Pupillometry: Consciousness reflected in the eyes

People with higher autistic traits display stronger fluctuations in pupil size when presented with an optical illusion.
  1. Woon Ju Park  Is a corresponding author
  2. Kimberly B Schauder  Is a corresponding author
  3. Duje Tadin  Is a corresponding author
  1. University of Rochester, United States
  2. University of Rochester School of Medicine, United States

Most of the tasks that our brain orchestrates in our body are performed outside of our conscious awareness. In addition to breathing, maintaining our balance and keeping our heart beating, these tasks include increasing or decreasing the size of our pupils as our environment becomes darker or lighter. This allows us to see in both darkened rooms and on bright sunny days. Although the area of a pupil can change by as much as a factor of ten (Winn et al., 1994), we are not aware of the small movements in the ocular muscles that open and close our pupils.

Recent studies have painted a complex and fascinating picture of what drives these changes in pupil size. Despite representing the lowest level of visual function, these pupil dynamics can reflect sophisticated processes that are closely linked to our everyday experience (including attention, decision making, and even aesthetic experiences; Binda and Murray, 2015; Hartmann and Fischer, 2014). Therefore, measurements of the pupils, also known as pupillometry, may be used as an indicator for cognitive and perceptual states. This method is also objective and non-invasive, and can, therefore be applied in a wide range of clinical contexts. For example, it enables us to communicate with patients suffering from ‘locked-in’ syndrome – a condition characterized by the paralysis of every muscle except the eye (Stoll et al., 2013).

Now, in eLife, Marco Turi, David Burr and Paola Binda – who are based at research institutes in Pisa, Florence and Sydney – report that fluctuations in pupil size may provide insight into clinical disorders (Turi et al., 2018). The researchers used a well-known optical cylinder illusion that consists of two overlapping sets of black and white dots moving in opposite directions on a 2D plane (Figure 1A). Due to the different speeds of the dots, anyone looking at the dots usually sees a rotating 3D cylinder (Andersen and Bradley, 1998; Video 1 in Turi et al., 2018). However, since the resulting illusion makes it difficult to detect which color is at the front (and, thus, the direction of rotation), our visual system selects one interpretation at any given time. As a result, the perceived rotation switches direction every few seconds (Figure 1B). Such approaches, where the physical stimulus remains constant but our subjective perception changes, have been widely used to study the mechanisms underlying visual awareness (Kim and Blake, 2005).

Changes in pupil size depend on subjective visual perception.

(A) Turi et al. used a visual stimulus in which two sets of dots (white and black) moved in opposite directions (thin arrows). (B) To an observer this stimulus appears as a 3D cylinder that rotates in a clockwise direction with the black dots at the front (top), or in a counter-clockwise direction with the white dots at the front (bottom). This perceived direction of rotation switches every few seconds. (C) Turi et al. found that pupil size increased when the black dots appeared to be at the front, and decreased when the white dots appeared to be at the front. The size of this change was correlated with Autism-Spectrum Quotient score.

To track any subjective change in perception, the participants reported how they experienced the rotation of the cylinder, while the researchers measured the pupil size. Although the overall intensity of light (the primary factor influencing pupil size) remained constant, the size of the pupils changed: the pupils became larger when the black dots seemed to be in the front, but became smaller, when the white dots appeared to be in front (Figure 1C).

In some participants, the size of the pupils changed substantially between the black and white phases, while in others the changes were only minimal. Turi et al. revealed that these differences strongly correlated with the Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ) scores of the participants (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001). The higher the number of autistic traits reported by an individual in the AQ questionnaire, the stronger the changes in pupil size became. This effect accounted for about half of the variance in AQ scores, which is considerably higher than the sizes of the effects reported for other sensory measures (e.g., Ujiie et al., 2015).

Turi et al. argue that the changes in pupil size reflect how individuals process visual information differently. Some people tend to focus on small, defined areas such as the surface at the front, while others concentrate on the entire cylinder. Consequently, people with a more detailed focus should have more fluctuations in pupil size, because their focus alternates between black and white dots. This observation is tightly linked with the long-standing hypothesis that individuals with autism spectrum disorders tend to focus more on the detail rather than the bigger picture (Happé and Frith, 2006).

The findings of Turi et al. provide compelling evidence that the size of a pupil reflects subjective differences in a way that is highly correlated to the reported autistic traits of the participants. The study provides numerous possibilities to investigate higher-level cognitive processes. This can be particularly valuable when studying individuals who may find it difficult to engage in complex behavioral tasks, such as individuals with minimal language skills or deficits in other cognitive abilities. Pupillometry is clearly a powerful tool for characterizing the individual differences in processing visual information and, potentially, for advancing our understanding of autism spectrum disorders.

References

    1. Winn B
    2. Whitaker D
    3. Elliott DB
    4. Phillips NJ
    (1994)
    Factors affecting light-adapted pupil size in normal human subjects
    Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science 35:1132–1137.

Article and author information

Author details

  1. Woon Ju Park

    Woon Ju Park is in the Center for Visual Science and the Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, University of Rochester, Rochester, United States.

    For correspondence
    woonju.park@gmail.com
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0003-1473-4876
  2. Kimberly B Schauder

    Kimberly B Schauder is in the Center for Visual Science and the Department of Clinical and Social Sciences in Psychology, University of Rochester, Rochester, United States.

    For correspondence
    kimberly.schauder@rochester.edu
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0001-6832-1044
  3. Duje Tadin

    Duje Tadin is in the Center for Visual Science and the Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, University of Rochester, Rochester, United States, and the Departments of Ophthalmology and Neuroscience, University of Rochester School of Medicine, Rochester, United States.

    For correspondence
    dtadin@ur.rochester.edu
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0003-1571-5589

Publication history

  1. Version of Record published: March 6, 2018 (version 1)

Copyright

© 2018, Park et al.

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.

Metrics

  • 1,780
    Page views
  • 164
    Downloads
  • 0
    Citations

Article citation count generated by polling the highest count across the following sources: Crossref, PubMed Central, Scopus.

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Woon Ju Park
  2. Kimberly B Schauder
  3. Duje Tadin
(2018)
Pupillometry: Consciousness reflected in the eyes
eLife 7:e35374.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.35374

Further reading

    1. Neuroscience
    Lies Deceuninck, Fabian Kloosterman
    Research Article Updated

    Storing and accessing memories is required to successfully perform day-to-day tasks, for example for engaging in a meaningful conversation. Previous studies in both rodents and primates have correlated hippocampal cellular activity with behavioral expression of memory. A key role has been attributed to awake hippocampal replay – a sequential reactivation of neurons representing a trajectory through space. However, it is unclear if awake replay impacts immediate future behavior, gradually creates and stabilizes long-term memories over a long period of time (hours and longer), or enables the temporary memorization of relevant events at an intermediate time scale (seconds to minutes). In this study, we aimed to address the uncertainty around the timeframe of impact of awake replay by collecting causal evidence from behaving rats. We detected and disrupted sharp wave ripples (SWRs) - signatures of putative replay events - using electrical stimulation of the ventral hippocampal commissure in rats that were trained on three different spatial memory tasks. In each task, rats were required to memorize a new set of locations in each trial or each daily session. Interestingly, the rats performed equally well with or without SWR disruptions. These data suggest that awake SWRs - and potentially replay - does not affect the immediate behavior nor the temporary memorization of relevant events at a short timescale that are required to successfully perform the spatial tasks. Based on these results, we hypothesize that the impact of awake replay on memory and behavior is long-term and cumulative over time.

    1. Neuroscience
    Sydney Trask, Nicole C Ferrara
    Insight

    Gradually reducing a source of fear during extinction treatments may weaken negative memories in the long term.