-hCMV-chI-Cre (ratio 7:1.5). The injection volume was 1 µL per hemisphere at the coordinates described previously. For the reversible disconnection experiment, mice were injected contralaterally in one LT and one BLA with AAV-5/2-mCaMKIIα-hM4D(Gi)_mCherry or with AAV-5/2-mCaMKIIα-mCherry (diluted 1:1 in PBS) in the BLA. Additional control groups were injected unilaterally with AAV-5/2-mCaMKIIα-hM4D(Gi)_mCherry in the LT or in the BLA.
Fiber photometry experiments
Mice were injected unilaterally with AAV-5/2-hSyn1-chI-jGCaMP7s (titer: 7.7 × 10–12 vg/ml) in the LT or with AAV-5/2-mCaMKIIα-jGCaMP8m (titer: 6.5 × 10–12 vg/mL) in the BLA. The mice injected with jGCaMP8m in the BLA were injected in the LT with AAV-5/2-hEF1α-dlox-(pro)taCasp3_2A_TEVp(rev)-dlox in AAV-1/2-hCMV-chI-Cre (ratio 7:1.5). The injection volume was 0.5 µL per hemisphere at the previously mentioned coordinates. Control mice were injected with AAV-5/2-hEF1α-dlox-(pro)taCasp3_2A_TEVp(rev)-dlox at the same dilution. Mono fiber-optic cannula (200/300 um, NA 0.37) was implanted in the BLA at the following coordinates AP: –1.6 mm; ML: +3.48 mm; DV: –3.45 mm from the skull. The fiber-optic cannula was fixed to the skull with Superbond (SUN MEDICAL, Japan).
All the viral vectors were bought from the Viral Vector Facility (VVF) of the Neuroscience Center Zurich (ZNZ).
Behavioral procedures
Looming stimulus
The apparatus consisted of an open-top arena (37 × 40 × 19.5 cm) with a monitor (16 inches) placed on the top. No shelter was used (Barbano et al., 2020; Shang et al., 2018). The exposure to the looming stimulus was performed during the dark period (between 6 PM and 10 PM). Mice were placed in the center of the arena and explored freely for 8–10 min before exposure to the looming stimulus. The looming stimulus consisted of an expanding black disk over a gray background, and it consisted of five repetitions, from 2° to 20° of visual angle (Yilmaz and Meister, 2013). The loom widens in 250 ms and remains at the same size for 250 ms with 2 s of pause between each loom. The experimenter was blind to the treatment and manually delivered the looming stimulus. The behavioral responses were recorded with a top camera (Phihong POE21U-1AF). All the animals were exposed at least 2–3 times to the looming stimulus, and only the one eliciting the greatest defensive response was analyzed. The defensive responses were analyzed automatically and manually using ANY-maze software (Stoelting, Ireland) for the 30 s following the onset of the looming stimulus. An experimenter, blind to the treatment, analyzed each mouse’s freezing percentage, tail rattling, escape events, and rearing events. Freezing was defined as a complete lack of movements, except for the respiratory movements, that lasted for at least 1 s. Tail rattling was defined as an event in which the mouse moved the tail vigorously. Escape events were defined as a sharp increase in the locomotor speed three times greater than the average speed before the exposure to the looming stimulus. Rearing was defined as an event in which the mouse stood on its hindpaws, and they were quantified for the 30 s preceding (baseline) and following the looming stimulus presentation. The differential score was calculated by subtracting the rearing events following the looming stimulus subtracted by the rearing events during the baseline period. Note that positive differential score values indicate an increase in rearing events compared with the baseline period, whereas negative scores indicate a decrease in rearing events compared with the baseline period.
For the reversible disconnection experiment, we tested the effect of the inhibition of the LT-BLA pathway on the locomotion and on the anxiety levels. Locomotion was indicated by line crosses in the looming arena during the first 8 min of habituation. Anxiety-like behavior was measured as the percentage of time spent in the center part of the looming arena during the same time period.
For the fiber photometry experiments, a white looming stimulus was used as a neutral control stimulus (Yilmaz and Meister, 2013). It was presented in a pseudorandom order alternated with the black looming stimulus. The white looming stimulus presented the same repetitions and speed as the black looming stimulus.
Aversive conditioning
The apparatus consisted of an open-top cage (24 × 20 × 30 cm) with metal floor bars placed in a soundproof cubicle (55 × 60 × 57 cm) (Ugo Basile, Italy). Two different behavioral protocols were used in this study. In one, the mice were conditioned by using five pairings consisting of 20 s, 5 kHz, sinewave tone (CS) co-terminating with a 2 s footshock (US) 0.6 mA. On the other hand, the animals were conditioned using four pairings consisting of 25 s, 7 kHz, sinewave tone co-terminating with 2 s foot shock 0.5 mA. After the conditioning, the animals stayed isolated for 10–15 min before returning to their home cage. Short-term (STM) and long-term memory (LTM) recall were assessed in a new context 2 hr or 24 hr after the conditioning, respectively. After 2 min of acclimation to the new context, the mice were presented to four or five CS presentations without the footshock. The intertrial interval ranged between 35 s and 120 s for both conditioning and testing sessions. The behavioral response was recorded by a top camera and/or side-view camera, and freezing was scored automatically by ANY-maze software. The freezing percentage indicates the time the mouse spent freezing during the CS presentation, divided by the CS length multiplied by 100. For the conditioning session, the freezing percentage is calculated for each CS presentation. For the recall session, the freezing percentage is the average of four CS presentations, with the only exception of the data shown in Figure 1 and Figure 1—figure supplements 1 and 2, in which the freezing percentage is the average of five CS presentations. The pre-CS freezing percentage is calculated as the average of the freezing time during the 25 s before each CS presentation.
For the fiber photometry experiments, the mice underwent an extinction protocol for 2 d. On each day, the CS was played 12 times in the recall context without the footshock.
Drugs
Propranolol hydrochloride (20 mg/kg, 500 µL; Merck, P0884) or sotalol hydrochloride (10 mg/kg, 500 µL; Merck, S0278) or clozapine N-oxide dihydrochloride (CNO water-soluble, HelloBio, HB6149; 10 mg/kg, 400 µL) or saline (0.9% NaCl, 400/500 µL) were administered by intraperitoneal injection. After the injection, the mice were isolated for 30 min before the beginning of the experimental procedures.
Electrophysiology recordings
As previously described, mice were injected with inhibitory DREADDs or mCherry in the BLA. The same cohort of mice was injected with undiluted AAV-5/2-hSyn1-chI-ChrimsonR_tdTomato (titer: 5.3 × 10E12 vg/mL) in the LT. After 4–5 wk of expression, the mice were anesthetized using 0.5 mg/kg FMM with the following mixture: 0.05 mg/ml of fentanyl ([Hameln, 007007] 0.05 mg/kg), plus 5 mg/mL of midazolam ([Hameln, 002124] 5 mg/kg), and 1 mg/mL of medetomidine (VM Pharma, 087896). After the induction of the anesthesia, the mice were placed on the stereotaxic frame. A 32-channel optoelectrode (Poly3, Neuronexus) was placed at the following coordinates: AP: 1.6 mm; ML: 3.48; DV: 3.5 ± 0.1. The neural data were amplified and digitized at 25 kHz. The input–output curve was recorded 30 min before and 1 hr after the CNO injection using a pulse of 0.5/1 ms, 638 nm.
At the end of the experiment, mice were sacrificed by cervical dislocation, and brains were extracted and kept in 10% formalin for 24 hr. Afterward, the brains were processed to confirm viral expression and electrode location.
Fiber photometry recordings and analysis
All the recordings were performed with Doric fiber photometry system composed of an LED-driver, a fiber photometry console, and a Doric minicube with 460–490 nm for GCaMP excitation, 415 nm for isosbestic excitation, and 580–650 nm for optical stimulation [ilFMC5-G2_IE(400-410)_E(460-490)_F(500-540)_O(580-680)_S]. A low-autofluorescence patch cord (200 nm or 300 nm, 0.37 NA) and a pigtailed rotary joint (200 nm, 0.37 NA) were used. The latter were bleached for 5 hr before each experiment via a 473 nm laser at 15–17 mW. The GCaMP signal was amplified with a Doric amplifier with a 10× gain and recorded with Doric Neuroscience Studio software (version 5.4.1.23) at 11 kHz. The signal was downsampled at 120 Hz for analysis. Light power at the patch cord tip was set between 30 and 35 µW for 470 nm excitation.
For synchronization with the looming stimulus presentation, a National Instrument board (NI USB 6003) was used to time-stamp the looming stimulus presentation over the calcium signal. For synchronization of the tone and shock presentation, an input–output box connected to the aversive conditioning system was used.
After 4–5 wk of virus expression, mice were handled for 2–3 d before the beginning of the experiments. Before each recording, the fiber-optic cannula was cleaned with CleanClicker (Thorlab; USA). To reduce bleaching during the behavioral experiments, the habituation to the looming stimulus and the extinction to the tone were conducted without recording the GCaMP signal.
A customized MATLAB script was used for the analysis. All the traces with a sudden change in the isosbestic signal were discarded in the final analysis. The code used for the analysis is freely available at the following link: https://github.com/NabaviLab-Git/Photometry-Signal-Analysis (copy archived at Nabavi Lab, 2022). Briefly, the signals were downsampled to 120 Hz using local averaging. A first-order polynomial was fitted onto the data using the least-squares method. To calculate the relative change in fluorescence, the raw GCaMP signal was normalized using the fitted signal according to the following equation: deltaF/F = (GCaMP signal fitted signal)/(fitted signal). Behavioral events of interest were extracted and standardized using the mean and standard deviation of the baseline period. In all the plots, we used only individual trials for each animal.
The area under the curve (AUC) was calculated by using a built-in function in GraphPad Prism 9. All the peaks with a distance below 10% between the minimum and the maximum were discarded. For the AUC analysis for the looming stimulus, a baseline period of 2 s was considered and the first 2 s of the looming stimulus presentation. For the CS, we used the 1.5 s as the baseline period and first 2 s of the CS presentation. For the US, we analyzed the AUC for 0.5 s preceding and following the footshock presentation.
Immunofluorescence
The mice were anesthetized with isoflurane and euthanized by cervical dislocation. The brains were harvested and stored for 24 hr in 10% formalin at room temperature. Then, the brains were sliced into 100–120-µm-thick slices in PBS on Leica Vibratome (VT1000S).
To visualize the extent of the lesion, the brains were stained for NeuN and GFP. Slices were permeabilized with PBS-Triton X 0.5% plus 10% of normal goat serum (NGS) and blocked in 10% bovine goat serum (BSA) for 90 min at room temperature. Subsequently, the slices were incubated with a mixture of anti-NeuN antibody mouse (Merck Millipore, MAB377; 1:500) and anti-GFP (Invitrogen, CAB4211, 1:1000) in PBS-Triton X 0.3%, 1% NGS, and 5% BSA and the incubation lasted for 72 hr at 4°C. At the end of the 72 hr incubation, the slices were washed three times in PBS. The slices were incubated in Cyanine 3 (Cy3) goat anti-mouse (Thermo Fisher Scientific, A10521, 1:500) and Alexa Fluor 488 goat anti-rabbit (Thermo Fisher Scientific, A-11008, 1:1000) in PBS-Triton X 0.3%, 1% NGS, and 5% BSA for 24 hr at 4°C. Nuclear staining was performed by using 1:1000 of DAPI (Sigma, D9542) for 30 min at room temperature. Brain slices were mounted on polysine glass slides with coverslips using Fluoromount G (Southern Biotech).
Imaging and cell counting
Imaging was performed by using a virtual slide scanner (Olympus VS120, Japan). Tile images were taken by the whole brain slides by using ×10 (UPLSAPO 2 ×10/0,40) or ×20 objective (UPLSAPO ×20/0.75). The emission wavelength for Alexa 488 was 518 nm with 250 ms of exposure time. For Cy3, the emission wavelength was 565 nm with 250 ms of exposure time.
GFP-positive cells were counted manually by using ImageJ. The experimenter, blind to the treatment, defined a region of interest and performed the cell counting. The GFP-positive cells were quantified in the LT at AP: –3.40 mm from bregma, the region with highest density of GFP-positive cells in the control group. The area was defined by overlaying the atlas landmarks over the image. The area dimension was calculated by using the built-in function in ImageJ.
Statistics
Statistical analyses were performed by using GraphPad Prism 9. All the data are represented as mean ± SEM, and they were tested for normality using Shapiro–Wilk and D’Agostino–Pearson normality test. If the data represented a normal distribution, a parametric test was used. The statistical methods and the corresponding p-values are reported in the figure legends. All the data were screened for outliers by using the ROUT test (Q = 0.5%).
Data availability
All data generated or analysed during this study are included in the manuscript and supporting file; source data for all the figures are deposited at Dyrad and available at https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.dbrv15f54 The codes generated for this work are available on GitHub at https://github.com/NabaviLab-Git/Photometry-Signal-Analysis (copy archived at Nabavi Lab, 2022).
-
Dryad Digital RepositoryDistinct representations of innate and learned threats within the thalamic-amygdala pathway.https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.dbrv15f54
References
-
Lesions of the basal amygdala block expression of conditioned fear but not extinctionThe Journal of Neuroscience 25:9680–9685.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2600-05.2005
-
Organization of the inputs and outputs of the mouse superior colliculusNature Communications 12:4004.https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-021-24241-2
-
Dopamine gates LTP induction in lateral amygdala by suppressing feedforward inhibitionNature Neuroscience 6:587–592.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1058
-
Mouse defensive behaviors: pharmacological and behavioral assays for anxiety and panicNeuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 25:205–218.https://doi.org/10.1016/S0149-7634(01)00009-4
-
Visual adaptation and novelty responses in the superior colliculusThe European Journal of Neuroscience 34:766–779.https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07805.x
-
Adrenergic transmission facilitates extinction of conditional fear in miceLearning & Memory 11:179–187.https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.71504
-
Involvement of subcortical and cortical afferents to the lateral nucleus of the amygdala in fear conditioning measured with fear-potentiated startle in rats trained concurrently with auditory and visual conditioned stimuliThe Journal of Neuroscience 15:2312–2327.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-03-02312.1995
-
I’ll take the low road: the evolutionary underpinnings of visually triggered fearFrontiers in Neuroscience 9:414.https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2015.00414
-
New perspectives on beta-adrenergic mediation of innate and learned fear responses to predator odorThe Journal of Neuroscience 28:13296–13302.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2843-08.2008
-
Cognitive control of escape behaviourTrends in Cognitive Sciences 23:334–348.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2019.01.012
-
Noradrenergic modulation of fear conditioning and extinctionFrontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 12:43.https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00043
-
Extinction of auditory fear conditioning requires MAPK/ERK activation in the basolateral amygdalaThe European Journal of Neuroscience 24:261–269.https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04893.x
-
Propranolol decreases fear expression by modulating fear memory tracesBiological Psychiatry 89:1150–1161.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2021.01.005
-
Topographic organization of neurons in the acoustic thalamus that project to the amygdalaThe Journal of Neuroscience 10:1043–1054.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.10-04-01043.1990
-
Dissociated role of thalamic and cortical input to the lateral amygdala for consolidation of long-term fear memoryThe Journal of Neuroscience 41:9561–9570.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1167-21.2021
-
Direct synaptic connections of axons from superior colliculus with identified thalamo-amygdaloid projection neurons in the rat: possible substrates of a subcortical visual pathway to the amygdalaThe Journal of Comparative Neurology 403:158–170.
-
The influence of subcortical shortcuts on disordered sensory and cognitive processingNature Reviews. Neuroscience 21:264–276.https://doi.org/10.1038/s41583-020-0287-1
-
Neural correlates of competing fear behaviors evoked by an innately aversive stimulusThe Journal of Neuroscience 23:3855–3868.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-09-03855.2003
-
SoftwarePhotometry-signal-analysis, version swh:1:rev:ed2e6a272977a1dbafc923796e873ebc8484daa9Software Heritage.
-
Neural and cellular mechanisms of fear and extinction memory formationNeuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 36:1773–1802.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.12.014
-
Is there anybody out there? Neural circuits of threat detection in vertebratesCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology 41:179–187.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2016.09.011
-
On the relationship between emotion and cognitionNature Reviews. Neuroscience 9:148–158.https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2317
-
Emotion processing and the amygdala: from a “low road” to “many roads” of evaluating biological significanceNature Reviews. Neuroscience 11:773–783.https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2920
-
A specific class of interneuron mediates inhibitory plasticity in the lateral amygdalaThe Journal of Neuroscience 30:14619–14629.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3252-10.2010
-
Equipotentiality of thalamo-amygdala and thalamo-cortico-amygdala circuits in auditory fear conditioningThe Journal of Neuroscience 12:4501–4509.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.12-11-04501.1992
-
Somatosensory and auditory convergence in the lateral nucleus of the amygdalaBehavioral Neuroscience 107:444–450.https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.107.3.444
-
Role of glutamatergic projections from ventral tegmental area to lateral habenula in aversive conditioningThe Journal of Neuroscience 34:13906–13910.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2029-14.2014
-
A novel role for the lateral habenula in fear learningNeuropsychopharmacology 47:1210–1219.https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-022-01294-5
-
Stressed rats fail to exhibit avoidance reactions to innately aversive social callsNeuropsychopharmacology 47:1145–1155.https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-021-01230-z
-
Modular organization of the brainstem noradrenaline system coordinates opposing learning statesNature Neuroscience 20:1602–1611.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4642
-
Rapid innate defensive responses of mice to looming visual stimuliCurrent Biology 23:2011–2015.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.08.015
Article and author information
Author details
Funding
Danish Council for Independent Research
- Sadegh Nabavi
Novo Nordisk (NNF16OC0023368)
- Sadegh Nabavi
AUFF NOVA
- Sadegh Nabavi
Danish Research Institute of Translational Neuroscience (19958)
- Sadegh Nabavi
Danish National Research Foundation (DNRF133)
- Sadegh Nabavi
European Research Council (Starting grant 22736)
- Sadegh Nabavi
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Acknowledgements
We thank R Malinow, J Piriz, F Ferenc Mátyás, J Lima, and members of the Nabavi laboratory for suggestions. We thank Jean-Charles Paterna and Viral Vector Facility (VVF) of the Neuroscience Center Zurich (ZNZ) for support. We thank Zachary Leamy for comments on the manuscript. This study was supported by Independent Research (DFF), Novo Nordisk Foundation (NNF16OC0023368), and AUFF NOVA grants to SN. Additionally, SN was supported by an ERC starting grant (22736), the Danish Research Institute of Translational Neuroscience (19958), and PROMEMO (Center of Excellence for Proteins in Memory funded by the Danish National Research Foundation) (DNRF133). Figures were created with BioRender.com.
Ethics
All the animal expermentations performed here were reviewed and approved by Danish Animal Experiment Inspectorate (permit number 2020-15-0201-00421).
Copyright
© 2023, Khalil, Faress, Mermet-Joret et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 1,573
- views
-
- 235
- downloads
-
- 9
- citations
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Neuroscience
Time estimation is an essential prerequisite underlying various cognitive functions. Previous studies identified ‘sequential firing’ and ‘activity ramps’ as the primary neuron activity patterns in the medial frontal cortex (mPFC) that could convey information regarding time. However, the relationship between these patterns and the timing behavior has not been fully understood. In this study, we utilized in vivo calcium imaging of mPFC in rats performing a timing task. We observed cells that showed selective activation at trial start, end, or during the timing interval. By aligning long-term time-lapse datasets, we discovered that sequential patterns of time coding were stable over weeks, while cells coding for trial start or end showed constant dynamism. Furthermore, with a novel behavior design that allowed the animal to determine individual trial interval, we were able to demonstrate that real-time adjustment in the sequence procession speed closely tracked the trial-to-trial interval variations. And errors in the rats’ timing behavior can be primarily attributed to the premature ending of the time sequence. Together, our data suggest that sequential activity maybe a stable neural substrate that represents time under physiological conditions. Furthermore, our results imply the existence of a unique cell type in the mPFC that participates in the time-related sequences. Future characterization of this cell type could provide important insights in the neural mechanism of timing and related cognitive functions.
-
- Neuroscience
Sour taste, which is elicited by low pH, may serve to help animals distinguish appetitive from potentially harmful food sources. In all species studied to date, the attractiveness of oral acids is contingent on concentration. Many carboxylic acids are attractive at ecologically relevant concentrations but become aversive beyond some maximal concentration. Recent work found that Drosophila ionotropic receptors IR25a and IR76b expressed by sweet-responsive gustatory receptor neurons (GRNs) in the labellum, a peripheral gustatory organ, mediate appetitive feeding behaviors toward dilute carboxylic acids. Here, we disclose the existence of pharyngeal sensors in Drosophila melanogaster that detect ingested carboxylic acids and are also involved in the appetitive responses to carboxylic acids. These pharyngeal sensors rely on IR51b, IR94a, and IR94h, together with IR25a and IR76b, to drive responses to carboxylic acids. We then demonstrate that optogenetic activation of either Ir94a+ or Ir94h+ GRNs promotes an appetitive feeding response, confirming their contributions to appetitive feeding behavior. Our discovery of internal pharyngeal sour taste receptors opens up new avenues for investigating the internal sensation of tastants in insects.