Hawkmoths evaluate scenting flowers with the tip of their proboscis
Abstract
Pollination by insects is essential to many ecosystems. Previously, we have shown that floral scent is important to mediate pollen transfer between plants (Kessler et al., 2015). Yet, the mechanisms by which pollinators evaluate volatiles of single flowers remained unclear. Here, Nicotiana attenuata plants, in which floral volatiles have been genetically silenced and its hawkmoth pollinator, Manduca sexta, were used in semi-natural tent and wind-tunnel assays to explore the function of floral scent. We found that floral scent functions to increase the fitness of individual flowers not only by increasing detectability but also by enhancing the pollinator's foraging efforts. Combining proboscis choice tests with neurophysiological, anatomical and molecular analyses we show that this effect is governed by newly discovered olfactory neurons on the tip of the moth's proboscis. With the tip of their tongue, pollinators assess the advertisement of individual flowers, an ability essential for maintaining this important ecosystem service.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.15039.001Introduction
Floral scent has been associated with insect pollination since the 18th century (Sprengler, 1793); however, the complex functions of floral volatiles have been only recently investigated in more detail, due to the availability of new molecular and analytical techniques (Raguso, 2008). Floral scent not only attracts pollinators (Klahre et al., 2011), but also manipulates them through chemical mimicry (Stökl et al., 2010) and repels herbivores (Junker and Blüthgen, 2010), altogether increasing plant fitness (Kessler et al., 2008). However, research studying the function of floral scent has been divided along two themes with little cross-fertilization: 1) studies examining the fitness effects of floral scent without the causal behavioral responses of pollinators (Kessler et al., 2008), or 2) studies examining the sensory physiology of pollinators, neglecting the ecological consequences for the plant (Raguso, 2008). Here, we meld these approaches and show that floral scent increases the fitness of individual flowers not only by increasing their detectability (Raguso and Willis, 2002), but also by enhancing the pollinator’s foraging motivation, and demonstrate that this is mediated by olfactory receptors on the tip of the moth’s proboscis which detect floral scent.
The hawkmoth Manduca sexta (Figure 1A, Video 1) is a major pollinator of the wild tobacco Nicotiana attenuata in the Great Basin Desert (USA) (Kessler et al., 2008; 2010; 2015). N. attenuata emits a relatively simple floral scent dominated by a single compound: benzyl acetone (BA) (Euler and Baldwin, 1996). In spite of this simplicity, producing BA might come at considerable metabolic but especially ecological cost, as BA might attract not only nectar thieves and florivores, but also female hawkmoths in search of oviposition sites (Baldwin et al., 1997; Kessler et al., 2010). Plants might therefore reduce the amount of floral volatiles released as much as possible without losing their pollination services. Field experiments using plants in which the emissions of BA had been silenced by RNAi of the biosynthetic gene NaChal1 (CHAL) have shown that BA is required to maximize pollination success (Kessler et al., 2008; 2015). It was suggested that lacking this scent made plants in nature 'invisible' to hawkmoth pollinators. However, the precise mechanisms by which odors of single flowers influence pollinator behavior and thereby plant fitness have rarely been examined in the direct interaction between plant and pollinator (Klahre et al., 2011; Riffell et al., 2008). Hence, how floral scent emitted by individual flowers functions in this mutualistic interaction remained unknown, particularly as it is unclear how pollinators detect single volatile compounds within complex natural environments (Hansson and Stensmyr, 2011; Riffell et al., 2014).
Results and discussion
We investigated the function of floral scent in the context of individual flower-moth interactions, by offering individual male moths the choice between BA-emitting flowers (i.e. empty-vector transformed flowers (EV)) and non-emitting flowers (i.e. CHAL) in a free flight tent (24 m × 8 m × 4 m, 10 CHAL and 10 EV plants, spaced 50 cm apart). The flight tracking revealed that moths chose to visit the same number of emitting and non-emitting flowers in a random sequence (Figure 1B, Figure 1—figure supplement 1A, probability of changing between EV or CHAL flowers during consecutive visits: 0.47). In a second bioassay conducted in a wind tunnel (2.4 m × 0.9 m × 0.9 m, moonlight [0.5 lux of sunlight spectrum]), we presented plants with either emitting or non-emitting flowers to individual moths and analyzed their flight patterns, approaches and flower contacts. In none of these analyses, we found any difference between plants with emitting and non-emitting flowers (Figure 1—figure supplement 1B,C). These results suggest that visual cues and general vegetative plant odors provided sufficient information for the moths to locate flowers, consistent with previous work using artificial flowers (Raguso and Willis, 2002) and clearly showing that non-scenting flowers are not 'invisible' to moths.
If non-scenting flowers are found by moths, why is plant fitness reduced? Does BA emission change the pollination probability? To test this, we loaded the moth’s proboscis with a standardized number of pollen grains using a fine brush. When such pollen-enhanced moths were allowed to forage freely on antherectomized N. attenuata flowers, seeds produced per flower of EV and CHAL plants differed significantly (Figure 1C). Scentless flowers matured very few seeds, reflecting the inferior pollination services provided by the moths despite a similar number of visits. This result highlights the importance of BA emission for the fitness of individual flowers and confirms the results of previous studies, which investigated the effects of BA emission on plant fitness at a population level (Kessler et al., 2008; 2015). But if the flowers were equally detectable by the moths, what behavioral mechanism was responsible for the plant fitness consequences?
To analyze the effect of BA emission on moth behavior in greater detail, we quantified the time invested by a moth at individual flowers in a wind tunnel assay. The moths spent significantly more time at emitting than at non-emitting flowers (Figure 2A) particularly while trying to insert their proboscis, so even before tasting the floral nectar (Figure 2—figure supplement 1A). However, having successfully inserted their proboscis, the time of nectar uptake was similar between them (Figure 2—figure supplement 1B). This suggests that BA emission increased the motivation of moths to forage when individual flowers were evaluated at a close range, possibly because BA emissions, are closely linked to the physiological state and thereby also to the potential nectar amount of a flower (Bhattacharya and Baldwin, 2012; Yon et al., 2015; Kessler et al., 2015). By increasing the probing time in BA-emitting flowers, moths increased their success rate at their first as well as at consecutive flower visits and, therefore, collected more nectar per flower visit in tent (Figure 2B, Videos 1 and 2, Figure 2—figure supplement 1C) and wind tunnel assays (Figure 2—figure supplement 1D). These results agree with a study using different Petunia lines which found that although flower scent aided navigation, increased nectaring was the most consistent effect of floral scent (Klahre et al., 2011).
The large fitness consequences of floral volatiles for both moth and plant beg the question: how do moths evaluate the headspace of individual flowers? The wide spread of the antennae and their distance from the flower resulting from the moth’s long proboscis which is fully extended during nectaring suggests that the olfactory spatial resolution of the antennae might be too low to resolve individual flowers in an inflorescence or even between neighboring plants (Willis et al., 2013). Hence, we inferred that the moth’s proboscis might play a role in flower perception (Goyret and Raguso, 2006). Using reverse transcription PCR, we qualified the accumulation of transcripts of olfactory genes in the proboscis of M. sexta. Similar to the mosquito Aedes aegypti, but contrasting with predictions for nectar feeding insects (Jung et al., 2015), we found that the olfactory co-receptor Orco was expressed in the tip region of the proboscis along with the ionotropic co-receptor, IR25a (Figure 3A). Notably, Orco was only expressed in the first centimeter of the proboscis whereas the ionotropic co-receptor, IR8a, was only found in upper sections. This heterogeneous distribution of olfactory genes is consistent with the idea that the moth proboscis plays a more complex role in chemoreception than previously thought (Reiter et al., 2015). Screening the proboscis tip by scanning electron microscopy (Figure 3B, Figure 3—figure supplement 1A,B) we found a sensillum type that was not previously described for M. sexta (Reiter et al., 2015). This sensillum resembled the known sensillum styloconicum, but instead of a single tip pore, had a multiporous cone (Figure 3B4). Similar sensillum types have been described in other lepidopteran species (Faucheux, 2013), but their function remained unknown, although the presence of odorant-binding proteins suggested a role in olfaction (Nagnan-Le Meillour et al., 2000). We used an antibody raised against Orco (Nolte et al., unpublished), and found a single Orco- positive cell only in the first multiporous sensilla styloconica (mSt) at the tip of the proboscis (Figure 3C, Figure 3—figure supplement 1).
Given the presence of potential olfactory sensilla on the proboscis of M. sexta, we wondered whether neurons housed in these sensilla play a role in the detection of BA and could thus help explain the pollination differences of EV and CHAL flowers. We performed single sensillum recordings and tested the response of neurons present in all sensillum types occurring at the tip of the proboscis to an air puff of BA at an ecologically relevant concentration (0.1 mM Kessler and Baldwin, 2007). Only neurons in the Orco-positive sensillum reacted to this compound (Figure 4B, Figure 4—figure supplement 1A). In a further test with 41 other ecologically relevant odorants, the first mSt was found to be more sensitive to BA and the structurally related benzylacetate (Figure 4C, Figure 4C—source data 1). Though these results show that neurons in the proboscis tip of M. sexta can detect volatile BA, it remained unclear whether the moth would also respond behaviorally to this compound based only on the input from neurons of the proboscis sensilla.
-
Figure 4—source data 1
- https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.15039.011
To disentangle the proboscis input from other chemosensory organs, we devised a behavioral experiment in which the corolla tube of a flower was replaced by a Y-maze choice assay (Figure 4D). Each arm of the Y-maze was either connected to a source of humidified air or humidified air scented with BA (0.1 mM). By drawing air directly behind the entrance of the Y-maze, the experimental set-up excluded antenna-based olfaction. Hence, as soon as a moth entered the flower aperture during free hovering flight, only the proboscis experienced the air stream containing either a solvent control, or BA (Video 3). During their first and subsequent insertions, the moths chose both Y-tube arms with equal frequency (Figure 4F, Figure 4—figure supplement 1B), but inserted their proboscis for a significantly longer time into the arm containing the BA-scented air (Figure 4E), demonstrating that the moth was able to detect BA with only the proboscis. Moths seem to use the olfactory input from the proboscis not for orientation on the corolla, but rather to assess the specific quality of an individual flower, consistent with the notion that the close-range orientation of the proboscis on the flower can be informed by mechanical and visual cues (Goyret and Raguso, 2006; Sponberg et al., 2015).
Our findings show that hawkmoths are well adapted to visit and detect volatiles of single flowers. Floral volatiles, such as BA, not only function as navigational cues (Haverkamp et al., 2016), but also inform pollinators about the identity and the physiological state of individual flowers (Bhattacharya and Baldwin, 2012; Yon et al., 2015). Only BA-emitting flowers encourage the moth to visit a flower long enough to lead to successful pollination. Our results show that floral scent is an essential chemical feature for hawkmoths to gain nectar from, and pollinate, a single flower. Interestingly, many flowers require that their pollinators acquire particular handling skills on their first visits, before the insects are able to use the flower efficiently (Laverty, 1994). This energy investment by the pollinator not only helps ensure outcrossing for the plant, but provides the insect with a more exclusive nectar source (Heinrich, 1979). In a recent study, inexperienced M. sexta were found to sometimes expend more energy on handling flowers than they gained from the nectar; if additional experience increases foraging efficiency, this would compel the moth to visit additional flowers of the same species (Haverkamp et al., 2016). Notably, the ability to smell BA with the tip of the proboscis may not only increase the motivation of M. sexta to invest energy into BA emitting flowers, but also strengthen the moth’s learning of these flowers, as the nectar reward becomes associated with the presence of BA. Such a BA-conditioned learning rate might help to ensure a positive energy balance for the moth while at the same time ensuring cross-pollination for the plant (Heinrich and Raven, 1972). However, the question to what extent the interaction of moths and plants relies on the moths’ learning ability requires additional attention in future studies.
Although both – metabolically costly and risky in terms of herbivory – thousands of plant species actively emit floral scent (Wright and Schiestl, 2009). These emissions might be a consequence of the physiological requirement for scent compounds by certain pollinator guilds when collecting nectar, even when visual cues would be sufficient to attract pollinators to a plant. The ongoing evolutionary interaction between plants and pollinators relies heavily on floral scent (Parachnowitsch et al., 2012; Schiestl and Johnson, 2013), and may explain the absence of scent-free plants in native N. attenuata populations (Kessler et al., 2015). To pollinators with the appropriate sensory system, floral scent provides a wealth of information, highlighting the importance of chemical communication in this mutualism, on which many of our crops rely (Radera et al., 2015).
Materials and methods
Plants
Request a detailed protocolWe used two transgenic Nicotiana attenuata Torr. (Solanaceae) lines derived from Agrobacterium tumefaciens (strain LBA 4404) transformation of wild type N. attenuata plants which were collected in a native population at the DI Ranch (Santa Clara, UT, USA) in 1988 and subsequently inbred for 22 generations (Krügel et al., 2002). Both lines have been described earlier, empty vector control plants (EV) transformed with pSOL3NC (line number A-04-266-3) (Bubner et al., 2006), as well and plants silenced by RNAi in the production of floral scent, CHAL line (N. attenuata chalcone synthase; pRESC5CHAL, line number A-07-283-5) (Kessler et al., 2008). Seeds were sterilized and germinated on Petri dishes with Gamborg’s B5 media as described in Krügel et al. (2002). Petri dishes with 30 seeds were maintained under LD (16 hr light and 8 hr dark) conditions in a growth chamber (Percival, Perry, Iowa, USA) for 10 days, and seedlings were transferred to small pots (TEKU JP 3050 104 pots, Pöppelmann, Germany) with Klasmann plug soil (Klasmann-Deilmann, Germany) in the glasshouse. After 10 days, plants were transferred to 9 cm × 9 cm pots for wind tunnel experiments or to 1 l pots for tent experiments. The glasshouse growth conditions are described in Krügel et al. (2002). For use in the wind tunnel, plants were transferred to a York Chamber (Johnston Controls, USA) with an inverted day/night cycle; daylight time was from 17–9, maintained at 25°C and night time temperatures were 22°C, with a humidity of 60–80%. Plants used in the tent experiment were cultivated in the Max Planck Institute for Chemical Ecology (MPICOE), Jena, Germany, main glasshouse. After attaining the rosette stage of growth, plants were transferred to a second glasshouse, maintained at the same conditions, located in Isserstedt, Germany, where the plants were cultivated until used for the tent experiments.
Insects
Moths used in the wind tunnel experiments were obtained from a colony maintained at the in Jena, Germany. Animals were reared as previously described (Koenig et al., 2015). Eggs were collected from female M. sexta moths, which could freely oviposit on N. attenuata plants. For the tent experiments, eggs from native M. sexta populations at the Utah field station were collected and shipped to Germany. After hatching, caterpillars were maintained on artificial diet (wind tunnel) or on Nicotiana tabacum plants (tent) at an ambient temperature of 27°C, 70% relative humidity and a light regime of 16:8 (light: dark). Fifth instar caterpillars were transferred into individual wood chambers for pupation. One week before hatching, pupae were sexed and male pupae were transferred to a flight cage with 15.5 hr daylight and 7.5 hr dim light (0.5 lux). Temperature and relative humidity were set to 25°C and 70% during day-light phase and to 20°C and 60% during the dim light phase. A transition phase of 30 min was used between phases. Animals were used for experiments 72–76 hr after hatching.
Wind tunnel
Request a detailed protocolNo-choice assays were performed in a Plexiglas wind tunnel (220 cm × 90 cm × 90 cm). Charcoal-filtered air was pushed through the tunnel at a speed of 0.37 m/s. Air temperature and relative humidity was adjusted to 25°C and 70%. Plants were transferred to the wind tunnel chamber at least 1 hr before the experiment; to avoid contamination, plants were cultivated in a separated compartment with an additional charcoal air filter. Directly before each trial, a single plant was placed at the front of the wind tunnel with the flower positioned 70 cm above the tunnel floor, 20 cm from the tunnel front and 45 cm from each tunnel side.
Moths were placed individually in mesh-cages (15 cm × Ø13 cm) 1 hr before the experiment and transferred to a pre-exposure chamber set to the same light and climate conditions as the wind tunnel. For each trial, a single M. sexta moth was placed on a platform 35 cm above the tunnel floor, 20 cm from the tunnel end and 45 cm from each tunnel side. After placement, every animal was given 5 min to innate wing- fanning. Animals which did not start wing-fanning (63% ) within this time frame were considered as non-responders and excluded from subsequent analyses. After take-off, the behavior of each moth was recorded for 4 min using a custom-made 3D video tracking system. The tracking system consisted of four cameras (Logitech C615, USA, infrared filter removed) recording at 30 Hz and a resolution of 800 × 600 pixels (each pixel 0.3 cm²). Using a background subtraction algorithm implemented in C, the 3D position of the moth was calculated at a rate of 10 Hz. Based on these tracking data, we analyzed the flight pattern of the moth during the last two seconds before encountering the flower in the wind tunnel using costume-written Matlab scripts (Mathworks, USA). All recorded flight tracks were cross-checked with video data and only complete recordings were used for further analyses. In order to avoid learning effects, we only considered the first flower approached by each moth.
Moth behavior at the flower was recorded at a rate of 30 Hz by a fifth camera (Logitech C615, infrared filter removed), which had been placed into the wind tunnel at a distance of about 30 cm from the flower. Recordings were automatically started by a custom-written movement detection algorithm. Flower probing times and total contact time were measured by manually analyzing the individual video files. Similar to the flight analyses, we only considered the first flower contact in the statistical analyses.
Tent
Request a detailed protocolTo emulate a natural environment, we conducted pollination experiments in a large tent (height, 4 m; width, 8 m; length, 24 m) with an enclosed roof to protect from rain and lateral mesh for natural airflow (Kessler et al., 2015). Plants were moved from the glasshouse to the tent which was located directly adjacent to the glasshouse. Experiments in the tent were conducted between August 27 and September 8 2014. Ten plants of each of the two lines EV and CHAL were aligned in rows at the central section of the tent. EV and CHAL plants were positioned directly next to each other, even touching inflorescences. The position of the EV and CHAL plants were changed after each moth, to minimize potential position effects. In order to use only freshly opened flowers, all open flowers were removed each morning, before experiments were conducted. Six to eight male M. sexta moths were released sequentially per night. A new moth was released only when the previous had stopped flying and every moth was only used once. Single flower visitations were observed, the genotype and time at a single flower was noted, and after the visitation, the approached flowers were removed to measure the remaining nectar. Each moth had up to 10 flower encounters and for each moth the mean nectar gain across all flower encounters was calculated.
Nectar and pollen analysis
Request a detailed protocolDirectly after each experimental wind tunnel trial, both the moth and the plant were removed. The remaining nectar in the flower was measured by carefully removing the flower base and removing the nectar with a pre-weighed capillary. The nectar amount was determined by reweighing the capillary and subtracting the two weights. In the tent trials, nectar volume was quantified directly using a BLAUDBRAND graduated capillary with a volume of 25 µL (Brand, Germany) by gently removing the corolla (Kessler et al., 2007).
The pollen load on the moth proboscis was determined by rinsing the proboscis three times in 1 mL of 1% Tween solution. 10 µL of 0.5% safranin (Sigma Aldrich, Germany) were added to each sample to stain the pollen outer layer. The samples were vortexed and centrifuged for 2 min at 10000 rpm. Thereafter, the supernatant was discarded without disturbing the pollen pellet and 100 µL distilled water was added to each sample. The samples were then vortexed and 10 µL were pipetted into a four-field Neubauer-counting chamber to determine the pollen number in each sample. Every sample was counted twice independently, and the mean value was used for statistical analysis.
Cross pollination experiment
Request a detailed protocolTo measure pollination rates in EV and CHAL plants by M. sexta in the wind tunnel, fully developed flowers were emasculated in the previous corresponding daylight morning cycle to avoid self-pollination (Kessler et al., 2008). For this, 3–4 flowers per plant of each line (EV and CHAL) were used in the wind tunnel, one plant and one moth at each time. Fresh pollen was collected in the corresponding morning from plants not being used for pollination. EV pollen which had been collected the previous night was rubbed on the hawkmoth proboscis using a fine brush prior to its release in the wind tunnel, in order to measure the pollen delivery to experimental flowers. If moths did not take flight voluntarily within 3 min after being placed in the wind tunnel they were excluded from the study. Moths, which took flight (73% ), were allowed to do so for four minutes in each wind tunnel trial. The numbers of matured capsules, as well as the seeds produced from each capsule were counted after ripening. Capsules were collected shortly before opening, approximately 14 days after the experiment, dried in a desiccator, and once opened, the seeds were counted in petri dishes. After each trial the pollen from each M. sexta was collected by washing the proboscis to ensure that similar amounts of pollen had been placed on the proboscis. For pollen counts, the same procedure as for the pollen retrieval was used. On average, we found that 548.75 (n= 41, SEM= 88.85) pollen grains had been placed on a single proboscis. No difference was found between moths tested with EV (n= 21, mean= 569, SEM= 92.3) or CHAL (n=20, mean= 500, SEM= 159.3) plants (Student’s t-test, p= 0.71).
Scanning electron microscopy
Request a detailed protocolM. sexta proboscises were cut 1 cm from the tip and fixed in 4% glutaraldehyde at 4°C overnight. Proboscises were then dehydrated in an ascending ethanol series (70%, 80%, 90%, 96%, 3x 100% ethanol, 10 min each), critical point dried (BAL-TEC CPD 030, Bal-Tec Union Ltd., Liechtenstein), mounted on aluminium stubs with conductive carbon cement (Agar Scientific, UK) and sputter coated with gold on a BAL-TEC SCD005 (Bal-Tec, Liechtenstein). Specimens were examined in a LEO 1530 Gemini scanning electron microscope (Zeiss, Germany) set at 8 kV and 11 to 15 mm working distance.
Immunohistochemistry and confocal laser scanning microscopy
Request a detailed protocolThe tip region of 20 M. sexta proboscises were carefully dissected into three small parts, cutting behind the first, before the fourth and behind the fifth sensillum styloconica. Directly after dissection, the proboscis parts were fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde (ROTH, Germany) in 1 M NaHCO3 (Sigma Aldrich, pH 9.5) overnight at 4°C. Subsequently, the samples were washed six times for 30 min in 1× phosphate-buffered saline containing 0.1% Trition X (PBS-T) (Sigma Aldrich, USA) and thereafter blocked for 3 hr in normal goat serum (NGS). The primary anti-body against Orco (kindly provided by Prof. Jürgen Krieger, University of Halle-Wittenberg, Germany) was applied at a 1:500 dilution in 2% NGS- PBS-T and incubated for 5 days at 4°C. Detection of the Orco antibody was performed by incubating in a goat-anti rabbit antibody linked to Alexa 488 (Invitrogen, USA) at a dilution of 1:200 in 2% NGS-PBS-T for 3 days at 4°C. In addition, we added an goat anti-horseradish peroxidase antibodies conjugated to Cy3 (Jackson Immuno Research, USA) at a dilution of 1:50 in 2% NGS-PBS-T to visualize neuronal tissue. For visualization, the samples were mounted in 50% glycerol on a microscope slide and scanned using confocal laser scanning microscopy (LSM 880, Zeiss, Germany). Alexa 488 was exited using the 488 nm line of the microscopes Argon laser, while a Helium Neon 543 laser was used to activate Cy3. Signals were detected by a spectral detector (quasar: 490–553 nm and 555–681 nm). All pictures were taken using a 20× air objective (N.A. 0.8). Scanning resolution was set to 1024 × 1024 pixel.
Total RNA isolation
Request a detailed protocolProboscises of ten male M. sexta were dissected and were cut 1 cm from the tip. Each tissue sample (tips and rest) was directly transferred to Tri-reagent (Sigma-Aldrich, USA). The samples were then homogenized with two 3 mm steel beads (Qiagen, Germany) using a TissueLyser (Qiagen, Germany) for 5 min at 50 Hz. Samples were stored at -20°C. Finally, RNA isolation was performed using TRI- Reagent (Sigma-Aldrich, USA) according to the manufacturer's instructions.
cDNA synthesis
Request a detailed protocolRNA samples were treated with TurboDNAse (Ambion, USA) according to the manufacturer's instructions. DNAse was removed using Tri-reagent following the instructions of the producer. RNA was dissolved in 25 µL RNA storage solution (Ambion, USA). For cDNA synthesis 1 µg total RNA per sample was used as template for the Super Script III kit (Invitrogen, Canada).
Reverse transcription-PCR
Request a detailed protocolFor RT-PCR dNTPS (Thermo Fisher Scientific, Lithuania), cDNA, gene-specific primers and the Avantage 2 Polymerase mix (Clontech, Canada) were used following the manufacturer’s instructions. Primers were designed according to Koenig et al. (2015): (RL31: GGA GAG AGG AAA GGC AAA TC and CGG AAG GGG ACA TTT CTG AC; MsexIR8a: CAA CCC CGA CGC GTA TCC GTA TCC and TTA CGG CCT ATA TTC ATT TTT AGG AAA AAC GCT TAT ATA TG; MsexIR25a: GGA GTC CGT ATA GCT ATC AGA ATA ATC GAG and TCA AAA TTT AGG TTT CAA ATT AGA TAA ACC TAA ATT TCT GGA TC; MsexORCo: ATG ATG GCC AAA GTG AAA ACA CAG G and CTA TTT CAG CTG CAC CAA CAC CAT G). Reaction was done in a thermocycler (GeneAmp PCR System 9700, PE Applied Biosystems, USA) with 95°C for 1 min, followed by 35 cycles of 95°C for 30 s, 60°C (for MsexIR25a: 62°C) for 30 s and 68°C for 90 s. The final step was incubation at 68°C for 3 min. The samples were loaded on a 1.5% agarose gel.
Electrophysiology
Request a detailed protocolFor electrophysiological recordings, moths were placed into a 15 mL reaction tube, from which the tip had been cut; in such a way that only the proboscis would extend from the tube. Eachanimal was then mounted on a microscope slide, and the proboscis was fixed with dental wax. Next we unrolled the first centimetre of the proboscis and fixed this part upside down on a small wax pedestal, so that most of the sensilla were approachable for electrophysiological recordings. Subsequently, the preparation was positioned under a microscope (BX51W1, Olympus, Japan) and a tungsten reference electrode was inserted into the proboscis shaft. The recording electrode was then inserted into the target styloconic sensillum via a motorized, piezo-translator-equipped micromanipulator (DC-3K/PM-10, Märzhauser, Germany). A constant air stream of humidified air was applied to the preparation. For stimulus delivery either 0.1 mg BA diluted in distilled water or distilled water only was loaded onto a filter paper, inserted in a glass pipette and puffed onto the proboscis using a Syntech stimulus controller (CS- 55, Syntech, The Netherlands). For the odor screen individual compounds were diluted in hexane (10–2 v/v) and 10 µL were loaded on to a filter paper and puffed as described before. A single puff lasted for 0.5 s. The recorded signal was then amplified (UN-06, Syntech, The Netherlands), digitally converted (IDAC-4, Syntech, The Netherlands), and recorded at a rate of 2400 Hz using AutoSpike v3.2. (Syntech, The Netherlands). Traces were exported as ASCII files and manually analyzed using R. Spikes were counted 2 s before the stimulus onset and 2 s thereafter. The number of spikes before the stimulus was then subtracted from the spikes counted after the stimulus onset. The resulting number of ∆ spikes was then divided by the number of seconds analyzed. In all experiments, three day old male moths were used.
Proboscis choice
Request a detailed protocolOlfactory preference of the proboscis was tested in a custom-built Y-maze (5 cm × 3 cm × 0.5 cm). Previous studies had found a 0.1 mM suspension of BA in nectar to be ecologically relevant in the interaction between M. sexta and N. attenuata (Kessler and Baldwin, 2007). Here, we tested 10 µL of 0.1 mM BA suspension in distilled water against the same amount of distilled water only. Both stimuli were pipetted onto a small filter paper discs and placed into 50 mL glass bottles. Bottles were connected to the Y-tube arms via Teflon tubing (Ø 6 mm). Charcoal-filtered air was pushed into the bottles so that the air flow at each Y-tube arm reached 0.1 L/ min. To prevent the moths’ antenna from contacting BA headspace and assure a homogenous flow, air was removed from the opening of the Y-tube at a rate of 0.2 L/ min. The movement of the moth proboscis was recorded via a video camera (Logitech C615, infrared filter removed) at 30 Hz. Videos were captured using the software package Media recorder (Noldus, The Netherlands) and subsequently viewed and manually analyzed using EthoVision (Noldus, The Netherlands). For tests, the Y-maze set-up was placed into the wind tunnel described above and moths were allowed to forage freely for 4 min. In order to attract the moths to the Y-maze, we attached the corolla of a freshly cut Nicotiana alata flower, which does not release BA (Raguso et al., 2003), onto the Y-maze opening.
References
-
Patterns and consequences of benzyl acetone floral emissions from Nicotiana attenuata plantsJournal of Chemical Ecology 23:2327–2343.https://doi.org/10.1023/B:JOEC.0000006677.56380.cd
-
The post-pollination ethylene burst and the continuation of floral advertisement are harbingers of non-random mate selection in Nicotiana attenuataThe Plant Journal : For Cell and Molecular Biology 71:587–601.https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-313X.2012.05011.x
-
Sensillum types on the proboscis of the Lepidoptera: A reviewAnnales De La Société Entomologique De France (N.S.) 49:73–90.https://doi.org/10.1080/00379271.2013.767514
-
The role of mechanosensory input in flower handling efficiency and learning by Manduca sextaThe Journal of Experimental Biology 209:1585–1593.https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.02169
-
Energetics and pollination ecologyScience 176:597–602.https://doi.org/10.1126/science.176.4035.597
-
Floral scents repel facultative flower visitors, but attract obligate onesAnnals of Botany 105:777–782.https://doi.org/10.1093/aob/mcq045
-
Making sense of nectar scents: The effects of nectar secondary metabolites on floral visitors of Nicotiana attenuataThe Plant Journal : For Cell and Molecular Biology 49:840–854.https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-313X.2006.02995.x
-
Changing pollinators as a means of escaping herbivoresCurrent Biology : CB 20:237–242.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2009.11.071
-
A reference gene set for chemosensory receptor genes of Manduca sextaInsect Biochemistry and Molecular Biology 66:51–63.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ibmb.2015.09.007
-
Bumble bee learning and flower morphologyAnimal Behaviour 47:531–545.https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1994.1077
-
Chemosensory proteins from the proboscis of Mamestra brassicaeChemical Senses 25:541–553.https://doi.org/10.1093/chemse/25.5.541
-
Non-bee insects are important contributors to global crop pollinationProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 113:146–151.https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1517092112
-
Start making scents: The challenge of integrating chemistry into pollination ecologyEntomologia Experimentalis Et Applicata 128:196–207.https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1570-7458.2008.00683.x
-
Spatiotemporal coding of individual chemicals by the gustatory systemThe Journal of Neuroscience : The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience 35:12309–12321.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3802-14.2015
-
Behavioral consequences of innate preferences and olfactory learning in hawkmoth-flower interactionsProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 105:3404–3409.https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0709811105
-
Pollinator-mediated evolution of floral signalsTrends in Ecology & Evolution 28:307–315.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2013.01.019
-
A deceptive pollination system targeting drosophilids through olfactory mimicry of yeastCurrent Biology : CB 20:1846–1852.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2010.09.033
-
Odor tracking flight of male Manduca sexta moths along plumes of different cross-sectional areaJournal of Comparative Physiology A 199:1015–1036.https://doi.org/10.1007/s00359-013-0856-0
-
Silencing Nicotiana attenuata LHY and ZTL alters circadian rhythms in flowersThe New Phytologist 209:1058–1066.https://doi.org/10.1111/nph.13681
Article and author information
Author details
Funding
European Research Council (advanced grant no. 293926 to Ian T. Baldwin)
- Ian T Baldwin
- Danny Kessler
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Acknowledgements
We thank Sandor Nietsche, the EMZ Jena, and Lydia Gruber for their help with the SEM images, Jürgen Krieger for providing the Orco anti-body, Tamara Spingler for supporting the Y-tube assays and Daniel Veit for engineering the set-ups for behavior experiments. Funding was provided by the European Research Council advanced grant no. 293926 to ITB and the Max-Planck-Society.
Copyright
© 2016, Haverkamp et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 3,281
- views
-
- 695
- downloads
-
- 55
- citations
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Neuroscience
The concept that dimeric protein complexes in synapses can sequentially replace their subunits has been a cornerstone of Francis Crick’s 1984 hypothesis, explaining how long-term memories could be maintained in the face of short protein lifetimes. However, it is unknown whether the subunits of protein complexes that mediate memory are sequentially replaced in the brain and if this process is linked to protein lifetime. We address these issues by focusing on supercomplexes assembled by the abundant postsynaptic scaffolding protein PSD95, which plays a crucial role in memory. We used single-molecule detection, super-resolution microscopy and MINFLUX to probe the molecular composition of PSD95 supercomplexes in mice carrying genetically encoded HaloTags, eGFP, and mEoS2. We found a population of PSD95-containing supercomplexes comprised of two copies of PSD95, with a dominant 12.7 nm separation. Time-stamping of PSD95 subunits in vivo revealed that each PSD95 subunit was sequentially replaced over days and weeks. Comparison of brain regions showed subunit replacement was slowest in the cortex, where PSD95 protein lifetime is longest. Our findings reveal that protein supercomplexes within the postsynaptic density can be maintained by gradual replacement of individual subunits providing a mechanism for stable maintenance of their organization. Moreover, we extend Crick’s model by suggesting that synapses with slow subunit replacement of protein supercomplexes and long-protein lifetimes are specialized for long-term memory storage and that these synapses are highly enriched in superficial layers of the cortex where long-term memories are stored.
-
- Neuroscience
Complex macro-scale patterns of brain activity that emerge during periods of wakeful rest provide insight into the organisation of neural function, how these differentiate individuals based on their traits, and the neural basis of different types of self-generated thoughts. Although brain activity during wakeful rest is valuable for understanding important features of human cognition, its unconstrained nature makes it difficult to disentangle neural features related to personality traits from those related to the thoughts occurring at rest. Our study builds on recent perspectives from work on ongoing conscious thought that highlight the interactions between three brain networks – ventral and dorsal attention networks, as well as the default mode network. We combined measures of personality with state-of-the-art indices of ongoing thoughts at rest and brain imaging analysis and explored whether this ‘tri-partite’ view can provide a framework within which to understand the contribution of states and traits to observed patterns of neural activity at rest. To capture macro-scale relationships between different brain systems, we calculated cortical gradients to describe brain organisation in a low-dimensional space. Our analysis established that for more introverted individuals, regions of the ventral attention network were functionally more aligned to regions of the somatomotor system and the default mode network. At the same time, a pattern of detailed self-generated thought was associated with a decoupling of regions of dorsal attention from regions in the default mode network. Our study, therefore, establishes that interactions between attention systems and the default mode network are important influences on ongoing thought at rest and highlights the value of integrating contemporary perspectives on conscious experience when understanding patterns of brain activity at rest.